MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY, 6, 363–378
Copyright © 2004, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Why People Watch Reality TV
Steven Reiss
James Wiltz
The Ohio State University
We assessed the appeal of reality TV by asking 239 adults to rate themselves on
each of 16 basic motives using the Reiss Profile standardized instrument and to
rate how much they watched and enjoyed various reality television shows. The
results suggested that the people who watched reality television had above-average trait motivation to feel self-important and, to a lesser extent, vindicated,
friendly, free of morality, secure, and romantic, as compared with large normative samples. The results, which were dose-dependent, showed a new method for
studying media. This method is based on evidence that people have the potential
to experience 16 different joys. People prefer television shows that stimulate the
feelings they intrinsically value the most, which depends on individuality.
Reiss (2000a) put forth a comprehensive theory of human motivation, variously
called sensitivity theory or the theory of 16 basic desires. The theory borrows
heavily from the philosophical ideas of Aristotle (trans. 1953), but it differs from
Aristotle in its analysis of individuality. Previous reports on sensitivity theory addressed diverse applications such as spirituality (Reiss, 2000, in press), personality
(Havercamp & Reiss, 2003), interpersonal relationships (Engel, Olson, & Patrick,
2002), psychopathology (Reiss & Havercamp, 1996), developmental disabilities
(Dykens & Rosner, 1999; Lecavalier & Tasse, 2002), and sports (Reiss, Wiltz, &
Sherman, 2001). In this article, the theory is applied to understanding reality television. The conceptual approach of this article may be expanded someday into a
general theory of culture.
Sensitivity theory holds that people pay attention to stimuli that are relevant to
the satisfaction of their most basic motives, and they tend to ignore stimuli that are
irrelevant to their basic motives. A person motivated by a strong desire for social
contact, for example, often looks for opportunities to socialize, whereas a person
Requests for reprints should be sent to Steven Reiss, Nisonger Center, The Ohio State
University, 1581 Dodd Drive, Columbus, OH 43210–1257. E-mail:
[email protected]
364
REISS & WILTZ
with a weak desire for social contact may not even know who is holding a party
over the weekend. A person with a strong desire for cleanliness (which falls under
the basic desires for order) may notice when cigarette ashes are left in a tray,
whereas a person with a weak desire for order may not even notice when dirty
dishes are left in the sink. If we could identify the most basic or fundamental motives of human life, we may be able to connect these motives to desires to pay attention to various media experiences. This may lead to insight into why certain categories of television programs, such as reality TV, appeal to many people.
In his search for basic motives, Aristotle (trans. 1953) distinguished between
means and ends. Means are motivational only because they produce something
else, whereas ends are self-motivating goals desired for no reason other than that is
what a person wants. When a professional athlete plays ball for a salary, the salary
is only a means of obtaining whatever is eventually purchased. When a child plays
ball for the fun of it, however, having fun and physical exercise (vitality) are ends.
Aristotle urged fellow philosophers to identify the end motives of human life, because these indicate the most fundamental purposes of behavior. Under sensitivity
theory, end motives are called basic desires.
In an effort to identify basic desires, Reiss and Havercamp (1998) asked thousands of people to rate the importance of hundreds of possible life goals.1 Mathematical factor analyses of these ratings showed that the participants’ responses
expressed 16 factors or root meanings. Both exploratory factor analysis (Reiss &
Havercamp, 1998) and three confirmatory factor analyses (Havercamp & Reiss,
in press; Reiss & Havercamp, 1998) showed the 16-factor solution to basic
motivation.
In conclusion, all motivation reduces to basic motivation,2 and basic motivation
influences what people pay attention to and what they do. The sensitivity theory of
motivation offers a unique analysis of basic motivation based on what thousands of
people rated to be their most important goals and motives. The results of the initial
studies on sensitivity theory showed 16 basic desires.
BASIC DESIRES
The 16 basic desires are shown in Table 1. At first blush, the list seems to leave
out a number of basic desires, such as those for wealth, survival, and spirituality.
