J. Neurolinguistics. Volume
7. Number 112. pp. 115-131, 1992
Printed in Great Britain
Articulatory
in Ataxic
Loop
0911-6044/92 ss.oo+.oo
PcrgamonPress Ud zyxwvutsr
Dysarthria
Laura Chiacchio
Luigi Trojan0
Fondazione Clinica de1 Lmoro,
Gzmpoli
Clinica Neurologica,
Seconda Facolta ’ di Medicina, Naples zyxwvut
Agostino Cusati
AlessandroFilla
Clinica Neurologica,
Seconda Facolta ’ di Medicina,
Naples
Clinica Neurologica,
Seconda Facolta ’ di Medicina,
Naples
Dario Grossi
Clinica Neurologica, Seconda Facolta ’ di Medicina, Naples
ABSMACT
The aim of the present paper was to investigate articulatory loop in patients with pure motor
speech disturbances. A homogeneous group of 18 patients affected by ataxic dysarthria and
without neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence of cortical damage was s&ctcd. The
study comprised four experiments which assessed: (i) immediate and delayed recall of word
sequences with and without articulatory suppression; (ii) phonological similarity and word length
effects on immediate recall; (iii) articulation rate; and (iv) verbal and spatial span. In agreement
with previous studies on patients affected by different types of dysarthria, articulatory loop was
found hmctional. However, patients had a slower atticulation rate, and it was argued that this
finding can represent a “capacity limitation” of articulatory rehearsal, contributing to the
patients’ slight verbal short-term memory defect,
A widely accepted mode1 of immediate memory is based on the working memory
theory (Badde19 and Hitch 1974; Baddeley 1986). The mode1 foresaw that verbal
short-term memory relied upon a language-related slave system, the so-called articulatory loop. Successively, it has been proposed that the articulatory loop is divided into
two subcomponents (Baddeley et al. 1984; Vallar and Baddeley 1984; Vallar and
Cappa 1987). The former is a passive input phonological store, the latter an active
articulatory rehearsal process. Phonologically coded information can be held in the
116
Journal of Neurolim,
Volume 7, Number 112(1992)
phonological store, and refreshed by the articulatory rehearsal in order to prevent
trace decay; on the other hand, the articulatory rehearsal feeds phonologically recoded
visual information to the store.
According to the working memory model, articulatory processes play a crucial role
in the short-term retention of verbal material, and this assumption is consistent with
many previous experimental studies (Levy 1971; Murray 1975). Recently, some
clinical and neuropsychological studies on patients affected by motor speech disturbances have explored possible consequences of selective artlculatory impairment
(Nebes 1975; Levine et zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIH
al. 1982; Villa and Caltagirone 1984; Baddeley and Wilson
1985; Vallar and Cappa 1987; Rochon er al. in press). All patients were totally mute
or had severe speech-out disturbances, due to cortical, subcortical, or brain-stem
lesions. Almost all studies reported normal memory span even in patients who could
not produce overt speech. Only Rochon et al. (in press) found a consistently lower
verbal span in five left-hemisphere damaged patients affected by so-called “speech
apraxia”. Taken together, these studies suggested that “peripheric” impairments of
articulation did not disrupt articulatory rehearsal, and were not sufficient to determine
a verbal memory deficit. By contrast, “central” disorders of articulation, as well as
those in apraxic patients, were thought to provoke short-term deficits, though not
necessarily excluding articulatory rehearsal.
The papers cited above usually collected together patients who had different anatomical lesions (both “peripheric”, i.e. pontine lesions and “central”, cortical
damage), and whose data at additional tasks (phonological similarity, and word length
effect) were not consistent with each other. Our research was aimed at verification of
this theoretical claim in a more homogeneous group of patients with non-linguistic
motor speech disturbances.
Motor speech disturbances are generally divided into two subgroups (Darley er al.
1975): (i) apraxia of speech, a disorder of the “capacity to program” speech muscular
movements, due to cortical damage; (ii) dysarthria, a group of different speech disorders with failure of muscular “control” and “strength”, not exclusively affecting
speech production. On the basis of speech-out features, it is possible to distinguish six
types of dysarthria, differing in lesion site and pathogenesis (Dailey et al. 1969a, b):
flaccid, spastic, hypokinetic, hyperkinetic, ataxic, and mixed. The above papers on
verbal short-term memory described patients affected by apraxia of speech or by
spastic, flaccid, or mixed dysarthria. Instead, we chose patients affected by ataxic
dysarthria, an articulatory derangement due to altered sequences of incorrect
movements. Such a picture is a symptom of cerebellar dysfunction, independent of
cortical lesions, and of any specific linguistic deficit. In particular, we studied patients
affected by hereditary ataxias, a group of rare diseases, with selective degeneration
of spino-cerebellar system and cerebellum, characterized by ataxia (i.e. failure in
coordination) of the extremities, nystagmus, and dysarthria. Heredoataxic patients
represent a sufficiently homogeneous group of patients suffering from impaired
coordination and control of articulation, phonation, and respiration.
oUrstudyconsistedoffourexperimentpaimedtoascertainwhetherttrcse”pcriferic”,
selective, well characterized, homogeneous, motor speech disturbances could affect
the articulatory loop.
