Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
www.elsevier.com/locate/visres
Colour unmasks dark targets in complex displays
Frederick A.A. Kingdom *, Reza Kasrai
McGill Vision Research Unit, Department of Ophthalmology, McGill University, 687 Pine Avenue W, Room H4-14, Montréal, PQ, Que., Canada H3A 1A1
Received 7 June 2005; received in revised form 1 August 2005
Abstract
Recent studies have suggested that colour (meaning chromatic) variations help the visual system segment luminance-variegated displays into their illumination and reflectance layers. This leads to the prediction that colour variations should unmask partially camouflaged achromatic transparencies on luminance-variegated backgrounds. We used ÔMondrian-likeÕ backgrounds that were either
achromatic, i.e., varying only in luminance, or chromatic, which in our stimuli meant varying in both luminance and colour. Both achromatic and chromatic backgrounds had the same luminance distribution. Thresholds for detecting simulated transparency targets were
found to be lower when on the chromatic compared to achromatic backgrounds. We hypothesised that the chromatic-background
advantage resulted from the extra cue provided by colour as to which borders were background and which transparency, predicting that
(a) randomising the colours on either side of the transparency border, (b) rotating the target to destroy its X-junctions, and (c) viewing
the target eccentrically, would each destroy the chromatic-background advantage. However, none of these predictions was upheld. We
suggest therefore that the chromatic-background advantage is due to a low-level, rather than border-disambiguation mechanism. We
suggest that chromatic variations reduce the noise, but not the signal, in the mechanism that detects dark targets in complex displays.
2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Colour vision; Transparency; Masking; Contrast detection
1. Introduction
Transparency, described phenomenally as Ôseeing
throughÕ, has long interested the vision science community.
The interest is often motivated by quite general questions
about scene analysis because a transparency, allthough a
material medium, can be considered to be a member of a
broad range of illumination effects that include shadows,
shading and highlights, all of which cause abrupt changes
in luminance and sometimes chromaticity. Many psychophysical studies have sought to determine the necessary
and sufficient conditions for generating an impression of
transparency (Beck, Prazdny, & Ivry, 1984; Gerbino,
1994; Kasrai & Kingdom, 2001a, 2002; Masin, 1997;
Metelli, 1974; Ripamonti & Westland, 2003; Robilotto,
Khang, & Zaidi, 2002; Singh & Anderson, 2002; DÕZmura,
Colantoni, Knoblauch, & Laget, 1997), and these condi*
Corresponding author.
E-mail address:
[email protected] (F.A.A. Kingdom).
0042-6989/$ - see front matter 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.018
tions can be roughly divided into two classes: surface and
figural. Surface conditions refer to the luminance and colour (meaning chromatic) requirements for perceived transparency, while figural conditions refer to its geometric
requirements, i.e., the arrangement and orientation of its
contours.
In this communication we are concerned with the surface conditions for transparency, but not as they concern
its impression but its detectability. Since we wish the present study to be relevant to shadows, we have restricted ourselves to transparencies that, like shadows, are achromatic
and have no additive component (transparencies with an
additive component, sometimes termed transluscencies,
are characterised by their milky appearance). When presented on densely variegated achromatic backgrounds,
achromatic transparencies may become partly camouflaged. In Fig. 1A, it is easy to appreciate that the achromatic transparency in the upper left of the display would
be difficult to detect if it were either fainter, presented briefly or viewed peripherally. The question we ask is whether
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
A
B
C
D
E
815
F
140
120
Frequency
100
80
60
40
20
0
0
100
200
300
400
500
Luminance bin
Fig. 1. Example test stimuli. (A) Achromatic background with transparency target; (B) chromatic background with transparency target; (C) achromatic
background with rotated target; (D) chromatic background with rotated target; (F) chromatic background with random-colour target. The arrow points
to an example distractor—there are six of these in all stimuli (see Section 2); (E) shows a frequency histogram of the luminances of the background
rectangles for both chromatic and achromatic backgrounds—see Section 2.
the addition of colour variations to the display—Fig. 1B
shows the stimulus—helps to unmask the achromatic
transparency under these conditions.
Why might one expect colour variations to unmask achromatic transparencies on luminance-variegated backgrounds? A number of recent studies have demonstrated
that colour variations help the visual system to segment
displays into what Barrow and Tennebaum (1978) have
termed Ôintrinsic imagesÕ,1 that is their reflectance and illumination layers (Kingdom, 2003; Kingdom, Beauce, &
Hunter, 2004; Kingdom, Rangwala, & Hammamji, 2005).