It is important to keep in mind that the 16 basic desires are considered to be elemental end motives. Whereas chemists have shown that all chemical compounds
can be analyzed as combinations from the Periodic Chart of Elements, sensitivity theory holds that many complex (herein called compound) human motives
TABLE 1
Reiss’s 16 Basic Motives
Motive Name
Power
Motive
Curiosity
Desire to influence (including
leadership)
Desire for knowledge
Independence
Desire for autonomy
Status
Desire for prestige (including desire for
attention)
Desire for peer companionship
(including desire to play)
Desire to get even (including desire to
win)
Desire to obey a traditional moral code
Social Contact
Vengeance
Honor
Idealism
Desire to improve society (including
altruism, justice)
Animal Behavior
Joy
Dominant animal eats more food
Efficacy
Animal learns to find food more
efficiently and learns to avoid
predators
Motivates animal to leave nest,
searching for food over larger area
Attention in nest leads to better
feedings
Safety in numbers for animals playing
in wild
Animal fights when threatened
Wonderment
Animal runs back to herd when stared
at by predators
Altruism in animals
Freedom
Self-importance
Fun
Vindication
Loyalty
Compassion
365
(continued)
366
TABLE 1 (Continued)
Motive Name
Motive
Physical Exercise
Desire to exercise muscles
Romance
Desire for sex (including courting)
Family
Order
Desire to raise own children
Desire to organize (including desire for
ritual)
Desire for food
Desire for approval
Desire for inner peace (prudence,
safety)
Desire to collect
Eating
Acceptance
Tranquility
Saving
Animal Behavior
Strong animals eat more and are less
vulnerable to predators
Reproduction essential for species
survival
Protection of young facilitates survival
Cleanliness promotes health
Nutrition essential for survival
Corresponding animal behavior unclear
Animal runs away from danger
(anxiety, fear)
Animal hoards food and other materials
Joy
Vitality
Lust
Love
Stability
Satiation
Self-confidence
Safe, relaxed
Ownership
REALITY TV
367
can be reduced to combinations among 16 basic desires. For most people, for example, the desire for wealth may be reduced to some compound of basic desires
for status, power, and saving. Readers interested in a more detailed discussion of
why certain desires are not included in the list of 16 should consult Reiss
(2000a).
Each of the 16 basic desires is thought to be universally motivating, but individuals differ in how they prioritize the 16 basic desires. Some people, for example,
are more strongly motivated by power than by curiosity, whereas others are more
strongly motivated by curiosity than by power. By definition, a Desire Profile
shows how strongly each of the 16 basic desires motivates a particular individual
(Reiss, 2000a).
When a basic (end) goal is obtained, people experience a joy (an intrinsically
valued feeling). As shown in Table 1, a different joy is experienced depending on
which basic goal is experienced. Freedom, for example, is experienced when we
obtain independence, whereas self-importance is experienced when we obtain status. Under sensitivity theory, pleasures (or joys) differ in kind. According to sensitivity theory, people seek to maximize their experiences of the 16 joys, especially
those that are most important to them according to their Desire Profile.
Basic desires imply core values (see Schwartz, 1994); we value whatever we
desire for its own sake. The logical connection between end motives and core values has been recognized since antiquity. Aristotle’s (trans. 1953) analysis of human motivation, for example, was published under the book title, The Nichomachean Ethics.
In conclusion, basic motivation influences what people pay attention to and
what they do. A new method for studying basic motivation—factor analysis of
what large numbers of people say motivates them—has shown 16 distinct basic desires. The characteristics of each basic desire include the following: end motivation, elemental motivation, universal motivation, individual differences in prioritization, associated joys when goal is obtained, and core value. According to
sensitivity theory, people behave as if they are trying to maximize the experience
of 16 joys, concentrating on those most important to them according to their individual Desire Profile.
