HYPOTHESIS
THE ROLE OF MINDFULNESS IN POSITIVE REAPPRAISAL
Eric Garland, MSW, LCSW,1# Susan Gaylord, PhD,2 and Jongbae Park, KMD, PhD3
Mindfulness meditation is increasingly well known for therapeutic efficacy in a variety of illnesses and conditions, but its
mechanism of action is still under debate in scientific circles. In
this paper, we propose a hypothetical causal model that argues
for the role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal coping. Positive reappraisal is a critical component of meaning-based coping
that enables individuals to adapt successfully to stressful life
events. Mindfulness, as a metacognitive form of awareness, involves the process of decentering, a shifting of cognitive sets that
enables alternate appraisals of life events. We review the concept
of positive reappraisal in transactional stress and coping theory,
then describe research and traditional literature related to mindfulness and positive reappraisal, and detail the central role of
mindfulness in the reappraisal process. With this understanding,
we present a causal model explicating the proposed mechanism.
The discussion has implications for clinical practice, suggesting
how mindfulness-based integrative medicine interventions can
be designed to support adaptive coping processes.
INTRODUCTION
Among the many interventions at the forefront of integrative
medicine, mindfulness meditation is increasingly well regarded
for its therapeutic efficacy in a broad range of illness, conditions,
and settings. Although the operationalization of the construct is
still under debate in academic circles, simply described, mindfulness is a mode of awareness characterized by a present-centered attention to raw experience liberated from cognitive
abstractions and preoccupations.1,2 Although mindfulness
practice is at the heart of ancient Buddhist traditions, and as such
has been practiced, analyzed, and debated for centuries, it is only
within the past decade that mindfulness has received significant
attention in the medical and psychological literatures. Indeed,
there is mounting empirical evidence of the role of mindfulness
in reducing stress and improving health outcomes across conditions as diverse as anxiety,3 depression,4 anger,5 cancer,6 substance abuse,7 fibromyalgia,8 and even psoriasis.9 Despite growing evidence for the clinical utility of mindfulness, there is
considerable debate in scientific circles over its therapeutic
mechanism of action.
The practice of mindfulness involves a variety of meditative
techniques designed to focus attention on experience in the
present moment.10,11 Although it is sometimes assumed that
meditative techniques promote health by triggering a relaxation
response,12 criticism has been leveled against the conceptualization of mindfulness practice as merely a relaxation technique.10
Putatively, the process of mindfulness extricates attention from
being fixated on evaluative language, enabling nonjudgmental,
metacognitive awareness of thoughts and feelings.2,10 This process is described as involving a shift in cognitive sets, known as
decentering—a stepping back from mental experience, which results in the realization that thoughts are veridical.13,14 How this
shift of attentional focus may result in salutogenesis is still unknown. In this paper, we explore a hypothetical mechanism
through which mindfulness promotes health via positive reappraisal, a form of meaning-based coping.
Since the work of the great medical sociologist Antonovsky,15
it has been empirically demonstrated that coping with adversity
is a critical component of health. Of the many forms of coping
outlined by Lazarus and Folkman16 in their seminal transactional theory of stress, the construct of positive reappraisal is
especially salient. Positive reappraisal, a form of meaning-based
coping, is the adaptive process by which stressful events are
reconstrued as benign, valuable, or beneficial. Research has demonstrated that the ability to find benefit from adversity is associated with improved health outcomes.17-19 In spite of the
known significance of positive reappraisal coping to healing, the
literature has largely ignored the question of how mindfulness
may leverage this adaptive coping process. In this paper, we
propose a hypothetical causal model that delineates the interactional dynamics between mindfulness and positive reappraisal.
Given the relevance of positive reappraisal for health, it behooves the clinician and researcher alike to discover the mechanism by which it operates. How does a person disengage from a
previously established stress appraisal to construct a more adaptive appraisal of their circumstances? Here, we propose that the
mechanism allowing one to shift from stress appraisals to posi-
1 School of Social Work, University of North Carolina - Chapel Hill,
Chapel Hill, North Carolina; and the Department of Physical Medicine
& Rehabilitation, School of Medicine, University of North Carolina Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina
2 Department of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, School of Medicine, University of North Carolina - Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North
Carolina
3 Dept. of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, School of Medicine,
University of North Carolina - Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina
The development of this manuscript was supported by a grant to E.G.
from the George H. Hitchings Fund for Health Research and Science
Education of the Triangle Community Foundation, Durham, NC.
# Corresponding Author. Address:
19 Copper Hill Ct, Durham, NC 27713.
e-mail:
[email protected]
© 2009 by Elsevier Inc. Printed in the United States. All Rights Reserved
ISSN 1550-8307/09/$36.00
Key words: Mindfulness, positive reappraisal, cognitive appraisal, coping, stress
(Explore 2009; 5:37-44. © Elsevier Inc. 2009)
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
doi:10.1016/j.explore.2008.10.001
37
tive reappraisals involves the metacognitive mode of mindfulness, a mode in which thoughts are experienced as transient,
psychological events rather than reflections of absolute reality.
The practice of mindfulness may facilitate and strengthen this
capacity for positive reappraisal.
