Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
2017, The Behavior Analyst
…
6 pages
1 file
BSimply too many notes…^is the line reportedly suggested to Austrian Emperor Joseph II as a comment on Mozart's brilliant first opera in Vienna. Well, the opera was brilliant but sometimes there really are too many notes-words-and much of operant conditioning seems to be made up of arguments about them. This is, presumably, a legacy of B. F. Skinner's devotion to and skill in using language. But science is more than language, and we should surely begin a discussion of consequential learning with some actual examples. What are we trying to explain? Killeen and Jacobs (2016) do not really make this clear, so an example may help. Here is one that makes more concrete some of the ideas there expressed. Many years ago, Derick Davis (Davis, Staddon, Machado, & Palmer 1993) looked again some data from an earlier experiment on reversal learning (Davis & Staddon, 1990; see also Staddon & Frank, 1974). The problem was simple: How best to explain the fact that pigeons trained on a daily discrimination-reversal task improve across days, but without, apparently, learning to reverse spontaneously? After considering several alternatives, we settled on a very simple, entirely deterministic model. But it did diverge from the usual behaviorist credo in one respect: it required us to assume a state, which sounds suspiciously cognitive but really is not. This simply means that the behavior of the organism cannot be accurately predicted from the single variable of response rate. Everybody knows this, of course. Numerous experiments, beginning with the phenomenon of behavioral contrast, show that response rate by itself cannot predict the effect of, for example, a change in relative reinforcement rate.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1984
Pigeons' key pecks turned off a key light, which also went off independently of their pecks. The pigeons were able to correctly discriminate the cause of the stimulus change, although their attributions were strongly affected by the amount and the delay of reward given for correct responses. Their discrimination was based on the asynchrony between a response and the change in the key light. A simple detection model that combined detectability and motivational factors provided a good description of the data. It was shown that the discriminative criteria did not change with changes in distribution of noise events in an optimal fashion and that the pigeons therefore were not "ideal detectors." In the final experiment, the pigeons were asked to discriminate the cause of a key light change, of a hopper illumination, and of a feeding. Performance decreased with each condition and with the duration of the last two events. It was noted that the memory trace for a stimulus change decays at the same rate as the primary reinforcement gradient but that it decays faster when the delay is filled with an event such as reinforcement. The possibility that the effects of reinforcement may be blocked by reinforcement is briefly discussed.
The Behavior Analyst, 2017
Animal Learning & Behavior, 1990
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior
The reinforcers that maintain target instrumental responses also reinforce other responses that compete with target responses for expression. This competition and its imbalance at points of transition between different schedules of reinforcement causes behavioral contrast. A model for this theory is constructed by expanding the coupling coefficient of MPR (Killeen, 1994). The coupling coefficient gives the degree of association of a reinforcer with the target response (as opposed to other competing responses). Competing responses, often identified as interim or adjunctive or superstitious behavior, are intrinsic to reinforcement schedules, especially interval schedules. In addition to that base-rate of competition, additional competing responses may spill over from the prior component, causing initial contrast; and they may be modulated by conditioned reinforcement or punishment from stimuli associated with subsequent component change, causing terminal contrast. A formalization of these hypotheses employed (a) a hysteresis model of off-target responses giving rise to initial contrast, and (b) a competing traces model of the suppression or enhancement of ongoing competitive responses by signals of following-schedule transition. The theory was applied to transient contrast, the following schedule effect, and the component duration effect.
The American Journal of Psychology, 2003
2007
An experiment in which a pigeon was trained in contextual discrimination of its own behavior was carried out. When the experimental chamber was illuminated with a constant light, the pigeon had to peck on a red (or green) key in the sample component after having been pecking to the left (or to the right). When the chamber was illuminated with an intermittent light, the reinforced sample-comparison sequences were the opposite. The subject learned the task in about 40 sessions and maintained high correct response ratios even though the reinforcement probability decreased from 1 to 0.2 after each correct trial. The results are discussed in terms of the kind of discriminative rule and the kind of hierarchic structure involved in the task.
Animal Learning & Behavior, 1986
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
When behavior suggests that the value of a reinforcer depends inversely on the value of the events that precede or follow it, the behavior has been described as a contrast effect. Three major forms of contrast have been studied: incentive contrast, in which a downward (or upward) shift in the magnitude of reinforcement produces a relatively stronger downward (or upward) shift in the vigor of a response; anticipatory contrast, in which a forthcoming improvement in reinforcement results in a relative reduction in consummatory response; and behavioral contrast, in which a decrease in the probability of reinforcement in one component of a multiple schedule results in an increase in responding in an unchanged component of the schedule. Here we discuss a possible fourth kind of contrast that we call within-trial contrast because within a discrete trial, the relative value of an event has an inverse effect on the relative value of the reinforcer that follows. We show that greater effort, longer delay to reinforcement, or the absence of food all result in an increase in the preference for positive discriminative stimuli that follow (relative to less effort, shorter delay, or the presence of food). We further distinguish this within-trial contrast effect from the effects of delay reduction. A general model of this form of contrast is proposed in which the value of a primary or conditioned reinforcer depends on the change in value from the value of the event that precedes it.
European Scientific Journal, ESJ, 2024
The study, which is being conducted in the context of the widespread use of information technology in Moroccan companies, focuses on examining the success factors of IT implementation. This is a crucial step in maximizing the value of information technology investments. The goal is to address concerns regarding the delay in digital transformation and the role of information technology in establishing a sustainable and irreplaceable competitive advantage. The research methodology utilizes a qualitative approach, specifically a single case study with semi-structured interviews for data collection. The theoretical framework incorporates the resource-based view (RBV) and contingency theories. The key findings of the study emphasize the challenges encountered during information technology implementation, especially concerning the complementarity of organizational and human resources with information technology resources. Based on these challenges, the factors contributing to successful information technology implementation are categorized into three groups: pre-implementation phase, alignment of information technology strategy, and collaboration and coordination among all stakeholders involved in the implementation process.
Archidiocesi di Bari-Bitonto
Primerjalna književnost, 2024
Rize İlahiyat Dergisi, 2023
VITRUVIO - International Journal of Architectural Technology and Sustainability
International Journal of New Economics and Social Sciences, 2018
… : revista de investigación en el campo …, 2011
Biuletyn Szadkowski, 2017
Revista Inter Legere, 2013
Journal of Coastal Research, 2020
Canadian Journal of Cardiology, 2013
Projeto História : Revista do Programa de Estudos Pós-Graduados de História, 2023
The Journal of General Physiology, 2007
Mansoura Veterinary Medical Journal, 2017