VICARIOUS EXPERIENCE
We have the potential to experience the 16 joys as a consequence of direct or
vicarious experiences. When we watch a love movie, for example, we may experience the joy of lust, or for a war movie, the joy of vindication. Love and vindi-
368
REISS & WILTZ
cation are essentially the same emotions when we experience them as a consequence of viewing movies or direct experience. Compared with joys that result
from experience, however, vicariously aroused joys may be more short-lived, of
lower quality or intensity, and less satisfying when experienced during recall.
Sensitivity theory holds that whether we pursue direct or vicarious experience
depends on many factors, such as upbringing, culture, opportunities, personal
skills, and personal history. According to sensitivity theory, we embrace television viewing as a convenient, minimal effort means of vicariously experiencing
the 16 joys repeatedly.
As far back as Aristotle (see Taylor, 1919/1955), media theories concerned with
vicarious experiences also were concerned with cathartic purging of one’s soul.
Catharsis theories express energy models of motivation; these models predict that
vicarious experiences release psychic energy, producing reductions in relevant behaviors. Catharsis theory predicts that release of aggressive energy produces a reduction in aggression, whereas release of tension produces a reduction in anxious
behavior. In contrast, sensitivity theory is a trait model of motivation, not an energy
model. Sensitivity theory holds that aggression (which falls under the basic desire
of vengeance), and anxiety (which falls under the basic desire of tranquility), are
enduring personality traits. Under sensitivity theory, aggressive people watch violent television programs partially because doing so arouses feelings of vindication,
which are joyful, not because viewing leads to a cathartic release of tension or energy. Thus, sensitivity theory predicts vicarious arousal of joys, but not a reduction
in criterion behavior following vicarious arousal.
The results of a number of studies support the hypothesis that motivational personality traits are linked to viewer preferences (cf. Bryant & Zillmann, 2002). Researchers have shown, for example, that aggressive children are attracted to aggressive television programs (Freedman, 1984), sex-oriented people are attracted
to programs with sexual themes (Greenberg & Woods, 1999; Ward & Rivadeneyra,
1999), religious people watch religious programs (Hoover, 1988), and curious
people like to watch the news (Perse, 1992). Inconsistent results have been reported, however, on the question of whether or not viewing gratifies or satiates motives. Aggressive children who view films with aggressive content, for example,
sometimes imitate the aggression, rather than show satiation (e.g., Bandura &
Walters, 1965; Kenny, 1952).
In conclusion, sensitivity theory holds that we have the potential to experience
the 16 joys as a consequence of both vicarious and direct experiences. Sensitivity
theory does not predict cathartic reductions in criterion behaviors following vicarious experiences of joys.
REALITY TV
369
THEORY COMPARISONS
Sensitivity theory represents a variant of the “uses and gratification” approach to
media psychology. Sensitivity theory expresses the following assumptions of this
approach (see Perry, 2002): (a) media use is motivated; (b) people select media
based on their needs; and (c) media compete with other activities for selection, attention, and use. Compared with previous uses and gratification theories, however,
sensitivity theory (a) connects media experiences to the 16 basic (end) desires
shown in Table 1 and (b) does not predict that gratification leads to increased
global satisfaction. Instead, sensitivity theory predicts that gratification leads to
the experience of joys specific to the basic motive that is gratified (see Reiss, in
press).
Sensitivity theory has both similarities and dissimilarities with mood management (Zillman, de Wired, King-Jablonski, & Jenzowsky, 1996) theoretical approaches in media psychology. On the one hand, both mood management theory
and sensitivity theory hold that people are motivated to balance motivational experiences. On the other hand, mood management theory holds that people balance
positive and negative moods, whereas sensitivity theory holds that people balance
separately each of 16 specific desires.3
Sensitivity theory is not an example of a “selective exposure” theory. Although
sensitivity theory holds that people pay attention to stimuli relevant to the satisfaction of their basic desires, in social psychology selective exposure implies motivation to confirm one’s beliefs and motivation to avoid disconfirmation of one’s beliefs (e.g., Oliver, 2002; Zillmann & Bryant, 1985.) Under sensitivity theory, such
motivation falls under the desire for acceptance, which is only one of the 16 basic
desires that are connected to media experiences.
