European Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 23,581-595 (1993)
zyx
z
zyxw
Effects of intergroup similarity on
intergroup relations
SONIA ROCCAS and
SHALOM H. SCHWARTZ
zyxwv
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel
Abstract
We examined the impact of intergroup similarity on two aspects of intergroup relations.
Drawing on social identity and belief congruence theory, we hypothesized that - at
high levels of intergroup similarity - increasing similarity has dual, seemingly opposed
effects: It increases ingroup favouritism in evaluations but also increases readiness
for social contact with the outgroup. We further hypothesized that both effects are
moderated by the strength of individuals’ identiJication with their ingroup. Finally,
we hypothesized that there is ingroup favouritism on dimensions relevant for defining
the group, but outgroup favouritism on dimensions irrelevant for this purpose. One
hundred and forty-nine students from two prestigious high schools, who were assigned
to one of three levels of manipulated similarity between their schools, evaluated both
schools on dimensions relevant and irrelevant to the school context and expressed their
readiness for social contact with the other school. Ingroup favouritism appeared on
relevant dimensions and outgroup favouritism on irrelevant dimensions. As predicted,
for those highly identified with their ingroup, intergroup similarity led to greater ingroup
favouritism in evaluations on relevant dimensions but to increased readinessfor outgroup
social contact. Implications for interpreting inconsistent results of past research and
for specgying conditions for intergroup bias are discussed.
zyxw
INTRODUCTION
The assumption that similarity between groups improves intergroup relations has
received broad research support. For example, research dealing with racial discrimination revealed that perceived similarity of beliefs consistently reduced discrimination
towards outgroup members (Insko, Nacoste and Moe, 1983). However, this assumption is sometimes contradicted. For instance, Genthner, Shuntich and Bunting (1975)
zyxwvu
zyxwvutsr
zyxwv
This research was supported by grant No. 88-00085 from the United States-Israel Binational Science
Foundation (BSF), Jerusalem, Israel, and by a grant from the Office of Science and Development of
the National Council for Research and Technology (Israel), both to the second author. Reprint requests
should be addressed to Shalom H. Schwartz, Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem, Israel, 91905.
0046-2772/93/060581-15$12.50
0 1993 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Received 21 April 1991
Accepted 20 January 1992
582
zyxwvutsr
zyxwvu
zyxw
S. Roccas and S. H. Schwartz
found that highly prejudiced white subjects were more aggressive towards a black
with similar beliefs than towards a black with dissimilar beliefs. The current study
seeks to clarify some of the conditions that determine whether similarity improves
or damages intergroup relations.
Research that examines the effects of similarity on intergroup relations has drawn
mainly upon two theories, belief congruence and social identity. We briefly sketch
these theories and relevant research, noting aspects helpful for identifying the conditions that interest us.
Belief congruence
Belief congruence theory (Rokeach, 1960)extrapolates from the similarity-attraction
paradigm of interpersonal relations (Byrne, 1971) to the effects of similarity on relations between members of different groups. Research in this framework shows that
perceived similarity between own beliefs and those of an outgroup member is negatively related to numerous types of discrimination (Insko, et al., 1983). However,
three characteristics of this research limit generalizing from its conclusions to the
effects of intergroup similarity on intergroup relations.
Focus on individuals
Experiments based on belief congruence theory typically inform subjects about the
amount of belief similarity between self and individual target persons either from
their ingroup or from a known, other group. Three aspects of this procedure limit
inferences to the intergroup situation. First, the information about similarityconcerns
a target individual and not a group. But, people tend to perceive information about
individuals that contradicts group stereotypes in a biased way that preserves the
stereotypes (Wilder, 1986). Consequently, information that single members of an
outgroup are similar may have different effects than information that the outgroup
itself is similar. Second, in these experiments, varied information is sometimes provided about multiple stimulus individuals. This focuses attention on individual characteristics and reduces the salience of group membership (Brown, 1988). Third,
behaviour toward individuals rather than toward groups is examined. But, behaviour
toward specific members of an outgroup may differ from behaviour toward the outgroup as a whole.
Nature of the dependent variable
Belief congruence research usually studies affective and behavioural dependent variables (e.g. liking, readiness for social contact) and not perceptual bias toward the
outgroup. But, behaviour toward an outgroup is not necessarily connected with
the way the outgroup is perceived in comparison to the ingroup. For example, Struch
and Schwartz (1989) reported that the behavioural intention to be aggressive toward
an outgroup was virtually uncorrelated with the tendency to perceive it as inferior
to the ingroup. It is therefore likely that the conditions that promote the expression
of outgroup discrimination in behavioural and affective modes differ from those
that promote its expression in perceptual biases.
zy
zyxwvut
zyxw
zyxwvu
Intergroup similarity
583
Amount of similarity between groups
Only increases in similarity at low levels have been shown to promote positive
responses to target persons in belief congruence studies. However, it is quite possible
that increases at higher levels of similarity may yield a negative relation between
similarity and responses. This is suggested by extension from uniqueness theory
(Snyder and Fromkin, 1980), optimal distinctiveness theory (Brewer, 199l), and other
models of individuation (e.g. Codol, 1984; Lemaine, 1974). These theories postulate
that people are motivated to feel moderately different from others, to find an optimal
balance between assimilation with and differentiation from others. Very low as well
as very high similarity with others promote negative responses. To determine whether
very high levels of intergroup similarity are also aversive, higher levels of similarity
than those previously studied are required.
