Sketch Grammar Sanzhi
Diana Forker
1. Introduction
1.1. Area, speakers and sociolinguistic situation
Sanzhi Dargwa is a Nakh-Daghestanian language from the Dargwa subbranch and belongs to the
South Dargwa varieties. It is spoken by approximately 250 speakers. More than 40 years ago all
Sanzhi speakers left the village of origin in the Caucasian mountains and moved to the lowlands.
Today, the majority of Sanzhi speakers live in the village of Druzhba in the Daghestanian lowlands
(Kajakentskij Rajon). Druzhba is an ethnically and linguistically heterogeneous settlement with
speakers of other South Dargwa varieties, other Nakh-Daghestanian languages such as Tabasaran,
Agul, Lezgian, and Lak and also very few Kumyk (Turkic) and Russian speakers.
Sanzhi Dargwa, like many other comparatively small languages and varieties spoken on the
territory of Daghestan, has no official status whatsoever. It is an unwritten language that is only
used for oral communication within the Sanzhi community. In school, Sanzhi children have around
two hours of mother tongue education per week, during which they learn Standard Dargwa, which
is mutually unintelligible with Sanzhi Dargwa. Russian serves as the main language of interethnic
communication and the only language used in education, administration and more generally in the
public sphere in Daghestan. Therefore, all Sanzhi speakers know at least some Russian. Before the
arrival of Russian in the remote parts of the central Daghestanian mountains where the original
village of Sanzhi is located Kumyk served as the language of interethnic communication in the area.
Nevertheless, among the Sanzhi speakers with whom I worked nobody claimed to have a significant
command of Kumyk.
Sanzhi speakers can be broadly divided into three groups. The oldest generation (60 years and
older) grew up speaking Sanzhi as their first language. Many women of the oldest generation are
dominant in Sanzhi and have only a limited command of Russian. Men learned Russian in school,
during their military service and during work stays in Russia. Most of the speakers of the middle
generation (age 30 to 60) were born in the lowlands and thus spent their entire life in linguistically
mixed settlements. They have been exposed to Russian from very early age and actively use both
Sanzhi and Russian on a daily basis. Most children and young people (30 years and younger) still
learn Sanzhi as their first language (depending on the family constellation), but they come in
contact with Russian right from the first day of their life. At latest when they attend kindergarten
Russian becomes the dominant language. Therefore, they only have a limited and mostly passive
command of Sanzhi and prefer to speak only Russian. They will probably not pass the language to
the following generation such that Sanzhi is heavily endangered.
1.2. State of research
As an unwritten language there is no long tradition of description or analysis. Since 2012 the
language is documented and described in the project “Documenting Dargi languages in Daghestan:
Shiri and Sanгhi”, financed bв the VА foundation and lead bв Forker. Detailed information about
the project, pictures, electronic dictionaries, texts, audio recordings and other materials can be
found on the project website www.kaukaz.net. The recordings and annotated texts are available to
the public via the DoBeS archive (http://dobes.mpi.nl/projects/shiri_sanzhi/). A glossed and
translated
corpus
(around
45,000
words)
can
be
found
at
http://webcorpora.net/SanzhiDargwaCorpus/search/?interface_language=ru. A comprehensive grammar will
be published soon (Forker Accepted[b]). Topics in the morphosyntax of Sanzhi and other aspects of
Sanzhi have been treated in Forker (2014; 2016; Accepted[a], [c], [d], [e], [f]). A collection of texts
with Russian translations and a Sanzhi-Russian and Russian-Sanzhi dictionary is Forker &
Gadzhimuradov (2017).
1
2. Phonology
2.1. Vowels and consonants
Sanzhi has four plain vowels and three pharyngealized vowels of which [iˁ] is very rare and its
phonemic status needs further clarification. Table 1 shows the vowel inventory. In addition, there is
one long voаel [aː], аhich is not phonemic, but occurs relativelв frequentlв.
Table 1: The vowel inventory
Front
Central Back
High [ı], [i]; [ıˁ], [iˁ]
[u], [ʊ]; [ʊˁ]
i
u; uˁ
Mid
[ε]
e
Low
[a], [aˁ]
a, aˁ
The voаel [a] often occurs as pharвngealiгed voаel [aˁ], but [uˁ] is also relativelв common (Section
2.5.5 pharyngealization), аhereas [iˁ] is restricted to verв feа аords. The voаel [aˁ] has phonemic
status (1).
(1) šaˁm ‘candle’
qːaˁp (preverb) in qːaˁp bikʼʷij (IPFV) ‘tаitch’
šam ‘one вear old ram’
qːap ‘sack’
Table 2 displays the consonant inventory. The three series of stops are voiceless nonejective,
voiceless ejective, and voiced. The two series of fricatives are voiceless and voiced.
Table 2: The consonant inventory
Lateral
Bilabial Dental
Palatal
Velar
Alveolar
[p] [b] [t] [d] [tʼ]
[k] [ɡ] [kʼ]
[pʼ]
t d tʼ
k g kʼ
p b pʼ
[kʷ] [gʷ] [kʼʷ]
kʷ gʷ kʼʷ
Stop
[tː]
[kː]
[pː]
tː
kː
pː
[kːʷ]
kːʷ
[s] [z]
[ʃ] [ʒ]
[x]
sz
šž
x
[бʷ]
xʷ
Fricative
[sː]
[ʃː]
[бː]
sː
šː
xː
Affricate
Nasal
[m]
m
[t͡s] [t͡sʼ]
c cʼ
[t͡sː]
cː
[n]
n
[t͡ʃ] [t͡ʃʼ]
č čʼ
[t͡ʃː]
čː
2
Pharyngeal
/ epiglottal
[q] [qʼ]
[ʡ]
q qʼ
ʡ
[qʷ] qʼʷ]
qʷ qʼʷ
[qː]
qː
[qːʷ]
qːʷ
[χ] [ʁ]
[ħ]
χʁ
ħ
[χʷ] [ʁʷ]
χʷ ʁʷ
[χː]
χː
[χːʷ]
χːʷ
Uvular
Glottal
[ʔ]
ʔ
[h]
h
[r]
r
Liquid
Semivowel
[w]
w
[l]
l
[j]
j
A non-phonemic glottal stop, which is not written, occurs before word-initial non-pharyngealized
vowels, e.g. aba [ʔaba] ‘mother’, including vowel-initial words in compounds, e.g. ca-ibil [t͡saʔibıl]
‘first’ (one-ORD). The semivowel /w/ is realized as a voiced labiodental fricative [v] or as a labialvelar approximant [w]. In addition to the segments listen in Table 2, the voiceless labiodental
fricative [f] is attested in the ideophone uf b-ik'ʷ-ij ‘blow’ (uf HPL-say.IPFV-INF) and in loan words,
mostly from Russian. In older loans it had been replaced with [p].
All plain consonants occur in initial, medial, and final position. All velars and uvulars occur also
in labialized form, predominantly in syllable-initial position. Minimal pairs for two labialized
consonants are given in (2).
(2) d-elq'-ij (PFV) ‘mill’
b-iχ-ij (PFV) ‘tie, fasten’
d-elq'ʷ-ij (PFV) ‘break’
b-iχʷ-ij (PFV) ‘be, become, be able’
All voiceless nonejective stops and fricatives (except for the pharyngeal/epiglottal and the glottal
sounds) and even a number of labialized consonants also occur in tense form (geminates). The
phonemic status of tense consonants can be proven by the minimal pairs and minimal oppositions
(3).
(3) iχ-i-j ‘DEM.DOWN-OBL-DAT’
buqij (PFV) ‘run, go’
iχː-ij ‘guard, protect, care’
buqːij ‘carrв, bring’
Tense consonants and three labialized consonants (qʼʷ, χʷ, ʁʷ) are never found in syllable-final
position.
Because Sanzhi is an unwritten language, there is no official orthography. In the published corpus
(Forker & Gadzhimuradov 2017), a Cyrillic orthography has been used that is largely based on the
orthography for Standard Dargwa. The transcription used here is given in the Tables 1 and 2.
2.3. Phonotactics
The minimal syllable consists of a single vowel. Initial vowels are always preceded by a nonphonemic glottal stop not indicated in the orthography. The syllables in monomorphemic native
words are V, VC, VCC, CV, CVC and CVCC (4). In other words, syllables never have complex
onsets, but can have complex codas. The most frequent syllable is CV, but CVC is also relatively
common. The only types of superheavy syllables are VCC and CVCC with only sonorants (/r/, /l/,
/n/, /m/, /j/) and /b/ permitted in the position of the first consonant in the coda.
(4) V
CV
VC
CVC
VCC
CVCC
u ‘2SG’
qu ‘field’
at ‘2SG.DAT’
dus ‘вear’
ims ‘moth’
laˁbz ‘mortar’
a.law ‘around’
χːʷe ‘dog’
eb.la ‘in spring’
ʁaj ‘аord, talk’
irk ‘threshing board’
daˁrqʷ ‘barn’
There are no native words with syllable-initial consonant clusters. Consonant clusters in (older
loans) are broken up by insertion of epenthetic vowels between initial consonant clusters or before
them, e.g. purust'in ‘bed sheet’, ispirt ‘alcohol’.
3
2.4. Prosody
Stress is not a very prominent category in Sanzhi Dargwa. The stress is quite weak and the stress
properties of words are very hard to determine. Stress is dynamic and has no fixed position, but it is
lexicalized. Some affixes attract stress such that the position of stress in roots and in inflected word
forms of one and the same lexeme may differ, e.g. plural suffixes of nouns: qːap ‘sack’ > qːup-né
‘sack’.
2.5. Morphophonemics
Sanzhi Dargwa has a variety of phonological and morphophonological alternations that affect
vowels and consonants. Some of the processes that target vowels result from the fact that hiatus is
not allowed. A number of processes such as vowel deletion, alternation in the form of enclitics /
affixes, and degemination are syllable repair mechanisms, but others do not serve this function.
Processes affecting vowels are vowel deletion, glide insertion, glottal stop insertion, long vowels
resulting from sequences of identical vowels, pharyngealization and formation of diphthongs, and
vowel mutation. Processes affecting mainly consonants are assimilation, palatalization, gemination
(in combination with devoicing) and degemination. Labialization and delabialization have an effect
on vowels and consonants.
2.5.1. Vowel deletion (vowel syncope)
Vowel deletion is one means of avoiding two subsequent vowels at a morpheme juncture. It is
found with plural suffixes of nouns and with encliticized negative auxiliaries.
(5) šuša > šuš-ne ‘bottle’
durħuˁ > durħ-ne ‘boв, son’
biχuble + akːu > b-iχ-ub-le=kːu (N-become.PFV-PRET-CVB=be.NEG)
2.5.2. Glide insertion
Glide insertion is found only with the palatal glide j and only before certain suffixes or enclitics that
start with the vowel a, for example with the derivational suffix -al / -jal that is used to form the
numerals 2-10, 20 as well as 100, e.g. aʁʷ-al ‘four’ vs. xu-jal ‘five’ (Section 3.5)
2.5.3. Glottal stop insertion
Another means of avoiding two adjacent vowels is the insertion of a glottal stop. This occurs when
spatial preverbs and negation prefixes are added to vowel-initial verbs. In the following examples
the glottal stops are given (although they are normally not written in this position).