REALITY TELEVISION
According to sensitivity theory, people go through life seeking to experience 16
basic (end) goals and associated joys, and they concentrate on those that are strongest and most highly valued (which depends on individuality). Soon after a basic
goal is obtained, the desire reasserts itself and must be satisfied anew. A few hours
after eating, for example, hunger re-emerges. A vengeful person who has experienced a few days of minimal conflict may become motivated to pick a fight or argument. Because basic desires quickly reassert themselves and, thus, can be satiated only temporarily, people seek ways to repeatedly satisfy their most important
370
REISS & WILTZ
basic desires. According to Reiss (2000a), one of the purposes of culture is to provide opportunities for people to experience repeatedly the 16 end goals and joys.
We applied Reiss’s (2000a, in press) sensitivity theory to viewing television
shows. The theory suggests that individuals prefer to watch those shows that
arouse the joys most important to them. People who are strongly motivated to socialize, for example, should be especially interested in shows that portray groups,
fun, or friendship. Those strongly motivated by vengeance should be especially interested in television programs with aggressive content.
If Reiss’s theory is valid, it should be possible to develop reliable motivational
profiles of viewers of particular types of television programs. In this investigation,
we tested the hypothesis that viewers of reality-based television programs rank order the 16 basic desires in a characteristic manner that departs significantly from
normative rankings. This is a fairly rigorous test of Reiss’s theory because nearly
every random group of 100 or so people produce approximate, normative rankings
of the 16 basic desires. Further, we made the test even more rigorous by assessing
“dose-dependent” associations—that is, the more reality television shows people
watch, the greater should be the departure from normality of the viewers rank orderings of the 16 basic desires. As far as we know, this investigation represented
the first significant effort to evaluate scientifically the appeal of reality television
using standardized measures. The current popularity of so-called reality television
has drawn interest from many social commentators and from some scholars (Johnson-Woods, 2002), but few scientific studies have been reported (Nabi, Biely, Morgan, & Stitt, 2003). As the term is used here, the defining characteristic of reality
television is that ordinary people (not professional actors) serve as the main characters of the television program. Included are shows such as Survivor (Burnett,
2001), Big Brother (Eligdoloff, 2001), and Temptation Island (Couan, 2001).
Whereas some have lamented the low level of morals on these shows and the exploitation of the participants (Peyser, 2001), others have seen these programs as
appealing to the basic human quest for truth and need for genuineness (Calvert,
2000).
METHOD
Participants
The participants were 239 adults (167 women and 72 men) who were recruited
from one of two sources—seminars for 121 persons working in human service
fields such as 4H youth groups and developmental disabilities programs—and 117
college students enrolled in courses at a large Midwestern university. We asked
REALITY TV
371
these two groups to participate in this study because we had access to them rather
than because of any specific characteristic they might show. They volunteered with
the understanding that they would be asked to complete anonymously a questionnaire about what they like and dislike but could not be told the purposes of the investigation until the study was completed.
Questionnaire
The questionnaires used in this study were presented in booklets entitled “Free
Time Activity.” The booklets asked 159 questions organized into three sections.