SUBJECTS
We examined a continuous series of twenty-five out-patients affected by heredodegenerative ataxias.
Eighteen subjects were selected without neuropsychological (Token Test; and Raven’s
Coloured Matrices), and neuroimaging (CT scan) evidence of cerebral cortiud
damage. Inclusion criteria were: more than 28/36 at Token Test; more than 22/36 at
Raven’s Coloured Matrices. Seven out of 25 patients (28%) were excluded, and this
figure can be thought of an an indirect expression of the prevalence of dementia in
heredoataxias. Some authors consider it “rare” (like Slaby and Wyatt 1974), while
other authors reported very much higher figures (e.g. Werdelin 1986; but there
dementia was roughly defined).
Thirteen patients (seven male, six female) received a diagnosis of Freidreich’s
disease according to the Ametee Cooperative Study on Friedreich’s disease (Geoftroy
et al, 1976). The mean age was 22.6 years (range 14-33, S.D. 6.4), mean education
was 9.2 years (range 5-13, S.D. 2.2). Three patients (two males, one female)
received a diagnosis of Autosomal Dominant Cerebellar Ataxia (A.D.C.A.), type 1,
according to Harding (1983). Mean age was 46 years (range 35-55), mean education
was 9.1 years (range 8-12). Two patients (two males) had Early Onset Cerebellar
Ataxia with retained tendon reflexes (E.O.C.A.), as defined by Harding (1983). Mean
age was 28.5 years (range 24-32), mean education was 8 years in both of them.
Only four of these patients (age range 16-30; schooling range 8-13), three affectad
by Friedreich’s disease (two male, one female) and one by E.O.C.A., accepted to
perform Experiment 2, which was given as a supplemental task.
Severity of the disease was evaluated by Inherited Ataxias Progression Scale
(I.A.P.S.; Campanella et zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJI
al. 1980), which varies from stage I @symptomatic patient)
to stage IV (patient confined to wheelchair or to bed). Our Friedreich’s disease
patients were grouped into stages II (six subjects), III (three subjects), and IV (four
subjects); A.D.C.A. and E.O.C.A. patients were included in stage II. Mean duration
of illness was 10.7 years (range 2-22) in Friedreich patients, 22 years (range 8-18)
in A.D.C.A., and 26 years (range 22-30) in E.O.C.A. subjects.
Dysarthria is found to be present in early stages of both Friedreich’s disease and
hereditary cerebellar ataxias, and in many cases it can be a debut symptom (Weredelm
1986). All of our patients showed dysarthria to a different degree: from difficulties
118
Journal of Neurdingulstics, Volume 7, Number 112(1992)
and hesitations in pronouncing some complex phonemes to a picture of overt dysarmria.
Fifty normal volunteers, without neurologic or psychiatric diseases, were examined
as controls. Age (tti = 0.1; p = 0.9) and schooling (t = 0.9; p = 0.30) matched
those of patients considered as a whole group (Table 1).
TABLE
1
Subjects
N
Patients
Controls
18
50
Schooling (years)
Age (Y-W
M
S.D.
M
S.D.
27.1
27.5
11.0
10.0
9.1
9.9
2.8
3.2
Patients’ and controls’ mean age and education, with standard deviation (S.D.). Thirteen
(seven male, six female) patients were affected by Friedreich’s disease; three patients (two
male, one female) by autosomal dominant cerebellar ataxia; two patients (male) by early onset
cere-bellar ataxia.
EXPERIMENT
1: IMMEDIATE AND DELAYED
WITHOUT ARTICULATORY
SUPPRESSION
RECALL
WITH AND
In the exploration of articulatory loop in dysarthric patients, one should first
evaluate verbal immediate memory abilities, and verify if patients use the same
strategy as control subjects. The present experiment had this two-fold aim: it
investigated patients’ immediate recall and the effect of articulatory suppression on
delayed recall.
Method
The experiment consisted of three different conditions of serial recall. In each
condition, twenty sequences of three bisyllabic high-frequency concrete words were
presented auditorily at a standard rate of one item/s.
(a) In the first condition, subjects were asked to repeat items immediately after
auditory presentation. Subjects had to reproduce sequences in correct serial order, and
in the case of omissions, they were invited to specify the serial position of items
recalled.
(b) In the second condition, serial recall was requested after a 6-s free interval.