(6) iʔa:
uʔi:
aʔi:
iʔi:
biʔat'un < b-i-at'-un (N-stick.into.PFV-PRET)
guʔičible < gu-ič-ib-le (SUB-occur.PFV.M-PRET)
maʔisːit (Don’t take it!)(alternative: majsːit)
biʔiʁitːe
(alternative: biːʁitːe)
As can be seen in the above examples, in some cases alternative processes can be applied, namely
the formation of a long vowel in the case of a sequence of two identical vowels and the change from
i to j if the first vowel is a and the second is i.
2.5.4. Sequences of identical vowels
Long vowels can be the result of a sequence of two identical vowels or of the vowel i plus the
semivowel j. The onlв long voаels are [aː] and [iː]. The emergence of long voаels from tаo
identical vowels is in many cases optional (7) with the insertion of a glottal stop as an alternative
(6).
(7) a-ag-ur
> aːgur ‘did not go’ (NEG-go.PFV-PRET)
či-w-ig-ul=de > čiːgulde ‘вou see him’ (SPR-M-see.IPFV-ICVB=2SG)
4
(alternative: čiwigulde)
2.5.5. Pharyngealization
Pharyngealization is a frequent process that is attested with verbal and nominal affixes containing u
or a. The pharyngealization feature of verbal and nominal stems with uvular and/or pharyngeal
consonants spreads to the closest prefixes or suffixes, but not to the entire word. Only those prefixes
and suffixes are affected that start with the vowels a and u. The nominal affixes that have
pharyngealized allomorphs are the plural suffix -upːe, oblique plural suffix -a and the suffix -a
deriving actions nouns from verbs. The verbal suffixes are a variety of derivational and inflectional
suffixes.
(8) qːuˁnqː-uˁpːe ‘nose-PL’
ruˁrq-uˁl ‘boiling’ (boil-ICVB)
b-iʡ-uˁn ‘stole’ (N-steal.PFV-PRET)
baliqː-aˁ-lla ‘fish-OBL.PL-GEN’
b-iħ-aˁq-ib ‘made fight’ (HPL-fight.PFV-CAUS-PRET)
b-aˁq-aˁjaˁ ‘Hit it!’ (N-hit.PFV-IMP.PL)
With some affixes pharyngealization is optional and one can find the same inflected word form with
and without affixes that contain pharyngealized vowels (9).
(9) b-aˁħ-uˁn-ce
guči b-aˁq-aˁraj
vs. b-aˁħ-un-ce
vs. guči b-aˁq-araj
‘аet’
‘to gather’ (gather HPL-assemble.PFV-SUBJ)
2.5.6. Formation of diphthongs
The diphthongs [aɪ̯ ] and [aʊ̯], аritten aj and aw, are found in a few roots. They also arise during
certain inflectional or derivational processes. When a spatial preverb ka-, ha-, sa- or the negation
prefixes (a-, ma-) are added to verbs having i as the root vowel and consisting only of one
consonant the result is a + i -> aj (10). This process is optional to a certain degree. The diphthong
[aʊ̯] arises аhen spatial preverbs or negation prefiбes аith the final voаel a are added to verbs with
the root vowel u (b-uC-) that are inflected for masculine singular gender agreement (10). The
masculine singular prefix w- is regularly dropped before verbs with the root vowel u, and then the
combination of the two subsequent vowels results in a diphthong (10).
(10) ma-isː-it > majsːit ‘do not shave!’ (NEG-shave.IPFV-PROH.SG) (alternative: maʔisːit)
sa-(w)-uq-un > sawqun ‘he came’ (HITHER-M-go.PFV-PRET)
2.5.7. Vowel mutation (apophony)
Vowel mutation is found with inflected nouns and verbs. In the first case it is triggered by
suffixation, in the second case by prefixation. Vowels in the final syllable of nouns ending in a
consonant are raised and/or backend when certain plural suffixes containing close-mid vowels are
added (11).
(11) a > u; e > u; e > i
qːap > qːup-re ‘sack’
nez > nuz-be ‘louse’
χabar > χabur-te ‘storв, neаs’
ʁez > ʁiz-be ‘hair’
Vowel mutation with verbs occurs when the spatial preverbs or negation prefixes with the final
vowel a are prefixed.
(12) a + i > e, i + a > e, a + e > e
sa-(w)-irʁ-an > serʁan ‘the one that comes’ (HITHER-M-come-PTCP)
či-ag-ur > čegur ‘s/he аent’ (SPR-go.PFV-PRET)
ha-erʔ-ul > herʔ-ul ‘tell’ (UP-tell.IPFV-ICVB)
5
2.5.8. Assimilation
Progressive assimilation occurs with all verbal and nominal suffixes that have initial l. The liquid
assimilates to a preceding sonorant n or r. Affected are the ergative case and the genitive case, e.g. li > -ni / -ri (34) the SPR-series (spatial case), the perfective converb / adverbializer: -le > -ne / -re
(44a, b) and the anteriority / causality converb -la > -na / -ra.
2.5.9. Palatalization
Palatalization of velar consonants occurs with verbs when suffixes starting with the front vowels i
and e, or the causative suffix -aq are added (13), or optionally when the masdar suffix -ni is
following.
(13) x > š, xː > šː, g > ž, k > č, kː -> čː, k' -> č'
b-ax-ul ‘going’ (N-go-ICVB)
vs. w-aš-e! ‘Go!’ (M-go-IMP.SG)
b-ug-ul ‘remaining’ (N-stay-ICVB)
vs. b-už-ib ‘remained’ (N-stay-PRET)
b-uk-ul ‘gathering’ (N-gather-ICVB)
vs. b-uč-ib ‘gathered it’ (N-gather-PRET)
2.5.10. Labialization and delabialization
Labialization of stops triggered by the round vowel u occurs only in two instances, namely with one
verb: w-i-ha-(w)-ulq-an > wihalqʷan ‘the one that goes inside’ (M-IN-go.IPFV.M-PTCP), and when the
two spatial preverbs gu- (SUB) and ha- ‘upwards’ are combined, e.g. gu-ha- > gʷa-.
Delabialization is more widespread and predictable. It occurs when verbs that contain labialized
stem consonants take suffixes beginning with the round vowel u (14). With nouns delabialization
occurs in the formation of the plural (14).
(14) b-elk'ʷ-ij ‘N-write.PFV-INF’ > b-elk-un ‘аrote’ (N-write.PFV-PRET)
daˁrqʷ ‘barn’ > duˁrq-be
q'ʷaˁl ‘coа’ > q'uˁl-e
2.5.11. Gemination and degemination
Final stops and fricatives of nouns become tense (i.e. geminates) when the plural suffixes -e and upːe are attached (15).
(15) juldaš > juldašːe ‘friend’
baliq > baliqːe ‘fish’
Optional gemination in combination with devoicing always involves at least one gender affix (b or
d) (16). In careful speech the two consonants are pronounced individually and no gemination and
devoicing takes place.
(16) d + d/t > tː
b/p + b > pː
le-d=de
> letːe ‘аe аere there’ (eбist-NPL=PST) (le-b=de)
gu-b-b-iči-b > gupːičib ‘it lost’ (doаn-N-N-occur.PFV-PRET)
Geminates are regularly degeminated when they end up in syllable-final position because geminates
in syllable-final position are prohibited. Therefore, after suffixation that leads to resyllabification
degemination takes place, i.e. tense consonants become lax, but voicing is not affected (17).
(17) c'eltːa > c'elt-me ‘gravestone’ ečːa > eč-ne ‘she-goat’,
ha-qː-ij (up-carry-INF) > haq-ni (masdar)
6
3. Morphology: word classes and inflection
3.1. Overview
The morphology can be characterized as agglutinative with a tendency for suffixes. Prefixes are
only found with verbs in in the form of gender prefixes. Lexical classes can be identified on the
basis of their morphosyntactic properties.
3.2. Nouns
3.2.1. Gender
Sanzhi has the typical Dargwa gender system of three genders that have a transparent semantic
basis: human masculine, human feminine, and neuter. The combined gender / number agreement
affixes are given in Table 10 below. All gender markers except the zero marking for masculine
singular agreement can occur as prefixes, suffixes, and infixes.
3.2.2. Number
Most nouns can be marked for plural by means of a suffix. Plural suffixes can be divided into three
groups:
- frequent and productive suffixes: -e, -te, -be, -me (17), (18)
- relatively frequent suffixes: - re, -ne, -upːe, -urbe (11), (17)
- very rare suffixes: -urme, -rme, -ube, -de, -une, -(u)bne (8)
The first group is the only one that can be used with recent loan words from Russian. The last group
is restricted to one or two lexical items. Many of the nouns undergo morphophonological processes
before the plural suffix is added. When case suffixes are added to nouns overtly marked for plural,
then the final vowel of the plural suffix changes from e to a (15), (8).
(18) t'ult' > t'ult'e ‘bread’
rursːi > rursbe ‘girl, daughter’
bajram > bajrumte ‘holidaв’,
peč > pečme ‘oven’
There is also an associate plural formed with the suffix -qal that probably originates from the
noun qal ‘house’. The associate plural is only used with nominals that have a specific reference
such as personal names, e.g. Pajt'ima-qal ‘Patimat and the people associated with her’, terms
denoting kinship relations, and the pronoun ča ‘who’.
3.2.3. Case
Sanzhi Dargwa has four grammatical cases and 19 core semantic cases as well as one minor spatial
suffix. The grammatical cases and the comitative are given in Table 3. The 18 core spatial cases are
provided in Table 5. The essive is shown in the neuter singular / human plural form with the
gender/number suffix -b. Illustrative partial paradigms of a few nouns can be found in Table 4.
Case suffixation is (almost) completely regular and predictable. As in many other NakhDaghestanian languages including other Dargwa varieties case suffixes in Sanzhi mostly do not
directly attach to the nominal root, but are preceded by a so-called oblique marker. For nouns in the
singular the oblique marker is identical to the ergative suffix and will be glossed with OBL. All
nouns and demonstrative pronouns in the plural have -a as oblique marker (Table 4).
Table 3: Grammatical cases
Case
Suffix
absolutive Ø
ergative
-l(i) (allomorphs -ri, -ni)
genitive
-la (allomorphs –lla, -ra, -na)
dative
-j
comitative -cːella
7
Table 4: Partial paradigms of a few nouns
ABS
ERG
GEN
DAT
COMIT
vowel-final stems
‘tree’
kːalkːi
kːalkːi-l
kːalkːi-la
kːalkːi-(li)-j
kːalkːi-cːella
ABS
ERG
GEN
kːalk-me
kːalk-m-akːalk-m-a-(l)la
singular
consonant-final stems
‘donkey’
‘friend’
‘clothes’
amχa
juldaš
paltar
amχa-l
juldaš-li
paltar-ri / -li
amχa-la
juldaš-la
paltar-ra / -la
amχa-j
juldaš-li-j
paltar-ri-j / -li-j
amχa-cːella
juldaš-lipaltar-ri-cːella / -licːella
cːella
plural
umχ-e
juldašː-e
paltur-te
umχ-a-l
juldašː-a-l
paltur-t-a-l
umχ-a-(l)la
juldašː-a-(l)la paltur-t-a-(l)la
The absolutive marks the single argument of an intransitive verb S (44a), the patient/theme
argument of a transitive verb P (33), the stimulus argument of affective verbs, the agent in the
antipassive construction (38b), noun phrases in the predicate nominal function (31), and
occasionally temporal duration.
The ergative marks the agentive argument of transitive verbs (33), occasionally the instrument,
and the patient in the antipassive constructions (38b).