The first section asked for demographic information, including age, sex, and state
of residence. A second section, Part B, asked participants to rate how much they
participated in and enjoyed travel, different types of travel, sports in general, specific sports, music, various types of music, and reality television. The participants
endorsed statements about how much they watched and enjoyed five different reality television shows—Survivor (Burnett, 2001), Big Brother (Eligdoloff, 2001),
Temptation Island (Couan, 2001), The Mole (Gunzo Productions, 2001), and The
Real World (Bunim/Murray Productions, 2001). The purpose of imbedding the
questions about reality television into a more general survey of leisure activities
was to disguise the investigators’ interest in reality television, minimizing any bias
or demand effects such as the participants’ desire to please the experimenter by
producing the results the experimenter is hoping to obtain.4
A third part of the questionnaire, Part C, consisted of the 128 items on the Reiss
Profile of Fundamental Motives and Motivational Sensitivities (Reiss & Havercamp, 1998). This is a standardized test of 16 “intrinsic” or “end” trait motives. A
list of the motives is presented in Table 1; they are defined in detail in Reiss (2000a,
in press). As noted already, previous research had shown a reliable factor solution
to the test in a series of studies with more than 10,000 total participants (Havercamp, 1998; Reiss & Havercamp, 1998). The results on the Reiss Profile have been
shown to predict real-life club participation (Havercamp & Reiss, in press), choice
of college major (Havercamp & Reiss, in press), spirituality (Reiss, 2000b), and
sports participation (Reiss et al., 2001). The reliability and validity coefficients for
the instrument significantly exceed those reported for many personality tests
(Havercamp & Reiss, in press).
In our research booklets, the order of presentation of Parts B and C were counterbalanced to minimize possible order effects. About half the participants completed booklets in which they reported their enjoyment of leisure activities before
completing the Reiss Profile, and about half had completed the Reiss Profile before reporting how much they enjoyed various leisure activities.
372
REISS & WILTZ
Procedure and Data Analysis
The questionnaires were completed anonymously and individually. After preliminary analysis showed no significant differences between the two samples, human
service workers and college students, and no significant differences resulting from
the order of presentation of the various questions, the data were collapsed across
these dimensions.
RESULTS
Table 2 shows mean scores on each of the 16 Reiss motives for groups indicating they
watched and enjoyed 0, 1, or 2 or more of the reality television programs included in
our survey booklets. After the data were submitted to a multivariate analysis of variance (ANOVA), which was highly significant at the .001 level, one-way ANOVAs
were calculated and effect sizes were estimated for statistically significant differences.
TABLE 2
Mean Motive Score for Three Viewing Groups
Number of Shows Liked
Motive
Zeroa
Oneb
Two+c
SD
F(2, 226)
p<
η2
Social Contact
Curiosity
Honor
Family
Independence
Power
Order
Idealism
Social Status
Vengeance
Eating
Romance
Physical Exercise
Acceptance
Tranquility
Saving
31.6
36.9
36.6
37.4
22.9
26.4
27.4
33.0
18.6
14.4
29.5
18.1
28.5
27.8
20.4
27.6
34.6
37.6
35.1
35.9
21.8
28.2
29.0
31.1
25.0
21.0
30.0
24.2
31.2
30.8
19.5
29.5
35.1
35.5
34.0
37.3
22.3
28.3
30.9
30.7
26.6
19.9
29.3
21.5
31.3
29.1
20.4
29.5
7.3
6.0
6.1
8.9
7.7
7.5
8.9
7.4
9.9
10.7
8.8
11.1
9.9
8.2
8.4
8.0
5.8
2.4
4.4
.05
.40
1.8
3.4
2.6
18.1
9.1
.06
5.5
2.1
.20
.20
1.5
.01
ns
.02
ns
ns
ns
.03
ns
.001
.001
ns
.01
ns
ns
ns
ns
.05
—
.04
—
—
—
.03
—
.14
.08
—
.02
—
—
—
—
an
= 94. bn = 48. cn = 84.
REALITY TV
373
By far, the largest significant effect was for the motive of status. The more reality TV shows a person liked, the more status-oriented was the person. The difference between the Zero and Two+ groups approached a full standard deviation,
which is a very large group difference.
The second largest significant difference concerned the basic motive of vengeance. People who watched and enjoyed reality television placed a higher value
on vengeance than did people who did not watch such shows.
Significant but small differences were reported on the motives of social contact,
honor, order, and romance. People who liked two or more reality television shows
on our list tended to be more motivated by social life, less motivated by honor,
more concerned with order, and more motivated by romance, as compared to those
who did not watch any of the reality television shows in our list.