To understand the role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal,
we first review the concept of positive reappraisal in transactional stress and coping theory. Next, we review research related
to mindfulness and cognitive reappraisal, based on a targeted
search and focused analysis of extant research. We then identify
traditional Buddhist views that implicate the role of mindfulness
in positive reappraisal. Based on this conceptual review, we detail the hypothesized central role of mindfulness in the reappraisal process, presenting a causal model explicating the proposed mechanism. Finally, we discuss implications for clinical
practice, suggesting a potential leverage point, a linchpin with
which mindfulness-based integrative medicine interventions can
be designed to support coping processes.
METHODS
Positive Reappraisal As an Adaptive Coping Process
Lazarus and Folkman16 identified appraisal as central to the
stress process. When a given stimulus is initially appraised as
challenging, harmful, or threatening, an activation of physiological systems involved in the stress response co-occurs with a
subjective experience of distress. For example, one such biological stress pathway involves the stimulation of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenal axis leading to elevated secretion of cortisol.20
Primary appraisal of the stimulus is then followed by a cognitive
process of secondary appraisal in which one’s resources and
coping options are weighed against the perceived demands of
the actual or potential harm. The biopsychosocial sequelae of
the stress reaction result from evaluation of one’s resources as
insufficient to negotiate the challenge presented by the threatening stimulus. Such stress appraisals may result in prolonged
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activation, leading to a disruption in the homeostasis of multiple body systems through
the cortisol-mediated stress response, which in turn is a significant regulatory factor for disease-generating events.21
However, this evaluative process is dynamic and mutable;
new data from the changing environment coupled with novel
information about one’s own reactions to the threat may initiate
a reappraisal process in which one’s original appraisal is changed
as a result of the feedback. A stimulus that was originally appraised as threatening may be reinterpreted as benign, such as in
the case of an encounter with an intimidating stranger who later
turns out to be benevolent. Conversely, what was once held to
be benign may be later appraised as threatening. Thus, the stress
reaction is potentially intensified or attenuated by reappraisals.
Research has found that people often experience positive outcomes from stressful events, even though the events themselves
did not have concretely beneficial resolutions. Such outcomes
may include perceiving benefit from facing adversity22 and feeling as if one had grown from dealing with the stressful event.23,24
These findings are incongruent with the original transactional
model of Lazarus and Folkman.16 In their classic model, positive
affect is the product of successful resolution of the stressful
38
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
event. However, this model does not detail the pathways by
which meaning-based coping generates positive affect in the
midst of unresolved stressful events, nor does it factor the effect
of positive emotion on adaptational responses.25
Positive reappraisal has been found to reduce distress in the
face of a number of medical conditions, including breast cancer,26 amyotrophic lateral sclerosis,27 traumatic brain injury,28
and myocardial infarction,29 among others. This finding is particularly evident in a seminal investigation conducted by Folkman and colleagues,30,31 who used mixed quantitative and qualitative methods to discern the relationship between coping and
stress in caregivers for partners who were terminally ill with
AIDS. They found that many caregivers in their sample coped
with chronic stress by actively striving to construe events as
positive and imbue events with positive meaning. After statistically controlling for the influence of other types of coping,
caregivers’ use of positive reappraisal was significantly correlated
with the experience of positive affect in the time leading up to
and following the death of their partners. Despite the inevitable
illness and death of their loved ones, caregivers used reappraisal
to feel positive emotions in the face of situations that could have
no benign resolution.
Positive reappraisal is an active coping strategy30 rather than a
defense mechanism used to repress or deny. Unlike suppression
of negative emotions, which can cause increased sympathetic
nervous system activation,32 positive reappraisal does not lead to
physiological or psychosocial complications.33,34 In addition,
positive reappraisal is often the first step toward a reengagement
with the stressor event. For instance, a person stricken with a
nonfatal heart attack might positively reappraise the event as an
opportunity to change their lifestyle and subsequently begin to
make changes in diet and exercise behaviors. Alternatively, a
person who has recovered from cancer might view their survival
of the disease as evidence of their strength and resilience, and
they might decide to dedicate their life to helping others make
similar recoveries. Hence, positive reappraisal is an adaptive
rather than an avoidant strategy— one that can be leveraged by
clinicians to optimize the well-being of their clients. Indeed, the
demonstrably efficacious process of cognitive restructuring in
the ubiquitous cognitive therapy of Beck and colleagues35-39
involves positive reappraisal training.
Hypothesized Role of Mindfulness in Positive Reappraisal: a
Metacognitive Framework
Regulating attention intentionally upon consciousness and its
contents1 in a moment-by-moment, nondiscursive, receptive
manner11 induces the naturalistic, metacognitive function of
mindfulness. Mindfulness is naturalistic in that it is an inherent
mental capacity of the human organism, although persons differ
in their ability and willingness to actualize this capacity.2,40
Mindfulness is metacognitive in the sense described by Nelson
et al41: it involves a metalevel of awareness that monitors the
object of cognition while reflecting back upon the processes of
cognition itself. Mindfulness is more like a mode than a trait, in
that attending in a mindful way generates a transitory metacognitive state that remains for as long as that form of attention is
sustained.42 Hence, mindfulness is an innate psychological function that can be fostered by training.
