Research in Nursing and Health, 1984. 7, 287-294
zyxwvut
The Male Nursing Student
as a Token
B. Kay Snavely and Gail Theus Fairhurst
The purpose of this research was to test the applicability of Kanter’s theory of tokenism
to male nursing students. Kanter’s theory states that numerical underrepresentation, not
cultural factors, causes tokens to experience greater performance pressure, social isolation, and role entrapment. Subjects were 322 mole and female nursing students from
two similar midwestern nursing schools. Subjects completed instruments measuring
social isolation, upward communication distortion, performance pressure, and communication apprehension. This research did not find support for Kanter’s claims. Possible
explanations for the findings include sex status differences and the nurturing character
of the nursing profession.
zyxwvuts
Most of the literature devoted to tokens and
minority groups in recent years focused on problems faced by minority females entering maledominated professions. Of note, however, is the
effect of an accompanying movement by males
into female-dominated professions. Nursing represents a primary area for males to branch away
from traditionally male occupations. Given the
predicted increase in demand for professionals in
the health care fields generally, and nursing particularly, it seems economically beneficial to the
nursing profession to encourage more males to
become nurses.
Individuals are identified as tokens when they
enter a job environment with a history of their
social category being numerically scarce in a given
occupation (Kanter, 1977). Social category refers
to some master status like age, sex, religion, or
other characteristic where a set of assumptions
about the relative status, expectations for behavior, andlor qualities possessed by the group are
presumed. According to Kanter, such characteristics affect the perception of tokens which in
turn results in three outcomes: social isolation,
performance pressure, and role entrapment. Isolation of the token occurs because majority mem-
bers tend to exaggerate their differences from tokens, a phenomenon which results in the majority
tightening its own boundaries and treating the
token as an outsider. Because tokens are more
visible than majority members, they are under
more performance pressure. Finally, when tokens enter an organization, majority members
stereotype them based on the token’s ascribed
characteristics such as sex or race which then
leads to role entrapment.
Based on research concerning female tokens in
business settings, Kanter (1977) argued that the
three token dynamics result, not because of cultural or sexual differences between majority and
minority group members, but because of the degree of imbalance in the numerical proportions
of the two groups. If this is true then males in
nursing ought to experience the same token dynamics reported by Kanter for women in business.
The literature about males in the nursing
profession suggested that males should encounter
the token dynamics Kanter named. Segal (1962)
reported that male nurses may feel lower prestige
when entering a female dominated occupation.
To reduce the amount of role strain, male nurses
zyxwvu
zyxwvutsr
zyxwvu
zyxw
Ms. B. Kay Snavely is an instructor in the Department of Management, Miami University, Oxford,
OH. Dr. Gail Theus Fairhurst is associate professor in the Department of Communication, Speech,
and Theater, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH.
This article was received February 25, 1983, was revised, and was accepted for publication on
November 2 2 , 1983.
Requests for reprints may be addressed to B. Kay Snavely, Instructor, Department of Management, Miami University, Oxford, OH 45056.
0 1984 Wiley 01 60-6891/84/040287-08 $04.00
287
288
zyxwvuts
zyxwv
RESEARCH IN NURSING AND HEALTH
(Bush, 1976; Greenberg & Levine, 1971) and
male nursing students (Bush, 1976) choose nursing specialties more closely aligned with traditional male responsibilities (e.g., anesthesiology,
psychiatry, urology, etc.). Fottler (1976) reported that sex segregation of nursing is based
on social, cultural, and economic values, and the
inconsistency of the traditional male image with
a traditionally nurturing female role.
In addition to the fact that much of the preceding literature is based on anecdotal data or suffers
from methodological limitations, such as small
sample size, the degree to which the male nurse
or nursing student experience is shaped by the
token dynamics identified by Kanter is not known.
According to Kanter (1977), males are high status tokens when they aspire to a majority group
occupation like nursing. This is because western
society accords a higher societal status to males
than to females. While Kanter said that deferential treatment may be substituted for the patronizing behavior female tokens receive, the
outcomes she associated with tokenism should
still prevail (i.e., performance pressure, social
isolation, and role entrapment).
