Neuroscience Letters 478 (2010) 102–106
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Neuroscience Letters
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet
Localisation of unilateral nasal stimuli across sensory systems
Johannes Frasnelli ∗ , Valérie La Buissonnière Ariza, Olivier Collignon, Franco Lepore
CERNEC, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 2 March 2010
Received in revised form 28 April 2010
Accepted 29 April 2010
Keywords:
Lateralization
Olfactory
Trigeminal
Somatosensory
Chemosensory
a b s t r a c t
Odor stimuli presented to one nostril can only be localised if they additionally activate the trigeminal
nerve’s chemosensitive fibers. In this study we aimed to investigate characteristics in the localisation
of unilateral trigeminal, olfactory and somatosensory nasal stimuli. We compared the ability of healthy
young subjects to localise monorhinally presented (a) pure olfactory stimuli (phenyl ethyl alcohol), (b)
mixed olfactory trigeminal stimuli (eucalyptol), and (c) somatosensory stimuli (air puffs). As expected,
subjects could localise the air puffs and eucalyptol, but could not phenyl ethyl alcohol. Interestingly,
we observed a significant correlation between localisation performance for eucalyptol and phenyl ethyl
alcohol but not between the ability to localise somatosensory and trigeminal or olfactory stimuli. These
observations show that on a behavioural level, the trigeminal chemosensory system is more intimately
connected to the olfactory system than to the somatosensory system despite the fact that anatomically
its information is conveyed via same nerve as the latter. Furthermore, they show that the trigeminal chemosensory system should therefore be considered a self-confined contributor to chemosensory
perception.
© 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
The trigeminal system represents a third chemical sense, next to
smell and taste. Its receptive structures are located in the nasal
and oral cavity; it allows for perception of the burning of chilli,
the cooling of mint, the sparkling of carbonated water and many
more via the activation of specific chemoreceptors (such as TRPV1
[33] or TRPM8 [46]) or free nerve endings [11]. In fact, most odorous substances also activate the trigeminal system, at least in
higher concentrations [12]. One interesting aspect of the intranasal
trigeminal system is that it allows for localisation of monorhinally
presented stimuli. Thus, we are able to correctly localise odorous
stimuli which have been presented to one nostril, only if the substance also activates the trigeminal system [28,29,44]. Accordingly,
we can localise mixed olfactory trigeminal stimulus, such as eucalyptol, but we cannot localise pure odors, such as the rose odor
phenyl ethyl alcohol [15,29]. The term “pure odors” refers to stimuli
which activate exclusively the olfactory nerve, without concomitant trigeminal stimulation. Only few pure odors are known; they
include phenyl ethyl alcohol, vanillin, and decanoic acid [12].
The olfactory and the trigeminal system are closely interconnected. As mentioned above, in higher concentrations, most odors
also stimulate the trigeminal system. Furthermore, simultaneous
stimulation with a trigeminal stimulus decreases the intensity
∗ Corresponding author at: Université de Montréal, Département de Psychologie,
Salle F-475, Pavillon Marie-Victorin, 90, ave. Vincent-d’Indy, Montréal, Québec, H2V
2S9, Canada. Tel.: +1 514 343 6111x10705; fax: +1 514 343 5787.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (J. Frasnelli).
0304-3940/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.04.074
of an odor [9,34], probably due to a central interaction between
both sensory systems [9]. In addition, subjects with a loss of
olfactory function also exhibit a decreased trigeminal sensitivity
[18,21,23]. Interestingly, this decreased sensitivity seems to be
limited to the chemosensory portions of the trigeminal system
only. When chemosensory trigeminal thresholds were compared
between healthy controls and patients with olfactory dysfunction,
the latter exhibited higher thresholds indicating lower sensitivity. When however somatosensory trigeminal thresholds were
compared, no difference between both groups could be observed
[19], although both types of information, i.e., chemosensory and
somatosensory, are conveyed via the same nerve. This suggests
that the close connection between olfactory system and trigeminal system is limited to the chemosensory portions of the latter.
In other words, the two sensory portions of the trigeminal nerve,
i.e., the chemosensory and the somatosensory system seem to be
relatively independent from each other.
Because of these close connections between trigeminal and
olfactory functions and the relative independence between trigeminal and somatosensory functions, we designed a study to further
understand the relations between these sensory systems in our
ability to localise odors. We thus monorhinally presented our subjects with (1) a pure odor, (2) a mixed olfactory trigeminal stimulus,
and (3) a somatosensory stimulus.