(c) In the third, the 6-s interval was filled with articulatory suppression (i.e. subjects
had to count forward starting from 1 at a regular, quick rate). In these two tasks, recall
instructions were the same as in (a).
The score was the total number of items recalled in correct serial position for each
condition (maximum score 60). All 18 patients were given this test, and Experiments
3and4.
R& zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIHGFEDCBA
a nd Disc ussion
Results are given in Table 2. Patients scored lower in all three conditions, but they
performed similarly to controls. Patients’ and controls’ best performance was at the
first condition: both groups showed a decrease in delayed recall (second condition),
and a greater drop with articulatory suppression. Two-way ANOVA showed evidence
of significant group (& ,& = 38.46,~ = 0.0001) and task (Qj2 = 198.69; p = O.ooOl)
effects. Posr-hoc comparisons were performed by means of the Scheffe’ test. Within
group analysis was consistent with the idea of parallel performance by patients and
controls. In both groups, no significant difference was found between the first and
second condition (patients: zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJ
F = 0.15, and controls: F = 1.59; p = N.S.), while the
differences between the first and third (patients: F = 68.21, and controls: F = 91.05;
p < 0.001). and between the second and third condition (patients: F = 61.84, and
controls: F = 68.54; p < 0.001) were very significant.
TABLE 2
Immediate zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIHGFEDCBA
and Delayed Verbal Recall with and witbout articalatory suppreadon
First Cond.
Patients
Controls
Second Cond.
Third Cond.
M
S.D.
M
S.D.
M
S.D.
56.9
59.4
2.9
1.6
55.6
57.3
3.7
3.0
30.2
43.9
11.0
9.4
Input ordered recall in three conditions: immediate (First Con&), a!& a 6-s free delay
(Second Cond.); after a 6-s delay filled with articulation of irrelevant material (articulatory
suppression: Third Cond.). Means (M) and standard deviations (S.D.) arc shown; maximum
score: 60.
Comparisons between groups showed a significant difference between patients and
controls at the first condition (F = 21.04, p = 0.001). suggesting that patients had a
deficit in verbal immediate memory (mean difference between patients* and controls’
scores: 2.51). Interestingly, only borderline significance was found for patientcontrol comparison at the second condition (F = 3.86; p = 0.54), where patients
showed a less evident decrease (mean difference: 1.72). Finally, in the third condition,
patient-control difference was clearly significant (F = 25.71; p = 0.001); in particular, under articulatory suppression, patients showed a very marked drop in recall
120
Journal
of Neurolingu&tics,Volume 7, Number 112(1992)
accuracy (mean difference: 13.67). Second condition results could account for
significant group-task interaction (F4,,ss = 590.0; p = 0.0001).
The present experiment indicated a verbal memory deficit in dysarthric patients, but
suggested that patients did rehearse items, as well as normal controls. Actually, if
dysarthric patients had chosen a somewhat different strategy, avoiding rehearsal
because of their articulatory troubles, articulatoty suppression would have been ineffective, with similar results at the second and third condition. Vice versa, articulatory
suppression produced a marked detrimental effect, while no difference was found
between immediate recall and the free-interval condition. In other words, patients
could not be considered as normal subjects under articulatory suppression, nohvithstanding their articulatory troubles. These findings were highly suggestive, but did not
definitely prove that patients were using articulatory rehearsal.
Two observations were worthwhile. The former was concerned with the unexpected
reduction of the mean difference between patients and controls at the second condition.
Both groups showed an insignificant decrement in performance at delayed recall, but
controls dropped more evidently. We are not able to explain the controls’ results, but
delayed recall findings supported the view that both groups were using the same
strategy.
The latter consideration regarded the very marked patients’ breakdown under
articulatory suppression. At least two interpretations are possible. The former would
foresee that patients suffered a greater disadvantage from performing two tasks at
same time (memory and articulation tasks), independently of their nature, just as
patients with “Central Executive System” deficit (Baddeley et al. 1986).
A second interpretation would foresee that the detrimental effect of articulatory
suppression was specifically dependent on the articulatory nature of the task. In this
case, irrelevant articulation could be more expensive for patients in terms of
attentional resource demand, because of the speech output difficulties. In other terms,
patients’ lower verbal span could be linked to reduced availability of attentional
resources during articulation. However, this interpretation would not follow directly
theoretical claims of working memory, according to which articulatory rehearsal has
minimal attentional demand. Alternatively, patients’ breakdown under suppression
could be due to the complete impossibility to rehearse, while normal subjects may
“intersperse an occasional subvocal rehearsal between suppression responses”
(Baddeley 1986). Additional, indepedent data are necessary to address this issue
properly.
A last remark about the present and following experiments: in contrast to previous
studies in dysarthric patients, we did not test serial recall by writing or pointing
because all patients were able to give oral responses. Also the possible implications
of this choice will be discussed below.