Because status usually is correlated with power
(Secord & Backman, 1974), and because status
outside the organization (i.e., in society at large)
adds to the structuring of control within the organization (Acker & Van Houten, 1974), male
tokens apparently have a source of power that
female tokens do not. The question thus becomes: What is the significance of this source of
status and power to token dynamics? Will males
in the nursing profession experience the performance pressure, social isolation, and role entrapment reported in recent studies of female tokens (Kanter, 1977; Spangler, Gordon, & Pipkin,
1978)? In this report a systematic empirical attempt to answer this question with male nursing
students is described. The token outcomes examined were: (a) social isolation in certain interactional domains, (b) upward communication
distortion, and (c) performance pressure. In addition, two other outcomes were communication
apprehension toward the female majority and toward the male minority.
The first outcome investigated was social isolation. According to Kanter (1977, p. 978), tokens should be more socially isolated than majority members. Further, social isolation is most
likely to surface in the areas of political information (e.g., ways of getting around formal rules,
strategies for impressing superiors, etc.) and personal problems affecting performance. Two other
topics, general school and social information, also
were studied because there is little information
about such variables.
Are male nursing students more socially isolated in
networks involving political information, personal
problems &fecting performance, general school and
social information than female nursing students?
The second outcome concerns entrapment in
stereotypical roles which may force tokens to distort their self-image to coworkers and superiors
more than majority members (Kanter, 1977). We
focused specifically on upward communication
distortion because the greater reward power of
superiors might increase the concern for the image one is projecting, and therefore, the tendency
to distort. For high status tokens, the distortion
would likely enhance rather than detract from the
perception of competence.
Do male nursing students distort their upward communication more than female nursing students?
The third outcome concerns the increased pressure to perform that tokens experience more than
majority members. This is brought about by the
heightened visibility of tokens.
Are male nursing students under greater pressure to
perform than female nursing students?
There are several behaviors that Kanter said
are endemic to tokenism that make communication particularly uncomfortable for tokens. This
is because communication is the primary means
through which a lowered status is conferred on a
token. Some of these status-lowering behaviors
are: reminding tokens about their deviance, making the token’s social category the object of humor or defamatory statements, being forced to
act in ways which reinforce social category stereotypes, and other self-imposed social isolation.
Consequently, tokens should experience communication apprehension, a construct defined as
anxiety asswiated with either real or anticipated
oral communication (McCroskey, 1977) with the
numerical majority.
Do male nursing students experience more communication apprehension than female nursing students when
interacting with the male minority?
Finally, if the males represent a threat to the
females, then they may engender communication
apprehension in the female majority. If they do
not represent a threat, then we should expect little
difference between males and females in their
reported level of apprehension.
zyx
Do fernale nursing students experience greater communication apprehension than male nursing students
when interacting with the male minority?
METHOD
Sample
zyxw
zyxwvutsrqp
MALE NURSING / SNAVELY AND FAIRHURST
Several nursing schools were examined and a
convenience sample was obtained from two midwestern 2-year diploma nursing schools. The two
selected schools were similar along the following
criteria: (a) type of program, (b) length of program, (c) distribution of ages in the student body,
(d) relative percentage of male students, (e) degree of autonomy provided to students, (f) admission standards, and (g) relative competitiveness in the environment. Both schools had a strong
emphasis on grades, class honors, and clinical
performance.
Due to the large number of females and small
number of males in each nursing school, pooling
of two schools was required for statistical comparison. For one school (with enrollment of 18
males and 202 females) the sample was composed of 17 males and 176 females. For the second school (with enrollment of 26 males and 147
females), the sample was composed of 24 males
and 105 females. A sample size of at least 30
males overall was sought to achieve stable parameter estimates. In addition, both first and second year classes within each school had to be
combined. Because tenure in the organization
could influence certain responses, the effects of
year in school were explored. Subjects included
322 nursing students, 41 males and 281 females;
all were Caucasian except for 5 females. The
average age was 22.5 years for the males and
22.4 years for the females.
289
formance at school, general school information,
and social information. Even though a student
could name another student as a contact, there
was no guarantee that the contact would reciprocate. This was treated as a response error, and
thus reciprocation was forced for unreciprocated
links.