Social identity
According to social identity theory (Hogg and Abrams, 1988; Tajfel, 1974), individuals are motivated to derive a positive social identity from their group membership
by perceiving their ingroup as positively distinctive compared to other groups. Perception of great intergroup similarity can be threatening because it blurs the distinction
between groups, making it difficult for individuals to derive a distinctive positive
social identity. Mere categorization into different groups elicits social comparison
that leads directly to intergroup competition for prestige. This competition is the
reason for ingroup bias -the widespread tendency to view the ingroup more favourably than outgroups (Mullen, Brown and Smith, 1992). Ingroup favouritism can be
expressed materially (in allocation of tangible rewards: e.g. Tajfel, 1974; Brewer
and Silver, 1978; Allen and Wilder, 1975) or cognitively (perceiving the ingroup
as better on important dimensions: Brewer and Silver, 1978; Mummendey and
Schreiber, 1983, 1984a). Group membership may produce intergroup bias even when
groups have no objective reason for antagonism toward one another.
Studies in the social identity framework have also examined the effects of similarity
on intergroup relations. These studies have yielded inconsistent outcomes. Allen
and Wilder (1975) found that degree of belief similarity with members of the ingroup
and of the outgroup had no effect on the allocation of a desired resource. Moghaddam
and Stringer (1988) found the usual ingroup bias when subjects perceived themselves
to be similar both to the ingroup and to the outgroup, but outgroup favouritism
when ingroup selection was based on chance and the outgroup was perceived to
be similar.
Turner (1978) studied the effect of similarity with the outgroup under conditions
of stable and unstable status relations between groups. When status relations were
stable, more ingroup bias was found for similar groups than for dissimilar groups.
The opposite results were found when status relations were unstable. Mummendey
and Schreiber (1984b) replicated the Turner study, but obtained very different results.
Diehl (1988) manipulated both interpersonal and intergroup attitudinal similarity.
When interpersonal similarity was manipulated, subjects favoured a similar outgroup
member. In contrast, when intergroup similarity was manipulated, they favoured
members of a dissimilar outgroup.
Finally, Brown and Abrams (1986) examined the effects of similarity on liking
584
zyxwvutsr
zyxwvut
zyxwvuts
zy
S, Roccas and S. H. Schwartz
for the outgroup and on evaluation of its performance. Students informed that outgroup members were similar to them either in their attitudes or in their scholastic
abilities evaluated the performance of the outgroup more highly than did those told
that outgroup members differed from them on both dimensions. Thus, similarity
increased evaluation. However, evaluation of the outgroup was least positive among
subjects told that the outgroup was similar to them in both its attitudes and its
scholastic abilities. Brown (1 988) interpreted this as indicating that intergroup similarity beyond a certain threshold is aversive and threatening. Interestingly, in all experimental conditions, similarity was positively related to liking for the outgroup.
In sum, studies in the social identity framework have shown some - albeit far
from consistent - evidence of a negative impact of similarity on intergroup bias.
This negative impact contradicts hypotheses derived from belief congruence theory.
Two other variables studied in research stemming from social identity theory are
relevant to our concerns, importance of the dimensions of group comparison and
identification with the ingroup. We discuss these variables next.
Importance of the dimensions of comparison
Intergroup bias is related to the importance of the dimensions on which the groups
are evaluated (Turner, 1978; Van Knippenberg, 1978). Mummendey (Mummendey
and Schreiber, 1984a; Mummendey and Simon, 1989) found greater ingroup favouritism on dimensions of high importance to the ingroup than on those of low importance
to it. Moreover, on dimensions of low importance to the ingroup but high importance
to the outgroup, she found outgroup favouritism. The latter shows that intergroup
bias may entail enhancement rather than devaluation of the outgroup. It contradicts
the need for a positive social identity and requires another motivation to explain
it'. Wilder (1986) proposed such a motivation - a norm of fairness. The fairness
norm motivates people to compensate ingroup favouritism on important dimensions
by outgroup favouritism on unimportant dimensions, if there is no cause to discriminate against the outgroup.
ZdentiJicationwith the ingroup
According to social identity theory, the motivation to positively differentiate the
ingroup arises only if individuals have internalized their group membership as an
aspect of their self-concept, that is, if they subjectively identify with the ingroup
(Tajfel and Turner, 1979). Hinkle and Brown (1990) review results of 14 studies
of the effects of degree of ingroup identification on ingroup bias. In most studies,
ingroup identification correlated positively with ingroup favouritism, but the pattern
was inconsistent and significant negative correlations were also found. Thus, the
relation of ingroup identification to intergroup bias clearly merits further research.