The reflectance layer is a representation of the surfaces in
a scene, and since most surfaces vary in both spectral as
well as intensive reflectance, this layer is in general both
colour- and luminance-defined. Shadows and achromatic
transparencies on the other hand, which form part of the
illumination layer of a scene, impose a divisive reduction
in the luminances of surfaces upon which they fall, and
are thus luminance-defined.2 In complex scenes colour is
therefore a potential cue for helping discriminate changes
in illumination from changes in reflectance via the following rule: luminance variations that are accompanied by col-
1
Gilchrist (1979, and see Gilchrist et al., 1983) have termed this Ôedge
classificationÕ.
2
Shadows can be tinted with colour; for example deep shadows on
sunny days tend to be bluish.
816
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
our variations are variations in reflectance, while luminance variations that are unaccompanied by colour variations are variations in illumination (Cavanagh, 1991;
Mullen & Kingdom, 1991; Rubin & Richards, 1982). In
the image processing domain, this rule has recently been
applied to help separate the shading and reflectance components of natural images (Marshall, Tappen, William,
Freeman, & Adelson, 2003; Olmos & Kingdom, 2004),
and psychophysical evidence has revealed that the rule is
exploited by the human visual system for Ôshape-fromshadingÕ (Kingdom, 2003; Kingdom et al., 2005; Kingdom,
Wong, Yoonessi, & Malkoc, 2005), and correctly identifying simulated shadows (Kingdom et al., 2004).
In the last of the aforementioned studies, Kingdom
et al. (2004) presented subjects with a stimulus comprising
three background sectors that were overlaid with a shadow/transparency that was either correctly or incorrectly
simulated. In the achromatic condition, the background
sectors differed only in luminance, whereas in the chromatic condition the sectors differed in luminance and colour. Subjects were better able to discriminate the correct
from incorrect shadows on the chromatic compared to
achromatic background. However, when the colours on
either side of the border of the shadow/transparency with
the background in the chromatic condition were randomised, performance was impaired. Kingdom et al. suggested
that the colour variations, provided they were continuous
across the border of the shadow/transparency with the
background, helped the visual system segregate the shadow/transparency from the background, enabling it to be
more efficiently processed. If colour variations improve
our ability to discriminate correct from incorrect shadows/transparencies, we might expect colour variations to
help our ability to detect shadows/transparencies that
are camouflaged. The first aim of this study is to test this
prediction. The targets used here, lacking penumbra,
arguably look more like achromatic transparencies than
shadows, and so we will refer to them as transparencies
from now on. However, we assume our results are equally
applicable to shadows.
If our prediction that achromatic transparencies are
more easily detected on chromatic compared to achromatic
backgrounds is born out, then other predictions follow.
Following the idea that continuity of colour across a luminance border is indicative of an illumination change, we
should expect that randomising the colours across the
transparency border should eliminate the chromatic background advantage. By the same argument, rotating the
transparency target, which also destroys the colour continuity, should also eliminate the chromatic background
advantage. Finally, viewing the stimuli eccentrically, which
renders imperceptible the precise colour composition of the
transparency border, should also eliminate the chromatic
background advantage.
To determine whether achromatic transparency targets
are detected more easily on chromatic (Fig. 1B) compared
to achromatic (Fig. 1A) backgrounds, it is essential, we ar-
gue, that the two types of background have the same
luminance distribution, otherwise any superiority in performance found with the chromatic display could not
unequivocally be attributed to its chromatic content.
Therefore, we emphasize at the outset that the chromatic
backgrounds used in this study are not isoluminant. The
use of achromatic and chromatic stimuli with the same
luminance distribution constitutes a defining characteristic
of the stimuli used here as well as in our previous study
(Kingdom et al., 2004), and we describe the method employed to achieve this in the following section.
Brief accounts of some of the results reported here have
been given elsewhere (Kasrai & Kingdom, 2001b; Kingdom
& Kasrai, 2001).
2. Methods
2.1. Stimuli
2.1.1. Generation
All stimuli were generated using the VSG2/3F videographics card (Cambridge Research Systems) hosted by a
Gateway 2000 P5 computer and displayed on a BARCO
CCID 7551 monitor.
2.1.2. Stimulus backgrounds
Fig. 1 shows example stimuli. Each stimulus was
7.3 · 7.3 at the viewing distance of 100 cm and was positioned in the middle of the monitor screen on a mid-grey
background of 13.9 cd/m2. The stimulus background contained 125 randomly positioned rectangles, clipped at the
border of the stimulus where necessary. Both the heights
and widths of the rectangles were randomly selected from
the range 0.43–1.29, producing rectangles with a variety
of areas and aspect ratios.
