姝 Academy of Management Review
2004, Vol. 29, No. 3, 379–387.
INTRODUCTION TO SPECIAL TOPIC FORUM
THE FUTURE OF WORK MOTIVATION THEORY
RICHARD M. STEERS
RICHARD T. MOWDAY
University of Oregon
DEBRA L. SHAPIRO
University of Maryland
The topic of employee motivation plays a central role in the field of management— both practically and theoretically. Managers see motivation as an integral part of the performance
equation at all levels, while organizational researchers see it as a fundamental building
block in the development of useful theories of
effective management practice. Indeed, the
topic of motivation permeates many of the subfields that compose the study of management,
including leadership, teams, performance management, managerial ethics, decision making,
and organizational change. It is not surprising,
therefore, that this topic has received so much
attention over the past several decades in both
research journals and management periodicals.
Whereas several recent articles have examined how far we have come in researching work
motivation, this special forum focuses on where
we are going.1 That is, we ask the questions:
What is the future of work motivation theories?
What are the critical questions that must be
addressed if progress in the field is to be made?
What is the future research agenda? How can
we extend or modify current models of work
motivation so they continue to be relevant in the
future? And where are entirely new models of
motivation needed to further our understanding
of employee behavior and job performance in
contemporary organizations?
To understand where the field is going, however, we must first understand where it has
been. This introduction represents an overview
of the field of work motivation from a theoretical
standpoint and lays the foundation for the articles that follow.2
The term motivation derives from the Latin
word for movement (movere.) Building on this
concept, Atkinson defines motivation as “the
contemporary (immediate) influence on direction, vigor, and persistence of action” (1964: 2),
while Vroom defines it as “a process governing
choice made by persons . . . among alternative
forms of voluntary activity” (1964: 6). Campbell
and Pritchard suggest that
motivation has to do with a set of independent/
dependent variable relationships that explain
the direction, amplitude, and persistence of an
individual’s behavior, holding constant the effects of aptitude, skill, and understanding of the
task, and the constraints operating in the environment (1976: 63–130).
These and other definitions have three common denominators. They are all principally concerned with factors or events that energize,
channel, and sustain human behavior over time.
In various ways, contemporary theories of work
motivation derive from efforts to explicate with
increasing precision how these three factors interrelate to determine behavior in organizations.
EARLY DEVELOPMENTS IN
MOTIVATION THEORY
The earliest approaches to understanding human motivation date from the time of the Greek
philosophers and focus on the concept of hedo-
We are indebted to the staff of AMR and to the editorial
review panel for their time and effort on behalf of this special forum.
1
For recent reviews of the research literature on work
motivation, see Kanfer (1990), Mitchell (1997), Ambrose and
Kulik (1999), and Mitchell and Daniels (2002).
2
For a more detailed examination of the evolution of work
motivation theories, see Pinder (1998) and Porter, Bigley, and
Steers (2003).
379
380
Academy of Management Review
nism as a principle driving force in behavior.
Individuals were seen as focusing their efforts
on seeking pleasure and avoiding pain. This
principle was later refined and further developed in the works of philosophers like Locke,
Bentham, Mill, and Helvetius, in the seventeenth
and eighteenth centuries.
Toward the end of the nineteenth century, the
issue of motivation began to migrate from the
realm of philosophy to the newly emerging science of psychology. Challenges immediately
arose over the use of hedonism as the basis for
the study of motivation. As Vroom explains, hedonism had
no clear-cut specification of the type of events
that were pleasurable or painful, or even how
these events could be determined for a particular
individual; nor did it make clear how persons
acquired their conceptions of ways of attaining
pleasure or pain, or how the source of pleasure or
pain might be modified by experience. In short,
the hedonistic assumption has no empirical content and was untestable (1964: 10).
As a result, behavioral scientists began searching for more empirically based models to explain motivation.
Among these early models were instinct theories, such as those proposed by James, Freud,
and McDougall. Instead of viewing behavior as
highly rational, these theorists argued that
much behavior resulted from instinct, defined by
McDougall as
“an inherited or innate psychological predisposition which determined its possessor to perceive,
or pay attention to, objects of a certain class, to
experience an emotional excitement of a particular quality upon perceiving such an object,
and to act in regard to it in a particular manner
(1908: 4).
James identified a list of such instincts that included locomotion, curiosity, sociability, fear,
jealousy, and sympathy.