We hypothesized that subjects could localise both stimuli which
activate the trigeminal nerve, i.e., the mixed olfactory trigeminal stimulus and the somatosensory stimulus, but not the pure
odor. As a consequence of the relative independence of the dif-
J. Frasnelli et al. / Neuroscience Letters 478 (2010) 102–106
ferent trigeminal fiber subtypes, we hypothesized the results for
the somatosensory and the mixed olfactory trigeminal stimulus not
to be correlated. In contrast, we expected the results for the pure
odorant and the mixed olfactory trigeminal stimulus to be correlated, as an expression of the intimate connection between both
chemosensory systems.
The study was conducted in agreement with the Declaration of
Helsinki. Subjects gave informed written consent prior to testing.
The protocol was approved by the Ethics Board of the University of
Montreal.
Subjects: We included 32 subjects (14 women) aged between 18
and 35 years (mean age 23 ± 3 (SD) years). No participant suffered
of any medical conditions at the time of the testing and did not
report any olfactory problems.
Stimuli: We used pure eucalyptol (eucalyptus odor; Galenova, St.-Hyacinthe, QC) and phenyl ethyl alcohol (rose odor;
SAFC, St. Louis, MO) as chemosensory stimuli, and air puffs
as somatosensory stimuli. Eucalyptol has a distinctive smell; it
can however clearly be perceived by anosmic subjects [23,30],
probably via the trigeminal receptor TRPM8 [2]. It is therefore considered to be a mixed olfactory trigeminal stimulus.
Phenyl ethyl alcohol on the other hand cannot be perceived
by anosmic subjects [12] and is therefore considered a pure
odorant which activates the olfactory nerve exclusively. The air
puffs, on the other hand, activate only somatosensory trigeminal
fibers.
Stimulus presentation: We adapted an fMRI compatible tactile stimulator (Institute for Biomagnetism and Biosignalanalysis,
University of Münster, Germany) in order to present stimuli in
an automated fashion. This portable multi-channel stimulator is
designed for generation and delivery of constant air puffs for
somatosensory stimulation during MEG and fMRI acquisition [14].
The stimulator provides air pressure pulses of well defined duration. Instead of connecting the outlets to balloon diaphragms, as it is
done for tactile stimulation, we connected them to odor chambers
via polyurethane tubing with 8 mm outer diameter and an inner
diameter of 4.8 mm (Fre-Thane 85A, Freelin-Wade, McMinnville,
OR). The odor chambers were glass bottles with a volume of 50 mL
and were filled with 4 mL of odorant. The outlet of the odor chambers was then connected to the subjects’ nose by means of the same
polyurethane tubing of approximately 50 cm length. By keeping all
tubings separated we could avoid cross contamination of odors.
During odor presentation, air with a flow of 2 L/min was switched
into the respective channel. All stimuli lasted 750 ms. Therefore,
subjects were stimulated with 25 mL of air per stimulated nostril.
Procedure: Subjects were blindfolded during the whole experiment. Stimuli were delivered to one nostril (monorhinal
stimulation). When pure odor (phenyl ethyl alcohol) and mixed
olfactory trigeminal (eucalyptol) stimuli were presented to one
nostril, an odor free air puff, equivalent in terms of pressure and
duration, was simultaneously delivered to the other nostril, so that
the subjects could not use somatosensory cues to localise the stimuli. An alerting high pitch (150 ms) was delivered that announced
the arrival of the next stimulus during a time interval of 2–4 s after
the alerting sound. In order to standardise the exploration of the
stimuli, subjects were asked to breathe when hearing the alerting
acoustic signal, hold the breath during stimulus presentation, and
breathe again after they had given their answer. After stimulus presentation, subjects’ task was to press one of two buttons as fast as
they could in order to indicate if they had perceived the stimulus
in the left or the right nostril. The next stimulus cycle started after
a resting period of 8000 ms.
Subjects carried out 2 blocs of 48 pseudo-randomized stimuli
(8 times the 6 different stimuli) and thus received a total of 96
stimuli for the whole experiment (32 each for air puffs, eucalyptol
and phenyl ethyl alcohol).
103
Fig. 1. Mean results (in %) when localising presented air puffs, eucalyptol stimuli
and phenyl ethyl alcohol (PEA) stimuli to the left and the right nostril. The dotted
line represents chance performance.