DISCUSSION
Because this was the first study to evaluate Reiss motivational profiles of a television audience, its significance may be the suggestion of a new method of potentially productive research in the fields of mass culture and communications. Prior
to this study, efforts to describe audiences in terms of personality traits were
mostly unsuccessful. Personality tests have a powerful tendency to yield the norms
every time groups of 100-plus people are tested; consequently, they often do not
show profiles for audiences of television shows. The Reiss Profile is a new kind of
personality instrument, however, based on motivational constructs rather than on
traditional personality constructs. The results of this study showed a statistically
significant, motivational profile for people who view reality television. This encourages future research aimed at developing motivational profiles of other groups
identified by their interest in particular shows or aspects of culture.
The results of our study on reality television supported the theoretical perspective that Reiss’s 16 basic desires and values are associated with viewing and enjoying reality television shows. The results showed that status is the main motivational
force that drives interest in reality television. The more status-oriented people are,
the more likely they are to view reality television and report pleasure and enjoyment. As shown in Table 1, people who are motivated by status have an above-average need to feel self-important. Reality television may gratify this psychological
need in two ways. One possibility is that viewers feel they are more important
(have higher status) than the ordinary people portrayed on reality television shows.
The idea that these are “real” people gives psychological significance to the viewers’ perceptions of superiority—it may not matter much if the storyline is realistic,
374
REISS & WILTZ
so long as the characters are ordinary people. Further, the message of reality television—that millions of people are interested in watching real life experiences of ordinary people—implies that ordinary people are important. Ordinary people can
watch the shows, see people like themselves, and fantasize that they could gain celebrity status by being on television.
Reality television viewers are more motivated by vengeance than are nonviewers. The desire for vengeance is closely associated with enjoyment of competition (Reiss, 2000a)—in prior psychometric research, the people who said they
value and enjoy getting even with others also tended to say they value and enjoy
competition. Further, people who avoid conflict, anger, and competition may avoid
viewing reality television shows because these shows often portray competition
and interpersonal conflict.
Because reality television is widely watched, it is often a topic of discussion at
the office. It is not surprising, therefore, that sociable people are significantly more
likely than nonsociable people to watch reality televisions, although the differences are small.
The finding that viewing reality TV shows is negatively associated with the extent to which a person embraces morality (honor) is not surprising because many
reality television shows champion expedience over ethics. These differences, although statistically significant, were small.
Small, significant effects also were obtained for the value of order. This finding
suggests that people who dislike rules may react negatively to the many rules that
must be followed by the participants of reality television shows. The finding concerning romance suggests that the sexual aspects of some shows attract viewers but
not very many because the effect is small in magnitude.
Some have questioned the intellectualism of reality television viewers, and others have questioned the physical laziness of people who like to watch television.
No support was found for either of these hypotheses. Both viewers and nonviewers
were equally motivated by curiosity, and the same was shown for the motive of
physical exercise.
Although reality television viewing is generally about status, specific shows
may appeal to different psychological needs. Temptation Island, for example, portrays infidelity, which may appeal to people who value expedience (low honor)
more than morality. Survivor, in contrast, has more of a competitive theme, perhaps
appealing to people who value vengeance.
The results of this study should be interpreted cautiously. Many different shows
are classified as “reality television,” so that current or future shows may have an
appeal different from the shows evaluated here. Although the results of this study
were not affected by gender, future studies may show gender preferences in how
REALITY TV
375
viewing habits are connected with basic desires. Gender effects probably occur but
perhaps at magnitudes too small to be identified by the methods used in this study.