The genitive marks various types of relations, e.g. adnominal attributes denoting possession (46),
material (44b), ingredients, properties, etc. Sanzhi does not distinguish between alienable and
inalienable possessors. Furthermore, the genitive marks arguments of most postpositions (44b) and
it occurs in partitive constructions.
The dative marks experiencers (35), (36), causes, prices, temporal duration (for / in) and points of
time. It also fulfils functions such as addressees (though for this role the IN-lative is more common),
recipients (34), beneficiaries, and other goals of extended intransitive verbs with experiential
semantics (39a) or occasionally spatial goals.
The comitative is used with nominals having animate referents in the comitative function as well
as with inanimate nouns in the instrumental function.
Table 5: Spatial cases
meaning on (SPR)
lative
essive
ablative
-le / -ja / -a
-le-b / -ja-b / -a-b
-le-r(-ka) / -ja-r(-ka) /
-a-r(ka)
to (AD)
-šːu
-šːu-b
-šːu-r(ka)
in, among
(IN)
-cːe
-cːe-b
-cːe-r(-ka)
under
(SUB)
-gu
-gu-b
-gu-r(-ka)
in front
(ANTE)
-sa
-sa-b
-sa-r(-ka)
behind
(POST)
-hara
-hara-b
-hara-r(ka)
Table 4 provides the core spatial cases. The spatial cases are formally and functionally rather
transparent and organized along two dimensions: location and direction (movement). There are five
suffixes that express different ways of locating an item with respect to a reference point:
- SPR-series -le / ja: on a reference point (42a), (50)
- AD-series -šːu: at, by, close to an (mostly) animate reference point
- IN-series -cːe: in a reference point (52)
- SUB-series -gu: under a reference point
- ANTE-series -sa: in front of a reference point
There is a three-way distinction in terms of direction (movement)
- lative (zero marked): direction to a goal (50), (52)
- essive (marked by means of the gender/number agreement suffixes): location at a reference point
(36)
8
- ablative (-r or -rka): movement away from a reference point or movement through or along a
reference point
The direction markers can be directly suffixed to spatial postpositions/adverbs and some other
nominals that have inherent locational meaning (toponyms). With all other items direction markers
only occur in combination with the location markers. In addition to the core spatial cases given in
Table 5 there is one minor spatial case whose use is somewhat restricted, the directional -GM-a
(37a).
The spatial cases are functionally and partially also formally close to spatial adverbs /
postpositions (Section 3.8) and can be used alone or together with them. Furthermore, there are
semantic and formal similarities with spatial preverbs (Section 3.6.8).
Spatial cases are also used for non-spatial purposes, e.g. as part of valency frames for semantic
roles such as addressees (43a), stimuli (36b) or recipients, in certain constructions such as
comparison (45) or to express causees in causative constructions.
Sanzhi has a number of derivational suffixes to form nouns. The most important are:
-či: agent nouns with from nouns denoting objects or places
-dex: abstract nouns open class words (verbs, adjectives, nouns, adverbs, bound stems)
-ala: action and event nouns ‘аaв of V-ing’ from verbal stems
(19) anq-či ‘gardener’
kːuš-dex ‘hunger’
irʁ-ala ‘understanding’
< anq ‘garden’
< kːuš ‘hungrв’
< irʁ- ‘understand’ (IPFV)
Other possibilities to extend the nominal lexicon are compounding (20) and to a limited extent
reduplication.
(20) kːalkːa zize ‘raspberrв’
kːaˁta-la maˁmre ‘blackberrв’
sːurrat helt'an ‘camera’
χːulaba ‘mother-in-laа’
< kːalkːi ‘tree’ + zize ‘straаberrв’
< ‘cat-GEN’ + ‘female breast’
< sːurrat h-elt'-an ‘picture UP-take.out.IPFV-PTCP’
< χːula ‘big, old’ + aba ‘mother’
3.3. Adjectives
Adjectives can clearly be distinguished from nouns or verbs since they are not lexically specified
for gender, and they cannot take tense suffixes or other inflectional morphology reserved for verbs.
They are formally nevertheless rather heterogeneous.
As characteristic for Dargwa varieties, Sanzhi has a class of underived short adjectives that occur
in the form of bare roots as attributes to nominals, but cannot be used substantively or predicatively.
Some of these adjectives are also used in compounding, especially in compound verbs (Section
3.6.8). All underived short adjectives can take the so-called attributive suffix -ce (plural -te) and
then fulfill all three functions: attribution (21a), predication (21b) and reference (21c). This
multifunctional suffix attaches not only to adjectival roots, but also to other parts of speech such as
inflected nouns or verbs.
(21) a. wahi-ce
xːun
b-irχ-i
nišːa-la
bad-ATTR.SG road
N-be.IPFV-HAB.PST
1PL-GEN
‘Аe had a bad road.’ (or ‘There аas a bad road in our (area).’)
b.
c.
χabacːi
dik'ar
wahi-ce
Khabaci
too
bad-ATTR.SG
‘Khabaci (=personal name) аas also not bad.’
c'il
wahi-te
a-d-arq'-ij
9
akːʷ-i
COP.NEG-HAB.PST
daˁʡle …
then
bad-ATTR.PL
NEG-NPL-do.PFV-INF
‘then like in order not to do bad (things) …’
as
Another suffix with a similar functional range is -il, which is added to spatial and temporal
adverbs inflected for the essive case in order to form adjectives denoting positions, and to verbs
(42a-d).
Derivation of adjectives is not very productive. Examples are:
(22) ʡaˁq'lu-či-b ‘intelligent’ (< ʡaˁq'lu ‘intellect, mind’)
qi-m-a-r ‘horned’ (< qi-me ‘horns’)
kam-b-azi-b ‘feа, little’ (< kam ‘verв feа, little’)
A few underived adjectives have agreement markers, e.g. b-ac' ‘emptв’, b-uqu ‘вelloа’. In
addition, all adjectives derived with certain suffixes have them (22).
3.4. Pronouns
Sanzhi has personal pronouns for the first and for the second person that have suppletive stems
(Table 6).
Table 6: Personal pronouns
1SG ‘I’ 2SG ‘you’
absolutive du
u
ergative
du-l
u-l
dative
dam
at
genitive
di-la
a-la
comitative di-cːella a-cːella
1PL ‘we’
nušːa
nušːa-l
nišːi-j
nišːa-lla
nišːi-cːella
2PL ‘you’
ušːa
ušːa-l
ašːi-j
ašːa-lla
ašːi-cːella
A rich system of demonstratives (Table 7) expresses number and case, but not gender. These
pronouns express deictic reference, serve as third person pronouns, and they also occur as definite
markers similar to definite articles. The pronouns are organized along several formal and semantic
dimensions:
- number (distinction singular vs. plural)
- proximity to speech act participants (speaker, hearer, third party)
- elevation (higher or lower than a reference point) (Forker, Accepted[f])
- usage as nominal modifier vs. independent nominal
- visibility, aformentionedness, familiarity, etc.
Table 7: The demonstrative pronouns
Singular
iC heC
iž hež
ij hej
il hel
it het
hiC
hiž
hij
hil
hit
Plural
i(C)tːi
ištːi
#
iltːi
itːi
ik' hek' hik' ixtːi
iχ heχ hiχ iχtːi
he(C)tːi
heštːi
#
heltːi
hetːi
hi(C)tːi
hištːi
#
hiltːi
hitːi
hextːi
heχtːi
hixtːi
hiχtːi
this / these; close to the speaker
this / these; close to the speaker
that / those; close to the addressee
that / those; not close to speaker or hearer,
undifferentiated
higher than a reference point (the speaker)
lower than a reference point (the speaker)
The demonstratives in Table 7 serve as basis for the derivation of manner adverbials and spatial
adverbials. Partial paradigms of demonstratives, reflexives and two interrogative pronouns are
displayed in Table 8.
10
Sanzhi Dargwa has simple reflexive pronouns (Table 8) and two types of complex reflexive
pronouns. Reflexive pronouns are marked for gender (in the absolutive only), for number and for
case. The absolutive case of the reflexive pronoun is identical to the copula and might be
diachronically related to it. For all other cases the pronoun has two suppletive stems, one for the
singular and one for the plural. Both types of complex reflexive pronouns consist of a reduplicated
form of the simple reflexive. For the first variant of the complex reflexive pronouns one part of the
reflexive undergoes case-copying from the controller, and the second part takes the appropriate
case-marking. In the second variant, the first part is invariably genitive. The simple reflexive
pronouns occur in local and non-local reflexivization (including logophoric contexts) and can even
establish reference across clausal boundaries, whereas the complex reflexive pronouns can only be
bound within the clause (Forker 2014).
There are three types of reciprocal pronouns. Two of these pronouns always consist of the
reduplicated numeral ca ‘one’. Except for the genitive they fully inflect for case, but do not
distinguish gender. One type of reciprocal pronouns is the equivalent of the genitive reflexive
because its first part is always in the genitive. The second reciprocal has always one part in the
absolutive. The third variant, ca<b>a, is also based on ca ‘one’ to which a plural suffix that
exhibits gender/number agreement is added. It can also be reduplicated.
The interrogative pronouns are given in (23).
(23)
ča
ce
čina
cet'le
‘who’
‘what’
‘where’
‘how’
ceʁuna
kutːi
cel
celij
‘which’
‘which’
‘why’
‘why’
ceqːel
čujna
kusa
čum
‘when’
‘how many times’
‘how much’
‘how many’
Table 8: Partial paradigms of demonstratives, reflexives and interrogatives
demonstratives
reflexives
interrogative pronouns
‘that’
‘those’
singular
plural
‘аho’
‘аhat’
absolutive hel
heltːi
ca-w /-r /-b ca-b /-d ča
ce
ergative
hel-i-l
heltː-a-l
cin-ni
ču-l
hi-l
ce-l-li
genitive
hel-i-la
heltː-a-lla
cin-na
ču-la
hi-la
ce-lla
dative
hel-i-j
heltː-a-j
cini-j
ču-j
hi-j
ce-lli-j
comitative hel-i-cːella heltː-a-cːella cini-cːella ču-cːella hi-cːella ce-lli-cːella
Sanzhi Dargwa has a rather wide range of indefinite pronouns that are formed on the basis of the
interrogative pronouns. Most of these pronouns make use of enclitics that are also otherwise used in
the grammar as embedded question marker, emphatic particle (=k'u) or additive enclitic. The
pronominal stems are inflected like the interrogative pronouns, and then the derivational markers
are attached. Examples of indefinites are:
(i) specific indefinite (-jal / -el, -k'u), e.g. ča=jal ‘somebody’, čina-b=el ‘somewhere’
(ii) non-specific indefinite (-del), e.g. ča=del ‘somebody’, čina-b=del ‘somewhere’
(iii) free-choice indefinite (-k'a), the pronoun is mostly followed by the verb b-iχʷ-ar=ra (Nbe.PFV-COND.3=ADD), e.g. ceqːel-k'a b-iχʷ-ar=ra ‘аhenever’
(iv) negative indefinite (in clauses with negative polarity), specific indefinite, free-choice
indefinite (-k'al), e.g. čina-b-k'al ‘anваhere, noаhere, someаhere’
(v) negative indefinite (in clauses with negative polarity), universal indefinite, free-choice
indefinite (=ra), e.g. ceqːel=ra ‘alаaвs, never, anв time’
3.5. Numerals
Sanzhi has a vigesimal numeral system, and the numerals are adjective-like. They can be used
attributively and nominally, and like demonstrative pronouns and nouns they distinguish a direct
from an oblique stem. There are cardinal (Table 9), ordinal, collective, multiplicative, distributive,
and group numerals.