Stimulus delivery and responses recording were controlled by
the “Presentation” software (Neurobs) running on a HP PC (AMD
Phenom X3 processor) with Windows XP. Performances of the subjects were evaluated in terms of hit rates (proportion of correct
responses).
Statistical analysis: The statistical analysis was performed by
means of SPSS 16.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). First, we compared the
performance against chance level using binomial statistics. Then,
we calculated repeated measures (rm) ANOVA on the dependent
variable “hit rate” with “stimulus” (phenyl ethyl alcohol, eucalyptol, air puff), “nostril” (left, right) as within subject factors. We also
included “sex” (women, men) as between subject factor. We computed post hoc t-tests when the ANOVA indicated significant main
effects. Furthermore, we calculated Spearman’s ranked correlation
coefficient between scores obtained for the different stimuli as well
as scores for the left and right nostril. In order to estimate task
accuracy we computed the sensitivity index d′ and response bias
criterion c, according to the signal detection theory [40]. Criterion
c can range from −1 to +1. A c of 0 denotes no tendency; negative values signify a tendency to the right, positive values signify a
tendency to the left. Significance level was set at 0.05.
Air puffs and eucalyptol were localised above chance (binomial;
air puffs: p < 0.001; eucalyptol: p = 0.03) while phenyl ethyl alcohol
was localised at chance.
In the rmANOVA we observed a significant effect of “stimulus” (F[2,58] = 110; p < 0.001), indicating that air puffs were better
localised than eucalyptol (air puffs: 91.3 [SEM: 1.9]%; eucalyptol:
68.1 [3.2]%; post hoc: p < 0.001) and that eucalyptol was better
localised than phenyl ethyl alcohol stimuli (41.8 [3.2]%; post hoc:
p = 0.001).
The factor “side” failed to reach significance (F[1,30] = 3.1;
p = 0.088). However, side differences have been reported in earlier studies; we thus compared the results for both nostrils for any
given stimulus. In fact, when using eucalyptol, subjects performed
significantly better on the right nostril (75.4 [3.6]%) than on the
left nostril (60.9 [4.8]%, p = 0.017; uncorrected). Although we also
observed higher scores on the right nostril for the other stimuli,
these differences were not significant (Fig. 1).
We did not observe sex differences and no significant interactions between factors.
Since the results for air puffs were not normally distributed
(Kolmogorov–Sminorff: p = 0.047), we next computed Spearman’s
ranked coefficient in order to investigated whether the scores
obtained for the different stimuli were correlated. Here, we
observed the results for eucalyptol and phenyl ethyl alcohol to
be correlated (rho[32] = 0.458; p = 0.008). In contrast, scores for air
puffs were not correlated neither to eucalyptol nor phenyl ethyl
alcohol scores (Fig. 2).
When looking at the single nostrils, we observed a significant
correlation for the results on the left and the right nostril, when air
puffs were used as stimuli (rho[32] = 0.363; p = 0.041), indicating
that subjects who performed well when localising air puffs on the
104
J. Frasnelli et al. / Neuroscience Letters 478 (2010) 102–106
Fig. 2. Individual scores when localising monorhinally presented stimuli. On the y-axis, the scores for eucalyptol are represented. On the x-axis scores for air puffs (left
diagram) and phenyl ethyl alcohol (right diagram) are depicted. Dotted lines represent chance performance.
right nostril, were good in doing so with left sided stimulation, too.
No such correlation between left and right nostril was observed
for eucalyptol or phenyl ethyl alcohol. In contrast, we observed a
significant correlation between results for eucalyptol and phenyl
ethyl alcohol stimulation in the left nostril (r[32] = 0.491; p = 0.004)
and in the right nostril (rho[32] = 0.509; p = 0.003). This indicates
that subjects were better in localising eucalyptol in a given nostril
also performed better for phenyl ethyl alcohol in the same nostril.
We did not observe such a correlation between air puffs and both
chemosensory stimulations.
On average, we observed a rightward tendency in all conditions,
which was smallest for the air puffs (−0.034), larger for phenyl ethyl
alcohol (−0.13), and largest for eucalyptol (−0.24). We observed
the rightward tendency to be significantly correlated for eucalyptol
and phenyl ethyl alcohol (r[32] = 0.57; p = 0.001). There was no such
correlation for air puffs.