The luminances and colours of the background rectangles were assigned as follows. For each stimulus three
image planes—red, green, and blue—were created in the
VSGÕs video memory. The three planes were alternated
in sequence at 160 Hz; thus each plane was refreshed
at approximately 53.3 Hz. While the red plane was displayed, only the red phosphor was activated, while the
green plane was displayed only the green phosphor
was activated, and while the blue plane was displayed
only the blue phosphor was activated. There were 256
linearly spaced intensity levels available for each image
plane, resulting in a total of 2563 possible luminancecolours in the combined three-plane image. Let each
luminance-colour be described as a ÔtristimulusÕ value,
rgb, where r, g, and b are phosphor intensities expressed
as proportions of their maximum luminances, rmax, gmax,
and bmax. The maximum phosphor luminances were
measured using the same frame-alternating sequence
employed for the presentation of the stimuli, but with
two of the phosphors set to zero luminance. The values
were rmax = 5.5 cd/m2, gmax = 19.7 cd/m2, and bmax =
2.6 cd/m2.
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
For the chromatic background, each rectangle was randomly allocated a tristimulus value rcgcbc from the full
gamut available. The luminance Lc of the rectangle is thus
Lc ¼ rc rmax þ gc gmax þ bc bmax .
ð1Þ
In order that the achromatic background rectangles were
selected from the same luminance distribution as the chromatic background rectangles, we first generated random
tristimulus values as for the chromatic background (i.e.,
rcgcbc), and then calculated the tristimulus value ra, ga, ba
that resulted in a luminance La equal to Lc but under the
constraint that ra = ga = ba. The formula is
r a ¼ g a ¼ ba ¼
rc rmax þ gc gmax þ bc bmax
.
ðrmax þ gmax þ bmax Þ
ð2Þ
This method for equating the luminance distributions
from which the chromatic and achromatic background
rectangles are selected is robust to any differences in
spectral sensitivity between observers. Suppose for example that our estimate of rmax underestimated the sensitivity to the red phosphor of a subject by a factor of two.
Doubling rmax however has on the equality between
Lc and La, nor therefore on the equality of the luminance distributions for the achromatic and chromatic
conditions.
Fig. 1E shows the theoretical distribution of the background rectangle luminances obtained from 16,000 randomly generated tristimulus values, using Eq. (1) to
calculate the rectangle luminances. As can be seen, the distribution has a flattish top with sloping tails.
2.1.3. Targets
All targets had an area of 1.3 square deg and a heightto-width aspect ratio that could vary randomly between
0.67 and 1.5. The targets were randomly positioned anywhere on the background with the constraint that the edges
of the target were at least 0.43 from the edge of the
background.
There were three types of target, termed ÔtransparencyÕ,
ÔrotatedÕ, and Ôrandom colourÕ, and examples of each are
shown in Fig. 1. The transparency target was formed by
multiplying the pixel intensities in the designated region
of all three image planes by t, where 0 < t < 1.0. A value
of t close to unity produced a target with a high physical
transparency/low opacity, whereas a value of t close to 0
produced a target with low physical transparency/high
opacity. Example transparency targets with t = 0.5 are
shown in Figs. 1A and B.
The rotated target was produced according to the same
principle as the transparency target, except that once created it was reflected across both the vertical and horizontal
axis before being superimposed on the background. The result was a target with the same luminance and colour composition as the transparency target, but without any Xjunctions (except by chance) along the border with the
background. Figs. 1C and D show example rotated targets
with t = 0.3.
817
The random-colour target was generated according to
the same principle as the transparency target, except that
once created, its internal colours were re-allocated new random colours, but without changing their luminances. This
condition was not applicable to the achromatic background, and an example with t = 0.5 is shown in Fig. 1F.
2.1.4. Target distractors
A final feature of our stimuli is the addition of rotated
target distractors with t = 1.0 to all stimulus backgrounds.
To understand the reason for these distractors, consider the
potential shape of the psychometric function for each of
the three types of target. Remember that the aim is to measure proportion correct detections as a function of t (actually log(1 t), see below), in order to calculate a threshold.