Mindfulness and Reappraisal
The metacognitive stance of mindfulness can moderate the
impact of potentially distressing psychological content through
the mental operation of stepping back from thoughts, emotions,
and sensations. The process by which persons step back from
their experience to shift cognitive sets, alternately termed decentering13 or reperceiving,43 has vital importance to the study of
stress and coping and may represent an unexplained link between appraisal and reappraisal. This link involves a secondorder rather than first-order change, a shift in mental process
rather than in contents.44
In their theoretical model, Shapiro et al43 consider the shift in
perspective involved in reperceiving a “meta mechanism” of
mindfulness.43 Reperceiving is thought to lead to an objectification of— or disidentification from—mental contents. In turn, this
mental shifting from the contents of consciousness to the process of consciousness itself eventuates in a number of directchange mechanisms, including self-regulation; values clarification; cognitive, emotional, and behavioral flexibility; and
exposure.43 Such therapeutic mechanisms may unfold from the
liberation of awareness from fixed or schematized narratives
about self and world. Shapiro et al43(p379) suggest that “through
reperceiving brought about by mindfulness, the stories (e.g.
about who we are, what we like or dislike, our opinions about
others, etc.) that were previously identified with so strongly become simply ‘stories.’” These authors contend that disidentification from socially conditioned narratives enables the selection
of values that are more congruent with the individual. However,
the mental shifting of reperceiving or decentering may afford an
even more fundamental cognitive flexibility. Ultimately, reperceiving may facilitate the flexible selection of cognitive appraisals, as “we become able to reflectively choose what has been
previously reflexively adopted or conditioned.”43(p380)
Thus, we argue that mindful decentering allows for the possibility of positive reappraisal. For one to reconstrue his or her
appraisal of a given event as positive, one must disengage and
withdraw from the initial appraisal into a momentary state of
metacognitive awareness that attenuates semantic evaluations
associated with the event. Hence, the cognitive shifting afforded
by the naturalistic state of mindfulness facilitates the attribution
of new meaning to previously stressful events. Once this state of
mindfulness consciousness is established, one may redefine or
reframe his or her circumstances as meaningful in a way that
engenders hope and resilience. In this way, mindfulness is an
intrinsic and central component of meaning-based coping. Accordingly, if the basic human capacity to make reappraisals involves the action of a naturally occurring mindfulness, then
mindfulness training should augment one’s ability to make positive reappraisals in the face of acute and chronic stressors. The
next section will detail empirical evidence in support of this
hypothesis.
Traditional Buddhist Concepts of Mindfulness That Support
Its Role in Positive Reappraisal
Buddhist understanding of the mechanism of mindfulness training (shamatha in Sanskrit, translated as “peacefully abiding,” or
satipatthana in Pali, translated as “the foundation of mindfulness”) lends theoretical support to its role in positive reappraisal.
Across Theravada, Mahayana, and Tibetan Buddhist traditions,
Mindfulness and Reappraisal
the basic objective of mindfulness is to develop a mind that is
free from obscuration and conflict, that is stable, clear, strong,
and supple.1,45,46
According to Buddhist theory, stability is developed by repeatedly placing one’s mind on an object, so that eventually
one’s attention can remain on the object without distraction.45,47 The object is usually the breath, but it can also be
external, such as a candle flame, or an internal visual image.
During the practice of mindfulness, thoughts often arise, and the
instructions are to simply note them nonjudgmentally, let them
go, and return to the object of meditation. The process of letting
go is central to the Buddhist theory of the beneficial effect of
mindfulness. By noticing and releasing distracting thoughts and
feelings, one is able to let go of clinging to memories of the past
and hopes and fears of the future, based on habitual patterns of
thought. Not only does one begin to perceive thoughts as mere
cognitions rather than reality, but in letting go of the obscuration of thoughts, one is therefore able to perceive and respond
freshly to present-moment experience. Moreover, by letting go
of thoughts and emotions, over and over, there is a reduction in
ego clinging–the serious, big-deal quality of who we think we
are–and a corresponding increase in spontaneity, humor, and
humbleness. In this spaciousness of the present moment, new
ways of perceiving and thinking about situations can arise. This
latter product of the practice of mindfulness is traditionally
known as insight (vipassana or vipashyana).47,48 “Insight is the
higher view that draws conclusions about what awareness
sees.”45(p189) It is in this spacious, present-centered, metacognitive state of mind that positive reappraisal can take place.
But why positive reappraisal? The psychological construct of
positive reappraisal is described as an adaptive coping strategy25
rather than as a defense mechanism, thus “positive” should not
be construed as being blinded by self-centered, wishful thinking.
This same perspective on reappraisal is described in the traditional Buddhist literature as the process of the mind’s becoming
less clouded by ego-centered thinking. The ego-clouded mind is
a self-absorbed state characterized by such negative emotions as
jealousy, resentment, and hatred. In Tibetan Buddhism, strong
negative emotions are characterized as migewa–Sanskrit for nonvirtue–and are considered the product of a weak, confused mind.
The practice of mindfulness results in a strengthening of the
mind, concomitantly decreasing migewa while increasing gewa,
or virtue.45 Virtuous states of mind include such positive qualities as generosity, compassion, gentleness, and patience. Thus,
the strong, peaceful mind cultivated by mindfulness practice
enhances qualities more akin to what is termed “Buddha nature,” or awakened mind–the perception of “things as they are.”