We will consider identification as a moderator of the effects of other variables. This
is suggested by the finding that various predictors of intergroup aggression were
'
zyxwvutsr
Hinkle and Brown (1990) cite other examples of outgroup favouritism as well as proposals to explain
it that they find problematic. In their meta-analysis, Mullen et al. (1992) report that the occurrence
of ingroup versus outgroup favouritism varies as a joint function of ingroup status and importance
of the dimension.
zy
zyxwv
Intergroup similarity
585
significantly stronger among respondents who identified highly with their ingroup
(Struch and Schwartz, 1989).
THE PRESENT RESEARCH
The present research sought to overcome the limitations of belief congruence studies
noted above. To that end, we focused on the intergroup rather than the interpersonal
level: We manipulated similarity information about the outgroup as a whole, rather
than about a specific outgroup member. We also assessed attitudes toward the group
as a whole. Furthermore, we studied high levels of similarity. This is because we
assumed that subjects would feel threat to their unique social identity only at very
high levels of similarity. In order to enable the subjects to express outgroup as well
as ingroup favouritism, we measured intergroup bias using dimensions with varying
importance to the definition of the group.
We tested three hypotheses. The first concerned effects of the nature of the dimensions of similarity on the direction of intergroup bias; the second concerned effects
of degree of similarity on group evaluation; the third concerned effects of similarity
on readiness to interact with the outgroup. We discuss each in turn.
In past research, the direction of intergroup bias depended upon whether the
dimensions of evaluation were important to the ingroup or to the outgroup. In
the present research, very similar groups were examined, so the same dimensions
were important to both groups. We postulate that the relevance of the dimensions
for dejining the group has effects similar to those of importance*. Perceiving the
ingroup as superior to an outgroup on dimensions relevant for group definition
is likely to enhance social identity. However, perceiving the ingroup as superior
on dimensions irrelevant to group definition may confer no benefit, and perceiving
the outgroup as superior may pose no threat. We also assume, following Wilder
(1986), that intergroup bias represents a compromise between motivations to achieve
a positive social identity and to act fairly. We therefore hypothesize: There is (1A)
ingroup favouritism on dimensions relevant for defining the group, but (1B) outgroup
favouritism on dimensions irrelevant for this purpose.
Following social identity theory, we assume that very high similarity threatens
the sense of distinctive social identity, motivating subjects to discriminate between
groups and to perceive the ingroup as superior. However, we postulate that identification with the ingroup moderates the relation between intergroup similarity and
ingroup bias. If a person does not identify with the ingroup, the distinctiveness
of his or her ingroup will not affect that person’s social identity. Consequently,
the person will not feel threatened by learning that an outgroup is very similar
to the ingroup. Moreover, as theorized above, only similarity on dimensions relevant
for defining the group poses a threat because it is superiority on these dimensions
that contributes to a positive social identity. We therefore hypothesize: (2) For group
members who identify with their ingroup, increased similarity on dimensions relevant
for defining the ingroup leads to increased ingroup favouritism in evaluation. Note
that this is the usual social identity theory prediction, but with the specification
of two limiting conditions.
zyxw
zyxwvuts
Our concept of relevance for defining the group differs from the use of relevance in Mullen e f al. (1992),
where it is equivalent to importance.
zyxwvuts
zyxwvuts
586
S. Roccas and S. H. Schwartz
Ingroup favouritism and a desire to avoid social contact with the outgroup do
not necessarily occur together (Struch and Schwartz, 1989). Ingroup bias apparently
can preserve the sense of distinct positive social identity without promoting hostility
to the outgroup. Although similarity with members of an outgroup may threaten
one’s social identity, such similarity can also make the prospect of interaction with
members of that same outgroup more desirable. Interaction with similar others is
more likely to provide the interpersonal rewards of reassurance and self-confirmation
(Byrne and Clore, 1970). Moreover, the expectation that those who are more similar
to us will reward us with more liking also makes interaction with them more attractive
(Condon and Crano 1988). Hence, increasing similarity with outgroups, even at
high levels, is likely to increase readiness to engage in social contact with their members. This prediction conforms, of course, with belief congruence theory.
We expect identification with the ingroup to moderate the effect of intergroup
similarity on readiness for social contact. Identification with one’s ingroup reflects,
in part, a sense of similarity to ingroup members (Skevington, 1981). For those
not identified with their ingroup, intergroup similarity implies little similarity between
self and outgroup members. Consequently, for them, interaction with a similar outgroup may promise little personal reinforcement. We therefore hypothesize: (3)
Increased similarity leads to greater readiness for contact with the outgroup among
those who identify with the ingroup.
METHODS
Sample and design
zyxwv
One hundred and forty-nine 10th and 1lth grade students from the three most prestigious high schools in Jerusalem, Israel responded to questionnaires during regular
class sessions. Prestigious schools were selected in order to permit the assumption
that attending these schools was a source of pride for students. A 2(comparison
dimension: relevanvirrelevant) x 3(similarity: very high/high/moderately high) factorial design was employed. Comparison dimension was a within-subjects factor
and similarity a between-subjects factor. Students were randomly assigned to similarity condition.