For the transparency target, as t approaches unity, performance must inevitably fall to chance, as the target literally
disappears when t = 1.0. However, this is not the case for
the rotated and random-colour targets. With t = 1.0, these
targets do not disappear, because they remain defined by a
continuous border around their edge. In pilot experiments
we found that performance was around 65% for the t = 1.0
rotated and random-colour targets on purely random-rectangle backgrounds. To obtain psychometric functions that
spanned 50–100% correct, we added six randomly positioned rotated target distractors with t = 1.0 to all stimulus
backgrounds prior to adding the target. The distractors
were added one at a time and therefore some distractors
by chance occluded parts of others. The target was always
added last so that it would never be occluded. The presence
of the target distractors was found to reduce performance
to chance for all types of target with t = 1.0. An example
distractor is shown by the arrow in Fig. 1F.
2.2. Procedure
We used the method of constant stimuli with a 2IFC
(two-interval-forced-choice) procedure. Subjects were
instructed to Ô. . .detect the dark, rectangular targets in the
display, which are of various degrees of transparencyÕ. Subjects were given plenty of practice trials to familiarise themselves with the different types of target and background. On
each trial, two stimuli were presented for 250 ms each, one
with and one without a target. The task for the subject was
to indicate by key press the interval containing the target.
A tone for an incorrect decision was provided for feedback.
In each block, the background was either chromatic or achromatic, one type of target was presented, and six levels of t
were presented (in random order). In each block there were
120 trials, so each t was presented 20 times. The t values for
each background–target combination were selected to produce performance in the range 50–100%, as determined by
pilot experiments. Examples of the ranges of t employed
for some of the conditions are given in Section 3, but t ranged overall from 0.95 to 0.05, the extremes corresponding
perceptually to Ôvery faint and highly transparentÕ and Ônear
black and opaqueÕ. The spacing of the six t values was ar-
818
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
ranged such that when converted to log(1 t), the values
were equally spaced. For each background–target condition six blocks were run, making a total of 720 trials per
psychometric function.
Measurements were made at 0, 5, and 10 eccentricity.
For the latter two eccentricities, subjects were required to
fixate a spot positioned to the right of the stimulus on
the horizontal meridian. Since the stimulus was 7.3 wide,
and the target randomly positioned within the stimulus,
the eccentricity of each target varied somewhat around its
specified mean value.
2.3. Subjects
A total of seven subjects were employed, the two
authors (FK and RK) and five naı̈ve subjects (LH, CB,
HW, AY, and SC) who were undergraduate volunteers.
All subjects had normal, or corrected-to-normal acuity,
and normal colour vision.
2.4. Data analysis
The
logistic
function,
0.5 + 0.5 * exp((x a)/b)/
(1.0 + exp((x a)/b)), with x = log(1 t), was fitted to the
proportion correct data. a and b were free parameters that
determined the threshold at 75% correct and the slope of
the psychometric function. The functions were fit using Igor
(Wavemetrics), with the reciprocal of the binomial standard
deviation of each data point used as a weighting function.
3. Results
Example psychometric functions for one of the naı̈ve
observers (CB) are shown in Fig. 2. Each plot shows proportion correct target detections against log(1 t). As
can be seen, all plots show a more-or-less monotonic increase in performance with log(1 t). The measure of
log(1 t) can be considered a measure of the darkness of
the targets relative to their background, and in the case
of the transparency targets, a measure of their opacity. In
terms of t, the abscissa values range 0.95–0.4 for the left,
0.95–0.3 for the middle and 0.8–0.05 for the right graph.
Proportion correct
1.0
0.9
0.8
The left graph shows psychometric functions for the 0
eccentricity transparency target on both chromatic and
achromatic backgrounds; the middle graph psychometric
functions for the 5 eccentricity transparency and random-colour targets on the chromatic background; the right
graph psychometric functions for the 10 eccentricity rotated targets on both chromatic and achromatic backgrounds.
The thresholds calculated from the psychometric functions are shown in Fig. 3. The top row of histograms shows
thresholds for the transparency target on both chromatic
and achromatic backgrounds, and the random-colour targets. The bottom row shows the thresholds for the rotated
targets on both chromatic and achromatic backgrounds.
Different graphs within each row are for different
eccentricities.
To illustrate more directly the relationship between
viewing eccentricity and stimulus condition, Fig. 4 plots
thresholds as a function of eccentricity for the three subjects who produced data for all eccentricities.
It is clear from Figs. 3 and 4 that thresholds for targets
on chromatic backgrounds are lower than for targets on
achromatic backgrounds, both for transparency and rotated targets. There appears to be little or no difference between thresholds for transparency and random-colour
targets in the chromatic background condition. Although
thresholds rise with eccentricity, there appears to be no
interaction between eccentricity and the magnitude of the
chromatic background advantage. To confirm these observations, we performed two types of statistical analysis: first,
t tests on the 0 eccentricity conditions, for which data on
five subjects was collected (though not identical subjects for
transparency and rotated targets); second, ANOVAS for
the three subjects who performed at all three eccentricities.
