Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2011
…
2 pages
1 file
O ver twenty years ago, when geneticists and paleontologists confirmed the early evolution of Homo sapiens in Africa, a new major problem emerged. If anatomical modernity emerged by around 200,000 years ago in the continent, where do the cultural innovations of the Later Stone Age (LSA) / Upper Paleolithic come from? These innovations, framed under the label of behavioral modernity, include modern symbolic and cognitive achievements, presented as the basis of our own success as a species. But what were they? The editors of this volume point out that we are still not certain of how the modern mind developed. These innovations appeared either late in human evolution and quickly (with the Upper Paleolithic and/or LSA around 40-50,000 years ago), earlier and more gradually (over the course of the African Middle Stone Age or MSA), or discontinuously, but rooted in both the African MSA and the Eurasian Mousterian. The last would argue that not only did early African moderns have a role, but also the Neanderthals and other archaic humans in Europe or Asia. This book reports on the research of an interdisciplinary network dealing with issues of how the human mind evolved. By the time they met at a conference in Cape Town in 2009, they had corresponded on a number of topics. The group includes a number of individuals from a wide variety of specializations. Their initial work and the conference were both supported by a number of institutions, including the John Templeton Foundation, which usually supports research on religion and belief systems. This is presumably why they included the word spirituality in the subtitle. The group aims to integrate new perspectives from a variety of disciplines, including archaeology, anthropology, primatology, philosophy, evolutionary biology, religious studies, paleoanthropology, and linguistics. Their aim is to link genetic, neural, cognitive, and behavioral development in order to understand the evolution of modern humans and their behavior. This aim is outlined in an introduction; otherwise, individual authors attempt to deal with these questions from their own disciplinary perspective. The first chapter is written by William McGrew, the primatologist who has spent his career documenting chimpanzee material culture across many populations in Africa. He reviews ape models for understanding human origins, and questions whether or not there can be something like a Pan symbolicus. While field studies of apes show that their communication and cognitive capacities are much greater than originally thought, it is still quite different from ours. In the second contribution, Sue Savage-Rumbaugh and
Australian Archaeology, 1998
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Journal of World Prehistory, 1995
The fossil record suggests that modern human morphology evolved in Africa between 150,000 and 50,000 years ago, when the sole inhabitants of Eurasia were the Neanderthals and other equally nonmodern people. However, the earliest modern or near-modern Africans were behaviorally (archaeologically) indistinguishable from their nonmodern, Eurasian contemporaries, and it was only around 50,000-40,000 years ago that a major behavioral difference developed. Archaeological indications of this difference include the oldest indisputable ornaments (or art broadly understood); the oldest evidence for routine use of bone, ivory, and shell to produce formal (standardized) artifacts; greatly accelerated variation in stone artifact assemblages through time and space; and hunting-gathering innovations that promoted significantly larger populations. As a complex, the novel traits imply fully modern cognitive and communicative abilities, or more succinctly, the fully modern capacity for Culture. The competitive advantage of this capacity is obvious, and preliminary dates suggest that it appeared in Africa about 50,000 years ago and then successively in western Asia, eastern Europe, and western Europe, in keeping with an African origin. Arguably, the development of modern behavior depended on a neural change broadly like those that accompanied yet earlier archaeologically detectable behavioral advances. This explanation is problematic, however, because the putative change was in brain organization, not size, and fossil skulls provide little or no secure evidence for brain structure. Other potential objections to a neural advance in Africa 50,000-40,000 years ago or to the wider “Out-of-Africa” hypothesis, include archaeological evidence (1) that some Neanderthals were actually capable of fully modern behavior and (2) that some Africans were behaviorally modern more than 90,000 years ago.
Dialectical Anthropology, 1981
Cognitive archaeology has undergone a quiet revolution in the past three to five years. What was once the study of a paltry prehistoric record is now open to the unlimited potential of modern neuroscience. Cognitive archaeology seeks to answer one of the most difficult questions in archaeology: What were these people thinking? In the distant past of the origin of genus Homo, the archaeological record reveals precious little information. The dawn of the modern human mind, perhaps the most important event in the history of life, was shrouded in unsolvable mystery. Until recently we were limited to a very narrow field of inquiry: the symmetry of tools, the spatial organization of sites, the first evidence of symbolism, and the growing complexity of technology. In 1976, Alexander Marshack argued for a very early origin of symbolism in the Mousterian, replete with personal adornment and ritual shamanism. The early origin of symbolism is supported today by Francesco d'Errico and Joao Zilhao, who have provided ample evidence for Neanderthals' and other archaic Homo advanced cognitive abilities, expressed in their symbolic material culture. At the same time, Margarent Conkey began the modern era of Paleolithic art interpretation by critiquing anthropologists' artificial categories. In 2000, McBrearty and Brooks summarized the evolution of human cognition and the history of cognitive archaeology in their article "The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior." Their conclusions align with Marshack's of 25 years earlier, that around 300,000 years ago the modern human mind began to appear in the archaeological record, and that behaviors that were previously limited, by predominant theories in archaeology, to the European 3 Upper Paleolithic were easily visible in the African Middle Stone Age. Determining what 'modern behavior' itself is has been half of the debate.
Conspiracies and Conspiracy Theories in American History, 2019
LUGARIT, 2024
Journal of Change Management, 2020
Engineering Failure Analysis, 2020
Interpretación de Methol Ferré sobre la Conferencia de Puebla de 1979 a partir del Ariel de Rodó., 2019
Public Affairs Quarterly, 2017
DOAJ (DOAJ: Directory of Open Access Journals), 2011
Journal of Korean society of spine surgery, 2015
Augusto Guzzo Revista Acadêmica, 2013
Journal of Biomechanics, 2006
UTMS Journal of Economics, 2016
Eurasian Journal of Business and Economics
Jurnal Sosial dan Sains, 2023