For the present study, we were able to design a fully automated
delivery system to carry out an olfactory localisation task. Compared to the usually used manual devices [23,45], an automated
delivery system has the advantage to not be influenced by subjecttester interactions and to ensure perfect time-control.
By using this device we compared for the, to the best of our
knowledge, first time, subjects’ ability to localise monorhinally
presented pure olfactory stimuli, trigeminal chemosensory stimuli, and trigeminal somatosensory stimuli in the same study. We
observe participants to be able to localise eucalyptol stimuli and
air puffs, but not phenyl ethyl alcohol stimuli. This is in line with
previous reports on the ability of humans to localise chemosensory
stimuli. In contrast to, e.g., rats [36], we are able to localise odorous stimuli only if they additionally stimulate the trigeminal nerve
[15,28,29,44,49]. In the present study for the first time we analyzed
correlations between performances in different sensory systems
while localising monorhinal stimuli; interestingly, we observed the
results for both chemosensory stimuli (phenyl ethyl alcohol and
eucalyptol), but not for eucalyptol and air puffs to be significantly
correlated. This correlation between scores obtained with eucalyptol and phenyl ethyl alcohol is notable if one considers the fact that,
on average, subjects’ performance when localising the pure olfactory stimulus phenyl ethyl alcohol was slightly below chance, in
line with earlier reports [15,20,39]. In order to explore this correlation closer, we looked at subsets of subjects. We observed the
10 subjects performing above chance with phenyl ethyl alcohol as
stimulus, with an average score of 62%, to also have a superior average performance of 80% with eucalyptol as the stimulus. Thus one
may speculate that subjects who are very sensitive when localising
eucalyptol may in fact also be able to localise phenyl ethyl alcohol.
Those 10 subjects with the lowest scores for phenyl ethyl alcohol
(average score: 21%) on the other hand could localise eucalyptol in
only 58% of the trials. Their score for phenyl ethyl alcohol was so
low that one could speculate that they are actually able to localise
it, but to the wrong side. Keeping this in mind, it would be interesting to know if any of these subject groups could be trained to
localise phenyl ethyl alcohol, e.g., in a feedback paradigm [45].
We know that the olfactory and the trigeminal system suppress
and enhance each other mutually [9,34]. One could speculate that,
in analogy, the olfactory input may reduce also the somatosensory
sensation. In this scenario, on the stimulated side, the olfactory
stimulation would lead to a reduced somatosensory sensation as
compared to the other nostril, where only an air puff was delivered.
If no additional chemosensory trigeminal input occurs simultaneously, the subject may localise the sensation on the side where
no olfactory stimulation had taken place, and thus to the wrong
side. There is further evidence that olfactory input may interfere
with odor localisation. In a study on the trigeminal properties of
the putative human pheromone androstadienone (AND), Boyle and
colleagues observed that subjects who were anosmic to AND were
better in localising it in a paradigm similar to the one we used. Subjects who could perceive the olfactory components of AND however
performed poorer localising AND [5]. Further research is needed to
put these different observations into one theoretical framework.
We observed a rightward response tendency, in line with
research from other sensory areas such as audition [13,31,32]. In
the visual domain, when subjects bisect horizontal lines, they generally lateralize the vertical center to the left. This is thought to
represent a rightward shift in the perceived location of this central point [3,6,7], reflecting a structural specialization of the right
cerebral hemisphere for spatial attention [35], which induces a
tendency to localise uncertain spatial percept to the weaker right
hemifield. However we observed the tendency to the right to be
only minimal for the somatosensory stimulus and most prominent for the mixed olfactory trigeminal chemosensory stimulus.
Again, we observed a significant correlation between the response
bias (rightward tendency) for both chemosensory stimuli, but not
for the somatosensory stimuli. A right-sided advantage/tendency
in the olfactory system has been found with regards to olfactory
discrimination [37,47], olfactory thresholds [8], and odor memory [27]. In addition, there is a right hemispheric predominance in
olfactory processing [17,38,48]. In addition to these olfactory tasks,
there is also a rightward tendency in tasks involving the trigeminal
chemosensory system [15]. We know that even pure trigeminal
chemosensory stimuli in addition to somatosensory brain areas
activate brain regions which are usually involved in the processing
of olfactory stimuli, including orbitofrontal, piriform and insular
cortex [1,4,22], mainly in the right hemisphere [4,24]. An intimate
J. Frasnelli et al. / Neuroscience Letters 478 (2010) 102–106
connection between olfactory and chemosensory has also been
shown functionally. If the sense of smell is impaired trigeminal
chemosensory sensitivity is also reduced. This has been shown
by means of psychophysical methods [18,23], electrophysiological measures [18,21], and brain imaging techniques [26]. From our
results one could therefore conclude that side effects are another
common feature of the trigeminal chemosensory system and the
olfactory system.