Beginning around the 1920s, however, as increased limitations of the theory began to
emerge, instinct theories began to be replaced
by models based on drive or reinforcement. Led
by such psychologists as Thorndike, Woodworth, and Hull, drive theorists introduced the
concept of learning in motivated behavior and
posited that decisions concerning present or future behaviors are largely influenced by the
consequences of rewards associated with past
behavior. Allport (1954) referred to this as hedo-
July
nism of the past. Past actions that led to positive
outcomes would tend to be repeated, whereas
past actions that led to negative outcomes
would tend to diminish. Thorndike (1911) referred to this as the law of effect, while Hull
(1943) suggested that effort or motivation was
largely determined by drive ⫻ habit.
Skinner (1953) and others later built on these
principles with the introduction of operant conditioning (referred to by some as reinforcement
theories), arguing that, over time, individuals
learn contingent relationships between actions
and their consequences and that these contingencies guide future behavior. Reinforcement
models continue to thrive today as explanatory
vehicles for understanding work motivation and
job performance, as well as in the workplace in
various performance management programs
(e.g., Komaki, 2003).
While psychologists were focusing on instincts and drives, managers were focusing on
more pragmatic issues. A key development here
was the work of Frederick Taylor and his colleagues in the scientific management movement. Coming from an industrial engineering
background, Taylor (1911), along with many of
his associates, focused his attention on the inefficiencies of factory production in an increasingly industrialized age. These colleagues proposed a new and paternalistic approach to
managing workers that relied on a combination
of job training, pay-for-performance incentive
systems, improved employee selection techniques, and job redesign, including the introduction of ergonomics. Far from being exploitative in intent, Taylor and his associates saw
scientific management as an economic boon to
both workers and management through the use
of improved manufacturing techniques, increased operating efficiency, and shared rewards. However, the subsequent rise of an increasingly sophisticated workforce, coupled
with company efforts to maximize productivity
without simultaneously increasing employee rewards, eventually served to discredit this system, leading to the widespread rise of unionization efforts in the 1930s.
Meanwhile, social scientists and managers
began to consider the role of social influences
on behavior in the 1930s. The role of group dynamics and the need to view employees as complex beings with multiple motivational influences were recognized as powerful influences
2004
Steers, Mowday, and Shapiro
on performance. Best noted among these research endeavors are Mayo’s (1933) and Roethlisberger and Dickson’s (1939) works. Bendix
summarized the principle contribution of this
human relations movement by observing that
the “failure to treat workers as human beings
came to be regarded as the cause of low morale,
poor craftsmanship, unresponsiveness, and confusion” (1956: 294). McGregor (1960) later built on
this in his classic early work, The Human Side of
Enterprise.
By the 1950s, several new models of work motivation emerged, which collectively have been
referred to as content theories, since their principal aim was to identify factors associated with
motivation. Included here is Maslow’s (1954)
need hierarchy theory, which suggests that, as
individuals develop, they work their way up a
hierarchy based on the fulfillment of a series of
prioritized needs, including physiological,
safety and security, belongingness, esteem, and
self-actualization. Maslow argued that the first
three needs on the list represent deficiency
needs that people must master before they can
develop into a healthy personality, while the
last two represent growth needs that relate to
individual achievement and the development of
human potential. Alderfer (1972) later adapted
this model to encompass just three needs: existence, relatedness, and growth.
A second need theory of the same era, first
introduced by Murray (1938) but more fully developed by McClelland (1961, 1971), ignored the
concept of a hierarchy and focused instead on
the motivational potency of an array of distinct
and clearly defined needs, including achievement, affiliation, power, and autonomy. McClelland argued that, at any given time, individuals
possess several often competing needs that
serve to motivate behavior when activated. This
contrasts with Maslow’s notion of a steady progression over time up a hypothetical hierarchy
as individuals grow and mature. By far, most of
the attention in McClelland’s model focused on
the needs for achievement (defined as behavior
directed toward competition with a standard of
excellence) and power (defined as a need to
have control over one’s environment). McClelland’s conceptualization offered researchers a
set of clearly defined needs as they related to
workplace behavior, in contrast to Maslow’s
more abstract conceptualizations (e.g., need for
achievement versus need for self-actualization)
381
and, thus, has found considerable popularity in
research on individual factors relating to work
motivation.
While Maslow and McClelland and their colleagues focused on the role of individual differences in motivation, Herzberg (1966; Herzberg,
Mausner, & Snyderman, 1959) sought to understand how work activities and the nature of
one’s job influence motivation and performance.