11
Table 9: Cardinal numerals
1 ca
11 wec'-nu ca-ra
2 k'ʷel (k'ʷi-) 12 wec'-nu k'ʷi-ra
3 ʡaˁb-al
13 wec'-nu ʡaˁb-ra
4 aʁʷ-al
14 wec'-nu aʁʷ-ra
5 xu-jal
15 wec'-nu xu-ra
6 urekː-al
16 wec'-nu urekː-ra
7 wer-al
17 wec'-nu wer-ra
8 kːaʔ-al
18 wec'-nu kːaʔ-ra
9 urč'em-al
19 wec'-nu urč'em-ra
10 wec'-al
20 ʁa-jal
21
22
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
ʁa-nu ca-ra
ʁa-nu k'ʷi-ra
ʡaˁb-c'al
aʁʷ-c'al
xu-c'al
urek-c'al
wer-c'al
kːaʔ-c'al
urč'em-c'al
daršː-al
61
71
200
300
400
500
1000
2000
1.234
urek-c'anu cara
wer-c'anu cara
k'ʷi-darš
ʡaˁb-darš
aʁʷ-darš
xu-darš
azir
k'ʷel azir
azir-lim k'ʷi-darš-lim
ʡaˁb-c'anu aʁʷ-ra
All numerals except for ca ‘one’ are morphologically complex containing a root and a
derivational suffix. Ordinal numerals are formed by adding suffix ʔib-il (allomorph -ʔubil with the
stem of the numeral ‘four’ that contains a labialized consonant) or its short variant -ʔib that
originates from the root of the verb ‘say’ (-ʔ-) plus the preterite suffix -ib. Collective numerals are
formed by adding the additive enclitic =ra to the cardinal numerals. Multiplicative numerals are
formed by means of the suffix -(j)na. Distributive numerals are formed by reduplicating the root.
Group numerals are formed by adding the suffix -GM-a to the root.
3.6. Verbs
The morphosyntactic categories of verbs in Sanzhi are person, gender, number, polarity, tense,
mood, aspect, evidentiality, and voice.
3.6.1. Stem formation
Based on their morphological make-up verbs can be divided into the following morphological
classes (i) simple verbal stems, (ii) derived verbs (using spatial preverbs, causativization), and (iii)
compound verbs. Derived verbs and compounds are treated in Section 3.6.8 below.
Simple underived stems come in pairs that can be treated as expressing the aspectual opposition
between perfective and imperfective. This opposition is found in most TAM forms and also
includes non-finite verb forms such as participles and converbs. Only some finite and non-finite
verb forms are available for perfective as well as for imperfective verb stems; most TAM forms can
be built only from imperfective or only from perfective stems. The formation of the aspectual pairs
is largely lexicalized and cannot be predicted. However, verbs can be divided into groups that
follow the same patterns. They can be distinguished on the basis of stem vowels, infix-like
segments from a closed class of phonemes and the presence or absence of a gender agreement slot
preceding the stem. A few examples of aspectual pairs are given in (24). In addition, there are a
number of defective verbs that have only one stem that is inflected for all TAM forms.
(24) it- (IPFV) / b-it- (PFV)
‘beat up’
b-irc'- (IPFV) / b-ic'- (PFV)
‘fill’
ruˁqː- (IPFV) / b-aˁrqː- (PFV) ‘educate’
b-ic'- (IPFV) / b-ac'- (PFV)
b-alχː- (IPFV) / b-aχː- (PFV)
lug- (IPFV) / d-elgʷ- (PFV)
‘thaа’
‘feed’
‘count’
The morphological structure of verbs in Sanzhi is fairly complex. There are up to five morphemes
that can precede the root and up to five that can follow it. These morphemes include first parts of
compound verbs and enclitics expressing person, tense, modality and illocutionary force. The root
can be followed by up to three suffixes; a derivational suffix for the causative that is directly
attached to the root; the other two suffixes express finite and non-finite TAM forms including
person agreement for some verb forms. To certain verb forms person or tense markers can be
12
encliticized. There are restrictions on the combinability of markers in the various slots, e.g. TAM
forms requiring person suffixes exclude the use of enclitic person or tense markers.
3.6.2. Agreement
Sanzhi has gender and person agreement. The two systems are formally and functionally
independent.
Most of the vowel-initial verbal stems and the two preverbs b-i- ‘in(side)’ and b-it- ‘away from
the speaker, thither’ have gender agreement affixes. Furthermore, the locational/existential copulas
(Section 3.6.7) and the copula-auxiliary ca-b have a slot for a gender agreement suffixes. The
agreement affixes are displayed in Table 10. The same agreement affixes, which can show up as
prefixes, infixes and suffixes, occur with all other parts of speech that agree (Section 4.2.2). The
agreement affix for masculine singular -w is always used when it occurs as a suffix. It is regularly
omitted when it occurs as a prefix to a verbal root beginning with u, e.g. ukː-unne=da (masc.) vs. rukː-unne=da (fem.) (eat.IPFV-ICVB=1SG) ‘I will eat’. It is optionally omitted when the root starts
with i, e.g. (w-)ik'-ul (masc.) vs. r-ik'-ul (say.IPFV-ICVB).
Table 10: Agreement affixes in Sanzhi
Singular First and second person plural Third person plural
Masculine w / Ø
d
b
Feminine r
Neuter
b
d
Person agreement is rather reduced with a clear opposition of speech act participants (first and
second person) vs. third person. There are a number of different sets for suffixal person agreement
that resemble each other and person enclitics (Table 11). The form of the agreement exponent varies
depending on the TAM form and not all TAM forms have person agreement markers at all. The
following verb forms distinguish person agreement (Table 11):
- suffixal person agreement: habitual present and habitual past; conditional forms; optative,
imperative and prohibitive
- enclitical person agreement: compound present and compound past, perfect, preterite, future, etc.
In the habitual present, the realis conditional and the past conditional the person suffix for first
and second persons is preceded by a stem augment vowel (i, u or occasionally a) that indicates the
valency of the verb (monovalent vs. bivalent or trivalent). Throughout this grammar the stem
augment vowel is not glossed separately but together with the following TAM suffix.
Table 11: Person suffixes in the habitual present and past and person enclitics
Habitual present (IPFV) Habitual past (IPFV) Person enclitics
Singular
Plural
Singular
Plural
Singular Plural
-a-di
-a-di
=da
=da
1 -V-d
-V-d
-a-tːe
-a-tːa
=de
=da
2 -V-tːe
-V-tːa
-i(ri), -ini
#
3
-u / -ar
Among the person enclitics only second singular has a unique marker (Table 11). For the third
person there are no person markers. Instead, the third person is left unmarked, or some other marker
fills the gap in the paradigm (e.g. the copula ca-b, which exhibits gender/number agreement or the
suffix -ne). Person agreement enclitics are widely used throughout the verbal paradigm, e.g. in the
compound present and past, in the perfect, in the preterite, in the future, etc. The person enclitics
belong to the predicative particles (Section 3.9).
3.6.3. Tense, aspect, and evidentiality in finite verb forms
13
3.6.3.1. Synthetic forms: Habitual present and habitual past
Sanzhi Dargwa has only two indicative synthetic verb forms that head independent clauses, the
habitual present and the habitual past. They are formed by adding stem augmentation vowels and
person agreement markers to verbal stems with imperfective aspectual semantics (Table 11).
The habitual present expresses habitual semantics, and future and potential future (35), (38a). The
habitual past expresses habitually occurring actions in the past (31), used in characterizing persons
(37b), when referring to occupations, etc., including the expression of future-in-the-past in the
protasis of past conditionals and irrealis conditionals.
3.6.3.2. Basic periphrastic verb forms
All verb forms consisting of a lexical verb bearing a participial or converbal suffix (and possible
other suffixes) followed by a person enclitic, the past enclitic or the copula ca-b are treated as basic
periphrastic verb forms. The copula can be replaced by the locational copulas or by other auxiliaries
to form additional periphrastic forms. The basic periphrastic verb forms can be divided into two
main groups: forms based on the imperfective stem (Table 12) and forms based on the preterite
(Table 13). The former convey mainly present time or future time reference and an imperfective
past, whereas the latter almost exclusively convey past time reference. Table 14 gives a partial
paradigm of the most important basic periphrastic verb forms for the verb b-irq'- (IPFV) / b-arq'(PFV) ‘do, make’.
Table 12: Basic periphrastic verb forms based on the imperfective stem
non-modal forms that employ the imperfective converb
compound present
+ imperfective converb
+ person enclitics/copula
compound past
+ past enclitic =de
modal forms that employ the participle -an
future
+ person enclitics/-ne
future in the past
+ past enclitic =de
+ participle -an
obligative
+ copula
obligative present
+ -ce
+ person enclitics /copula
obligative past
+ past =de
Non-modal forms: Compound present and compound past
The compound present is the default tense for conveying present time reference and widely used
in the corpus (36b), (39a). It covers various imperfective meanings such as progressive, habitual,
and continuative.
The Compound past corresponds to the aspectual semantics of the compound present (progressive,
habitual, continuative), but conveys past time reference.
Modal forms: Future, future in the past and the obligative forms
There are five forms based on the participle -an, the future, the future in the past, and three
obligative forms. They are not only formally, but also functionally closely related to each other.
The future expresses future time reference, potential situations, predictions of future situations, and
it has a modal meaning, namely the expression of obligation (37c, d).
The future in the past expresses irrealis modality, referring to situations and actions that should
have been taken place or performed in the past. It is also used in the counterfactual apodosis of
irrealis conditional clauses.
The obligative is formally and functionally closely related to the future, but it makes use of the
copula for all third persons instead of person enclitics. The meaning of the obligative and its two
derivatives (obligative present and obligative past) is usually modal referring to needs and
obligations, close to deontic necessity.
14
Table 13: Forms based on the preterite
imperfective stem
preterite
+ preterite + person
enclitic / zero
resultative (only third
+ preterite + copula
person)
preterite + perfective converb -le + X
perfect
(unattested in corpus)
past perfect
(unattested in corpus)
(pluperfect)
preterite + attributive marker -ce + X
experiential I
(unattested in corpus)
experiential past I
(unattested in corpus)
preterite + participle -il + X
experiential II
(unattested in corpus)
experiential past II
(unattested in corpus)
perfective stem
+ preterite + person enclitic / zero
+ preterite + copula
+ preterite + perfective converb -le + person
enclitics / copula
+ preterite + perfective converb -le + past
enclitic =de
+ preterite + attributive -ce + person enclitics /
copula
+ preterite + attributive -ce + past enclitic =de
+ preterite + attributive -il + person enclitics /
copula
+ preterite + attributive -il + past enclitic =de
The preterite (-ib, -ub, -un, and -ur) is the most important verbal suffix in Sanzhi not just because
it is extremely common in terms of token frequency and used as the base for a wide range of TAM
forms (Table 13) including the preterite participle, but also because it is the major indicator for
verbal inflection classes.
In principle, many verbs can inflect the imperfective as well as the perfective stem for the
preterite, but it seems that not all verbs have this possibility. There are only very few corpus
examples of imperfective verb stems bearing the preterite suffix. All other forms can be elicited, but
speaker do not seem to have clear intuitions about the meanings and context of use of these forms
and translations suggest that the forms are not truly part of verbal paradigms.