The t tests for the 0 eccentricity data were within-subject
and tested for significant differences (two-tailed) between
(a) transparency targets on chromatic versus achromatic
backgrounds, (b) rotated targets on chromatic versus achromatic backgrounds, and (c) transparency versus randomcolour targets on chromatic backgrounds. The ANOVAS
were also within-subject and tested for the same significant
differences as the t tests, but included the factor eccentricity. The results of both analyses are shown in Table 1.
1.0
CB
0.9
o
0
0.7
0.6
1.0
CB
5
0.9
o
0.8
0.8
0.7
0.7
0.6
chro.
achro.
0.5
0.4
-1.2
-0.8
log(1-t)
-0.4
CB
o
10
0.6
0.5
Transp.
Rand. Col.
0.4
-1.2
-0.8
log(1-t)
-0.4
0.0
Chro.
Achro.
0.5
0.4
-0.8 -0.6 -0.4 -0.2
0.0
log(1-t)
Fig. 2. Example psychometric functions for one of the naı̈ve subjects (CB). Each plot shows the proportion correct detections as a function of log(1 t).
The continuous lines are best-fitting logistic functions. Error bars are binomial standard deviations. Left 0, middle 5, and right, 10 eccentricity. Chro.,
chromatic; achr., achromatic; transp., transparency; rotated (rot.); rand. col., random colour.
819
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
Threshold (1-t)
0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
LH
0.8
0.6
CB
FK
HW
CB
FK
0.2
0.2
CB
LH
5
0.4
0.2
0.2
LH
CB
CB
o
FK
0.8
0.6
o
0.4
AY
10
FK
0.8
0.6
SC
0.8
0.6
o
0.4
LH
0.2
LH
5
0.4
RK
achr. rot.
chro. rot.
o
0
0.4
Threshold (1-t)
0.8
0.6
achr. trans.
chro. trans.
rand. col.
o
0
o
10
LH
FK
CB
FK
Threshold (1-t)
Fig. 3. Thresholds for detecting the dark target. Top: thresholds for chromatic transparency (chro. trans.), achromatic transparency (achr. trans.) and
random-colour (rand. col.) conditions. Bottom: thresholds for chromatic rotated and achromatic rotated conditions. Left 0, middle 5, and right, 10
eccentricity.
1.0
0.8
0.6
0.8
0.6
CB
FK
0.4
0.4
0.4
1.0
0.8
0.6
0.2
achr. trans.
chro. trans.
rand. col.
0.2
0
Threshold (1-t)
1.0
0.8
0.6
LH
2
4
6
8
0.2
10
0
1.0
0.8
0.6
LH
0.4
2
4
6
8
10
0.8
0.6
CB
0
2
4
6
8
Eccentricity (deg)
10
4
6
8
10
4
6
8
10
FK
0.2
0.2
achr. rot.
chro. rot.
2
0.4
0.4
0.2
0
0
2
4
6
8
10
Eccentricity (deg)
0
2
Eccentricity (deg)
Fig. 4. Thresholds for detecting the dark target as a function of type of background and eccentricity for three subjects. chro., chromatic; achr.,
achromatic; transp., transparency; rot., rotated; rand. col., random colour.
As can be seen from the right-hand column of Table 1,
both analyses reveal significant differences in thresholds between targets presented on chromatic versus achromatic
backgrounds, for both transparency and rotated targets.
There are no significant differences between the transparency and random-colour targets on the chromatic backgrounds. Eccentricity is a significant main effect, but
there are no significant interactions between eccentricity
and other factors.
4. Discussion
Achromatic transparency targets on luminance-variegated backgrounds are more easily detected when the back-
820
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
Table 1
Statistical analysis of results
Eccentricity
# Ss
Factors and comparisons
Test
Significance *p < 0.05
0
0
0
5
5
5
Transparency target, chromatic V achromatic background
Rotated target, chromatic V achromatic background
Transparency V random-colour target, chromatic background
t(4) = 5.549
t(4) = 4.657
t(4) = 0.75
p = 0.005*
p = 0.01*
p = 0.495
All
3
A Transparency target, chromatic V achromatic background
B Eccentricity
A·B
F(1,2) = 26.1
F(2,4) = 12.1
F(2,4) = 4.63
p = 0.036*
p = 0.02*
p = 0.091
All
3
A: Rotated target, chromatic V achromatic background
B: Eccentricity
A·B
F(1,2) = 366
F(2,4) = 13.32
F(2,4) = 0.22
p = 0.003*
p = 0.017*
p = 0.815
All
3
A Transparency V random-colour target, chromatic background
B: Eccentricity
A·B
F(1,2) = 1.08
F(2,4) = 13.32
F(2,4) = 1.653
p = 0.408
p = 0.017*
p = 0.3
ground is also colour-variegated. This confirms the first
prediction of the study. For the remaining three predictions, namely that the chromatic background advantage
in target sensitivity would disappear when (a) the colours
were randomised across the transparency target border,
(b) when the transparency targets were rotated, and (c)
when the targets were viewed eccentrically, there was no
support. We will return to an examination of these unsupported predictions after first considering the significance of
the main finding of the study.