ArtkulatoryLoopinAtukDgsutbrh zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihg
121
EXPERIMENT
2: ARTICULATORY
LOOP EFFECTS
To confirm that patients used articulatory rehearsal, we looked for effects generally
considered to be due to an articulatoty component of the immediate verbal memory
system: the phonological similarity effect for visually presented material and word
length effect for both auditory and visual stimuli (Vallar and Cappa 1987). A pattern
clearly indicative of disruption of articulator-y rehearsal was found in only one (B.O.)
out of the five apraxic patients with verbal span deficit as described by Rochon et al.
(in press). Of the remaining four patients, two showed standard effects of visual
phonological similarity and word length (indicative of normal articulatory rehearsal),
while two (A. M. and G. B.) did not show clear patterns. Also Vallar and Cappa’s
paper (1987) reported contrasting results in two anarthric patients, suggesting a spared
articulatory rehearsal with normal verbal span. By contrast, and strikingly, the pattern
expected in the case of articulatory rehearsal disruption was found, and attributed to
a peculiar strategy choice, in a patient (P. V.) with defective PS and reduced verbal
span (Vallar and Baddeley 1984).
Method
We searched for phonological similarity effect and for word length effect by means
of the same procedure already used in Italian studies (Vallar and Baddeley 1984;
Vallar and Cappa 1987). Only four out of eighteen patients accepted to perform this
experiment, as described in a previous section.
(a) Phonological similarity effect: we used two pools of letters (10 phonologically
similar and 10 phonologically dissimilar) to build up sequences of three and four
items. Ten different sequences from each set were both aurally and visually presented,
and patients had to recall items immediately afterwards. The score was the total
number of items repeated in correct serial position (maximum score thirty for three
item sequences and forty for four item sequences).
(b) Word length effect: we used two sets of words (10 bisyllabic and 10 pentasyllabic) to build up sequences of three and four items. Ten different sequences from
each set were both aurally and visually presented, and patients had to repeat them
immediately afterwards. The score was the total number of items repeated in correct
serial position (maximum score was thirty for three item sequences, and forty for four
item sequences).
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Results, reported in Tables 3 and 4, are expressed as percentages of maximum
score. About phonological similarity effect, a higher accuracy in reproducing dis-
122
Journal of Neudh@da,
Volume 7, Number 112(1992)
similar phonemes was observed both with shorter and longer sequences, independently of presentation modality. A two-way ANOVA showed that phonological
similarity approached significance (F, ,, = 4.1; p = 0.052), while modality (F,,, =
1.8; p = 0.18) and similarity-modality interaction (F,,*s = 0.35; p = 0.56) did not.
In this case, results were clear-cut even though a fully significant phonological similarity effect was not found. According to the theoretical framework, articulatory
rehearsal conveys phonologically recoded visual information to the phonological
store, thus determining phonological similarity effect for visual material. In this case,
the insignificant modality effect and, in particular, the insignificant similaritymodality interaction testified that phonological similarity effect occurred to the same
extent with both auditory and visual presentation, i.e. dysarthric patients were using
articulatory rehearsal.
TABLE 3 zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSR
phonological similarity effect
Visual pres .
Acoustic pres .
Length
Ph.S.
Ph.D.
Ph.S.
Ph.D.
3
4
0.95
0.71
0.99
0.92
0.89
0.67
0.97
0.81
Input ordered recall of sequences of three or four phonologically similar (Ph.S.) and phonologically dissimilar letters, presented aurally (acoustic pres.) and visually (visual pres.). Results
are expressed as mean probability of correct recall. Four patients were given this task.
TABLE 4
Word Length Effect
Visual pres.
Acoustic pres .
3
4
Short
Long
Short
Long
0.99
0.85
0.95
0.73
0.95
0.85
0.91
0.66
Input ordered recall of sequences of three or four short and long words, aurally (acoustic
pres.) and visually (visual pres.) presented. Results are expressed as mean probability of correct
recall. Four patients were given this task.
As for the word length effect, a similar analysis was done. A higher overall accuracy
for short words was evident, both with three item and four item sequences, independently of presentation modality. A two-way ANOVA showed a significant word length
AcWWoryLoopinAtuicDy-
123
effect (F,., = 4.5; zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIHGFEDCBA
p = O&l),
while there was not a modality effect (Ft.7 = 0.9;
0.35), nor a word length-modality interaction (F,,,, = 0.803; p = O.%).
That is
to say a clear word length detrimental effect was found in verbal immediate memory.
In addition, the lack of modality effect and of length-modality interaction was
consistent with the view that our patients were not under articulatory suppression.
Otherwise, suppression being present during input, it would have been possible to
observe differences between auditory and visual presentation (Baddeley et al. 1984).
Once more, our findings suggested that patients were using articulatory rehearsal, and
in this case evidence seemed even stronger.