In his motivation-hygiene theory, Herzberg argued that work motivation is largely influenced
by the extent to which a job is intrinsically challenging and provides opportunities for recognition and reinforcement. Herzberg saw the context surrounding a job (which he referred to as
hygiene factors) as being far more temporal in
terms of leading to satisfaction and future motivation. Herzberg deserves credit for introducing
the field to the role of job design—specifically,
job enrichment—as a key factor in work motivation and job attitudes. In subsequent work,
Hackman and Oldham (1976) and others have
extended this line of research as it relates to
work design, motivation, and job performance,
while others, including Deci (1975; Ryan & Deci,
2000), have articulated theories focusing specifically on task-based intrinsic versus extrinsic
factors in motivation (e.g., self-determination
theory).
THE “GOLDEN AGE” OF
WORK MOTIVATION THEORIES
Beginning in the mid 1960s, a new approach to
the study of work motivation emerged, which
focused on delineating the processes underlying work motivation. Process theories contrast
sharply with the earlier content theories, which
focused on identifying factors associated with
motivation in a relatively static environment.
Process theorists view work motivation from a
dynamic perspective and look for causal relationships across time and events as they relate
to human behavior in the workplace.
Central to the process theory genre is a series
of cognitive theories of motivation that collectively attempt to understand the thought processes that people go through in determining
how to behave in the workplace. In our view, the
theories generated during the late 1960s and
early 1970s make this period something of a
“golden age” of work motivation theories. Never
before and, some would argue, never since has
382
Academy of Management Review
so much progress been made in explicating the
etiology of work motivation.
Perhaps best known of the cognitive theories
is expectancy (or expectancy-valence) theory.
Expectancy theory derives from the early work
of Lewin (1938) and Tolman (1959), who saw behavior as purposeful, goal directed, and largely
based on conscious intentions. Vroom (1964) presented the first systematic formulation of expectancy theory as it related to the workplace.
He argued that employees tend to rationally
evaluate various on-the-job work behaviors
(e.g., working harder) and then choose those behaviors they believe will lead to their most valued work-related rewards and outcomes (e.g., a
promotion). Thus, the attractiveness of a particular task and the energy invested in it will depend a great deal on the extent to which the
employee believes its accomplishment will lead
to valued outcomes.
Porter and Lawler (1968) expanded Vroom’s
initial work to recognize the role of individual
differences (e.g., employee abilities and skills)
and role clarity in linking job effort to actual job
performance. Porter and Lawler also clarified
the relationship between performance and subsequent satisfaction, arguing that this relationship is mediated by the extent and quality of the
rewards employees receive in exchange for
good job performance. Finally, Porter and
Lawler incorporated a feedback loop to recognize learning by employees about past relationships. That is, if superior performance in the
past failed to lead to superior rewards, future
employee effort may suffer as incentives and the
reward system lose credibility in the employee’s
eyes.
Since its initial publication, a number of
scholars have worked to extend or further refine
the basic cognitive expectancy framework to reflect emerging research findings and new theoretical developments (e.g., Kanfer, 1990; Mitchell, 1997). For example, expectancy theory has
been used to study forms of work behavior other
than job performance, including employee absenteeism, turnover, and organizational citizenship behavior (Mobley, 1977; Mowday, Porter, &
Steers, 1982; Organ, 1988; Porter & Steers, 1973;
Steers & Rhodes, 1978). Researchers have also
linked group expectations and social influences
to individual work motivation decisions (Porter,
Lawler, & Hackman, 1975). Finally, basic expectancy principles have been incorporated into
July
several emerging models of cross-cultural influences on work motivation and job performance
(Bhagat & McQuaid, 1982; Earley, 1997; Steers &
Sanchez-Runde, 2001; Triandis, 1995).
In addition to expectancy theory, a number of
other important cognitive theories of work motivation have been developed since the 1960s,
each with its own focus. Adams (1963), for example, introduced equity theory to explain how employees respond both cognitively and behaviorally to perceived unfairness in the workplace
(see also Mowday & Colwell, 2003, and Weick,
Bougon, & Maruyama, 1976). Adams argued that
both conditions of underpayment and overpayment can influence subsequent behavior. Recent work on procedural and distributive justice
further develops this area using the fundamental concept of equity and its consequences (Cropanzano & Rupp, 2003; Folger, 1986; Greenberg,
1993; Sweeney & McFarlin, 1993).