How large is the chromatic background advantage, at
least as measured in our displays? If we average across subjects and eccentricities, the percentage difference in opacity
(1 t) thresholds between the chromatic and achromatic
conditions is 41% for the transparency targets and 31%
for the rotated targets. These are both less than a factor
of two. Therefore while the chromatic background advantage appears to be robust and significant, it is not large. It
remains to be seen whether there are more optimal conditions for eliciting the chromatic background advantage.
Many studies have considered whether luminance- and
chromatic-sensitive mechanisms interact in visual tasks.
Of these, the studies that have measured contrast detection
thresholds for luminance- and colour-defined patterns such
as sine-wave gratings or disks are most directly relevant to
the present study. The results of these studies have for the
most part indicated that chromatic- and luminance-sensitive mechanisms operate independently at detection threshold (e.g., Cole, Hine, & McIlhagga, 1993; Mullen &
Losada, 1994; Mullen & Sankeralli, 1999; Wandell, 1985;
exceptions are Gur & Akri, 1992; Switkes, Bradley, &
DeValois, 1988). For example, Mullen and Losada (1994)
found that near-threshold levels of a chromatic grating
mask failed to facilitate the detection of an achromatic
grating test, and vice versa, provided positional cues to
the test were eliminated. This lack of facilitation of an achromatic test by a chromatic mask would seem at odds with
the results found here, but it must be borne in mind that
the intensive properties of our stimuli are quite different
from those employed by Mullen and Losada. In our stimuli, both the chromatic and achromatic backgrounds had
the same, suprathreshold level of luminance contrast. For
this reason, we consider our findings an instance of chromatic unmasking rather than chromatic facilitation, since
the thresholds for our targets are presumably much higher
than they would be were the background a uniform field.
The analogous experiment to ours using gratings would
be to measure contrast thresholds for a luminance grating
test in the presence of a suprathreshold luminance grating
mask (i.e., a contrast increment threshold), and repeat
the measurements with an added chromatic grating mask.
However, to our knowledge there is no published data
for this stimulus condition, and unpublished experiments
in our laboratory using gratings have so far failed to find
evidence for chromatic unmasking.
What might be the mechanism of chromatic unmasking?
First, consider the information content of our stimuli. In
terms of luminance content, the chromatic and achromatic
conditions have the same information, as we equalised the
luminance variance in the two conditions (see Section 2). In
terms of colour content, this depends on the condition. For
the transparency targets, the chromatic stimulus is more
information rich than the achromatic stimulus. In the chromatic condition, the background borders change in luminance and colour, whereas the transparency borders
change in luminance but not colour, and this provides a
cue to the target not present in the achromatic stimulus.
By the same reasoning, the information content in the
chromatic transparency condition is greater than that of
the chromatic random-colour condition. In the case of
the rotated targets, the information content of both chromatic and achromatic stimuli is presumably the same.
Therefore, if information content determined performance,
we would expect lower thresholds for the transparency targets on chromatic compared to achromatic backgrounds,
which we found. However, we would also expect lower
thresholds for the transparency compared to random-colour targets on the chromatic background, and similar
thresholds for rotated targets on both chromatic and achromatic backgrounds, neither of which was found. Taken
together, our results are not consistent with the idea that
information content alone determines performance.
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
The second candidate for chromatic unmasking is reflectance-illumination disambiguation. In Section 1 we discussed how it has been shown that chromatic variations
help the visual system to segment complex displays into
their illumination (which here includes transparency) and
reflectance layers, by reducing the uncertainty as to which
borders are illumination and which reflectance. This led
us to propose three manipulations that should eliminate
the chromatic background advantage. However, none of
the manipulations had the desired effect, and we must
therefore conclude that it is unlikely that the chromatic
background advantage with our stimuli is due to any kind
of border disambiguation. In this respect, the data here are
quite different from those of Kingdom et al. (2004) where
the task was to discriminate correctly from incorrectly simulated shadows; in that study we found that randomising
the colours across the shadow border significantly impaired
discrimination.