Procedure
The experiment was presented as a study of students’ views of different high schools.
After completing the experimental questionnaire, students were debriefed. They then
completed a questionnaire concerning ethical aspects of the research. The following
variables were manipulated or measured in the experimental questionnaire.
Manipulation of similarity
Subjects read that researchers had considerable objective information about various
schools and were now interested in the students’ evaluations of their school as compared to other schools. Students were then provided with information about the
degree of similarity between their school and one other school with regard to success
Intergroup similarity
zy
587
in national examinations, choices of topics for national examinations, readiness to
exert themselves during compulsory military service, percentage volunteering for
combat units, percentage of graduates fit to serve as army officers, choices of careers,
and academic success.
In the ‘moderately high’ similarity condition, subjects were told that graduates
of both schools were very similar on the aforementioned characteristics. In the ‘high’
similarity condition, they were told, in addition, that previous research had revealed
that graduates of these two schools report similar school experiences that are different
from the experiences reported by graduates of other high schools, and that students
of other schools consider these two schools to be virtually identical. In the ‘very
high’ similarity condition they were told, in addition, that-due to budget deficits-the
Ministry of Education was considering unifying various high schools and that these
two schools were candidates for future unification because they were so similar.
zyxwv
zyx
zyxwvu
zyxwvut
zyx
Evaluation of the ingroup and the outgroup
Subjects estimated the proportion of students in their school (ingroup) and in the
other school (outgroup) who do each of the following six things: (1) Study hard,
(2) are involved in politics, (3) are interested in popular music, (4) are active in
youth organizations, ( 5 ) want to go to college, (6) consider the teachers in their
school better than average. Three items were chosen from a pool of activities that
judges assessed a priori as relevant to the school context (1,5,6) and three from
a pool assessed as important to high school students but irrelevant to the school
context (2,3,4). Responses were given on a 7-point scale labelled at its extremes:
1-Almost no one, and 7-almost everyone. Subjects also indicated whether they considered the type of behaviour described by each of the items as positive, neutral,
or negative3.
Importance of the dimensions
To provide an estimate of the importance of each dimension for evaluating a school,
subjects rated each of the six items on a scale that ranged from 1 - not important
at all to 7 -very important.
Indexes of bias
Two indexes of bias in intergroup evaluation were computed, one based on items
relevant to the school context and the other based on the items irrelevant to this
context. For each item, the evaluation of the outgroup was subtracted from the
evaluation of the ingroup. This difference score was then multiplied by the importance
rating the subject gave to the item as a dimension for evaluating a school. The
mean of these weighted difference scores for the three relevant items (alpha = 0.65)
zy
Subjects evaluated the schools on an additional item - cheating on examinations. This item was not
included in analyses because student’s appraisals of the desirability of this behaviour varied greatly:
1 1 per cent considered cheating positive, 42 per cent negative, and 47 per cent neutral. All other items
were rated as non-negative by at least 97 per cent of subjects.
588
zyxwvuts
zyxw
zyxwvu
zyx
S. Roccas and S. H. Schwartz
and for the three irrelevant items (alpha = 0.30) served as measures of intergroup
bias4.
Readiness to engage in social contact
For each of the following six activitiesthat might be carried out together with another
school, subjects were asked if they would rather participate in the activity with members of the designated other school which they had evaluated or with members of
another, unspecified school: (1) A graduation party, (2) a youth delegation to travel
abroad, (3) an organization for volunteer activities, (4)seminars on social problems,
(5) a week of pre-military training, (6) a programme of extracurricular courses. An
index of readiness for social contact with the designated other school (the outgroup)
was computed by summing the number of activities students preferred to be engaged
in with that school (alpha = 0.90).
Identijication with the ingroup
To measure identification, we summed responses on 7-point scales to the following
three questions: (1) How proud are you of attending your school? (2) How satisfied
are you with having chosen your school? (3) How willing are you to recommend
your school to other potential students? The internal reliability of this index of
identification was alpha = 0.89. Subjects above and below the median on the identification index were defined as constituting the high and low identification groups.
PILOT STUDY
A pilot study was conducted to check whether the manipulation of intergroup similarity was effective, and to validate the a priori classification of items as relevant or
irrelevant to the school context. Thirty-two high school students were randomly
assigned to one of three similarity conditions parallel to the conditions in the main
study: Very high/high/moderately high. The manipulation of similarity differed only
in that the two schools described were not named.
Subjects were asked to evaluate the similarity between the two schools on two
items, using a scale anchored by 0 (not at all similar) and 100 (completely similar).
An ANOVA on the perceived similarity (average of two items) of the schools confirmed the effectiveness of the manipulation. Perceived similarity was highest in the
very high condition ( M = 87.8), lower in the high condition ( M = 77.8), and lower
still in the moderately high condition ( M = 69.8) (F(2,29df) = 8.75, p < 0.05; all
means different by Newman-Keuls test, p <0 .05). As intended, moreover, the perceived level of similarity was quite high in all conditions.