The results of this study are consistent with those reported by Nabi et al. (2003)
regarding the psychological appeal of reality-based television. Nabi et al. showed
that curiosity (including need for cognition) was not a significant motive for
watching reality television; the results of this study also showed no correlation between curiosity and viewing of reality television. Nabi et al. also showed that voyeurism (“getting a peek”) does not motivate viewing reality television. Because
Reiss and Havercamp (1998) and Reiss (2000a) implied that voyeurism is motivating only as a means to 1 of more the 16 basic goals, we did not study voyeurism. (It
is not a fundamental or intrinsically desired motive.) Nabi et al. reported a small
correlation between the “unscripted nature” of reality shows and impulsivity. In
our work, impulsivity is not a universal or fundamental motive (it is a personality
trait), but flexibility falls under low or weak desire for order. We found a small correlation between order and viewing. Although the shows may be “unscripted,”
rules are salient features of these shows, so that arguably any expected association
with order should be positive, not negative as Nabi et al. assumed.
In conclusion, these results supported the general hypothesis that cultural
events such as reality television shows arouse specific combinations of 16 intrinsic
feelings or joys. The appeal of reality events is influenced by the degree of match
between (a) the pattern of intensities of 16 intrinsic joys the show arouses and (b)
the individual’s valuations of the 16 basic joys (called a Reiss Desire Profile). Future researchers can study the relevance of this model for a much wider range of
television shows and cultural events. It is unlikely to work every time, of course,
but it may produce reliable empirical results much more frequently than was the
case with alternative methods.
NOTES
1Surprisingly,
the most influential analyses of basic motivation in history were put forth
without having first asked large numbers of people what their motives are. Plato’s (trans.
1966) suggestion that justice and knowledge are basic motives was based on philosophical
analysis, not on what fellow Athenians said motivated them. When Kant (1783/1953) discussed the overriding importance of moral imperatives, he did not ask large numbers of fellow Germans if “moral imperatives” motivated them. Darwin (1859/1990) held that reproduction and survival were the two most fundamental motives guiding behavior. He based his
conclusion on detailed observations of animals, not on what fellow Brits said were their
overriding goals. Freud (1916/1963) reduced all motivation to sex and aggression even
376
REISS & WILTZ
though his patients denied that that was their motivation. Even motivational personality theorists such as Murray (1938) and Maslow (1943) developed lists of basic motives without
having asked large numbers of people about their motives.
2This is true a priori (as a matter of logic given what the terms mean), according to
Aristotle.
3Reiss (in press) suggested that global concepts such as “positive mood” express logical
errors and exaggerate the motivational significance of pleasure in human behavior. Historically, the idea that pleasure motivates human behavior above all else is hedonism. According to sensitivity theory, pleasure usually is a nonmotivational byproduct of end goal
obtainment, not a motivating cause. In other words, we desire knowledge for its own sake
and seek it even when doing so is frustrating. The fun experienced at attainment is a consequence, not cause, of our having desired knowledge.
4Copies
of Parts A and B are available on request from Steven Reiss.
REFERENCES
Aristotle. (trans. 1953). The nicomachean ethics (J. A. K. Thomson, Trans.). New
York: Penguin.
Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1965). Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt.
Bryant, J., & Zillman, D. (Eds.). (2002). Media effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Burnett, M. (Producer). (2001). Survivor [Television series]. New York: CBS.
Calvert, C. (2000). Voyeur nation. Boulder, Co: Westview Press.
Couan, C. (Producer). (2001). Temptation island [Television series]. New York:
Fox Entertainment Network.
Darwin, C. (1990). The origin of species. New York: Modern Library. (Original
work published 1859)
Dykens, E. M., & Rosner, B. A. (1999). Redefining behavioral phenotypes: Personality-motivation in Williams and Prader-Willi Syndromes. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 104, 158–169.
Eligdoloff, G. (Producer). (2001). Big brother [Television series]. New York:
CBS.
Engel, G., Olson, K. R., & Patrick, C. (2002). The personality of love: fundamental
motives and traits related to components of love. Personality and Individual
Differences, 32, 839–853.
REALITY TV
377
Freedman, J. L. (1984). Effect of television violence on aggressiveness. Psychological Bulletin, 96(2), 227–246.