It is unquestionable that a successful localisation of eucalyptol is based on the activation of intranasal trigeminal fibers. Here
we compared subjects’ ability to localise trigeminally mediated
chemosensory and somatosensory stimuli. Interestingly we did not
observe a correlation in subjects’ ability to localise air puffs and
chemosensory stimuli, although in both cases the necessary information is conveyed via the same cranial nerve. However, there are a
number of reports on dissociations between the chemosensory and
the somatosensory portions of the trigeminal nerve. We know, for
example, that different regions of the nasal mucosa respond differently to chemosensory and somatosensory trigeminal stimulation.
After stimulation with carbon dioxide (chemosensory stimulation)
larger cerebral electrophysiological responses and greater intensity ratings were obtained after stimulation of the anterior portion
of the nasal cavity, when compared to the posterior one. For air
puffs (somatosensory stimulation), this was the other way round
[16]. A similar dissociation between somatosensory and chemosensory sensitivity has been observed in patients with anosmia, who
are known to also exhibit reduced chemosensory trigeminal sensitivity [21,23,42,43]. Thus, as expected, patients with anosmia
exhibited higher chemosensory thresholds when compared to controls. However, patients and controls had similar thresholds to
(somatosensory) electrical cutaneous stimulation [19]. Finally, differences between chemosensory and somatosensory trigeminal
perception have been described with regards to brain activation
patterns. Intranasal trigeminal chemosensory stimulation leads to
activations of brain regions which are usually activated after olfactory and/or gustatory stimulation, such as the orbitofrontal cortex,
piriform cortex and insula [1,4]. After somatosensory stimulation
activation of these brain areas is usually not observed [10]. In
fact, a direct comparison between chemical (carbon dioxide) and
mechanical (air puffs) trigeminal stimulation revealed that the former led to a significantly higher activation in the left insula and the
right frontal lobe [25].
In summary, we show that the ability to correctly localise a
monorhinally presented trigeminal chemosensory stimulus is not
related to the ability to localise a somatosensory stimulus but rather
to the localisation of an olfactory stimulus. Both the localisation
of trigeminal chemosensory stimuli and olfactory stimuli share a
tendency for a right-sided bias. From the literature we know that
the chemosensory trigeminal and the olfactory system share neuroanatomical [1,4] and functional [21,23,41,43] characteristics. On
the other hand, the somatosensory and the chemosensory portions of the trigeminal nerve have been shown to exhibit different
characteristics on neuroanatomical [25] and functional [19] levels.
Together with earlier reports our results therefore further support
the notion of an intimate connection between the chemosensory
trigeminal and olfactory systems.
References
[1] J. Albrecht, R. Kopietz, J. Frasnelli, M. Wiesmann, T. Hummel, J.N. Lundström,
The neuronal correlates of intranasal trigeminal function—an ALE metaanalysis
of human functional brain imaging data, Brain Res. Rev. 62 (2010) 183–196.
[2] H.J. Behrendt, T. Germann, C. Gillen, H. Hatt, R. Jostock, Characterization of the
mouse cold-menthol receptor TRPM8 and vanilloid receptor type-1 VR1 using
a fluorometric imaging plate reader (FLIPR) assay, Br. J. Pharmacol. 141 (2004)
737–745.
[3] D. Bowers, K.M. Heilman, Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line
bisection task, Neuropsychologia 18 (1980) 491–498.
105
[4] J.A. Boyle, M. Heinke, J. Gerber, J. Frasnelli, T. Hummel, Cerebral activation
to intranasal chemosensory trigeminal stimulation, Chem. Senses 32 (2007)
343–353.
[5] J.A. Boyle, J.N. Lundstrom, M. Knecht, M. Jones-Gotman, B. Schaal, T. Hummel,
On the trigeminal percept of androstenone and its implications on the rate of
specific anosmia, J. Neurobiol. 66 (2006) 1501–1510.