We also assessed the relevance to the school context of the six items used to
measure evaluation of the ingroup and the outgroup in the main study. Subjects
rated the relevance of each item as a determinant of the prestige of a school, using
a 7-point scale. A principal components factor analysis of the responses, constrained
to a two-factor solution, accounted for 57.3 per cent of the variance. The first factor
zyxwvut
Given the low alpha for the index of bias on the irrelevant dimensions, analyses of irrelevant items
were run separately on each item.
Intergroup similarity
zy
zy
589
was composed of the three items classified a priori as irrelevant (31 per cent of
the variance, loadings 0.62 to 0.84) and the second factor of the three items classified
a priori as relevant (26 per cent, loadings 0.48 to 0.82).
RESULTS
Direction of intergroup bias on relevant and irrelevant dimensions
Before examining the effects of similarity on intergroup bias, we sought to establish
whether the usual ingroup favouritism was present here. We hypothesized that (1A)
ingroup favouritism is found on dimensions relevant to the school context, and
(1 B) outgroup favouritism is found on irrelevant dimensions.
In order to measure intergroup bias, the evaluation of the outgroup was subtracted
from the evaluation of the ingroup, for each item. Positive scores on intergroup
bias indicate ingroup favouritism and negative scores indicate outgroup favouritism.
A MANOVA on the bias scores for the six items was significant (F(6,141 df) =
12.76, p < 0.05). Column one of Table 1 presents the ingroup bias scores for each
item, and column two presents the univariate F-values. As hypothesized (1A), ingroup
favouritism was expressed on all three items relevant to the school context. In support
of hypothesis lB, outgroup favouritism was expressed on two of the three items
irrelevant to the school context. The third item showed no bias.
zy
zy
Table 1. Ingroup bias scores and importance ratings for dimensions relevant and irrelevant
to the school context
~
Items
Relevant to the school context
Want to go to college
Consider teachers above average
Study hard
Irrelevant to the school context
Active in youth organizations
Interested in popular music
Involved in politics
Bias
score
0.18
0.46
0.60
0.01
-0.46
-0.37
Univariate
F test
(df1,146)
~~
Importance
ratings
zyxwv
4.30*
13.14*
36.14*
4.92
6.27
4.85
0.00
17.17*
12.80*
3.57
3.16
4.92
*p < 0.05 for the difference between evaluation of the ingroup and the outgroup.
Recall that the rationale for these hypotheses developed in the Introduction was
based on the idea that only dimensions relevant for defining the group affect social
identity. Past theorizing and findings on ingroup and outgroup favouritism referred
to the importance rather than the relevance of dimensions for social identity. In
order to establish whether our findings might reflect the impact of importance rather
than the hypothesized effects of relevance, we examined the importance ratings of
the items. These ratings, shown in column three of Table 1, reveal that - on average
-items relevant to the school context were evaluated as more important than irrelevant items (M = 5.36 and 3.88 respectively, t(152’d = 1 2 . 6 7 , ~< 0.05). However,
one of the items irrelevant to the school context (involvement in politics) was also
590
zyxwv
zyxwvutsr
S. Roccas and S. H. Schwartz
one of the more important items. This suggests at least partial overlap of relevance
with importance in the current study. Moreover, on this item, outgroup favouritism
was shown, as predicted from its irrelevance, rather than ingroup favouritism, as
predicted from its high importance.
Effects of manipulatedsimilarity on intergroup bias
We hypothesized (2) that for group members who identify with their ingroup,
increased similarity on dimensions relevant for defining the ingroup leads to ingroup
favouritism. That is, in the presence of high initial levels of similarity, manipulated
similarity increases ingroup favouritism. Note that this hypotheses is limited to people
who identify with the ingroup and refers to dimensions relevant for defining the
group. The relevant findings are presented in Table 2. The bias indexes are the
means of the weighted difference scores for the sets of school relevant items. Positive
scores indicate ingroup favouritism, negative scores outgroup favouritism.
zyxw
Table 2. Intergroup bias on dimensions relevant to group definition as a function of similarity
level and identification with the ingroup
Level of manipulated similarity
High
Very high
High
Moderately high
2.99
1.95
-0.20
Identification
(N)
Low
(N)
(30)
(26)
(33)
(22)
(21)
(17)
5.29
3.04
4.98
zyxwvu
zyxw
zyxw
The bias scores in column one indicate that, as predicted, the greater the manipulated similarity between ingroup and outgroup, the greater the ingroup favouritism.
To test the reliability of the predicted difference on ingroup favouritism among those
highly identified with the ingroup, we conducted a planned comparison, using the
error term from the 3 x 2 (similarity X identification) between-subjects ANOVA.
This comparison revealed that, as predicted, highly identified subjects expressed significantly more ingroup favouritism in the very high similarity condition than in the
moderately high similarity condition (t(140df) = 2 . 1 4 , ~< 05)’.