Awakened mind is not a blank or neutral state, but a fundamentally “positive” state of being. The Shambhala Buddhist literature describes this quality as “basic goodness.”49 Such traditional
conceptualizations support the notion of mindfulness’ role in
positive reappraisal.
Empirical Scientific Evidence in Support of the
Hypothesized Mechanism
Fresco et al50 addressed the connection between the ability to
shift cognitive sets and positive reappraisal, examining the correlation between these constructs as part of an attempt to vali-
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
39
date a self-report measure of decentering, the Experiences Questionnaire-Decentering factor. This factor contains items such as
“I can separate myself from my thoughts and feelings” and “I can
observe unpleasant feelings without being drawn into them.”
Data obtained from a sample of 61 college students who were
administered Decentering factor questions and the Emotional
Regulation Questionnaire51 evidenced a significant but modest
correlation of (r ⫽ 0.25, P ⬍ .05) on measures of decentering and
positive reappraisal, indicating that the two constructs are distinct but interrelated.
Coffey and Hartmann52 used structural equation modeling to
depict the mediational relationships between mindfulness and
psychological distress. Two samples of college students (N ⫽
204 and N ⫽ 258) were administered a number of self-report
instruments, including a measure of trait mindfulness, the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale,11 and a measure of emotion regulation, the Repair subscale from the Trait Meta-Mood Scale,53
which includes items related to positive reappraisal, such as “No
matter how badly I feel, I try to think about pleasant things.”
Statistical mediation was tested using a path analysis, which
found that mindfulness exerted a significant indirect effect on
psychological distress through emotion regulation ability. The
authors offer the interpretation that mindfulness leads to increased awareness of negative affective states, alerting the individual to the need to implement coping strategies as a means of
dealing with the stressful event. However, in light of our hypothesized mechanism, an equally plausible but overlooked interpretation is that mindfulness leads to emotion regulation via decentering from emotions and their cognitive antecedents, thereby
enabling the engagement of salutary coping processes. Regardless of the interpretation, it is clear from this research that cognitive emotion regulation strategies (of which positive reappraisal is an exemplar) partially mediate the distress-reducing
action of mindfulness.
In addition to these two cross-sectional studies, a longitudinal
study of the effects of mindfulness training on attentional subsystems provides suggestive support for the hypothesis that
mindfulness undergirds positive reappraisal.54 The attentional
functioning of naïve meditators participating in an eight-week
mindfulness-based stress reduction course was measured by their
performance on the Attention Network Test.55 At posttest, Jha
et al54 found that relative to controls, participants in the mindfulness-based stress reduction course evidenced significantly
greater increases in the ability to shift focus from one object to
the next. Although this study demonstrates that mindfulness
training facilitates visual-orienting functions, mindfulness may
have similar facilitative effects on cognitive switching, since visual and conceptual switches involve interrelated neural networks. Cognitive switching is the ability to shift stimulus response sets encoded as cognitive representations, restructuring
associative processes to adapt to environmental demands.56
Studies using electroencephalographic event–related potential
methodologies and functional magnetic resonance imaging suggest that attentional orienting, whether visual, auditory, or cognitive, is controlled by a common network of brain structures.
These structures, including regions of frontal and parietal cortex,
exert a top-down influence over perceptual processes.57
40
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
Given the findings of Jha and colleagues,54 one can speculate
that the effects of mindfulness training on orienting may generalize to cognitive switching as well, for as Brown et al2 assert,
“Thoughts, then, including mental images, narratives, and other
cognitive phenomena, can be regarded as objects of attention
and awareness, just as are sights, sounds, and other sensory phenomena.” Cognitive reappraisal may be conceptualized as a specific case of attentional reorienting, involving a disengagement
or decentering of the attention from an initial semantic object
(the primary or secondary appraisal) to move to engage a new
semantic object (the reappraisal). If mindfulness meditation
training enhances one’s ability to shift from one visual object to
another, then it is possible that such training may facilitate the
cognitive-semantic shifts involved in positive reappraisal by engaging the metacognitive, nonconceptual, decentered mode of
mindful awareness.
Another recent study of mindfulness training may offer support for the hypothesis that mindfulness undergirds positive
reappraisal via cognitive switching. Chambers et al58 used a waitlist control group design to study the impact of a 10-day mindfulness retreat on novice meditators. In addition to examining
the effect of mindfulness on self-reported affect, the authors
tested the process of attention switching, or the ability to shift
cognitive sets, via a novel experimental task, the internal switching task (IST), which involves maintaining a count of how many
affective words have been serially presented on a computerized
display. During this task, subjects are asked to imagine being
characterized by these emotion words and then ascertain
whether the word was positively or negatively valenced. Subjects
press a key when they have updated their mental count and are
ready to be presented with the next word, and reaction times
between word presentation and key press are recorded. The
mindfulness training group exhibited a significant decrease in
reaction times on the IST from preretreat to postretreat, which
was not observed in the control group. In addition, prereduction/postreduction in reaction times on the IST significantly
correlated with decreases in depressive symptoms. These findings suggest that mindfulness training facilitates attention
switching from negative to positive emotional stimuli, and consistent with our hypothesis, training-related enhancements in
attention switching are associated with improved affect. Such
attention switching may indeed be at the heart of positive reappraisal.