In synthesis, dysarthric patients behaved differently from normal subjects under
articulatory suppression. They showed the typical effects attributed both to articulator-y rehearsal and to phonological store, as well as normal Italian speakers (Vallar
and Baddeley 1984), and in general as well as normal subjects. Pure motor speech
disorders did not disrupt articulatory loop. It must be noted that this conclusion is valid
both for patients considered as a whole group, and for single patients. Actually, all
subjects presented clear phonological similarity and word length effects, when patients
did not achieve ceiling results.
p =
EXPERIMENT
3: ARTICULATION
RATE
The articulatory loop was found to be unimpaired. Another feature of speech output
relevant to the issue of immediate memory is articulation rate, and the present experiment was aimed at measuring it. In previous studies it was implicitly assumed that
articulatory rehearsal features overlapped those of overt realization of speech. For
instance, normal articulation rate testified for normal rehearsal processes in the patient
P. V., who showed a selective impairment of phonological store (Baddeley and Vallar
1984).
Moreover, systematic relationships between articulation time and verbal memory
abilities were found: the faster the articulation rate, the larger the verbal memory span
(Baddeley et al. 1975; Nicolson 1981; Schweickert and Boruff 1986; Standing and
Curtis 1989).
Method
We used two simple measures of articulation flow, already described in Italian
speakers (Baddeley and Vallar 1984).
We measured time spent in: (a) counting forwards from 1 to 10 five successive
times; and (b) articulating letters of the Italian alphabet from A to Z. Subjects were
asked to articulate aloud as quickly as possible, pronouncing items correctly.
124
zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIHGFEDCBA
Journal of Neurolinguistics,Volume 7, Number 112(19912)
Results zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIHGFEDCBA
and Discussion
As expected, patients’ mean articulation time was very high (Table S), even though
the lowest patients’ results overlapped into a normal range. The next question is
whether these findings are sufficient to account for a defective immediate serial recall.
In other words, can patients’ verbal memory deficit be thought of as the clinical
counterpart of articulation time-verbal span relationships? This claim would be in
agreement with the model and also with the idea that articulatory rehearsal is involved
in memory span (Hitch 1980; Vallar and Papagno 1986). However, even though we
have selected patients without neuropsychological deficits, it remains plausible that the
verbal memory deficit is part of a more general deficit of short-term memory, seen
either as a defect of the “Central Executive” or as a consequence of a decreased
information processing speed (Hart et al. 1986).
TABLE 5
Articulation Rate
Alphabet
Patients
Controls
Numbers
M
S.D.
Range
M
SD.
Range
23.2
11.0
13.6
2.6
10-75
8-18
10.2
5.3
9.1
1.5
8-35
3-10
Means (M), standard deviations (S.D.), and range of articulation times (in s) for Italian
Alphabet (A-Z) and numbers (l-10; five times successively).
Since patients have been shown to rehearse at a slower rate, it could be possible that
the testing procedure, which implied oral responses, could account for patients’ lower
results in immediate verbal recall. Actually, a study on prefix/sufftx effect (Baddeley
and Hull 1979) pointed out that the longer the time after stimulus the presentation is,
the poorer the retention, and oral responses induced a larger delay between presentation and response completion in patients, because they took a longer time to pronounce
items. Indeed, contrasting data have been reported on the effect of the delay between
presentation and response (for a discussion, see Frick 1988). Therefore, we suggest
that larger time delays after presentation can be considered as a minor factor in verbal
memory performance.
EXPERIMENT
4: IMMEDIATE
MEMORY SPANS
To ascertain whether our patients showed a general short-term memory defect or
a selective verbal one, we gave patients verbal and nonverbal memory span tests. We
chose a nonverbal test, the Corsi’s block tapping test, which, according to the working
memory model, is believed to tap a different slave system, devoted to visuo-spatial
immediate memory.
We tested memory for both digits and words.
(a) The Verbal Memory Span for digits was given and scored according to the
standard instructions of the W.A.I.S. Digits Forward subtest (Wechsler 1981), which
is well standardixed in Italian speakers (Orsini et al. 1987).
(b) The Verbal Memory Span for words was included to assess verbal span with
different material. We asked subjects to repeat series of bisyllabic high-fquency
concrete words. Three series for each sequence length were given. The score was the
longest sequence the patient could repeat correctly. Such a further measure of verbal
span has already been used in Italian clinical studies (Orsini et al. 1988).
(c) Furthermore, we used Corsi’s block tapping task, following the standard procedure (Orsini et (II. 1987).
Results are shown in Table 6. Patients’ mean score was lower both at the verbal and
spatial span, but score ranges overlapped. One-way ANOVA applied to data for each
independent variable (digit span, word span, Corsi’s span) showed that only digit span
(F,,, = 4.40; p = 0.04) and word span (F,,& = 7.49; zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWV
p = 0.008)
dif f er ed
significantly in the two groups, while spatial span difference failed to reach significance
(F,,, = 3.5; p = 0.09).