[6] J.L. Bradshaw, N.C. Nettleton, G. Nathan, L. Wilson, Bisecting rods and lines:
effects of horizontal and vertical posture on left-side underestimation by normal subjects, Neuropsychologia 23 (1985) 421–425.
[7] J.L. Bradshaw, N.C. Nettleton, G. Nathan, L. Wilson, Head and body space to left
and right, front and rear. II. Visuotactual and kinesthetic studies and left-side
underestimation, Neuropsychologia 21 (1983) 475–486.
[8] W.S. Cain, J.F. Gent, Olfactory sensitivity: reliability, generality, and association
with aging, J. Exp. Psychol. 17 (1991) 382–391.
[9] W.S Cain, C.L. Murphy, Interaction between chemoreceptive modalities of
odour and irritation, Nature 284 (1980) 255–257.
[10] A.F. DaSilva, L. Becerra, N. Makris, A.M. Strassman, R.G. Gonzalez, N. Geatrakis,
D. Borsook, Somatotopic activation in the human trigeminal pain pathway, J.
Neurosci. 22 (2002) 8183–8192.
[11] W.W. Dawson, Chemical stimulation of the peripheral trigeminal nerve, Nature
196 (1962) 341–345.
[12] R.L. Doty, W.P.E. Brugger, P.C. Jurs, M.A. Orndorff, P.J. Snyder, L.D. Lowry,
Intranasal trigeminal stimulation from odorous volatiles: psychometric
responses from anosmic and normal humans, Physiol. Behav. 20 (1978)
175–185.
[13] A. Dufour, P. Touzalin, V. Candas, Rightward shift of the auditory subjective
straight ahead in right- and left-handed subjects, Neuropsychologia 45 (2007)
447–453.
[14] T. Elbert, H. Flor, N. Birbaumer, S. Knecht, S. Hampson, W. Larbig, E. Taub, Extensive reorganization of the somatosensory cortex in adult humans after nervous
system injury, Neuroreport 5 (1994) 2593–2597.
[15] J. Frasnelli, G. Charbonneau, O. Collignon, F. Lepore, Odor localization and sniffing, Chem. Senses 34 (2009) 139–144.
[16] J. Frasnelli, S. Heilmann, T. Hummel, Responsiveness of human nasal mucosa
to trigeminal stimuli depends on the site of stimulation, Neurosci. Lett. 362
(2004) 65–69.
[17] J. Frasnelli, J.N. Lundstrom, J.A. Boyle, J. Djordjevic, R.J. Zatorre, M. JonesGotman, Neuroanatomical correlates of olfactory performance, Exp. Brain Res.
201 (2010) 1–11.
[18] J. Frasnelli, B. Schuster, T. Hummel, Interactions between olfaction and the
trigeminal system: what can be learned from olfactory loss, Cereb. Cortex 17
(2007) 2268–2275.
[19] J. Frasnelli, B. Schuster, T. Zahnert, T. Hummel, Chemosensory specific reduction
of trigeminal sensitivity in subjects with olfactory dysfunction, Neuroscience
142 (2006) 541–546.
[20] J. Frasnelli, M. Ungermann, T. Hummel, Ortho- and retronasal presentation of
olfactory stimuli modulates odor percepts, Chemosens. Percept. 1 (2008) 9–15.
[21] T. Hummel, S. Barz, J. Lotsch, S. Roscher, B. Kettenmann, G. Kobal, Loss of olfactory function leads to a decrease of trigeminal sensitivity, Chem. Senses 21
(1996) 75–79.
[22] T. Hummel, R.L. Doty, D.M. Yousem, Functional MRI of intranasal chemosensory
trigeminal activation, Chem. Senses 30 (2005) i205–i206.
[23] T. Hummel, T. Futschik, J. Frasnelli, K.B. Huttenbrink, Effects of olfactory function, age, and gender on trigeminally mediated sensations: a study based on the
lateralization of chemosensory stimuli, Toxicol. Lett. 140/141 (2003) 273–280.
[24] T. Hummel, E. Iannilli, J. Frasnelli, J. Boyle, J. Gerber, Central processing of
trigeminal activation in humans, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1170 (2009) 190–195.
[25] E. Iannilli, C. Del Gratta, J.C. Gerber, G.L. Romani, T. Hummel, Trigeminal activation using chemical, electrical, and mechanical stimuli, Pain 139 (2008)
376–388.
[26] E Iannilli, J. Gerber, J. Frasnelli, T. Hummel, Intranasal trigeminal function in
subjects with and without an intact sense of smell, Brain Res. 1139 (2007)
235–244.