These findings that mindfulness may influence positive reappraisal through attentional mechanisms have support from basic
psychological science. It has been empirically demonstrated that
positive emotional states are strongly related to broadened attentional focus. The work of Isen59 has demonstrated that positive
emotions enhance creative thinking, enlarge semantic categories
to include more disparate concepts, and increase one’s ability to
make meaningful associations. Fredrickson and Branigan60
found that pleasant feelings expand visual attention from a focus
on fine visual details to a more global, holistic view (ie, seeing the
forest before the trees). Recent experimental research by Rowe et
al61 complements and extends this work, showing that positive
affective states result in enhancements in semantic associations
between remotely related terms, and this enhanced associative
process correlated with increased breadth of visual attention.
Mindfulness and Reappraisal
Hence, the relationship between positive emotion and attention
appears to be causal, in that experimentally inducing positive
feelings leads to broadened conceptual-semantic domains and
expanded visual attention. However, it is plausible that this is a
bidirectional causal relation, such that attentional broadening
may also lead to increased capacity for positive emotions. If so,
mindfulness practice, which expands attention, may facilitate
reappraisal by relaxing inhibitory cognitive control, leading to a
generative, flexible, and positively valenced state of mind.
Figure 1. The mindful coping model.
Pilot Research on the Role of Mindfulness Practice in
Positive Reappraisal
Our own research lab is currently conducting a clinical investigation of how mindfulness training ameliorates stress-related
illness through its impact on cognitive coping strategies. Preliminary analyses of two cohorts (N ⫽ 17) of participants in our
mindfulness-based stress and pain management program support our hypothesis that mindfulness undergirds positive reappraisal. Class participants were administered a battery of questionnaires before and after an eight-week intervention based on
the mindfulness program designed by Kabat-Zinn.62 The group
intervention involved training in both formal and informal
mindfulness practice, including sitting and walking meditation,
as well as the use of mindfulness during daily activity.
Self-reported mindfulness was measured with the Five Facet
Mindfulness Questionnaire.63 Positive reappraisal was measured
with items from the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire,64 which asks to what extent one copes with adverse life
events, with thoughts such as “I think I can become a stronger
person as a result of what happened,” “I think I can learn something from the situation,” and “I think that the situation also has
positive sides.” A number of statistical relationships were found
to support our hypotheses that mindfulness facilitates positive
reappraisal. First, paired samples t tests revealed that positive
reappraisal scores significantly increased from preintervention to
postintervention (t ⫽ 3.275, df ⫽ 16, P ⫽ .005), as did overall
mindfulness scores (t ⫽ 5.084, df ⫽ 14, P ⬍ .001). Second,
postintervention mindfulness was found to strongly positively
correlate with postintervention positive reappraisal (r ⫽ 0.786, P
⬍ .001).
These findings, although preliminary and based on a small,
uncontrolled pilot study, have implications for our proposed
theoretical mechanism. Mindfulness training appeared to increase positive reappraisal as a means of coping with stressful
events. By the end of the eight-week intervention, persons with
higher self-reported mindfulness also reported higher levels of
positive reappraisal. Given that the intervention amplified
mindfulness, increased engagement of the mindful coping pathway outlined in this paper may have augmented the use of
positive reappraisal. However, due to a lack of statistical power,
mediational analyses were not conducted to fully test this hypothesis. In addition, due to the uncontrolled nature of our
design, these findings are subject to a number of serious threats
to internal validity, including maturation, history, and testing
threats that render our results tentative at best. Without a comparison group, it is impossible to determine whether positive
reappraisal increased due to mindfulness training, nonspecific
therapeutic factors (eg, attention by a caring instructor, social
Mindfulness and Reappraisal
support, motivation to participate in an intervention), or simply
due to the passage of time. Nevertheless, the observed correlations suggest that future investigation of our hypothesis is warranted using growth curve analysis of a full-scale, randomized
controlled trial to explore the trajectories of positive reappraisal
as mediated by levels of mindfulness.
The Mindful Coping Model
Based on this theoretical proposal and bolstered by empirical
support, we present the following causal model of the role of
mindfulness in the positive reappraisal process (Figure 1). Only
the pathways between mindfulness and positive reappraisal are
detailed in the mindful coping model.
This model builds upon Garland’s earlier conceptual framework of stress, metacognition, and coping.65 As seen in Figure 1,
the transactional model of stress and coping16,30 has been unpacked in the mindful coping model. According to this model,
when a given event is appraised as a threat, harm, or loss that
exceeds one’s capabilities, the individual may initiate an adaptive response by decentering from this stress appraisal into the
mode of mindfulness, wherein one attends to the dynamic process of consciousness itself rather than its contents. This mode
increases attentional flexibility and broadens awareness. From
the vantage point of this expanded, metacognitive awareness,
one can then reappraise the given event in a positive manner by
attributing to it new meaning. This new attribution may arise
either through a conscious process of reflection or a more automatic process, based on spontaneous insight. The reappraisal of
the event then results in positive emotions such as compassion,
trust, confidence, and equanimity, which reduce stress and in
turn influence subsequent appraisal processes.