Preterite and resultative
The preterite is the default past tense with respect to form and function. It conveys past time
reference and is very frequent, especially in autobiographical narratives and in daily conversations
about past events. However, it can also occur in traditional narratives and in other narratives about
the past that are not related to the personal experience of the speaker, (33), (34).
The resultative is obtained from the third person of the preterite if the copula is added. The use of
the copula conveys perfectivity/resultativity. This form is usually not used in personal narratives,
but it is very frequent in other texts such as traditional narratives and other third-person perspective
narrations. The focus on the resulting state can lead to an indirect evidential interpretation that
becomes particularly obvious to speakers when they are asked to compare the preterite to the
perfective resultative.
Perfect and past perfect
The perfect is not particularly frequent. Its semantic range primarily covers resulting states; it
mostly occurs with verbs such as ‘sit’, ‘lay down’, ‘die’, ‘get/become hungry’, etc. that denote a
change of state.
The past perfect has past resultative meaning, such that with verbs that imply a change of state in
the patient the past perfect denotes that state that obtains at some moment in the past. It also
expresses non-firsthand evidentiality. Usually this means that the speaker concludes from an
observed result that an event has taken place.
15
Experiential forms
There are two variants of the experiential. They both involve verbal forms that function as
participles. The experiential forms have a perfect-like and past perfect-like semantics, but are
predominantly used when speakers talk about their own experiences and about situations they were
personally involved, so most of the examples contain first person core arguments.
Table 14: Exemplary paradigms of basic periphrastic forms for the verb ‘do, make’
Forms based on the imperfective stem
imperfective stem
perfective stem
compound present
b-irq'-ul=da / =de / ca-b
#
compound past
b-irq'-ul=de
#
future
b-irq'-an=da / =de / =ne
#
future in the past
b-irq'-an=de
#
Forms based on the preterite
preterite
b-irq'-ib=da / =de / ca-b
b-arq'-ib=da / =de / ca-b
resultative (only third person) b-irq'-ib ca-b
b-arq'-ib ca-b
preterite + perfective converb -le + X
perfect
(b-irq'-ib-le=da / =de / ca-b) b-arq'-ib-le=da / =de / ca-b
past perfect (pluperfect)
(b-irq'-ib-le=de)
b-arq'-ib-le=de
preterite + attributive marker -ce + X
experiential I
(b-irq'-ib-ce=da / =de / ca-b) b-arq'-ib-ce=da / =de / ca-b
experiential past I
(b-irq'-ib-ce=de)
b-arq'-ib-ce=de
3.6.4. Mood and modality
Non-indicative verb forms are imperative (8), prohibitive (43a), optative and the modal
interrogative. The imperative, optative and the modal interrogative are restricted in their use to the
second person for the first two forms and the first person for the last form.
The imperative and the prohibitive share the (partial) distinction between intransitive (-e / -u) and
transitive verbs (-a / -i) expressed through the use of dedicated suffixes. The same distinction is
found in the stem augmentation vowels of synthetic verb forms (Section 3.6.3.1) and conditionals
(Section 3.6.6).
Table 15: Optative, imperative and prohibitive
optative (PFV)
imperative (PFV)
singular plural
singular
plural
1 -ab-a
#
2 -ab-e
-ab-a / -ab-aj / -ab-aja -a / -e / -en -aj(a) / -ene(ja)
3 -ab
#
prohibitive (IPFV)
singular plural
#
-V-t(ːa) -V-tːaj(a)
#
The imperative and the optative are mostly formed from the perfective stem, whereas the
prohibitive is formed from the imperfective stem. Imperative and prohibitive are only used with
second person; the pronoun referring to the addressee can be expressed. As in other Dargwa
varieties (e.g. Icari, Sumbatova & Mutalov 2003: 98, Belyaev, In Preparation), the imperative
cannot be used when the P argument of a transitive verb is first person. The optative is used instead
(25). Otherwise the functions of the optative cover wishes, blessings, curses as, for instance, used in
greetings and other idiomatic phrases and it expresses indifference, when the speaker does not care
about a situation or event. There is also the possibility of using the bare verbal stem in the optative
function.
(25) w-at-ab-aja
du!
16
M-let.PFV-OPT-2PL
‘Leave me!’
1SG
Sanzhi has a suffix -ide, called ‘modal interrogative’ that is only used in content questions with
first person subject-like arguments of verbs of all valency classes. These questions have a modal
meaning covering possibility, deontic modality and future.
3.6.5. Negation
Negation can be expressed through prefixes or through the negative copula. In contrast to other
Dargwa varieties (e.g. Icari, Shiri), Sanzhi Dargwa does not express negation through reduplication
of the verbal stem. There are two negative prefixes a- and ma- that occur after right before
deixis/gravitation preverbs and root-initial gender markers, if there are any. The negative copula has
the root (b-)akːʷ- (allomorphs (b-)akʷ-, (b-)akː-) of which the initial vowel is dropped when it is
encliticized to a preceding predicate (=kːu and =kːʷi).
3.6.6. Non-finite forms
Non-finite verb forms are infinitive, subjunctive (i.e. agreeing infinitive), masdar, general converbs,
enclitics used for the formation of specialized converbs, conditional and concessive forms and
participles.
In principle, the infinitive (-ij) can be formed from imperfective and perfective stems but it is
almost exclusively used with perfective stems (21c). In its plain form it is used in complement
clauses and purpose clauses. In addition, the infinitive can take a number of suffixes and enclitics,
namely the attributive suffix -ce for the formation of complement and purpose clauses, various
subordinating enclitics for the formation of adverbial clauses (see below), and the dative case for
the expression of causes. Sanzhi Dargwa has another verb form, the subjunctive, that is functionally
largely equivalent to the infinitive, but shows person agreement in the second person (-itːaj / -utːaj)
and third person (-araj / -anaj) (43c). The masdar (-ni) is used for the formation of deverbal nouns
that can be used like other nominals, i.e. in the position of arguments or adjuncts, and for
complement clauses.
Converbs
Sanzhi has two general (i.e. contextual) converbs, the imperfective converb and the perfective
converb. The imperfective converb (-ul(e), -unne) can only be formed from imperfective stems and
from stems of which the aspect is not specified. It is used for the formation of the compound present
(36b) and the compound past, for adverbial clauses that express temporal simultaneity or
precedence of the event in the adverbial clause with the event expressed in the main clause (47),
(44b) and for one type of complement clause (49). The perfective converb is formed by adding the
suffix -le to the preterite participle. It is used for the formation of the perfect and past perfect, for
temporal adverbial clauses that refer to situations that taking place before the situation expressed in
the main clause or simultaneously with it (44a), and for certain complement clauses.
The functions of specialized converbs are more specific, and in their formation mostly enclitics
with specific temporal/causal meanings are employed which can also be added to other parts of
speech than verbs (Table 16). The enclitic can be a postposition or adverb or some other temporal
marker; it is attached to the preterite participle, the modal participle, the subjunctive and the
infinitive. Other ways of obtaining specialized adverbial clauses involve the locative participle and
the noun zamana ‘time’.
Table 16: Specialized converbs and subordinating enclitics
enclitic / suffix / other formant
meaning
=qːel(la) ‘when, while, because’ (47)
simultaneity, anteriority, causality
=er ‘when, as’
simultaneity
=sat / =satːin / =satːinna ‘until, before, as much
posteriority, manner
17
as, as long as’
=sar(ka) ‘until, before’
(h)itːi ‘after, because’
-la ‘since, after’
b-el-le ‘while, as long as, as soon as, until, when’
zamana ‘time’
=xːar ‘although, even if’
the locative participle -na
bahanne / bahandan ‘because of’
posteriority
anteriority, causality
anteriority, causality
simultaneity, immediate anteriority
simultaneity
concession
causality
causality
Conditional and concessive verb forms
All conditional and concessive forms are non-finite verb forms that head dependent clauses, thus
they are normally followed by a finite clause. Conditional and concessive clauses have person
agreement expressed by suffixes that strongly resemble the suffixes used in simple verb forms of
main clauses (habitual present, habitual past) (37a). These suffixes are added to the stem augment
vowels that express transitivity in case of the realis conditional and past conditional forms. For the
imperfective realis conditional and the imperfective past conditional, which are formed from
imperfective verb stems, the suffix -aχː-a is added to the stem and to it the person suffixes are
added. The conditional forms can acquire a concessive conditional meaning (‘even if’) when the
additive marker =ra is encliticized to the conditional suffixes.
Participles
Participles and functionally related forms occur in headed and headless relative clauses and partially
also in complement clauses and certain periphrastic verb forms such as the preterite, future forms
and experiential forms.
The preterite participle is formally identical with the preterite (Section 3.6.3.2), but when used as
participle it does not convey past time reference. The modal participle (-an) can only be formed
from imperfective stems. Its semantics covers modality (obligation, deontic necessity) and future
time reference (37c). However, in relative clauses the modal meaning is often absent. The
attributive markers -il and -ce can combine with the preterite and the modal participle in order to
form headed and headless relative clauses (42a-d) or for the use in complement constructions.
The formation of the locative participle (-an) is also restricted to imperfective stems. It has a
locative meaning that corresponds to the semantics of the lative case.
3.6.7. Copulas and other auxiliaries
The default copula and its negative counterpart are widely used for the formation of periphrastic
tenses (Section 3.6.3.2) and in copula clauses (Section 4.7). For all tenses or subordinate clause
types, in which the copula cannot be used, the verb b-irχʷ- / b-iχʷ- ‘be, become, occur, can’ is
employed. This verb has the full inflectional paradigm. In addition, there are four existential /
locational copulas that have a similar functional range as the copula (i.e. copula clauses with
existential / locational semantics and to a certain degree the formation of periphrastic tenses).
Table 17: Copulas
ca-b default copula, present tense
(b-)akː-u negative copula, present tense
le-b close to the speaker
k'e-b
higher than a reference point
te-b away from the speaker or undifferentiated χe-b
lower than a reference point
The verb b-už- (PFV) ‘be, stay, remain’ is used in copula clauses with evidential semantics and,
more generally, as an auxiliary in evidential constructions.
18
3.6.8. Verbal derivation (including preverbs)
Derived verbs contain spatial preverbs or are causativized. In their original spatial meaning preverbs
express location, direction and deixis/elevation preverbs and occur in the order [(location)(direction)]-(deixis/elevation)-root. They are generally optional, but there are bound verbal roots for
which the prefixed preverbs are obligatory.
Location preverbs just like the spatial cases express location and direction. All location preverbs
are identical to spatial postpositions, spatial adverbials or spatial cases (26). The deictic and
elevation preverbs are given in (27) (Forker Accepted[f]).
(26) či- ‘on’
gu- ‘under’
GM-i- ‘in, inside’
hitːi- ‘behind, after’
kʷi- ‘in(to) / to, in(to) the hands’
(27) ha- ‘up, upаards’
sa- ‘to the speaker, hither’
sa- ‘in front of’
tːura- ‘outside’
ka- ‘doаn, doаnаards’
GM-it- ‘aаaв from the speaker, thither’
Causativization is the only valency-changing derivation that exists in Sanzhi because the
antipassive construction does not make use of derivational affixes. It is a productive means of
deriving causative verbs from base verbs. It can be applied to most if not all verbs, including
intransitive, transitive and affective verbs. The causative suffix -aq is added directly to the stem
prior to TAM suffixes and the aspectual meaning is preserved.