Corsi
Patients
COnaOlS
Digits
words
M
S.D.
M
S.D.
lu
S.D.
4.8
5.2
0.8
0.9
4.9
5.4
0.8
0.9
3.9
4.5
0.5
0.9
Means (hi) and standad deviations
(S.D.) for immediatemmrory span.
The present experiment was aimed at ascertaining whether these patients had a
selective verbal short-term memory, or a more general defti of short-term memory.
NsL
7:1/2-a
126
zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIHGFEDCBA
Journalof NewoIinguistics,Volume 7, Number 112(1992)
From the theoretical point of view, a selective verbal STM defect would rule out the
hypothesis of a Central Executive defect in these patients, formulated to explain the
relevant drop of delayed recall under articulatory suppression.
Our span results are not strong enough to allow definite conclusions. Nonetheless,
since spatial span, which relies more heavily on “Central Executive”, both in
encoding and decoding processes (Morris 1987), appears to be less affected in our
patients, it is possible to suggest that the defect of “Central Executive” at least is less
likely. Actually, an opposite pattern (selective deficit at Corsi’s block tapping task
with spared verbal immediate memory) was found in patients with defective “Central
Executive”, such as mildly demented patients (Orsini er al. 1988). Consequently,
even though we cannot exclude a slight, general short-term memory defect, we think
that the more prominent verbal memory defect has to be related to a more specific
mechanism. The slower articulation rate in dysarthric patients could induce a slight
verbal memory defect per se. To verity this interpretation we searched for correlations
between articulatory rate and short-term memory tasks. As a measure of articulation
rate, we used the mean of the two articulation times (articulation index) for each
subject. We also tested the eventual correlation between the articulation index and
spatial span, to exclude the possibility that articulation index could relate to STM
defect non-specifically. Correlation coefficients are shown in Table 7 for patients and
controls separately.
TABLE 7
Correlation Coef’ficients Between Articulatory Index and Memory Span
Patients
Controls
Digit span
word span zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYX
Spatial span
r
P
r
P
r
P
0.57
0.3
0.01
0.03
0.28
0.06
0.25
0.65
0.16
0.14
0.5
0.33
All controls’ coefficients were lower, probably because of the lower variability in
their articulation index. Articulatory index was not correlated with spatial span, while
it was significantly correlated with digit span in both groups. However, the correlation
of articulatory index with word span only approached significance for patients, while
it was not significant for controls. So, on one hand it seems clear that articulation time
is not only an index of general cognitive impairment, while on the other hand, it seems
to relate well to digit span while it was not highly correlated with word serial recall
task. This last finding, i.e. the lack of correlation between articulatory index and word
span, could be explained by the heterogeneity of the material to be pronounced
(digits, letters) and to be recalled (words). Indeed, studies about articulation-span
Articulatory Loop
in Ataxic
Dysvthrip
127
relationships used the same verbal material for articulating and recalling.
In summary, memory span results could not provide firm evidence supporting a
“pure” verbal immediate memory defect, but it is possible to suggest that slower
articulatory rate plays a valuable role in patients’ verbal span deficit.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The aim of this paper was to verify possible influences of motor speech disturbances
upon articulatory loop. We selected patients presenting ataxic dysarthria without
general cognitive impairments and investigated their performance. The outstanding
result was that articulatory loop was unimpaired in this homogeneous group of
subjects. Patients did use the same strategy in verbal immediite recall as normal
controls, and showed all standard effects of phonological similarity, and of wordlength for both auditory and visual presentation. Nevertheless, dysarthric patients
showed slow articulation rate and a slight deficit of immediate memory, more
prominent for verbal material. It was argued, also on the basis of a significant negative
correlation between articulation times and verbal span, that the low articulation rate
can at least contribute to, if not account for, the verbal memory deficit.
This hypothesis has the merit of being entirely compatible with all previous
experimental studies and it is consistent with normal articulatory loop effects, found
in ataxic patients. Actually, one could hypothesize that patients were still using
articulatory rehearsal but with reduced efficiency depending on its rate. In other
words, if articulatory loop is able to maintain verbal material that can be pronounced
in approximately 2 s (Baddeley et al. 1975), dysarthric patients can be seen to have
only a “capacity limitation” of articulatory rehearsal. Analogously, patient P.V. was
shown to have only a “capacity limitation” of a functional phonological store
(Baddeley and Vallar 1984), because she presented the standard phonological similarity effect, and normal articulation rate, but had a reduced auditory verbal short-term
memory. In both cases, auditory phonological similarity and word length effects
represent qualitative indices of preserved articulatory rehearsal and phonological store,
respectively, whereas they cannot detect capacity limitations or reduced efficiency of
the subcomponents.