Goal-setting theory also emerged in the late
1960s, as researchers began to discover that the
simple act of specifying targets for behavior enhanced task performance (Locke, 1968, 1996;
Steers & Porter, 1974). Research in this arena
showed that goal specificity, goal difficulty, and
goal commitment each served to enhance task
performance. Based on numerous empirical
studies, Locke and Latham (1990) subsequently
proposed a formal theory of goal setting. Earley
and Erez (1991) later added a time dimension to
this topic by examining the role of cognitive
processing on motivation, while Crown and
Rosse (1995) examined the role of group goals, in
addition to individual goals, on performance.
Applications of goal-setting theory in the form of
individual and team management-by-objectives
programs are now used widely throughout industry (Ambrose & Kulik, 1999).
Finally, this period saw significant developments focusing on the role of social cognition
and self-efficacy on behavior and performance
by such leading researchers as Bandura
(1977a,b, 1997). Bandura proposed a social cognitive theory, suggesting that self-confidence
lies at the heart of an individual’s incentive to
act or to be proactive. Indeed, after a major
review of the research literature on social cognition and self-efficacy, Stajkovic and Luthans
(1998, 2003) found considerable support for the
role of self-efficacy in determining workrelated performance, particularly as moderated by task complexity and locus of control.
2004
Steers, Mowday, and Shapiro
Based on this research, Luthans (2001) has proposed extending this concept into the workplace through a model labeled positive organizational behavior.
RECENT DEVELOPMENTS IN
WORK MOTIVATION
Many of the ideas emerging from the 1960s
and 1970s have subsequently been extended
and further developed to reflect an expanded
pool of research findings and more sophisticated research methods. Indeed, the 1980s witnessed a series of refinements and extensions of
existing theories. For example, researchers
made great strides in conceptual developments
and empirical work focusing on social learning
theory, as they did in new work focusing on
goal-setting theory, job design, reward systems,
punishment, procedural justice, innovation and
creativity, and cross-cultural influences on work
behavior.
However, by the 1990s, intellectual interest
in work motivation theory—at least as measured by journal publications—seemed to decline precipitously. As evidence of this, consider the number of theoretical (as opposed to
empirical) articles published in leading behavioral science journals over the past decade
(e.g., see Ambrose & Kulik, 1999, or Mitchell &
Daniels, 2002). You will find few articles that
focus on genuine theoretical developments in
this area. Instead, you will see minor extensions, empirical tests, or applications of existing theories. While clearly helpful, this hardly
leads to breakthrough developments in our
understanding of the principles underlying
work motivation. At the same time, a review of
the most recent editions of textbooks in the
field of management and organizational behavior reveals that most of the theories discussed date from the 1960s and 1970s, with
only fleeting references to more recent work.
(It is also curious that some early motivation
theories that have subsequently been widely
discredited continue to permeate such texts.)
In short, while other fields of management
research (e.g., leadership, decision making,
negotiations, groups and teams, and organization design) continue to develop conceptually,
substantive theoretical developments focusing on work motivation have not kept pace.
383
An outside observer might conclude from
this situation that either we have lost interest
in the subject of work motivation (perhaps because it is no longer a pressing issue in organizations) or that we solved the work motivation
problem long ago, thereby eliminating the
need for additional work. Neither of these conclusions seems very plausible. On the contrary, in the new economy, replete with its
dot.coms, e-commerce, and increased globalization (as well as the more traditional manufacturing and service firms), a motivated workforce is frequently cited as a hallmark of
competitive advantage. Indeed, MIT economist Lester Thurow (1992) observed over a decade ago that successful companies (and
countries) will compete in the future based
principally on the quality of both their technology and their human resources. A motivated workforce becomes a critical strategic
asset in such competition.3 Why, then, has
there been so little intellectual activity focusing on this important topic? Perhaps we have
yet to develop the breakthrough ideas that can
push us to the next level of understanding.
While theoretical developments on work motivation may have declined in recent years,
the world of work has changed dramatically.
Indeed, one can argue that the past decade
has witnessed greater workplace changes
than any other decade in memory. Companies
are both downsizing and expanding (often at
the same time, in different divisions or levels
of the hierarchy). The workforce is characterized by increased diversity with highly divergent needs and demands. Information technology has frequently changed both the manner
and location of work activities. New organizational forms (such as those found in e-commerce) are now commonplace. Teams are redefining the notion of hierarchy, as well as
traditional power distributions. The use of
contingent workers is on the rise. Managing
knowledge workers continues to perplex experienced managers across divergent industries.