Compounding is a very productive way of extending the verbal lexicon. The compounds consist
of two parts: a first part that can be a noun (often a loan word), short adjective, ideophone, bound
lexical stem, or, very rarely, another verbal stem.
(28) er ‘life’ + b-iχʷ- (HPL-be.PFV-)
sːalam ‘greeting’ + b-ikː- (N-give.PFV-)
ʡaˁħ ‘good’ + b-arq'- (N-do.PFV-)
ʡaˁħ ‘good’ + b-iχʷ- (N-be.PFV-)
qaˁš + k-aʁ- (DOWN-do.PFV-)
‘live’
‘greet’
‘improve, correct’
‘be, become good, get healthв’
‘cut off, cut into pieces’
The bound lexical stems form a closed class that occurs only in compound verbs. The second part
of compound verbs is a light verb. The most frequent ones are b-iχʷ- ‘be, become, can’, b-ik'ʷ- ‘say’
and b-arq'- ‘do’. The verb b-ik'ʷ- is widely used in compounds that denote verbs of speech and the
production of other sounds, but also in many verbs of movement.
3.7. Adverbs
Sanzhi has some basic spatial adverbs, but most of them are derived. Several series of deictic spatial
adverbs can productively be derived from demonstrative pronouns (Table 7). All spatial
postpositions given in Section 3.8 can also be used adverbially without a dependent NP. Some of
them have not only spatial, but also temporal semantics. Basic temporal adverbs denote times of the
day, temporal relations, etc. Deictic manner adverbs are also derived from demonstratives and other
manner adverbs are formed by means of the suffix -le. The latter suffix also forms perfective
converbs (Section 3.6.6).
3.8. Postpositions
Sanzhi has spatial and non-spatial postpositions. Some of the spatial postpositions also have
temporal readings. The majority of the spatial postpositions is widely used as adverbs and then
occurs without a dependent noun phrase. (29) shows them in the essive form. Spatial postpositions
govern spatial cases and/or the genitive. The non-spatial postpositions are given in (30). Depending
on their semantics they govern the comitative and the absolutive.
19
(29) sala-b
sa-b
hitːi-b
xːari-b
urkːa-b
‘in front’
‘in front, ago’
‘after, behind’
‘at the bottom, doаn, under’
‘betаeen, in the middle’
(30) b-alli
bahanne / bahandan
q'atːin(na)
qari-b
b-i-b
šːule-b
či-b
tːura-b
‘together, аith’
‘because of’
‘for the sake of, because of’
canille
akːʷar
ħaˁsible
‘at the top, above, on, about’
‘in(side)’
‘aside, neбt to’
‘on’
‘outside’
‘together, аith’
‘аithout, eбcept, apart’
‘according to’
3.9. Minor classes
Sanzhi does not have native conjunctions. The main way of conjoining phrases is the use of the
additive enclitic, and at the clause level converbs are employed (Section 4.6). Major discourse and
modal enclitics are the additive enclitic =ra, the and the modal enclitics =q'al (Forker Accepted[d]),
=q'ar and =n(u). Focus-sensitive enclitics with a more specific semantics are, for instance, =cun
‘only’, =gina ‘alone, only’. The genitive reflexive pronouns cinna (singular) and čula function as
pause fillers, and =kːʷa is a politeness particle. Common address particles are ja, wa ‘ey, hey’, haj
‘hi, oh’, (informal answer to greeting and astonishment), ma ‘take’, and hara ‘come, go, here is’.
In recent studies of Dargwa varieties researchers have introduced the term ‘predicative particles’
to refer to a closed class of grammatical elements that fulfill the functions of copula-like auxiliaries
(e.g. Sumbatova & Mutalov 2003, Kalinina & Sumbatova 2007, Sumbatova & Lander 2014). This
means that they function as heads of nominal predicate clauses and similar clauses that do not
contain other verbs, and they are used in analytic verb forms together with non-finite verb forms in
order to form full main clauses. In other words, they are responsible for the finiteness of certain
clauses, and their use depends on the clause type and the TAM form. Table 18 presents the
predicative particles of Sanzhi. They are enclitics because they cannot form their own phonological
word. They always need a host to which they attach, but unlike suffixes they can be added to
various parts of speech or phrase types, i.e. to verbs, but also to nominals (NPs), adjectives, or
adverbs. Due to this freedom in host selection they can be used in term focus constructions (Section
4.8). However, most commonly they occur in the position in which auxiliary verbs in periphrastic
verb form occur, namely following the lexical verb. In copula clauses they are normally attached to
the head of the predicate (Section 4.7). They partially express verbal categories such as person or
tense. But they are not verbs themselves.
Table 18: Predicative particles
Particle
Gloss
=da
1SG, 1PL,
2PL
=de
2SG
=de
PST
=q'al
MOD
=e / =ja
Q
=w / =uw / =ew / =aw
Q
=l / =jal / =el
INDQ
Function
person enclitic for first person singular and plural, second
person plural (Section 3.6.2)
person enclitic for second person singular (Section 3.6.2)
past tense marker
modal particle (Section 3.9)
marker for content questions (Section 4.3)
marker for polar questions (Section 4.3)
marker for embedded questions (Section 4.4.2)
4. Syntax
4.1. Noun phrase and postpositional phrase
Noun phrases are head-final (35), (41) but quantifiers and genitives occasionally occur after the
head noun (21a). Within the noun phrase there is gender and number agreement. Main targets for
gender / number agreement at the phrase level are a handful of adjectives and adjectival quantifiers
20
that have agreement affixes, and participles. Number agreement without gender agreement is found
with demonstrative pronouns.
Sanzhi does not have a special class of articles. Instead, demonstrative pronouns (Section 3.4) and
the numeral ca ‘one’ can be used in the function of definite and indefinite articles respectivelв, but
often their interpretation is ambiguous between definite article and demonstrative pronoun or
indefinite article and numeral.
Postpositional phrases consist of a postposition that is preceded by a noun phrase and thus are
always head-final (44b).
4.2. Clause structure
4.2.1. Word order
The constituent order in main clauses is basically free in the sense that every logically possible
order can be found in texts and can easily be elicited. The most frequent order is SOV (35), but
SVO is also very common (33), and other order are found as well (33), (34). Verb-initial orders
(VSO and VOS) are rare, but not ungrammatical. The pragmatically neutral order in which none of
the constituents is particularly emphasized is SV in intransitive clauses and SOV/SVO in transitive
clauses including elicited sentences without any context. In subordinate clauses, verb-final order is
almost obligatory (44a, b) (49).
4.2.2. Case and agreement in simple clauses
Valency patterns
Valency patterns of predicates can be divided into one-place (monovalent), two-place (bivalent) and
three-place patterns (Table 19). Cases used to encode the arguments are absolutive, ergative, dative,
and to a lesser extent genitive and spatial cases such as the ANTE-ablative or the IN-lative.
Table 19: Valency classes
subject-like argument/valency
absolutive
dative
ergative
monovalent
intransitive
monovalent affective
one verb only (b-us-)
bivalent
extended intransitive
bivalent affective
transitive
trivalent
#
#
ditransitive
Table 20 provides an overview about the major valency classes. All verbs in the table are given in
the order imperfective / perfective if they have two stems.
Table 20: Major valency classes
Case marking
# of
patterns
arguments
intransitive (absolutive)
S-ABS
one
Predicates and examples
b-ubk'- / b-ebk'- ‘die’; či-r-ha-b-ulq- / či-r-ha-b-uq- ‘vomit’
(31)
monovalent affective verbs (dative)
S-DAT
one
ʡaˁħ-le ca-b ‘feel good, be аell’; c'aχ-ka-b-ircː- / c'aχ-ka-bicː- ‘feel ashamed’ (36)
extended intransitive (absolutive + dative / spatial case)
A-ABS, P-DAT
two
kːač-b-irk- / kːač-b-ik- ‘touch’, gu-lik'- ‘listen to’ (39a)
A-ABS, P-IN-lative/- two
b-ik'ʷ- ‘talk to’; xʷit'-b-ik'ʷ- ‘аhistle at’ (32)
DAT
A-ABS, P-ANTEtwo
uruχ-b-irχʷ- / uruχ-b-iχʷ- ‘get/be afraid of’; uruc-b-irχʷ- /
ablative
uruc-b-iχʷ- ‘be/get ashamed / embarrassed of’ (36b)
bivalent affective verbs (dative + absolutive / other)
A-DAT, P-ABS
two
či-b-ig- / či-b-ag- ‘see’; b-irʁ- / b-arʁ- ‘understand’ (35)
21
A-DAT, P-ANTEtwo
c'aχ-le ca-b ‘to feel/be ashamed in front of’; b-irt'- / b-et'ablative
‘long for, miss’
transitive (ergative + absolutive)
A-ERG, P-ABS
two
b-irc- / b-ic- ‘sell’; b-urχ- / b-arχ- ‘seа’ (33), (38a, b)
other bivalent verbs (ergative + dative)
A-ERG, P-DAT
two
b-aˁq- / b-uˁrq- ‘hit’; zaˁnʁ d-aˁq- / zaˁnʁ d-uˁrq- ‘phone’
ditransitive (ergative + absolutive + other)
Aditr-ERG, T-ABS, G- three
lukː- / b-ikː- ‘give’; či-b-iž-aq- / či-b-až-aq- ‘shoа’; haʔ- /
DAT/-IN-lative
herʔ- ‘saв, tell’; b-urs- ‘saв, tell’; xar-b-irʁ- / xar-b-eʁ- (34)
Monovalent verbs have three possibilities for marking their single argument (Table 19). The
majority of the monovalent verbs assign the absolutive case (31), though dative or ergative are also
possible.
(31) χalq'
mic'ir
b-irχʷ-i
people
alive
HPL-be.IPFV-HAB.PST.3
‘The people staвed alive.’
Extended intransitive verbs are two-place predicates that, in addition to an argument in the
absolutive have a further argument in the dative or another case (32).
(32) du
at
r-iχči-a-arg-ud
1SG
2SG.DAT
F-believe-NEG-go.IPFV-PRS.1
‘I (fem.) do not believe (in) вou.’ (E)
Simple transitive verbs form the major class of bivalent predicates (33).
(33) du-l
a-b-iʡ-uˁn=da
qːarqːa. ča-k'al
1SG-ERG NEG-N-steal.PFV-PRET=1 stone
who-INDEF
a-kax-ub=da
NEG-kill.PFV-PRET=1
‘I did not steal the stone. I did not kill anвone.’
du-l
1SG-ERG
Extended transitive (i.e. ditransitive) verbs are three-place verbs that besides having two
arguments in the ergative, and absolutive, have an additional argument marked with the dative or in
some other way. Person agreement is controlled by the absolutive or the ergative argument, never
by the third argument.
(34) dam
b-ičː-ib
iž
1SG.DAT
N-give.PFV-PRET this
‘The snake gave it to me.’
maˁlʡuˁn-ni
snake-ERG
Affective predicates are a clear-cut class of mostly experiential predicates that express the
experiencer argument in the dative and the stimulus argument, if there is one, in the absolutive (35),
(36a).