Consequently, it would not be possible to rule out an impairment of phonological
store in our patients only on the basis of standard detrimental effect of auditory
phonological similarity. In other terms, subjects could have a phonological store
capacity limitation, not detectable with articulatory loop effects, that would be
sufficient to explain the reduction of verbal memory span. However, at a glance it
would seem unlikely that patients with subcortical lesions could show a phonological
store capacity limitation. Furthermore, our dysarthric patients differed from patients
with defective phonological store by a constant, though not significant, superiority of
128
Journal of Neurolinguistics, Volume 7, Number l/2 (1992)
auditory modality in all verbal memory tasks and for normal comprehension abilities
(as assessed by means of the Token Test).
On the basis of our results, we confirm that articulatory rehearsal operates by means
of “central” components, because articulatory troubles due to non-cortical disorders
did not affect articulatory rehearsal processes. However, we found that our patients
did show slight verbal memory deficit, in contrast to patients affected by more
severe speech-out disorders previously described. These inconsistencies deserve some
consideration.
Patients with massive pontine lesions are reported to show normal verbal span, but
only on a pointing task, with standard effects of word length and phonological
similarity. As discussed above, standard effects tell us nothing about the features,
accuracy, and efficiency of the “inner voice”. That is to say, standard articulatory
loop effects cannot make quantitative predictions. The verbal memory impairment we
found, has been ascribed to motor speech disturbances due to sub-tentorial selective
lesions of the coordination and control (cerebellar) pathways. In other patients,
defined as “peripheric”, as G. F. (Vallar and Cappa 1987), massive pontine lesions
involved efferent pathways but probably also control pathways (cerebellar) and
general activating cortical afferences (reticular midbrain formation). Therefore, these
patients ’ “inner voices” could present alterations of accuracy and rate sufficient per
se to provoke deficits in verbal immediate memory. This deficit would be impossible to detect, but standard articulatory loop effects are not a sufficient argument
for excluding its existence. From this point of view, we suggest some cautions
about claims of normal verbal STM in patients with gross disturbances of overt
articulation. Only more detailed assessment of each subject, or, on the other hand,
group studies of homogeneous patients can allow for such general conclusions about
unimpaired short-term memory.
In conclusion, “peripheric” motor speech disorders of ataxic dysarthria do not
affect articulatory loop subcomponents. Nonetheless, it was argued that the slower
articulation rate due to articulatory troubles can represent a kind of “capacity
limitation” of articulatory rehearsal and can account for (at least partially) a slight
verbal immediate memory deficit. From a methodological point of view, the notion
of partial impairments (“capacity limitations”) of single subcomponents, as well as
that of possible exceptions to the model conceivable in terms of strategical choices
(likewise in patients G. F., and P. V.), could extend the theoretical framework and
make it more compliant. However, the more flexible and comprehensive a framework
becomes, the less falsifiable it becomes, and hence it is only with strong, convergent
evidence that theoretical exceptions are allowed into a framework. Bearing in mind
these considerations, we were very cautious about drawing conclusions which are
entirely compatible with previous experimental data and with theoretical constraints.
Otherwise we had to admit that the data did not fit into working memory theory.
Artkuhtory
Loop
in Ataxk
zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZY
Dysduia zyxwvutsrqponm
129 zyxwvu
NOTES
1.
Address for correspondence: Dr Luigi Trojano, Fondazione Clinica de1 Lavoro,
Centro Medico di Campoli M.T., I.R.C.C.S., 82030 Campoli M.T. (BN) Italy.
REFERENCES
Baddeley, A. D.
1986 zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPONMLKJIHGFEDCBA
W orking M emory , Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Baddeley, A. D. and G. J. Hitch
1974 “Working Memory,” in Recent Advances in Learning and M otivation,
Vol. VIII, Bower G. (ed.), New York: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D. and A. Hull
1979 “Prefix and Suffix Effect: Do They Have a Common Basis?” Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18. 12940.
Baddeley, A. D., V. Lewis and G. Vallar
1984 “Exploring the Articulatory Loop,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psy chology 36. 233- 52.
Baddeley, A. D., R. Logie, S. Bressi, S. Della Sala and H. Spinnler
1986 “Dementia and Working Memory,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psy chology 38a. 603- 18.
Baddeley, A. D., N. Thomson and M. Buchanan
1975 “Word Length and the Structure of Short-term Memory,‘* Journal of
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14. 575- 89.
Baddeley, A. D. and B. Wilson
1985 “Phonological Coding and Short-term Memory in Patients Without
Speech,” Journal of M emory and Language 24. 490- 502.
Campanella, G., A. Filla, F. De Falco, D. Mansi, A. Durivage and A. Barbeau
1980 “Friedreich’s Ataxia in the South of Italy. A Clinical and Biochemical
Survey of 23 Patients,” Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences 7.