DISCUSSION
The mindful coping model is designed to elucidate a mechanism
central to emotionally focused coping processes. In so doing, it
provides a conceptual map to guide future research on the underpinnings of positive reappraisal. Additionally, this model
provides both rationale and impetus for viewing mindfulness as
a fulcrum for clinical interventions that, when leveraged, can
bolster the meaning-based coping of clients under duress.
Clinical interventions can be developed to utilize the mindfulness processes inherent in positive reappraisal by explicitly
training clients to first decenter from stress appraisals into a
metacognitive mode, and then to reappraise the stressor event as
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
41
an impetus for growth or a source of benefit. By teaching a client
to intentionally cultivate nondiscursive, receptive metacognition in the face of a stressor, they may more easily detach from
maladaptive appraisals of the event, thereby facilitating new conceptualizations that reduce negative affect and empower the
individual.
In this way, mindfulness meditation can be incorporated into
cognitive therapy to potentiate cognitive restructuring efforts.
This use of mindfulness is innovative. Although mindfulnessbased cognitive therapy integrates the principles of mindfulnessbased stress reduction within the frame of cognitive therapy,13
the approach suggested by our conceptual review would marry
complementary aspects of both treatments, targeting both the
process (via mindfulness training) and content (via cognitive
restructuring) of consciousness. This hybridized therapy would
target the metalevels and object levels of cognition that drive
self-perpetuating “depressive interlock” schemata implicated in
the maintenance of mood disorders.41,66
Despite its potential clinical utility, the mindful coping model
is a work in process and does have limitations. First, the presence
of such a mechanism may be difficult to substantiate due to a
temporal dilemma; initial stimulus perception, appraisal, coping
efforts, and reappraisal processes manifest as a phenomenally
integrated holism. It is likely that the phase of mindfulness consciousness integral to the positive reappraisal process of nonmeditators is extremely brief, thereby making its detection difficult. Second, mindfulness may facilitate coping through other
pathways in addition to positive reappraisal. For example, mindfulness may mitigate stress-related problems by reducing one’s
tendency to utilize maladaptive coping strategies. Lastly, other
executive cognitive control functions may mediate positive reappraisal. Prepotent response inhibition and the updating and
monitoring of working memory representations are important
subcomponent processes of cognitive control that are implicated in the performance of higher-order, complex executive
tasks.67
It must be noted that this paper proposes an apparent paradox: mindfulness is a mode of nonevaluative and nonjudgmental awareness, whereas positive reappraisal attributes a positive
valence to experience. Hence, striving to reconstrue situations as
positive would seem to be contrary to mindfulness. Indeed, this
conceptualization may not wholly accord with classic Buddhist
literature.1 Traditionally, mindfulness would not be used to
make positive reappraisals, because in the Asian cultures in
which it was first described, positivity and negativity are held to
be indivisible aspects of a complementarity.68 The pursuit of
positive experience inevitably results in emotional pain, as the
transience of time leads to change and the loss of what was
sought after.68,69 Loss then drives one in search of the means to
improve his or her situation, which inexorably leads to the same
frustration: a double bind in which the seeker is caught by the
very act of seeking.69,70 In traditional cultural descriptions of the
construct, mindfulness offers the way out of this circular trap of
dualistic thinking,65 attenuating emotional distortions of stimuli
perception by encouraging nonevaluative contact with phenomenological experience.44
Given this traditional conceptualization of nonstriving and
mindfulness, how can the claim be made that mindfulness is a
42
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
critical component in the mechanics of positive reappraisal?
Again, it is our contention that mindfulness facilitates positive
reappraisal in that it allows for a decentered mode of awareness
from which new cognitive appraisals of self and world can be
made. Mindfulness may therefore be seen as a precondition or
initial phase in the reappraisal process but should not be considered synonymous with positive reappraisal itself. We hypothesize that the two components are separable, in much the same
way that mindful awareness is distinct from but a part of loving
kindness meditation. In some cultures, such as Theravada or Zen
Buddhism, that emphasize austerity or asceticism, mindfulness
may not be used to make positive reappraisals, whereas in other
traditions, such as Tibetan Buddhism with its concept of gewa
and migewa, mindfulness might lead to benevolent attributions
about the nature of reality. In Western cultures, where attribution of linguistic meaning seems to be part of the developmental
process of mind in society,71 mindfulness and positive reappraisal may be more tightly coupled. The Western mind is embedded in stories and narratives; although mindfulness may
temporarily suspend evaluative language, it is inevitable that
Westerners will reengage their narratives. Mindfulness may allow for the possibility of a positive reengagement with the conditions of life.
Despite the limitations and cautions discussed above, the implications of this conceptualization of mindfulness and its role
in positive reappraisal are manifold. Randomized controlled trials of mindfulness training could investigate how decentering
partially mediates positive reappraisal, and subsequently, how
positive reappraisal partially mediates the therapeutic impact of
mindfulness training. Such mediational models could be further
explicated by systematic, first-person accounts of how mindfulness training influences emotionally focused coping processes.
Such a mixed methods approach would capture the interpenetrating qualitative and quantitative aspects of the phenomena in
question.
Additionally, the aforementioned hypothesis could guide a
biopsychosocial research program, using neuroimaging technologies, psychophysiology data, and performance-based
psychological tasks to complement self-reports of persons
using positive reappraisal to cope with stressful life events. In
so doing, one could explore the bio-behavioral correlates of
the influence of mindfulness on the positive reappraisal process. Such translational research would further illuminate the
underpinnings of successful adaptation in the face of irresolvable adversity, thereby affording new targets for clinical interventions.