The overall ANOVA also revealed a difference that was not predicted: Those
low in ingroup identification showed stronger ingroup bias than those high in identification (F(1,140 df) = 7.92, p < 0.05). Phrased differently, the less strongly students
identified with their school, the more they biased their evaluations in favour of
their school6.
Effects of manipulatedsimilarity on readiness for social contact
We hypothesized (3) that increased similarity leads to greater readiness for social
contact with the outgroup, among those who identify with their ingroup. Table
’ Analyses of bias on the irrelevantitems revealed no significanteffect of similarityor of identification.
The other effects in the overall ANOVA (similarity and the two-way interaction) were not significant.
zy
zyxwv
Intergroup similarity
591
3 presents the mean index of readiness for social contact as a function of similarity
level and identification with the ingroup.
Table 3. Readiness for social contact with the outgroup as a function of similarity level
and identificationwith the ingroup
~~
Level of manipulatedsimilarity
Very high
High
Moderately high
~
High
Identification
(N)
Low
(N)
4.67
4.56
4.12
(30)
(26)
(33)
(22)
(21)
(17)
3.39
3.84
4.28
zyxwv
zyxw
As shown in column one of the table, which refers to those highly identified with
their ingroup, the experimental groups were ordered as expected on the index of
readiness for outgroup contact. The appropriate planned comparison, using the error
term from the 3 x 2 (similarity x identification)between-subjectsANOVA, indicated
that those told the two schools were very highly similar, were significantly more
ready to engage in outgroup contact than those told the schools were only moderately
highly similar (t(143 df) = 1 . 6 6 , ~< 0.05)7.
DISCUSSION
The major goal of this research was to examine the impact of intergroup similarity
on two aspects of intergroup relations: Biased perception (evaluation) and readiness
for social contact. We first sought to clarify the types of dimensions on which perceptual bias appears.
Relevance and importance of dimensions of evaluation
Ingroup favouritism was found on dimensions of evaluation relevant to the school
context and outgroup favouritism on two of three dimensions irrelevant to this context. These results parallel Mummendey’s findings for dimensions varying in importance: Ingroup favouritism on dimensions important to the ingroup and outgroup
favouritism on dimensions important to the outgroup (Mummendey and Schreiber,
1983, 1984a; Mummendey and Simon, 1989). Mummendey examined only importance, whereas we measured both importance and relevance. Relevance for group
definition predicted the direction of intergroup bias better than importance did:
Political involvement, which was highly important but irrelevant, yielded outgroup
rather than ingroup favouritism. Our findings raise the possibility that, in general,
the relevance of dimensions rather than their importance accounts for the direction
of intergroup bias. Since relevance and importance are often confounded, research
explicitly designed to tease out possible differences between the effects of these two
variables is necessary.
Social identity theory does not explain why there was outgroup favouritism on
Neither the main effect nor the interaction was significant in the overall ANOVA.
592
zyxwvuts
S. Roccas and S. H. Schwartz
dimensions irrelevant to group definition. We based outgroup favouritism hypothesis
on Wilder’s (1986) idea that subjects may also be motivated by a fairness norm
in intergroup situations. Our findings support the view that subjects compensate
for ingroup favouritism on relevant dimensions with outgroup favouritism on irrelevant dimensions. What conditions affect the tendency to compensate in this way?
To the best of our knowledge, this phenomenon appears robustly only in studies
of existing (non-experimental) groups in which subjects evaluated an outgroup whose
basic characteristics were similar to their own. This suggests that compensating
ingroup with outgroup favouritism may occur only in circumstanceswhere important
bases of identification are shared by the ingroup and the outgroup. A second condition
that may be necessary is the absence of real intergroup conflict (cf. Hinkle and
Brown, 1990). In the presence of such conflict, compensatory outgroup favouritism
may not emerge because the perceived conflict may justify discrimination.
zy
Effects of similarity
We hypothesized that increased intergroup similarity has dual, seemingly opposed,
effects on the two aspects of intergroup relations: It increases readiness for outgroup
contact but also increases bias in favour of the ingroup. This research identified
several conditions that effect the associations of intergroup similarity with these
aspects of intergroup relations. We discuss each condition in turn.
As predicted, subjects who identified with the ingroup showed greater ingroup
favouritism on dimensions relevant to the school context the greater the manipulated
similarity. For subjects who did not identify with the ingroup, ingroup favouritism
did not vary systematically as a function of similaritylevel. These results are congruent
with the social identity theory analysis that high similarity between an ingroup and
an outgroup threatens distinctive social identity (Brown and Abrams, 1986).
In past research, manipulated intergroup similarity had contradictory impacts on
ingroup favouritism. We will integrate past results with our findings to clarify some
conditions that moderate the influence of similarity on intergroup bias.
zyxwvutsr
Type of group
Similarity has promoted intergroup bias in studies of existing groups but not of
groups created for research purposes. The social identity linked to membership in
the latter is transitory and probably of little significance for the self-concept (Abrams
and Hogg, 1988). Intergroup similarity apparently poses a more serious and effective
threat to social and hence to personal identities in existing groups, where emotional
and self-concept investments in membership are greater.