The two indicators of social isolation employed in this study were (a) outputs from a network analysis procedure (Richards, 1975) providing individual, group, and systemic information
and (b) an integrativeness score measuring the
association of a subject’s immediate contacts. The
first indicator of social isolation, at the group
level of analysis, is the role assigned to each
individual on the basis of their position within
the networks. Richards developed an algorithm
which distinguishes between (a) isolates, those
who have no links or only one other link to persons who are themselves isolated; (b) dyad members, those who are linked to only one other person and in terms of the group function as isolated
pairs; (c) group members, those individuals belonging to a group meeting the following criteria:
zyxwv
1. There must be at least three members.
2. Members must have 50% of their contacts
with members in the group.
3. Each member must be linked to every other
member within the group (through a path
lying entirely within the group).
4. There must be no single individual or link,
which when removed, causes the group to
fail to meet the preceding criteria.
zyxwvutsr
zyxwvutsr
Measures
Social isolation. An instrument to measure
social isolation was constructed and pretested on
a freshman class (N = 97) at a third midwestern
nursing school. After modifications, a revised
questionnaire was administered to the four nursing classes in this sample. Students were given
a list of the other students in their class and asked
to respond to the following: (a) Whom do you
get information from regarding the politics of being
a student (i.e., how to get around formal rules,
strategies for impressing superiors, overall strategies for getting what you want)? and (b) Whom
do you give information to regarding the politics
of being a student? In addition to identifying the
direction of influence, students weighted their responses by specifying both the frequency and importance of the contacts. Similar sociometric
questions were posed regarding three other topics: personal problems that could affect per-
(d) liaisons, individuals who are linked to two or
more groups but do not meet the criteria for group
membership themselves; and (e) others, those
persons who possess a number of links to other
persons but fail to qualify for membership in any
identifiable group.
Following Richards’ definition, a person was
categorized as a social isolate if there were no
links to other persons or the links were to isolated
people. If an individual was classified as a liaison, an other, or a group member, that person
was considered integrated into a network of work
relations. Dyad members were analyzed separately because members are not themselves isolates, but as a pair they function as one.
The second indicator of social isolation, at the
individual level of analysis, was an integrativeness score which is a measure of association of
a subject’s immediate contacts. An integrativeness score was based on the number of links between persons connected to the focal person, divided by the largest possible number of such links.
290
zyxwvutsrq
zyxw
RESEARCH IN NURSING AND HEALTH
zyxwv
zy
zyx
The values ranged from 0 to 1 (Goldhaber &
Rogers, 1979), with values closer to 1.00 indicating a greater degree of integrativeness into the
network. Eight integrativeness scores, one for each
network topic, were tabulated for each subject.
Upward communication distortion. The
measure of upward communication distortion
closely followed a procedure developed by
Athanassiades (1974). Subjects were given duplicate lists of 36 personality traits. The 36 personality traits, both positive and negative, were
selected by the researchers for the two roles of
Student and Nurse. For the first list, which appeared early in the overall questionnaire, students
were asked to indicate the degree to which they
felt the traits were representative of “how they
see themselves.’’ For this exercise, students were
assured that the faculty would not see their responses. Later in the questionnaire, students again
received the same list (although the order was
transposed) and essentially asked to do the same
thing. With the second list, however, students
were told that the faculty would be seeing their
evaluations for purposes of counter-evaluating the
responses. If students were distorting their selfimage to instructors, then there should be some
difference in how they see themselves and how
they portray themselves to their instructors. Each
student received a distortion score by summing
the differences between the first and second responses to each scale.
Performance pressure. Two indicators of
performance pressure were employed. The first
indicator, suggested by Kanter (1977), was that
performance pressure would be manifested by a
tendency to under- or overachieve. To determine
achievement level, high school grades (obtained
with students’ permission) and class rank were
used to predict nursing school grades. Those
students whose grades were correctly predicted
were dropped from consideration. Students whose
grades were lower than expected were labeled
underachievers, while those whose grades were
greater than expected were labeled overachievers.
The second indicator of performance pressure
was drawn from Spangler et al. ’s ( 1978) research
on token women in law school. They operationalized this construct in terms of contemplationof
withdrawal from school. Accordingly, in a single
item response format, the nursing students in this
sample were asked, “How frequently have you
contemplated withdrawing from nursing school?”
Subjects were given a 5-point scale with endpoints ranging from frequently to never.