They follow the same agreement rules as intransitive and transitive verbs
(35) dam
ʡaˁħ
1SG.DAT
good
‘I knoа a good place.’
musːa
place
b-alχ-ad
N-know.IPFV-PRS.1
22
Gender/number agreement
Gender/number agreement is a pervasive feature of Nakh-Daghestanian languages including
Sanzhi. Agreement targets at the clausal level are all verbs with agreement affixes (e.g. most vowelinitial simple verbs, and others) including all copulas, reflexive pronouns in the absolutive and one
reciprocal pronoun, all items that can be inflected for the essive case and all items that inflect for
the directional, i.e. mostly spatial adverbs. Agreement affixes are given in Table 10. At the clausal
level, gender agreement is controlled in most of the cases by the absolutive argument of the clause
(31), (33), (35). However, certain verb forms allow the ergative or the dative argument as controller,
and other clauses lack absolutive arguments and resort to default agreement (36a).
Person agreement
Person agreement, which works completely independent of gender agreement, is only found with a
subset of inflectional forms, mostly verb forms in finite main clauses and conditional forms (Table
11 shows a selection). With monovalent predicates and extended intransitive predicates person
agreement is always controlled by the argument in the absolutive, if there is any (31), (36b).
Otherwise third person agreement is used (36a).
(36) a. dam
wahi-l
1SG.DAT
bad-ADVZ
‘I feel bad there.’
b.
ca-b
COP-N
heχ-tːu-b
DEM.DOWN-LOC-N
“xːunul-li-sa-r
uruχ
Ø-ik'-ul=de=w”,
woman-OBL-ANTE-ABL fear
M-say.IPFV-ICVB=2SG=Q
ca-w,
“u?”
COP-M
2SG
‘“Are вou afraid of вour аife?” he saвs.’
Ø-ik'-ul
M-say.IPFV-ICVB
In clauses with transitive, ditransitive and bivalent affective predicates, person agreement follows
the rule 1, 2 > 3. This means that speech act participants control the agreement if the clause has any
(37a-d). If there are only first and second person arguments, in principle, both persons can control
agreement independently of their grammatical role, although there might be a small tendency for
second person arguments to outrank first person arguments.
(37) a. Realis conditional 2 > 3
wot
tak, het
hetːu-b-a
sa-qː-itːel
well so
that there-N-DIR HITHER-carry.PFV-COND.2SG.PRS
‘like this, if (вou) put this here, in the middle’
b.
c.
d.
Habitual past 3 > 1
šːamχal
acːi-l
Shamxal
uncle-ERG
‘Uncle Shamkhal led me (fem.).’
r-ik-a-di
F-lead.IPFV-HAB.PST-1
2 > 1, agent controls agreement
u-l
du
kʷi
2SG-ERG
1SG
in.the.hands
‘You аill keep me (masc.) in the hands.’
2 > 1, patient controls agreement
u-l
du
kʷi
2SG-ERG
1SG
in.the.hands
‘You аill keep me (masc.) in the hands.’
23
urc-an=da
keep.M.IPFV-PTCP=1
urc-an=de
keep.M.IPFV-PTCP=2SG
urkːa
between
Most non-finite verb forms such as converbs and participles do not inflect for person. The only
exceptions are conditional and concessive forms (37a) and the subjunctive (43c).
4.2.3. Changing case and agreement in simple clauses
The major ways of changing the valency classes of predicates (and consequently case marking and
agreement) are detransitivization in the antipassive construction and transitivization in the causative
construction.
The antipassive is formed by reversing the case marking of A and P in a clause with a canonical
transitive predicate (38a, b). The verb remains unmarked, but the gender/number agreement on the
verb changes.
(38) a. it-i-l
kːurtːi
3SG-OBL-ERG dress
‘S/he seаs a dress.’
b.
b-urχu
N-sew.IPFV
it
kurtːi-l
r-urχu
3SG
dress-ERG
F-sew.IPFV
‘She is a dressmaker.’ or ‘She habituallв seаs dresses.’
Syntactically, the antipassive is a detransitivizing operation. However, the use of antipassives is
semantically rather than syntactically motivated. It has habitual semantics which is typical for
antipassives (Comrie et al. Accepted). Apart from being restricted to only one predicate class,
canonical transitive verbs, the antipassive is additionally constrained in some other ways. Only a
limited number of TAM forms such as the compound present, the habitual present and the habitual
past allow for it. Transitive verbs for which it is unclear what the result of the action that they
denote would be do not form the antipassive. It is not available with first or second person patients.
It is impossible for A and P to be both animate or both inanimate. The last two constraints are not
really syntactic in nature since the resulting clauses are grammatical. However, the meaning would
not be what is intended. The outcome is simply a normal clause in which A and P have been
reversed.
In the majority of cases, causativization (suffix -aq) adds one argument to the valency frame of
the base verb, i.e. intransitive verbs become transitive (41) and transitive verbs become ditransitive.
Causativization normally applies only once to the verbal stem, but in elicitation it can also be added
twice to a small number of verbs.
4.2.4. Reflexive and reciprocal constructions
In reflexive constructions the reflexive pronouns refer only to third person. For first and second
person reflexivization personal pronouns are used. The controller of a reflexive pronoun in a clausebound reflexive construction can be A thereby taking various case suffixes (absolutive, ergative,
dative). The reflexive pronoun occurs as P (39a, b) or in another semantic role.
(39) a. Rašid
ca-w
cin-i-j
Rashid REFL-M REFL-OBL-DAT
‘Rashid is looking at himself.’
b.
Rasul-li
cin-ni
Rasul-ERG
REFL-ERG
‘Rasul is praising himself.’
er-či-w-ik'-ul
look-SPR-M-look.at.IPFV-ICVB
ca-w
REFL-M
gap.w.irq'-ul
praise.M-ICVB
24
ca-w
COP-M
ca-w
COP-M
However, the controller as well as the pronoun can switch places in some positions, namely A vs.
P with As of canonical transitive and experiential predicates. This means that the case marking of
controller and controlee is flexible in such cases (40). There are a few restrictions on the position of
the reflexive pronoun, but in general the position is quite free, i.e. it can also precede the controller
(Forker 2014). The case switch is impossible with extended intransitive predicates.
(40) Rasul ca-w
cin-ni
gap.w.irq’-ul
ca-w
Rasul REFL-M REFL-ERG
praise.M-ICVB
COP-M
‘Rasul is praising himself.’ (PABS=name; AERG=reflexive pronoun)
Reciprocal pronouns are very similar to complex reflexive pronouns in form as well as in
morphosyntactic behavior including the possibility of reversing the case marking. They are always
locally bound.
4.3. Major Sentence Types
There is no special marking for declarative clauses. Only finite verb forms can serve as heads and
negation is expressed through suffixes or negative forms of the copula (Section 3.6.5).
Interrogative clauses are mainly marked by interrogative enclitics. There are three sets of
interrogative enclitics for the three types of interrogative clauses: polar, content and subordinate.
The interrogative enclitics are usually added to the predicate (verbal or nominal), and in content
questions they can also be added to the question word. They belong to the class of predicative
particles and are given in Section 3.9. This means that they can co-occur with certain non-finite
verb forms in analytic tenses and turn the verb plus enclitic complex into a finite verb form that
heads a main clause. Thus, in many questions there is no copula, auxiliary or other predicative
particle (person enclitic, past enclitic), but only a non-finite lexical verb and an interrogative
enclitic. Content questions contain interrogative pronouns (Section 3.4) and the interrogative
enclitic.
The constituent order in questions is the same as in declarative clauses (41), (36b). In content
questions the topical items mostly precede the interrogative pronoun and the verb appears in clausefinal position. It is also possible but rather uncommon to put the interrogative pronoun in clauseinitial position.
(41)
bek'
akːʷ-ar
admi
celij
w-i-ha-w-q-aq-un=da=ja
head
be.NEG-PRS person why
M-IN-UP-M-go.PFV-CAUS-PRET=1=Q
nušːa-li?
1PL-ERG
‘Аhв did аe send a man аithout a head there into (the cave)?’
Subordinate questions are marked with an enclitic that is also used as a complementizer with
verbs of speech and cognition (43b), and for the formation of specific indefinite pronouns (Section
3.4).
4.4. Complex sentences
4.4.1. Relative clauses
Sanzhi Dargwa uses participles for the formation of relative clauses. As other modifiers, relative
clauses normally precede the head (Section 4.1). There are three simple participles that consist of a
suffix added to the verbal stem (preterite participle, modal participle, and locative participle), and
complex participles that make use of additional attributive suffixes.
Relativization does not single out any syntactic position or grammatical relation, because
basically almost all positions can be relativized by making use of the participial strategy, including
S, A, P, stimulus, theme, goal, etc.
25
kːalkːi-le-r
či-r
kajč-ib-il]
ABS
tree-SPR-ABL
on-ABL fall.M-PRET-PTCP
‘the boв аho fell from the tree’ (S)
(42) a. [_
durħuˁ
boy
t'amsːa
b-arq'-ib-il]
ERG
carpet
N-make.PFV-PRET-PTCP
‘the girl аho made the carpet’ (A)
rursːi
girl
b. [_
c.
[rursːi-l
_
b-arq'-ib-il]
girl-ERG
ABS
N-make.PFV-PRET-PTCP
‘the carpet made bв the girl’ (P)
t'amsːa
carpet
d.
[it
_
er-či-w-erč'-ib-il]
3SG
DAT
look-SPR-M-look.PFV-PRET-PTCP
‘the girl that he looked at’ (goal)
rursːi
girl
Relative clauses can have a head or be headless. The head is normally a common noun, but it can
also be a personal pronoun, an indefinite pronoun, a demonstrative pronoun, or a personal name.
Thus, relative clauses can be restrictive or non-restrictive without any difference in their
morphosyntactic form. Within the relative clause the head is usually indicated by a gap. Sanzhi does
not have relative pronouns. Reflexive pronouns can, in principle, used to express co-reference
between an item in the main clause and another item in the relative clause.
4.4.2. Complementation
Complementation strategies vary according to the semantics of the complement-taking predicates
and the semantics of the complement clause (e.g. potential vs. activity vs. fact type) as well as coreference and control relations between the subject of the matrix predicate and the arguments in the
complement clause. Major complementation strategies are:
zero strategy
quotative particles bik’ul, haʔible (43a)
attributive markers -ce and -il
masdar -ni
perfective converb -le
embedded question marker (=l / =jal / =el) (43b)
infinitive -ij (21c) / subjunctive (43c)
(43) a. xːunul-li
woman-ERG
tiladi
request
b-arq'-ib
N-do.PFV-PRET
ca-b
COP-N
hel-i-cːe
that-OBL-IN
“ma-ax-utːa”
r-ik'-ul
PROH-go-PROH.SG
F-say.IPFV-ICVB
‘His аife begged him "Do not go!"’
b.
aχːu
not.know
dam
1SG.DAT
[ce-lla
what-GEN
qari-či-w
on.top-SPR-M
Ø-ik'-ul=el]
M-say.IPFV-ICVB=INDQ
‘I don’t knoа about аhat he is thinking.’
c.
ca-b=ra
[kaxʷ-araj]
a-b-iχ-ub
26
pikri
thought
REFLHPL=ADD
NEG-HPL-be.able.PFV-PRET
kill.PFV-SUBJ.3
‘Theв themselves аere not able to kill them.’
The number of verbal categories expressed depends on the complementation strategy. Zeromarked complements and those bearing the embedded question marker or containing quotative
particles express the same number of categories as main clauses, i.e., person marking, TAM
marking and illocutionary force marking is fully retained. For all other strategies (converbs,
attributive-markers, infinitive, subjunctive, masdar, etc.) the number of categories expressed in the
complement clause is smaller than in the main clause. The constituent order in complement clauses
is more frequently verb final than in main clauses, but this is not a strict requirement. Sanzhi has
complement control constructions with obligatory subject omission in the complement clause if the
latter is headed by the infinitive or subjunctive (43c). In case of co-referent arguments, the overt
argument normally occurs in the matrix clause.