Degree of similarity
Brown and Abrams (1986) found that manipulated similarity led to a lower evaluation
of the outgroup only when subjects were informed that the groups were similar
both in attitudes and status. Brown (1988) interpreted this as indicating that only
high levels of similarity pose a threat to distinctive social identity. Our research
supports this assertion. It also demonstrates a monotonic (positive) relation between
zy
zyxwvut
zyxw
zy
Intergroup similarity
593
similarity and intergroup bias for the first time by manipulating three relatively
high levels of similarity.
Importance of group distinctiveness
One other condition that may influence whether intergroup similarity is experienced
as threatening is the importance of distinctiveness to the group’s justification for
its existence. Political parties, for example, justify their existence by pointing to
their distinctive ideologies. Hence, perceived similarity with another party is likely
to be highly threatening. In contrast, the establishment of a public elementary school
is primarily justified by the number of children in the neighbourhood, not by the
distinctiveness of the goals of the school. Hence, teachers told that their school
is very similar to another may experience little identity threat. This condition has
yet to be studied empirically, but a related idea - the degree to which a group
has a ‘comparativeideology’ -was suggested by Hinkle and Brown (1990).
Identification with the ingroup
Identification with the ingroup was negatively related to ingroup favouritism: Students less identified with their own school showed more ingroup favouritism. This
unpredicted finding replicates findings in previous studies conducted in occupational
settings (Brown, Condor, Wade and Williams, 1986; Brown and Williams, 1984;
Oaker and Brown, 1986). It contrasts, however, with findings in research on political
parties (Kelly, 1988, 1990). We discuss why schools are more similar to work-groups
and then seek to explain this pattern of findings.
Following Kelly (1988), we suggest that social identification is less central to membership in occupational than in political groups. Work-groups are mainly sources
of income and friendship. In contrast, political party membership is largely based
on identification with group goals. For students, schools are clearly more analogous
to work-groups than to political parties. Students rarely enter schools out of personal
identification with the schools’ goals.
Low identification with a group probably reflects dissatisfaction with the benefits
of group membership, including identity benefits. Tajfel(l978) suggested three alternative member responses when a group does not contribute positively to social identity. (1) Leave the group, if possible; (2) engage in social action to promote desirable
changes, if leaving is impossible, or (3) reinterpret group attributes so that unwelcome
features are either justified or made acceptable. In both work-groups and schools,
dissatisfied members usually face serious barriers to leaving, inclining them to engage
in social action or, more likely given the constraints, in reinterpretation. In contrast,
it is usually much easier to quit or decrease investment in a political party.
These responses may explain the negative relation between identification and
ingroup favouritism among students and work-group members. Dissatisfied group
members, unable to leave or to change the system, report low identification. When
evaluating the group, however, they reinterpret its attributes, viewing them as more
positive than the attributes of similar groups of which they are not members. This
ingroup bias enables them to achieve some degree of positive social identity despite
their dissatisfaction with their own group.
zyxwvutsrq
zyxwv
zyxwvuts
594
S. Roccas and S. H. Schwartz
Readiness to engage in social contact with the outgroup
As with intergroup bias, the effects of manipulated similarity on readiness to engage
in social contact with the outgroup depended upon the degree of identification with
the ingroup. For highly identified subjects, similarity increased readiness for contact.
For not especially identified subjects, readiness for contact was unrelated to similarity
level. The results for readiness for contact are compatible with belief congruence
theory. They join the results of a long series of studies demonstrating that similarity
leads to attraction. This study adds three new points. First, similarity increases readiness for contact even when the similarity concerns a whole group and not a specific
outgroup member. Second, similarity relates monotonically to readiness for contact
across three levels of similarity. Third, similarity increases readiness for contact with
an outgroup even as it simultaneously increases ingroup favouritism at the expense
of the outgroup.
REFERENCES
zyx
Abrams, D. and Hogg, M. (1988). ‘Comments on the motivational bias of self-esteem in
social identity and intergroup discrimination’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 18:
317-334.
Allen, V. L. and Wilder, D. A. (1975). ‘Categorization, belief similarity and group discrimination’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32:971-977.
Brewer, M. B. (1991). ‘The social self:Being the same and different at the same time’, Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17:475482.
Brewer, M. B. and Silver, M. (1978). ‘Ingroup bias as a function of task characteristics’,
European Journal of Social Psychology, 8: 393-400.
Brown, R. J. (1988). Group Processes: Dynamics Within and Between Groups, Basil Blackwell,
London.
Brown, R. J. and Abrams, D. (1986). ‘The effect of intergroup similarity and goal interdependence on intergroup relations’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22:78-92.
Brown, R. J., Condor, S., Wade, G. and Williams, J. (1986). ‘Explainingintergroup differentiation in an industrial organization’, Journal of Occupational Psychology, 59: 273-286.