Communication apprehension. The Situational Communication Apprehension Measure
(SCAM) developed by Snavely and Phelps (1979)
was used to measure communication apprehension. The scale required subjects to state their
degree of agreement (on a 7-point scale) with the
following statements: When communicating with
my male (female) coworkers (1) I feel nervous,
(2) I feel shy, (3) I feel relaxed, (4) I feel intimidated, ( 5 ) I feel afraid of what they will think
of me, (6) I find it difficult to talk. Cronbach’s
alpha for the scale was .80. After pretesting, the
SCAM scale was revised to accommodate three
additional items: (1) I feel self-conscious, (2) I
feel womed, and (3) I am troubled with shyness.
In a pilot study, the principal component analysis
showed the first factor accounted for some 73%
of the variance, and the reliability was consistent
with previous (Snavely & Phelps, 1979) research
(Cronbach’s alpha = .92). Students filled out
two SCAM scales, the first with males as the
referent and the second with females as the referent.
Procedure
After a signed informed consent was obtained
from each subject, a questionnaire containing the
four instruments was administered in large group
settings ( at different times for each class). The
response rates of the questionnaires were 88%,
90%, 94%, and 98% for the four classes.
RESULTS
Social Isolation
zy
Role designation. Separate network analyses
were performed for each class. The first indicator
of social isolation was the network role assigned
to each participant. In Table 1 are the percentage
of males and females falling into each role classification. Since the two class networks did not
differ, the percentages were averaged. An examination of the isolate category in Table 1 shows
an extremely low percentage of both males and
females playing the isolate role in all four networks; there are no isolates in the school information network for either sex. For males there
are no isolates in the Flitics or social networks;
for females there is less than 1% in the politics
and social networks. The personal problems network is the only network where any isolates exist
for either sex, although the percentages are still
extremely low. Additionally, no dyad members,
who as pairs function as isolates, surfaced.
lntegrativeness scores. The second indicator
of social isolation was the integrativeness scores
assigned to each subject by network. The eight
integrativeness scores for each individual were
zyxwvu
zyxw
zyxwv
z
zyxwvutsrq
zyxw
29 1
MALE NURSING / SNAVELY AND FAIRHURST
Table 1. Network Role Classificationsfor Male
Students
(N=41)and Female (N=281)
Nursing
Group
Member
liaison
Isolate
Dyad
Member
Other
YoMales
YOFemales
68.3
69.6
3.7
1.1
0.0
0.5
0.0
0.0
28.1
28.5
% Males
YOFemales
47.6
69.6
2.5
3.4
3.6
2.3
0.0
0.0
46.4
24.5
91.5
80.8
2.4
2.3
0.0
0.0
0.0
0.0
5.1
16.9
Social
Information
YOMales
YOFemales
YOMales
YOFemales
6.1
4.5
0.0
0.8
0.0
0.0
9.8
20.3
Combined
% Males
% Females
72.9
73.7
3.7
2.8
0.9
0.9
0.0
0.0
22.6
22.6
Network
PoIit ics
Personal
Problems
School
Information
84.2
74.6
zyxwvu
_.
zyxwvuts
subjected to a principal components analysis with
iterations to arrive at a more parsimonious set of
network variables. Variables that loaded .5 or
above on one factor with no secondary loading
greater than . 3 on another were retained. This
criterion was chosen because factor loadings have
been shown to be somewhat unstable (Dorans &
Drasgow, 1978), and this criterion gave an indication of the relative unidimensionality of the
variables retained. The analysis produced a 2factor solution as shown in Table 2. It is immediately apparent that the directional information
for the different topics (i.e., whom do you get
information from, give information to?) is strongly
correlated suggesting a mutual exchange of information. The factor matrix in Table 2 shows
that general school information, social information, and political information correlate strongly
with the first factor, although social information
also has high loadings on the second factor. The
general nature of this topic is thought to be the
reason for the dual factor loadings. To aid interpretability, “get information from,” and “give
information to” on social matters were dropped
from the analysis. The first factor then is composed of the topics school information and political information, both of which involve work
knowledge. The second factor is composed solely
of the topic personal problems affecting performance. Composite scales for these two factors
were formed by adding together the variables that
loaded on that factor. This procedure resulted in
sacrificing the orthogonality of the factors.