The third possible cause for chromatic unmasking is
some kind of low-level contrast gain adjustment. Some
years ago, Switkes et al. (1988) suggested that it would
make good sense for the visual system to suppress luminance borders in favour of chromatic ones, because chromatic borders are more reliable indicators of object
boundaries. At first glance, our results are inconsistent with
this idea, as we found that a luminance-defined target was
better detected when chromatic variations were present
than when not. Nevertheless, there is still pertinence to
Switkes et alÕs idea. What might be happening in our stimuli is that the chromatic variations partially suppress the
luminance contrasts within but not between target and background. In signal detection terms, this translates to the notion that chromatic variations suppress the noise but not
the signal. But how might this happen? One can rule out
a simple contrast gain control mechanism whereby the
chromatic contrasts reduce the variance in luminance-contrast response to the stimuli, as this would impact equally
both signal and noise. A possible mechanism is one in
which luminance-contrast-sensitive neurones receive inhibitory inputs from similarly tuned neurones outside of their
classical receptive fields, as has been demonstrated for a
class of neurones in monkey V1 (Kapadia, Westheimer, &
Gilbert, 2000; Knierim & van Essen, 1992; Nothdurft, Gallant, & van Essen, 1999). If these neurones were disinhibited
by inputs from colour-sensitive neurons in their surrounds,
they would become more sensitive to luminance contrast.
What is it about our stimulus arrangement that produces chromatic unmasking? Is the articulated background the
critical feature, and if so, is it important that the chromatic
and luminance borders are spatially aligned? At this stage
we do not have answers to these questions. Such questions
will not be easy to answer because of the small size of chromatic unmasking, which leaves little room for exploration.
However, we are presently carrying out studies to explore
the possible factors.
In conclusion, we have shown a positive role for colour
vision in the analysis of image structure that is not explica-
821
ble in terms of the reflectance–illumination disambiguation
role of colour vision that we anticipated might be the cause.
Rather, our results point to a role for colour vision in suppressing the luminance noise that in complex displays impairs the detectability of luminance-defined targets. If this
is indeed a new role for colour vision then it is one that
not only complements colour visionÕs beneficial role in
intrinsic-image segmentation, but also its beneficial role
in the analysis of other aspects of image structure, such
as the detection of fruit and flowers in dense foliage (Domini & Lucas, 2001; Mollon, 1989; Sumner & Mollon, 2000).
Acknowledgment
This research was supported by Canadian Institute of
Health Research Grant #MOP-11554 given to F.K.
References
Barrow, H. G., & Tennebaum, J. M. (1978). Recovering instrinsic scene
characteristics from images. In A. R. Hanson & E. M. Riseman (Eds.).
Computer vision systems (Vol. 99, pp. 3–26). New York: Academic Press.
Beck, J., Prazdny, K., & Ivry, R. (1984). The perception of transparency
with achromatic colors. Perception and Psychophysics, 35, 407–422.
Cavanagh, P. (1991). Vision at equiluminance. In J. J. Kulikowski, I. J.
Murray, & V. Walsh (Eds.), Vision and visual dysfunction volume V:
Limits of vision (pp. 234–250). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Cole, G. R., Hine, T. J., & McIlhagga, W. H. (1993). Detection
mechanisms in L-, M-, and S-cone contrast space. Journal of the
Optical Society of America A, 10, 38–51.
Domini, N. J., & Lucas, P. W. (2001). Ecological importance of
trichromatic vision to primates. Nature, 410, 363–365.
Gerbino, W. (1994). Achromatic transparency. In A. L. Gilchrist (Ed.),
Lightness, brightness, and transparency (pp. 215–255). Hove, UK:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Gilchrist, A. L. (1979). The perception of surface blacks and whites.
Scientific American, 240, 112–123.
Gilchrist, A. L., Delman, S., & Jacobsen, A. (1983). The classification and
integration of edges as critical to the perception of reflectance and
illumination. Perception and Psychophysics, 33, 425–436.
Gur, M., & Akri, V. (1992). Isoluminant stimuli may not expose the full
contribution of color to visual functioning: Spatial contrast sensitivity
measurements indicate interaction between color and luminance
processing. Vision Research, 32, 1253–1262.