351- 8.
Darley, F. L., A. E. Aaronson and J. R. Brown
1969a “Differential Diagnostic Patterns of Dysarthria,” Journal of Speech and
Hearing Research 12, 146- 69.
1969b
“Clusters of Deviant Speech Dimentions in Dysarthrias,”
Journal of
Speech and Hearing Research 12. 462- 9.
1975
M otor Speech Disorders, Toronto: W. B. Saunders.
Frick, R. W.
1988 “Issue of Representation and Limited Capacity in the Auditory Short-term
Store,” British Journal of Psy chology 79. 2 1340.
130
Journal of Neurolinguistics, Volume 7, Number 112 (1992)
Geoffroy, G., A. Barbeau, G. Breton, B. Lenieux, M. Aube, C. Leger and J. B.
Bouchard
1976 “Clinical Description and Roentgenologic Evaluation of Patients with
Friedreich’s Ataxia,” zyxwvutsrqponmlkjihgfedcbaZYXWVUTSRQPON
Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences 3.
279- 86.
Harding, A. E.
1.
1983 “Classification of the Hereditary Ataxias and Paraplegias,” Lancer zyxwvutsrq
1151-5.
Hart, R. P., G. K. Henry, J. A. Kwentus and R. T. Leshner
1986 “Information Processing Speed of Children with Friedreich’s Ataxia,”
Developmental M edicine and Child Neurology 20. 310- 3.
Hitch, G. J.
1980 “Developing the Concept of Working Memory,” in Cognitive Psy chology :
New Directions, Claxton G. (ed.), London: Routledge &LKegan Paul.
Levine, D. N., R. Calvanio and A. Popovics
1982 “Language in the Absence of Inner Speech,” Neuropsy chologia 20.
391- 409.
B. A.
1971 “Role of Articulation in Auditory and Visual Short-term Memory,” Journal
of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 10. 123-32.
Morris, R. G.
1987 “Exploring the Visuo-Spatial Scratch-pad,*’ Quarterly Journal of Experi-
Levy ,
mental Psy chology 39. 409- 30.
Murray, D. J.
1975 “Articulation
and Acoustical Confusability in Short-term Memory,”
Journal of Experimental Psy chology 78. 679- 84.
Nebes, R. D.
1975 “The Nature of Internal Speech in a Patient with Aphemia,” Brain and
Language 2. 489- 97.
Nicolson. R.
1981 “The Relationship Between Memory Span and Processing Speed,” in
Intelligence and Learning, M. Friedman, J. P. Das and N. O’Connor
(eds), London: Plenum Press.
Orsini. A., D. Grossi, E. Capitani, M. Laiacona, C. Papagno and G. Vallar
1987 “Verbal and Spatial Immediate Memory Span: Normative Data from 1355
Adults and I I12 Children,” Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences 8.
539- 48.
Orsini, A., L. Trojano, L. Chiacchio and D. Grossi
1988 “Immediate Memory Spans in Dementia,” Perceptual and M oror Skills
67. 267- 72.
Al- tkUhtO ~Lo o piDAtUkDyssrthrlr zyxwvutsrqponmlkji
131
Rochon, E., G. Waters and D. Caplan
n.d.
“Short-term Memory Processes in Patients with Apraxia of Speech:
Implications for the Nature and Structure of the Auditory Verbal Shortterm Memory System,” Journal of Neuroiinguistic in press.
Schweickert, R. and B. Boruff
1986 “Short-term Memory Capacity: Magic Number or Magic Spell?” Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 12.
419-25.
Slaby, A. E. and R. J. Wyatt
1974 Dementia in the Presenium, Springfield (Jll.): C. C. Thomas Publishers.
Standing, L. and L. Curtis
1989 “Subvocalization Rate Versus Other Predictors of the Memory Span,”
Psychological Repot% 65. 487-95.
Vallar, G. and A. D. Baddeley
1984 “Fractionation of Working Memory: Neuropsychological Evidence for a
Phonological Short-term Store,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Behavior 23. 151-61.
Vallar. G. and S. F. Cappa
1987 “Articulation and Verbal Short-term Memory: Evidence from Anarthria,”
Cognitive Neuropsychoiogy 4. 55-78.
Vallar. G. and C. Papagno
1986 “Phonological Short-term Store and the Nature of the Recency Effect:
Evidence from Neuropsychology,” Brain and Cognition 5. 428-42.
Villa, G. and C. Caltagirone
1984 “Speech Suppression Without Aphasia after Bilateral Perisylvian Softenings,” Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences 5. 77-83.
Wechsler, D.
1981 WA&R Manual, New York: Psychological Corporation.
Werdelin, L.
1986 “Hereditary Ataxias. Occurrence and Clinical Features,*’ Acta Neurologica
Scandinavica 73. Supp. 106.