Because the correlations between the dependent network factors and some of the other
variables were high, multivariate analysis of the
data was used to test for group differences. Thus,
the effects of sex and year in school on the factored network variables, the communication apprehension variables, and contemplation of withdrawal from nursing school were examined. The
multivariate profile analysis (Morrison, 1976),
which tested for an overall treatment effect, was
significant (F (12,815) = 5.40, p < .0001). The
multivariate analyses of variances which followed tested the model for specific treatment ef-
Table 2. Rotated Factor Matrix for Network lntegrativeness Scores
of 322 Nursing Students
School Information
School Information
Social Information
Social Information
Political Information
Politica I Information
Personal Problems
Personal Problems
Gets information from
Gives information to
Gets information from
Gives informution to
Gets information from
Gives information to
Gets information from
Gives information to
YOof Variance
=
Factor 1
Factor 2
.867
,888
,673
,642
.776
,773
.227
.2 1 5
.141
.1 77
.4 1 5
,452
.2 13
,239
.932
,870
62.4
16.6
zyxwvutsr
zyxwv
zyxwv
zyxwvut
zyxwvutsr
292
RESEARCH IN NURSING AND HEALTH
fects. The effect for sex was significant (F (5,272)
= 6.68, p < .001), while the effect for year was
not significant. The interaction effect for sex and
year was not significant.
Univariate F tests were subsequently used to
probe the meaning of the multivariate difference
(Spector, 1977). A Statistical Analysis System
(SAS) (Helwig & Council, 1979) Type IV sums
of squares solution was used in all cases because
the design was unbalanced and the effects were
not orthogonal. This solution partials for all other
effects in the model, the most conservative approach. The overall F was significant for the work
knowledge network factor [F (3,276) = 5.21,
p < .002, R2 = .05],with a year effect surfacing
(F = 15.23, p < .05). The overall F was not
significant for the personal problems network
factor.
Sex composition of group and individual contact networks. Since a sex effect failed to surface for those falling into the isolate category as
well as in terms of their integrativeness scores,
a separate analysis of the sex composition of both
the group structures that emerged, as well as the
contacts named by each student was undertaken.
The logic of this inquiry stemmed from the belief
that if males primarily associated with other males,
this would still be evidence of social isolation
and thus the preceding finding would be spu-
especially in the area of personal problems affecting school performance where discussion of
male-female relationships is likely to surface. The
data in Table 3 bear this out. Thus, the finding
that males are socially integrated does not appear
spurious.
Upward Communication Isolation
zyxwvut
zyxwv
zyxwv
rious.
Of the 76 groups that emerged from the four
networks, 1 group contained only males, 30 groups
contained only females (partly due to the vast
majority of students being female), and 45 groups
contained both sexes.
When individual data were examined to see
the percentage of males and females named as
contacts, little evidence of exclusive contact within
males were found, as shown in Table 3, males
name other males as comprising no more than
one-third of their contacts. While this is 27 to 4
times as many males as named by females, this
finding is not unusual. That is, we would expect
males to associate a little more with other males
The upward communication distortion measure
was not included in the preceding multivariate
analysis because the amount of missing data for
this variable, when combined with the other variables, dropped too many males from the analysis.
Consequently, a univariate F test was performed.
Although the test should be interpreted with some
caution, there does not appear to be much redundancy between the upward communication distortion measure and the other dependent variables. The correlations ranged from - .06 to .15.
A sufficierit number of males was retained for
analysis (N = 29) when this variable was considered alone. The results of the analysis of sex
and year in school on upward communication
distortion were not significant. However, the effect for sex was the most prominent of the sources
of variation (F = 5.54, p < .02).
Performance Pressurg
Contemplation of withdrawal from school was
tested as part of the multivariate analyses reported for Social Isolation. The univariate F tests
showed that the effect for contemplation of withdrawal from nursing school was not significant.
Under- and overachievement were determined
by the residuals yielded by the multiple regression of nursing school grades on high school grades
and class rank. The residuals ranged from
- 121.514 to 113.433. The top 25% were deemed
overachievers and the bottom 25% underachievers. The cut-off points were 31.20 and -31.18,
respectively. Class rank and high school grades
accounted for a significant [F (2,194) = 19.36,
Table 3. Percentage of Males Named a5 Contacts by Male
and Female Students
Network
Politics
Personal Problems
School Information
Social Information
Percentage of
male students
naming males
as contacts
Percentage of
female students
naming males
as contacts
33%
33%
13%
9%
12%
12%
2 7%
29%
z
zyxwvut
zyxw
zyxwvu
zyxwv
zyxwv
MALE NURSING I SNAVELY AND FAIRHURST
p < .oOl, R2 = .17], albeit a comparatively
small proportion, of the variance in nursing school
grades. Consequently, this measure is not as strong
an indicator of performance pressure as expected.