4.4.3. Adverbial clauses
Sanzhi has different types of adverbial clauses that can be distinguished according to the semantics
and the morphological make-up of the verb forms in the subordinate clause. Semantically, we can
distinguish between simple converbs with a fairly general meaning (imperfective and perfective
converb) and specialized converbs with a rather specific temporal or non-temporal meaning (Table
16). To this conditional and concessive forms are added.
The syntactic characteristics of constructions with general converbs have repeatedly been
discussed because they exhibit a mixed behavior showing features of subordination as well as of
coordination (see, e.g., Kazenin & Testelets 2004, Creissels 2010, 12; Forker 2013). Sentences in
Sanzhi can be fairly complex containing a number of adverbial clauses that are combined with one
main clause. Semantically, these clauses either resemble coordination as in (44a) or subordination
when the meaning is more causal (44b).
(44) a. [amχa
donkey
b-arcː-ur-re]
N-get.tired.PFV-PRET-CVB
[ka-b-ič-ib-le]
DOWN-N-occur.PFV-PRET-CVB
b-ebč'-ib
N-die.PFV-PRET
ca-b
COP-N
‘The donkeв got tired, fell doаn and died.’
b.
[bahsar
first
ʡaˁbal
three
heχ
DEM.DOWN
qal-la
house-GEN
cin-na
REFL.SG-GEN
xːari
down
či-ka-d-irxː-ul]
SPR-DOWN-NPL-put.IPFV-ICVB
atːa-la
father-GEN
jurt-la
house-GEN
k-ag-ur-re]
DOWN-go.PFV-PRET-CVB
[ha-d-iqː-ul]
UP-NPL-carry.IPFV-ICVB
qːarqːa
stone
[qːaq-li-j
back-OBL-DAT
qːarqːa=ra
stone=ADD
gu-r-h-aqː-ib=da
DOWN-ABL-UP-carry-PRET=1
‘First (because) the stones of father's house had fallen down three floors (? houses), we
put them on the back and carried them, carried the stones.’
27
4.5. Comparative constructions
In comparative constructions, inequality between two items is expressed by means of the SPRablative that is added to the standard of comparison. Neither the comparee nor the parameter of
comparison bears any special marking.
(45) atːa-ja-r
χːula-te=ra
father-SPR-ABL big-ATTR.PL=ADD
‘There аere those older than father.’
b-irχ-i
HPL-be.IPFV-HAB.PST.3
4.6. Co-ordination and chaining
For the coordination of noun phrases the additive enclitic =ra is encliticized to the head of every
member of the conjunction. Sanzhi Dargwa does not have native words or strategies for the
coordination of independent clauses, except for simple juxtaposition. Instead, the main way of
combining clauses such that they are semantically equivalent to coordinated clauses in European
languages is the use of simple converbs, predominantly of the preterite converb, but the
imperfective converb is also possible (44a), (44b). Alternatively, juxtaposition is attested or the use
of the Russian coordinator i ‘and’. The particle ja(ra) is used as a bisyndetic particle in clause initial
position. In affirmative clauses it means ‘either … or’ (not attested in the corpus) and in negative
clauses it means ‘neither …nor’. For the disjunctive coordination of affirmative clauses, the Russian
disjunction ili is used.
4.7. Non-verbal predication
Items used as copulas are the default copula ca-b, four locational copulas (Section 3.6.7), all
predicative particles (Section 3.9), and a number of other auxiliaries. Which copula item is chosen
depends on the person of the copula subject, on the meaning of the copula construction, on the
temporal reference of the clause and on further categories such as evidentiality, illocutionary force,
and clause type. Copula constructions can express identity, group membership, attribution (31),
(36a), (45), possession (46), benefaction, and also location and existence. The copula subject is
always in the absolutive case. It may be any kind of noun phrase (common noun, proper name,
pronoun, etc.) or nominalized clause. The copula complement, which bears the absolutive case or
any other case, may be a noun phrase, but it may also be an adjective, an adverbial, a postpositional
phrase or a nominalized clause (46).
4.8. Information structure
The information structure of clauses can be manipulated through word order, clefts, other
constructions such as predicate doubling, focus-sensitive particles and other particles (Section 3.9),
and intonation (Forker & Belyaev 2016). Focal items frequently appear in the preverbal position,
but postverbal focus is also attested. Topical items occur at clausal boundaries, mostly in clauseinitial position, though clause-final topics are also found. In cleft constructions that express term
focus, the focal item in the cleft is usually followed by an item serving as copula and a participial
clause expresses the presupposition (Forker 2016) (46).
(46) [ala
mašin] ca-b
[zaja b-iχ-ub-il],
ʡaˁli-la
2SG.GEN car
COP-N spoil N-be.PFV-PRET-PTCP Ali-GEN
‘It is вour car that is spoiled, not Ali’s.’ (E)
akːu
be.NEG
5. Lexicon
The Sanzhi lexicon is composed of items inherited from Proto-Dargwa and of numerous loans that
mostly originate from Turkic, Arabic, Persian and nowadays form Russian. A simple RussianSanzhi and Sanzhi-Russian dictionary can be found in Forker & Gadzhimuradov (2017). An
28
electronic dictionary is currently under preparation. A preliminary version can be found at
http://www.kaukaz.net/cgi-bin/blosxom.cgi/english/dargwa/dict_exp.
6. Sample text
This is a famous anecdote among the Sanzhi people who has been told to me by several speakers.
This version originates from my main language assistant, Gadzhimurad Gadzhimuradov.
С
я
,
ъ
Ц
ЦӀ
Ӏ
ь
ь
ь
Ӏ
,-
ъ , я
.
ь .
я.
ь Ӏ
,
,
,
я
Ӏ
Ӏя
Ӏ
ь
.
.
- ь
.
, ь
,
я.
zija
horsefly
The horsefly
(47) [sunglan-te
Sanzhi-PL
b-ah
HPL-owner
b-ircː-an=qːel]
N-bathe.IPFV-PTCP=when
[zija-me
horsefly-PL
q'ac'
bite
d-ik'-ul]
ʡaˁsi
b-iχ-ub
ca-b
NPL-say.IPFV-ICVB
angry
HPL-be.PFV-PRET
COP-HPL
‘Аhen the Sanгhi people аere bathing, theв got angrв from the bites of the horseflies.’
(48) ag-ur
ca-b
[k-aqː-araj
go.PFV-PRET COP-HPL DOWN-carry-SUBJ.3
‘Theв аent to take weapons.’
(49) b-aʔ
ašː-ib
HPL-begin
begin.PFV-PRET
‘Theв began to kill them.’
ca-b
COP-HPL
tupang-e]
weapon-PL
[hel-tːi
that-PL
(50) ca-lla
antːa-le
či-ka-b-iž-ib
one-GEN forehead-SPR SPR-DOWN-N-sit.PFV-PRET
‘A horseflв sat doаn on the forehead of one (man).’
(51) [il-i-l
that-OBL-ERG
cin-na
REFL.SG-GEN
antːa-le
forehead-SPR
t'up
finger
kerx-ul]
kill.IPFV-ICVB
ca-b
COP-N
zija
horsefly
či-b-aršː-ib-le]
SPR-N-direct.PFV-PRET-CVB
cara-lli-cːe
ik'-ul
ca-w
“hara,
hara,
zija”
other-OBL-IN
say.IPFV-ICVB
COP-M
here
here
horsefly
‘He points with his finger on his forehead and says to the other man, “Here is the horseflв.”’
(52) [itilil-li
other-ERG
ix-ub-le
throw.PFV-PRET-CVB
tupang
weapon
29
antːa-l-cːe]
forehead-OBL-IN
zija=ra
horsefly=ADD
kax-ub
ca-b
il
admi=ra
kill.PFV-PRET
COP-N
that
person=ADD
‘The other shot at the forehead and killed the horseflв and the man’
“ca
one
(53) c'il
ik'-ul
ca-w
then
say.IPFV-ICVB
COP-M
‘Then he saвs: “One of you, one of us.”’
ašːa-la,
2PL-GEN
ca
one
nišːa-la”
1PL-GEN
References
Creissels, Denis. 2010. Specialized converbs and adverbial subordination in Aбaбdərə Akhvakh. In
Isabelle Bril (ed.), Clause linking and clause hierarchy: Syntax and pragmatics, 105–142.
Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Comrie, Bernard, Diana Forker & Zaira Khalilova. Accepted. Antipassives in Nakh-Daghestanian
languages: Exploring the margins of a construction. In Katarzyna Janic, Alena WitzlackMakarevich, & Denis Creissels (eds.), The multifaceted aspects of Antipassive. Amsterdam:
Benjamins.
Forker, Diana. 2013. Microtypology and the Tsezic languages: A case study of syntactic properties
of converbal clauses. SKY Journal of Linguistics 26, 21–40.
Forker, Diana. 2014. Are there subject anaphors? Linguistic Typology 18, 51–81.
Forker, Diana. 2016. Floating agreement and information structure: The case of Sanzhi. Studies in
Language 40, 1–25.
Forker, Diana. Accepted[a]. Grammatical relations in Sanzhi Dargwa. In Balthasar Bickel & Alena
Witzlack-Makarevich (eds.), Handbook of grammatical relations. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Forker, Diana. Accepted[b]. Reported speech constructions in Sanzhi Dargwa and their extension to
other areas of grammar. In Patrizia Noel Aziz & Barbara Sonnenhauser (eds.), The syntax of
pragmatics: Addressing, adding, signaling. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Forker, Diana. Accepted[c]. A grammar of Sanzhi Dargwa. Submitted to Language Science Press.
Forker, Diana. Accepted[d]. More than just a modal particle: The enclitic =q'al in Sanzhi Dargwa.
To appear in Functions of Language.
Forker, Diana. Accepted[e]. Sanzhi-Russian code switching and the Matrix Language Frame
Model. To appear in International Journal of Bilingualism.
Forker, Diana. Accepted[f]. Elevational deixis and insubordination in Sanzhi Dargwa. To appear in
Linguistic Typology.
Forker, Diana & Oleg Belyaev. 2016. Information structure in Nakh-Daghestanian languages. In
M.M. Jocelyne Fernandez-Vest & Robert Van Valin Jr (eds.), Information structure and spoken
language from a cross-linguistic perspective, 239–261. Berlin: De Gruyter.
Forker, Diana & Gadzhimurad Gadzhimuradov. 2017. Sanzhinskie skazki i rasskazy. Makhachkala.
Kalinina, Elena & Nina Sumbatova. 2007. Clause structure and verbal forms in NakhDaghestanian. In Irina Nikolaeva (ed.), Finiteness: Theoretical and empirical foundations, 183–
249. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kazenin, Konstantin & Yakov G. Testelets. 2004. Where coordination meets subordination:
Converb constructions in Tsakhur (Daghestanian). In Martin Haspelmath (ed.), Coordinating
constructions, 227–240. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Sumbatova, Nina R. & Rasul O. Mutalov. 2003. A grammar of Icari Dargwa. Munich:
Lincom.
Sumbatova, Nina R. & Yury Lander. 2014. Darginskij govor selenija Tanty: grammatičeskij očerk,
voprosy sintaksisa. Moscow: Jazyki slavjanskoj kul'tury.
30