Kapadia, M. K., Westheimer, G., & Gilbert, C. D. (2000). Spatial
distribution of contextual interactions in primary visual cortex and in
visual perception. Journal of Neurophysiology, 84, 2048–2062.
Kasrai, R., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2001a). The precision, accuracy, and
range of perceived achromatic transparency. Journal of the Optical
Society of America A, 18, 1–11.
Kasrai, R., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2001b). Articulated chromatic
backgrounds facilitate detection of illumination boundaries. Perception, 30(Suppl.), 3.
Kasrai, R., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2002). Achromatic transparency and
the role of local contours. Perception, 31, 775–790.
Kingdom, F. A. A. (2003). Colour brings relief to human vision. Nature
Neuroscience, 6, 641–644.
Kingdom, F. A. A., Beauce, C., & Hunter, L. (2004). Colour vision brings
clarity to shadows. Perception, 33, 907–914.
Kingdom, F., & Kasrai, R. (2001). Discriminating material from
illumination changes in complex scenes. Proceedings of the Society
for Imaging Science and Technology: PICS, 2001, 5–8.
Kingdom, F. A. A., Rangwala, S., & Hammamji, K. (2005). Chromatic
properties of the colour-shading effect. Vision Research, 45, 1425–1437.
822
F.A.A. Kingdom, R. Kasrai / Vision Research 46 (2006) 814–822
Kingdom, F.A.A., Wong, K., Yoonessi, A. & Malkoc, G. (2005). Colour
contrast influences perceived depth in combined shading and texture
patterns. Spatial Vision, in press.
Knierim, J. J., & van Essen, D. C. (1992). Neuronal responses to static
texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 67, 961–980.
Masin, S. C. (1997). The luminance conditions of transparency. Perception, 26, 39–50.
Marshall, F., Tappen William, T., Freeman & Adelson, E. H. (2003).
Recovering intrinsic images from a single image in advances. In Neural
Information Processing Systems (NIPS) (pp. 15). MIT Press.
Metelli, F. (1974). The perception of transparency. Scientific American,
230, 90–98.
Mollon, J. D. (1989). ÔThoÕ she kneelÕd in that place where they grew. . .Õ
The uses and origins of primate colour vision. Journal of Experimental
Biology, 146, 21–38.
Mullen, K.T., Kingdom, F.A.A., 1991. Colour contrast in form perception. In: Gouras, P. (Ed.), The perception of colour. Vol. 6 in Vision
and visual dysfunction, by J. Cronly-Dillon (Ed.) Macmillan: Oxford,
198–217.
Mullen, K. T., & Losada, M. A. (1994). Evidence for separate pathways
for color and luminance detection mechanisms. Journal of the Optical
Society of America A, 11, 3136–3151.
Mullen, K. T., & Sankeralli, M. J. (1999). Evidence for the stochastic
independence of the blue–yellow, red–green and luminance detection
mechanisms revealed by sub-threshold summation. Vision Research,
39, 733–745.
Nothdurft, H. C., Gallant, J., & van Essen, D. C. (1999). Response
modulation by texture surround in primate area V1: Correlates of
‘‘popout’’ under anesthesia. Visual Neuroscience, 16, 15–34.
Olmos, A., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2004). A biologically-inspired algorithm
for separating reflectance and shading images. Perception, 33,
1463–1473.
Ripamonti, C., & Westland, S. (2003). Prediction of transparency
perception based upon cone excitation ratios. Journal of the Optical
Soceity of America A, 20, 1673–1680.
Robilotto, R., Khang, B., & Zaidi, Q. (2002). Sensory and physical
determinants of perceived achromatic transparency. Journal of Vision,
2, 388–403.
Rubin, J. M., & Richards, W. A. (1982). Color vision and image intensities:
When are changes material? Biological Cybernetics, 45, 215–226.
Singh, M., & Anderson, B. L. (2002). Towards a perceptual theory of
transparency. Psychological Review, 109, 492–519.
Sumner, P., & Mollon, J. D. (2000). Catarrhine photopigments are
optimised for detecting targets against a foliage background. Journal
Experiment Biology, 23, 1963–1986.
Switkes, E., Bradley, A., & DeValois, K. K. (1988). Contrast dependence
and mechanisms of masking interactions among chromatic and luminance gratings. Journal of the Optical Soceity of America A, 5,
1149–1162.
DÕZmura, M., Colantoni, P., Knoblauch, K., & Laget, B. (1997). Color
transparency. Perception, 26, 471–492.
Wandell, B. A. (1985). Color measurement and discrimination. Journal of
the Optical Soceity of America A, 2, 62–71.