The ANOVA failed to reveal a significant sex
effect.
Communication Apprehension
Both communication apprehension variables
were tested as part of the multivariate analyses
reported for Social Isolation. The univariate F
tests showed that the effect for communication
apprehension towards females was not significant. However, the effect for communication
apprehension towards males was significant
(F (3,276) = 4.36, p < .005, R2 = .05). When
the model was further broken down, a sex effect
surfaced ( F = 11.61, p < .001). Females felt
significantly more communication apprehension
than males.
293
that male nursing students knew “how to play
the game” and manipulate the system toward their
own ends; thus, males were described as politically astute. (b) Female majority members felt
more communication apprehension toward token
males than the token males felt toward females.
The apprehension felt by majority members may
be a recognition of the power of the tokens due
to their high status.
The implications of this argument suggest that
being numerically dominant is only one among
many sources of status and control within an organization and these sources must be considered
when examining token dynamics. Future research should continue to explore the extent to
which token dynamics intensify when other sources
of status and control (e.g., race, age) favor the
majority and lessen when such sources favor the
token minority.
A second explanation comes from Cook’s
(1969) work on race relations. Organizations go
through stages with regard to the integration of
any specific minority group. These stages are
marked by the degree of contact with the minority. They range from a rather uncertain preinteraction period to a postinteraction stage where the
experience with minorities has been sufficient to
draw conclusions about them. Fromkin (1974)
made the point that proportional imbalance may
be most critical in the early stages of integration
rather than later on. If the male nurses in previous
studies or the women in Kanter’s study were
newer, as a minority group, to their setting than
this study’s male nursing students were to theirs,
this may explain why token dynamics surfaced
for the former groups. While Kanter (1977) argued that token dynamics will surface whenever
the relative numbers are sufficiently imbalanced,
she allowed that the newness of a token minority
to a setting may heighten token dynamics. It may
be, however, that scarcity is necessary but not
sufficient to produce tokenism with newness also
being a necessary condition.
A third and final explanation for these results
stems from idiosyncracies introduced by the
nursing profession itself. Consider the following:
First, nursing by its definition and duties demands a nurturing approach of its members. People who enter nursing must have some need to
help others. That helping nature very likely permeates the socialization process when new members enter the profession. Thus, the majority group
members may seek to reduce any uncomfortable
dynamics that male nursing students are likely to
face. This nurturing approach is not endemic to
other professions where tokenism has been studied; compare, for example, the difficulties faced
zyxwv
DlSCUSSlON
This research failed to find the token dynamics
reported in studies of female tokens (Kanter, 1977;
Spangler et al., 1978). When male nursing student behavior was compared to the female majority’s behavior, the males were neither more
socially isolated, nor distorting in their upward
communication, nor under greater pressure to
perform. Under- or overachievement was not as
strong an indicator of performance pressure as
expected, perhaps because the average age of the
sample was 22, some 4 to 5 years after high
school graduation. If Kanter’s theory applies to
all tokens, then we should have found significant
differences in the amount of performance pressure, social isolation, and role entrapment between male and female nursing students in this
investigation.
One intuitively appealing explanation for these
results could be sex status differences (i.e., the
higher status of males and the lower status of
females in society). According to Acker and Van
Houten (1974), a sex based status difference outside the organization adds to the structuring of
control within the organization because organizations tend to reflect the cultures they are a part
of. For the high status male nursing students, this
translates to a token minority that has a power
base from which to resist majority group assertions of control, like social isolation, role entrapment, and performance pressure. Other findings
in this study supportive of this argument include
the following: (a) The nursing school faculty, the
male students, and the female students reported
zyxwv
294
zyxwvutsrq
zyxwv
RESEARCH IN NURSING AND HEALTH
by female tokens in competitive business settings.