[27] M. Jones-Gotman, R.J. Zatorre, Odor recognition memory in humans: role of
right temporal and orbitofrontal regions, Brain Cogn. 22 (1993) 182–198.
[28] A.M. Kleemann, J. Albrecht, V. Schopf, K. Haegler, R. Kopietz, J.M. Hempel, J.
Linn, V.L. Flanagin, G. Fesl, M. Wiesmann, Trigeminal perception is necessary
to localize odors, Physiol. Behav. 97 (2009) 401–405.
[29] G. Kobal, S. Van Toller, T. Hummel, Is there directional smelling? Experientia
45 (1989) 130–132.
[30] M. Laska, H. Distel, R. Hudson, Trigeminal perception of odorant quality in
congenitally anosmic subjects, Chem. Senses 22 (1997) 447–456.
[31] J. Lewald, Gender-specific hemispheric asymmetry in auditory space perception, Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 19 (2004) 92–99.
[32] J. Lewald, H. Foltys, R. Topper, Role of the posterior parietal cortex in spatial
hearing, J. Neurosci. 22 (2002) RC207.
[33] L. Liu, S.A. Simon, Capsaicin, acid and heat-evoked currents in rat trigeminal
ganglion neurons: relationship to functional VR1 receptors, Physiol. Behav. 69
(2000) 363–378.
[34] A Livermore, T. Hummel, G. Kobal, Chemosensory event-related potentials in
the investigation of interactions between the olfactory and the somatosensory (trigeminal) systems, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 83 (1992)
201–210.
[35] M.-M. Mesulam, Attentional networks, confusional states, and neglect syndromes, in: M.-M. Mesulam (Ed.), Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive
Neurology, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2000, pp. 174–256.
106
J. Frasnelli et al. / Neuroscience Letters 478 (2010) 102–106
[36] R. Rajan, J.P. Clement, U.S. Bhalla, Rats smell in stereo, Science 311 (2006)
666–670.
[37] I. Savic, H. Berglund, Right-nostril dominance in discrimination of unfamiliar,
but not familiar, odours, Chem. Senses 25 (2000) 517–523.
[38] I. Savic, B. Gulyas, PET shows that odors are processed both ipsilaterally and
contralaterally to the stimulated nostril, Neuroreport 11 (2000) 2861–2866.
[39] R.A. Schneider, C.E. Schmidt, Dependency of olfactory localization on nonolfactory cues, Physiol. Behav. 2 (1967) 305–309.
[40] J.G. Snodgrass, J. Corwin, Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia, J. Exp. Psychol. 117 (1988) 34–50.
[41] J.C. Stevens, W.S. Cain, Aging and the perception of nasal irritation, Physiol.
Behav. 37 (1986) 323–328.
[42] J.C. Stevens, W.S. Cain, Smelling via the mouth: effect of aging, Percept. Psychophys. 40 (1986) 142–146.
[43] J.C. Stevens, A. Plantinga, W.S. Cain, Reduction of odor and nasal pungency
associated with aging, Neurobiol. Aging 3 (1982) 125–132.
[44] E. von Skramlik, Über die Lokalisation der Empfindungen bei den niederen
Sinnen, Z. Sinnesphysiol. 56 (1924) 69.
[45] C.J. Wysocki, B.J. Cowart, T. Radil, Nasal trigeminal chemosensitivity across the
adult life span, Percept. Psychophys. 65 (2003) 115–122.
[46] K.L. Zanotto, A.W. Merrill, M.I. Carstens, E. Carstens, Neurons in superficial
trigeminal subnucleus caudalis responsive to oral cooling, menthol, and other
irritant stimuli, J. Neurophysiol. 97 (2007) 966–978.
[47] R.J Zatorre, M. Jones-Gotman, Right-nostril advantage for discrimination of
odors, Percept. Psychophys. 47 (1990) 526–531.
[48] R.J. Zatorre, M. Jones-Gotman, A.C. Evans, E. Meyer, Functional localization and
lateralization of human olfactory cortex, Nature 360 (1992) 339–340.
[49] R. Zernecke, A.M. Kleemann, K. Haegler, J. Albrecht, B. Vollmer, J. Linn, H. Bruckmann, M. Wiesmann, Chemosensory properties of human sweat, Chem. Senses
35 (2010) 101–108.