In conclusion, the question of what makes positive reappraisals possible is critical for the application of transactional stress
and coping theory to the practice of integrative medicine. Clinicians are often faced with assisting patients suffering from
stress-related illness, and hence it is essential that practitioners
have tools to prevent, treat, and mitigate these chronic and
pernicious conditions. By adding mindfulness to the clinical
armamentarium, integrative interventions can augment meaning-based coping to improve biopsychosocial outcomes. Mindfulness may enable us to grasp the “handle of cognition” to cope
with and gain agency over our lives.72
Mindfulness and Reappraisal
Acknowledgment
We thank Drs Matthew Howard and Gary Bowen from the
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, School of Social
Work, for their helpful reviews of this manuscript.
REFERENCES
1. Kalupahana DJ. The Principles of Buddhist Psychology. Albany, NY:
State University of New York Press; 1987.
2. Brown KW, Ryan RM, Creswell JD. Mindfulness: theoretical foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychol Inquiry. 2007;18:
211-237.
3. Kabat-Zinn J, Massion AO, Kristeller J, et al. Effectiveness of a
meditation-based stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149:936-943.
4. Teasdale JD, Moore RG, Hayhurst H, Pope M, Williams S, Segal
ZV. Metacognitive awareness and prevention of relapse in depression: empirical evidence. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2002;70:275-287.
5. Speca M, Carlson LE, Goodey E, Angen M. A randomized, wait-list
controlled clinical trial: the effect of a mindfulness meditationbased stress reduction program on mood and symptoms of stress in
cancer outpatients. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:613-622.
6. Carlson LE, Speca M, Faris P, Patel KD. One year pre-post intervention follow-up of psychological, immune, endocrine and blood
pressure outcomes of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in
breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Brain Behav Immun. 2007;21:
1038-1049.
7. Bowen S, Witkiewitz K, Dillworth TM, et al. Mindfulness meditation and substance use in an incarcerated population. Psychol Addict
Behav. 2006;20:343-347.
8. Grossman P, Tiefenthaler-Gilmer U, Raysz A, Kesper U. Mindfulness training as an intervention for fibromyalgia: evidence of
postintervention and 3-year follow-up benefits in well-being. Psychother Psychosom. 2007;76:226-233.
9. Kabat-Zinn J, Wheeler E, Light T, et al. Influence of a mindfulness
meditation-based stress reduction intervention on rates of skin clearing in patients with moderate to severe psoriasis undergoing phototherapy (UVB) and photochemotherapy (PUVA). Psychosom Med.
1998;60:625-632.
10. Bishop SR. What do we really know about mindfulness-based stress
reduction? Psychosom Med. 2002;64:71-83.
11. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: mindfulness
and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84:
822-848.
12. Benson H, Beary JF, Carol MP. The relaxation response. Psychiatry.
1974;37:37-46.
13. Segal Z, Williams JM, Teasdale JD. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2002.
14. Teasdale JD, Segal Z, Williams JM. How does cognitive therapy
prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control
(mindfulness) training help? Behav Res Ther. 1995;33:25-39.
15. Antosky A. Unraveling The Mystery of Health. San Francisco, Calif:
Jossey-Bass; 1987.
16. Lazarus R, Folkman S. Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York, NY:
Springer; 1984.
17. Carver CS, Pozo C, Harris SD, et al. How coping mediates the effect
of optimism on distress: a study of women with early stage breast
cancer. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;65:375-390.
18. Himelein MJ, McElrath JV. Resilient child sexual abuse survivors:
cognitive coping and illusion. Child Abuse Negl. 1996;20:747-758.
19. Major B, Richards C, Cooper ML, Cozzarelli C, Zubek J. Personal
resilience, cognitive appraisals, and coping: an integrative model of
adjustment to abortion. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74:735-752.
Mindfulness and Reappraisal
20. Kaye JM, Lightman SL. Psychological stress and endocrine axes. In:
Vedhara K, Irwin M, eds. Human Psychoneuroimmunology. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press; 2005:53-80.
21. Rosmond R. Role of stress in the pathogenesis of the metabolic
syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:1-10.
22. Affleck G, Tennen H. Construing benefits from adversity: adaptational significance and dispositional underpinnings. J Pers. 1996;64:
899-922.
23. Park CL, Cohen LH, Murch RL. Assessment and prediction of
stress-related growth. J Pers. 1996;64:71-105.
24. Zoellner T, Maercker A. Posttraumatic growth in clinical psychology–a critical review and introduction of a two component model.
Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:626-653.
25. Folkman S, Moskowitz JT. Positive affect and the other side of
coping. Am Psychol. 2000;55:647-654.
26. Manne S, Ostroff J, Winkel G, Goldstein L, Fox K, Grana G. Posttraumatic growth after breast cancer: patient, partner, and couple
perspectives. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:442-454.
27. Young JM, McNicoll P. Against all odds: positive life experiences of
people with advanced amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Health Soc Work.
1998;23:35-43.
28. Moore AD, Stambrook M. Coping strategies and locus of control
following traumatic brain injury: relationship to long-term outcome. Brain Inj. 1992;6:89-94.