Second, there may be other token dynamics
outside the coworker relationship which impact
the majority-minority interaction. Specifically, it
seems likely that larger societal roles may greatly
affect token dynamics because it is outside the
hospital setting where the male entering nursing
finds considerable prestige loss. Parents, particularly fathers, are most overt in their attempts to
dissuade males from becoming nurses (Bush,
1976). Outside the family, it is generally “first
contact situations and superficial relationships”
(Schoenmaker & Radosevich, 1976) where disapproval of male nurses is highest. Though these
may not be the most important relationships males
face, the sheer numbers of superficial relationships and first meetings encountered could be
substantial. Patients in hospital settings, though
initially uncomfortable with male nurses, tend to
‘‘come around” eventually according to selfreports by male nurses (Greenberg & Levine,
1971). Nevertheless, the need to examine token
dynamics which may accrue from outside the immediate organization may be important because
both this study and Kanter’s (1977) examined
dynamics within the organization and, by their
design, largely ignored the outside environment.
In light of the findings in this study which suggest the relative lack of token dynamics as far as
coworker relationships are concerned, and previous research suggesting the relative presence of
token dynamics, the nursing profession should
encourage and participate in further study of token dynamics faced by males in the profession.
Such study could: (a) potentially prompt an increase in the number of males in the profession,
a profession currently faced with a shortage of
personnel; (b) lead to a clearer explanation of the
situation faced by males and females in minority
and majority groups; and ( c ) contribute to our
understanding of token dynamics in general.
Cook, S. W. Motives and conceptual analysis of attitude-related behavior. In W. J. Arnold & J. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on motivation.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1969.
Dorans, N, & Drasgow, F. Alternative weighting
schemes for linear prediction. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1 978,2 I , 3 1 6-345.
Fottler, M. D. Attitudes of female nurses toward the
male nurse: A study of occupational segregation.
Journai of Health and Social Behovior, 1976, 17,
z
zyxwv
98-1 10.
zyxwvut
Fromkin, H. L. Social psychological analysis of organizational integration. In H. L. Fromkin & J. J.
Sherwood (Eds.), lntegrotingthe Organization. New
York: Free Press, 1974.
Goldhober, G. M., & Rogers, D. P. Auditing organizational communication systems. Dubuque: Kendall/Hunt, 1979.
Greenberg, E., & Levine, 8. Role strain in men nurses:
A preliminary investigation. Nursing Forum, 1971,
10, 417-430.
Helwig, J. T., & Council, K. (Eds.) Statistical analysis
systems user‘s guide, Raleigh, NC: Statistical Analysis System Institute, 1979.
Konter, R. M. Some effects of proportions on group
life: Skewed sex ratios and responses to token
women. American Journal of Sociology, 1977, 82,
965-990.
McCroskey, J. C. Oral communication apprehension: A summary of recent theory and research.
Human Communication Research, 1977,4, 78-96.
Morrison, D. G. Multivoriate statistical methods. New
York: McGraw-Hill, 1976.
Richards, W. D., Jr. A manual for network analysis.
Buffalo, NY: Institute for Communication Research, 1975.
Schoenmaker, A., & Radosevich, D. M. Men nursing
students: How they perceive their situation. Nursing Outlook, 1976, 24, 298-305.
Secord, P., & Backman, C. Social psychology (2nd
ed.). New York: McGraw Hill, 1974.
Segal, B. E. Male nurses: A case study in status contradiction and prestige loss. Social Forces, 1962,
41, 31-38.
Spangler, C., Gordon, M., & Pipkin, R. Token women:
An empirical test of Kanter‘s hypothesis. American
Journal of Socidogy, 1978, 84, 160-1 70.
Spector, P. E. What to do with significant mvltivariate analysis of variance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1977, 62, 158-1 63.
zyxwvutsr
REF€R€NC€S
Acker, J., 81 Van Houten, D. R. Differential recruitment ond control: The sex structuring of organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 1974, 19,
152-1 63.
Athanassiodes, J. C. An investigation of some communication patterns of female subordinates in hierarchical organizations. Human Relations, 1974,
27, 195-209.
Bush, P. J. The mole nurse: A challenge to traditional role identities. Nursing Forum, 1976, 15,
390-405.
REF €R€NC€ NOT€
Snavely, W. B., & Phelps, L. A. Development of a
situational communication apprehension measure.
Paper presented at the annual convention of the
Eastern Communication Association. Philadelphia, May 5-7, 1979.