And globalization and the challenges of managing across borders are now the norm instead of the exception.
3
See a special issue of Harvard Business Review (January
2003) focusing on the importance of employee motivation as
a key strategic asset in competition and corporate performance.
384
Academy of Management Review
These changes can have a profound influence
on how companies attempt to attract, retain, and
motivate their employees. Yet we lack new models capable of guiding managerial behavior in
this new era of work. As Cappelli notes, “Most
observers of the corporate world believe that the
traditional relationship between employer and
employee is gone, but there is little understanding of why it ended and even less about what is
replacing that relationship” (1999: 1). We believe
that the time has come to redirect our intellectual energies into discovering new models—and
research toward new models— of work motivation and job performance commensurate with
this new era.
THE ROAD AHEAD
With this in mind, in 2001 AMR issued a call
for papers on the topic of the future of work
motivation. A special seminar was held at the
2001 annual meeting of the Academy of Management to stimulate interest and discussion in the
topic. In response to the call, researchers submitted fifty-six papers, which were subsequently reviewed. In view of space limitations of
the journal, many worthy papers could not be
accommodated. However, following multiple review cycles, six papers emerged that seem to
offer new and useful ideas and insights into
future directions for the theoretical development
of the topic.
What these papers have in common is a
genuine effort to build on existing theories of
work motivation by adapting and extending
them to fit the realities of the changing contemporary workplace. Today’s workplace is
characterized by an increasingly short-term
focus, time as a critical performance variable,
increasing interdependence among employees (often manifested in some form of team
organization), evolving affective responses to
the workplace experience, increasing value
and motive conflicts on the part of employees,
and a clear recognition of the transitory nature
of careers.
The six papers appearing in this special issue
address a variety of issues critical to advancing
our understanding of motivation theory and motivation in the workplace. The first paper, by
Edwin A. Locke and Gary P. Latham, focuses on
the development of metatheories on work motivation. These authors present six recommenda-
July
tions for developing more complex theories of
work motivation that are more valid, more complete, broader in scope, and, by implication,
more useful to practitioners than existing
theories.
In the second, Yitzhak Fried and Linda Haynes
Slowik examine ways in which time factors can
influence goal-setting processes and job performance in work organizations. They argue that
the addition of time as a key variable in goalsetting theory adds to its dynamism and validity
in helping explain employee behavior in increasingly complex, continually evolving work
environments.
Next, Myeong-Gu Seo, Lisa Feldman Barrett,
and Jean M. Bartunek draw on both psychological and neurobiological theories of core affective experiences to identify a set of direct and
indirect paths through which work-related affective feelings can influence three dimensions of
behavioral outcomes: direction, intensity, and
persistence. In addition to direct influence, affective experiences can also influence behavior
indirectly, through their effects on goal level
and goal commitment, as well as on the key
judgment components of motivation relating to
expectancy, utility, and progress.
Ruth Kanfer and Phillip L. Ackerman then use
life-span and adult development theories to facilitate an understanding of the implications of
aging on workplace motivation. Although aging
is generally viewed as leading to declining cognitive and intellectual capabilities, these authors argue that this view may be overly simplistic. Instead, they argue that aging is a more
complex process, in which declining cognitive
abilities are accompanied by growth in other
intellectual abilities, reorganization of motives
and goals, and changing personality traits.
Fully understanding how aging influences motivation, therefore, requires a comprehensive
understanding of the different and often compensatory changes taking place.
Following this, Naomi Ellemers, Dick de
Gilder, and S. Alexander Haslam use selfcategorization theory and social identity processes to examine the ways in which individual
and group processes interact to determine work
motivation. The fact that work in organizations
is increasingly organized around teams suggests it is important to understand how groups
influence work motivation. Their paper explores
how participation in groups can have a power-
2004
Steers, Mowday, and Shapiro
ful influence on motivation above and beyond
what can be understood by focusing exclusively
on individual-level effects.
Finally, Hugo M. Kehr synthesizes several
lines of research on motivation by examining
the influences of explicit and implicit motives
and perceived abilities on motivation in the
workplace using a compensatory model. Kehr’s
model helps answer some intriguing, unresolved questions concerning individual goal attainment and why self-set goals may sometimes
be nonmotivating.