29. Santavirta N, Kettunen S, Solovieva S. Coping in spouses of patients
with acute myocardial infarction in the early phase of recovery.
J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2001;16:34-46.
30. Folkman S. Positive psychological states and coping with severe
stress. Soc Sci Med. 1997;45:1207-1221.
31. Moskowitz JT, Folkman S, Collette L, Vittinghoff E. Coping and
mood during AIDS-related caregiving and bereavement. Ann Behav
Med. 1996;18:49-57.
32. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106:95103.
33. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology. 2002;39:281-291.
34. Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JD. Rethinking feelings:
an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:1215-1229.
35. Beck AT. Cognitive therapy. A 30-year retrospective. Am Psychol.
1991;46:368-375.
36. Beck AT. The past and future of cognitive therapy. J Psychother Pract
Res. 1997;6:276-284.
37. Beck AT, Rush AJ. A cognitive model of anxiety formation and
anxiety resolution. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 1985;7:349-365.
38. Butler AC, Chapman JE, Forman EM, Beck AT. The empirical
status of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses.
Clin Psychol Rev. 2006;26:17-31.
39. Rush AJ, Beck AT. Cognitive therapy of depression and suicide.
Am J Psychother. 1978;32:201-219.
40. Goldstein J. One Dharma: The Emerging Western Buddhism. New York,
NY: Harper One; 2002.
41. Nelson TO, Stuart RB, Howard C, Crowley M. Metacognition and
clinical psychology: a preliminary framework for research and practice. Clin Psychol Psychother. 1999;6:73-79.
42. Bishop SR, Lau M, Shapiro S, et al. Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2004;11:230-241.
43. Shapiro SL, Carlson LE, Astin JA, Freedman B. Mechanisms of
mindfulness. J Clin Psychol. 2006;62:373-386.
44. Hayes SC, Wilson KG. Mindfulness: method and process. Clin
Psychol Sci Pract. 2003;10:161-165.
45. Mipham S. Turning the Mind into an Ally. New York, NY: Riverhead
Books; 2003.
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
43
46. Mipham S. Taming the Mind and Walking the Bodhisattva Path. Halifax, NS: Vajradhatu Publications; 2000.
47. Thrangu K. The Practice of Tranquility and Insight. a Guide To Tibetan
Buddhist Meditation. New York, NY: Snow Lion Publications; 1993.
48. Goldstein J. The Experience of Insight: a Natural Unfolding. Santa Cruz,
Calif: Unity Press; 1976.
49. Trungpa C. Shambhala: The Sacred Path of the Warrior. Boston, Mass:
Shambhala Publications; 1985.
50. Fresco DM, Moore MT, van Dulmen MH, et al. Initial psychometric properties of the experiences questionnaire: validation of a selfreport measure of entering. Behav Ther. 2007;38:234-246.
51. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation
processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J
Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85:348-362.
52. Coffey K, Hartman M. Mechanisms of action in the inverse relationship between mindfulness and psychological distress. Complement Health Pract Rev. 2008;13(2):79 –91.
53. Salovey P, Mayer JD, Goldman S, Turvey C, Palfai T, eds. Emotional
Attention, Clarity, and Repair: Exploring Emotional Intelligence Using the
Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association; 1995. Pennebaker JD, ed. Emotion, Disclosure, and
Health.
54. Jha A, Krompinger J, Baime M. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7:109-119.
55. Fan J, McCandliss B, Somer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the
efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:340-347.
56. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A,
Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their
contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000;41:49-100.
57. Kok A, Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M. The control of attention
and actions: current research and future developments. Brain Res.
2006;1105:1-6.
44
EXPLORE January/February 2009, Vol. 5, No. 1
58. Chambers R, Lo BCY, Allen NB. The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research. February 23, 2007.
59. Isen AM. Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior.
Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1987;20:203-253.
60. Fredrickson BL, Branigan C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of
attention and thought-action repertoires. Cogn Emot. 2005;19:313-332.
61. Rowe G, Hirsh JB, Anderson AK. Positive affect increases the breadth
of attentional selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:383-388.
62. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for
chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp
Psychiatry. 1982;4:33-47.
63. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using
self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness.
Assessment. 2006;13:27-45.
64. Garnefski N, Kraaij V. The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire: psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and anxiety in adults. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2007;23:141-149.
65. Garland EL. The meaning of mindfulness: a second-order cybernetics of stress, metacognition, and coping. Complement Health Pract
Rev. 2007;12:15-30.
66. Teasdale JD. Metacognition, mindfulness, and the modification of
mood disorders. Clin Psychol Psychother. 1999;6:146-155.
67. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulusdriven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201-215.
68. Watts A. The Way of Zen. New York, NY: Pantheon Books; 1957.
69. Watts A. Psychotherapy East & West. New York, NY: Random
House; 1961.
70. Bateson G. Steps to an Ecology of Mind. Chicago, Ill: The University of
Chicago Press; 1972.
71. Vygotsky LS. Mind in Society: the Development of Higher Psychological
Processes. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 1978.
72. Depraz N, Varela F, Vermersch P. On Becoming Aware. Philadelphia,
Pa: John Benjamins North America; 2003:163.
Mindfulness and Reappraisal