Freud, S. (1963). Introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. London: Hogarth Press.
(Original work published 1916)
Greenberg, B. S., & Woods, M. G. (1999). The soaps: Their sex, gratifications, and
outcomes. The Journal of Sex Research, 36, 250–257.
Havercamp, S. M. (1998). The Reiss profile of motivational sensitivity: Reliability,
validity, and social desirability. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Ohio
State University.
Havercamp, S. M., & Reiss, S. (2003). A comprehensive assessment of human
striving: Reliability and validity of the Reiss Profile. Journal for the Assessment of Personality, 81, 123–132.
Hoover, S. M. (1988). Television myths and ritual; the role of substantive meaning and spatiality. In J. W. Carey (Ed.), Media, myths, and narratives (pp.
161–178). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover. (Original work
published 1890)
Johnson-Woods, T. (2002). Big bother: Why did that reality-tv show become such
a phenomenon? St. Lucia, Queensland, Australia: University of Queensland
Press.
Kant, I. (1968). Critique of pure reason (N. Kemp Smith, Trans.). New York: St.
Martin’s Press. (Original work published 1781)
Kenny, D. T. (1952). An experimental test of the catharsis theory of aggression.
University of Washington, Seattle.
Lecavalier, L., & Tasse, M. J. (2002). The sensitivity theory of motivation and
psychopathology: An exploratory study. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, 105–115.
Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of motivation. Psychological Review, 50, 370–396.
The mole. (2001). Europe: ABC.
Murray, H.A. (1938). Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental
study of fifty men of college age. New York: Oxford University Press.
Nabi, R. L., Biely, E. N., Morgan, S. J., & Stitt, C. R. (2003). Reality-based television programming and the psychology of its appeal. Media Psychology, 5,
303–330.
Oliver, M. B. (2002). Individual differences in media effects. In J. Bryant &
D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp.
507–522). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Perry, D. K. (2002). Theory and research in mass communications: Contexts and
consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
378
REISS & WILTZ
Perse, E. M. (1992). Predicting attention to local television news: Need for cognition and motives for viewing. Communication Reports, 5, 40–49.
Peyser, M. (2001, April 16). When ‘chains of love’ become a chain of fools.
Newsweek, p. 58.
Plato. (trans. 1966). The republic (F. M. Cornford, Trans.). New York: Oxford
University.
The real world. (2001). New York: MTV.
Reiss, S. (2000a). Who am I; The 16 basic desires that motivate our actions and define our personalities. New York: Tarcher/Putnam.
Reiss, S. (2000b). Why people turn to religion. Journal for the Scientific Study of
religion, 39, 47–52.
Reiss, S. (2003). The 16 strivings for God. Zygon, 39, 303–320.
Reiss, S. (in press). Multifaceted nature of intrinsic motivation: The theory of 16
basic desires. Review of General Psychology.
Reiss, S., & Havercamp. S. M. (1996). The sensitivity theory of motivation: Implications for psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 621–632.
Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (1998). Toward a comprehensive assessment of
fundamental motivation: Factor structure of the Reiss Profile. Psychological
Assessment, 10, 97–106.
Reiss, S., Wiltz, J., & Sherman, M. (2001). Trait motivational correlates of athleticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 1139–1145.
Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Are there universal aspects in the structure and contents of
human values. Journal of Social Issues, 50, 19–45.
Taylor, A. E. (1955). Aristotle. New York: Dover. (Original work published 1919)
Ward, L. M., & Rivadeneyra, R. (1999). Contributions of entertainment television
to adolescents’ sexual attitudes and expectations: The role of viewing amount
versus viewer involvement. The Journal of Sex Research, 36, 237–249.
Zillmann, D., & Bryant, J. (1985). Selective exposure to communication. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Zillmann, D., de Wired, M., King-Jablonski, C., & Jenzowsky, S. (1996). Dramainduced affect and pain sensitivity. Psychosomatic Medicine, 58, 333–341.