Throughout, these papers contribute to the
long tradition of substantive research and theoretical development in the field of work motivation that benefit both organizational researchers
and practicing managers alike.
REFERENCES
385
Crown, D. F., & Rosse, J. G. 1995. Yours, mine and ours:
Facilitating group productivity through the integration
of individual and group goals. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 64: 138 –150.
Deci, E. L. 1975. Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
Earley, P. C. 1997. Face, harmony, and social structure: An
analysis of organizational behavior across cultures. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Earley, P. C., & Erez, M. 1991. Time dependency effects of
goals and norms: The role of cognitive processing on
motivational models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76:
717–727.
Folger, R. 1986. Rethinking equity theory: A referent cognitions model. In H. W. Beirhoff, R. L. Cohen, & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Justice in social relations: 145–162. New York:
Plenum.
Greenberg, J. 1993. The social side of fairness: Interpersonal
and informational classes or organizational justice. In
R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: Approaching fairness in human resources management: 79 –103.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Adams, S. J. 1963. Towards an understanding of inequity.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67: 422– 436.
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. 1976. Motivation through the
design of work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16: 250 –279.
Alderfer, C. P. 1972. Existence, relatedness, and growth. New
York: Free Press.
Herzberg, F. 1966. Work and the nature of man. Cleveland:
World Publishing.
Allport, G. W. 1954. The historical background of modern
psychology. In G. Lindzey (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. 1959. The motivation to work. New York: Wiley.
Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. 1999. Old friends, new faces:
Motivation research in the 1990s. Journal of Management, 25: 231–292.
Atkinson, J. W. 1964. Introduction to motivation. Princeton, NJ:
Van Nostrand.
Bandura, A. 1977a. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84: 191–215.
Bandura, A. 1977b. Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs,
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. 1997. Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New
York: Freeman.
Hull, C. L. 1943. Principles of behavior. New York: AppletonCentury-Crofts.
Kanfer, R. 1990. Motivation theory and industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. D. Hough
(Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology: 75–170. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
Press.
Komaki, J. 2003. Reinforcement theory at work: Enhancing
and explaining what employees do. In L. W. Porter, G. A.
Bigley, & R. M. Steers (Eds.), Motivation and work behavior (7th ed.): 95–113. Burr Ridge, IL: Irwin/McGraw-Hill.
Bendix, R. 1956. Work and authority in industry. New York:
Wiley.
Lewin, K. 1938. The conceptual representation and the measurement of psychological forces. Durham, NC: Duke
University Press.
Bhagat, R. S., & McQuaid, S. J. 1982. Role of subjective culture
in organizations: A review and directions for future research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67: 653– 685.
Locke, E. A. 1968. Towards a theory of task motivation and
incentives. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3: 157–189.
Campbell, J. P., & Pritchard, R. D. 1976. Motivation theory in
industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational
psychology: 63–130. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Locke, E. A. 1996. Motivation through conscious goal setting.
Applied and Preventive Psychology, 5: 117–124.
Cappelli, P. 1999. The new deal at work. Boston: Harvard
Business School Press.
Cropanzano, R., & Rupp, D. E. 2003. An overview of organizational justice: Implications for work motivation. In
L. W. Porter, G. A. Bigley, & R. M. Steers (Eds.), Motivation
and work behavior (7th ed.): 82–95. Burr Ridge, IL: Irwin/
McGraw-Hill.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. 1990. A theory of goal setting and
task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Luthans, F. 2001. The case for positive organizational behavior. Current Issues in Management: 1(1): 10 –21.
Maslow, A. H. 1954. Motivation and personality. New York:
Harper & Row.
Mayo, E. 1933. The human problems of an industrial civilization. New York: Macmillan.
386
Academy of Management Review
McClelland, D. C. 1961. The achieving society. Princeton, NJ:
Van Nostrand.
McClelland, D. C. 1971. Assessing human motivation. New
York: General Learning Press.
McDougall, W. 1908. An introduction to social psychology.
London: Methuen.
McGregor, D. 1960. The human side of enterprise. New York:
McGraw-Hill.
Mitchell, T. R. 1997. Matching motivational strategies with
organizational contexts. Research in Organizational Behavior, 19: 57–94.
Mitchell, T. R., & Daniels, D. 2002. Motivation. In W. Borman,
D. Ilgen, & R. Klimoski (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook
of psychology. Volume 12: Industrial and organizational
psychology: 225–254. New York: Wiley.