Brown, R. J. and Williams, J. (1984). ‘Group identification: The same thing to all people?
Human Relations, 37:547-564.
Byrne, D. (1971). The Attraction Paradigm, Academic Press, New York.
Byme, D. and Clore, G. L. (1970). ‘A reinforcement model of evaluation processes’, Personality: An International Journal, l: 103-128.
Codol, J.-P.(1984). ‘Social differentiation and nondifferentiation’. In: Tajfel, H. (Ed.) The
Social Dimension, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Condon, J. W. and Crano, W. D. (1988). ‘Inferred evaluation and the relation between attitude
similarity and interpersonal attraction’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54:
789-797.
Diehl, M. (1988). ‘Social identity and minimal groups: The effects of interpersonal and intergroup attitudinal similarity on intergroup discrimination’, British Journal of Social Psychology, 27:289-300.
Genthner, R. W., Shuntich, R. and Bunting, K. (1975). ‘Racial prejudice, belief similarity
and human aggression’, Journal of Psychology, 91: 229-234.
Hinkle, S. and Brown, R. (1990). ‘Intergroup comparisons and social identity: Some links
and lacunae’. In: Abrams, D. and H o g , M. (Eds) Social Identity Theory: Constructive
and Critical Advances, Wheatsheaf, Hemel Hempstead.
Hogg, M. and Abrams, D. (1988). Social Ident$cations, Routledge, London.
Insko, C., Nacoste, R. and Moe, J. (1983). ‘Belief congruence and racial discrimination:Review
of the evidence and critical evaluation’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 13:153-174.
zyxwvut
zy
Intergroup similarity
zy
zy
595
Kelly, C. (1988). ‘Intergroup differentiation in a political context’, British Journal of Social
Psychology, 27: 319-332.
Kelly, C . (1990). ‘Social identity and intergroup perception in minority-majority contexts’,
Human Relations, 43: 583-592.
Lemaine, G. (1974), ‘Social differentiation and social originality’, European Journal of Social
Psychology, 4: 17-52.
Moghaddam, F. and Stringer, P. (1988). ‘Outgroup similarity and intergroup bias’, Journal
of Social Psychology, 128: 105-1 15.
Mullen, B., Brown, R. and Smith, C. (1992). ‘Ingroup bias as a function of salience, relevance,
and status: An integration’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 103-122.
Mummendey, A. and Schreiber, H. J. (1983). ‘Better or just different? Positive social identity
by discrimination against or by differentiation from outgroups’, European Journal of Social
Psychology, 13: 389-397.
Mummendey, A. and Schreiber, H. J. (1984a). “Different just means ‘better’: Some obvious
and some hidden pathways to in-group favoritism”, British Journal of Social Psychology,
23: 36S367.
Mummendey, A. and Schreiber, H. J. (1984b). ‘Social comparison, similarity and ingroup
favoritism’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 14: 231-233.
Mummendey, A. and Simon, B. (1989). ‘Better or different? 111: The impact of importance
of comparison dimension and relative in-group size upon intergroup discrimination’, British
Journal of Social Psychology, 28: 1-1 6.
Oaker, G. and Brown, R. (1986). ‘Intergroup relations in a hospital setting’, Human Relations,
3 9 767-778.
Rokeach, M. (1960). The Open and ClosedMind, Basic Books, New York.
Skevington, S. (1981). ‘Intergroup relations and nursing’, European Journal of Social Psychology, 11: 43-59.
Snyder, C. R. and Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Uniqueness: The Human Pursuit of Dz@erence,
Plenum Press, New York.
Struch, N. and Schwartz, S. H. (1989). ‘Intergroup aggression: Predictors and distinctiveness
from ingroup bias’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56: 364-373.
Tajfel, H. (1974). ‘Social identity and intergroup behaviour’, Social Science Information, 13:
65-93.
Tajfel, H. (1978). ‘Social categorization, social identity and social comparison’. In: Tajfel,
H. (Ed.) Differentiation Between Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of Intergroup
Relations, Academic Press, London.
Tajfel, H. and Turner, J. C. (1979). ‘The social identity theory of intergroup behavior’. In:
Worchel, S. and Austin, W. G. (Eds) Psychology of Intergroup Relations, Nelson-Hall,
Chicago.
Turner, J. C. (1978). ‘Social comparison, similarity and ingroup favouritism’. In: Tajfel, H.
(Ed.) Differentiation Between Social Groups: Studies in the Social Psychology of Intergroup
Relations, Academic Press, London.
Van Knippenberg, A. (1978). ‘Status differences,comparative relevance and intergroup differentiation’. In: Tajfel, H. (Ed.) Differentiation Between Social Groups: Studies in the Social
Psychology of Intergroup Relations, Academic Press, London.
Wilder, D. A. (1986). ‘Social categorization: Implications for creation and reduction of intergroup bias’. In: Berkowitz, L. (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 19,
Academic Press, Orlando, FL.
zyxwvut
zy
zyxwvut
All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them i