Mobley, W. H. 1977. Intermediate linkages in the relationship
between job satisfaction and employee turnover. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62: 237–240.
Mowday, R. T., & Colwell, K. A. 2003. Employee reactions to
unfair outcomes in the workplace: The contributions of
Adams’ equity theory to understanding work motivation.
In L. W. Porter, G. A. Bigley, & R. M. Steers (Eds.), Motivation and work behavior (7th ed.): 65– 82. Burr Ridge, IL:
Irwin/McGraw-Hill.
Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. 1982. Employeeorganization linkages: The psychology of commitment,
absenteeism, and turnover. New York: Academic
Press.
Murray, H. A. 1938. Exploration in personality. New York:
Oxford University Press.
July
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. 2000. Self-determination theory and
the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55: 68 –78.
Skinner, B. F. 1953. Science and human behavior. New York:
Macmillan.
Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. 1998. Self-efficacy and workrelated performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 124: 240 –261.
Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. 2003. Social cognitive theory
and self-efficacy: Implications for motivation theory and
practice. In L. W. Porter, G. A. Bigley, & R. M. Steers
(Eds.), Motivation and work behavior (7th ed.): 126 –140.
Burr Ridge, IL: Irwin/McGraw-Hill.
Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. 1974. The role of task-goal
attributes in employee performance. Psychological Bulletin, 81: 434 – 452.
Steers, R. M., & Rhodes, S. R. 1978. Major influences on employee attendance: A process model. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 63: 391– 407.
Steers, R. M., & Sanchez-Runde, C. 2001. Culture, motivation,
and work behavior. In M. Gannon & K. Newman (Eds.),
Handbook of cross-cultural management: 190 –215. London: Blackwell.
Sweeney, P. D., & McFarlin, D. B. 1993. Workers’ evaluations
of the “ends” and “means”: An examination of four models of distributive and procedural justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 53: 23–
40.
Taylor, F. 1911. Scientific management. New York: Harper.
Organ, D. W. 1988. Organizational citizenship behavior: The
good soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Thorndike, E. L. 1911. Animal intelligence. New York: Macmillan.
Pinder, C. 1998. Work motivation in organizational behavior.
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Thurow, L. 1992. Head to head: The coming economic battle
among Japan, Europe, and America. New York:
Morrow.
Porter, L. W., Bigley, G. A., & Steers, R. M. 2003. Motivation
and work behavior (7th ed.): Burr Ridge, IL: Irwin/
McGraw-Hill.
Porter, L. W., & Lawler, E. E. 1968. Managerial attitudes and
performance. Homewood, IL: Irwin.
Porter, L. W., Lawler, E. E., & Hackman, J. R. 1975. Behavior in
organizations. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Porter, L. W., & Steers, R. M. 1973. Organizational, work, and
personal factors in employee turnover and absenteeism.
Psychological Bulletin, 80: 151–176.
Roethlisberger, F., & Dickson, W. J. 1939. Management and
the worker. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tolman, E. C. 1959. Principle of purposive behavior. In
S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of science, vol. 2: 239 –
261. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Triandis, H. C. 1995. Motivation and achievement in collectivist and individualistic cultures. In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement,
vol. 9: 1–30. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Vroom, V. H. 1964. Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
Weick, K. E., Bougon, M. G., & Maruyama, G. 1976. The equity
context. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 15: 32– 65.
Richard M. Steers is the Kazumitsu Shiomi Professor of Management in the Lundquist
College of Business, University of Oregon. He received his Ph.D. from the University of
California at Irvine. His current research focuses on employee motivation and crosscultural management.
Richard T. Mowday is the Gerald B. Bashaw Professor of Management in the Lundquist College of Business, University of Oregon. He received his Ph.D. from the
University of California at Irvine and focuses his teaching and research on leadership
in organizations.
2004
Steers, Mowday, and Shapiro
Debra L. Shapiro, formerly the Willard Graham Distinguished Professor of Management at UNC–Chapel Hill, is now professor of management and organization in the
R. H. Smith School of Business, University of Maryland at College Park, and a member
of the Academy of Management’s Board of Governors. She received her Ph.D. from
Northwestern University. Her research focuses on issues regarding how to manage
conflict in organizations that tend to motivate unproductive employee behaviors and
the cross-cultural challenges of managing conflict effectively.
387