Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-019-01111-z
ORIGINAL PAPER
Parent Mindfulness, Parenting, and Child Psychopathology in China
Zhuo Rachel Han 1 & Nigela Ahemaitijiang 1 & Jia Yan 2 & Xiaoyi Hu 3 & Justin Parent 4 & Chelsea Dale 4 & Karissa DiMarzio 4 &
Nirbhay N. Singh 5
Published online: 13 February 2019
# The Author(s) 2019
Abstract
Objectives Considering the Western-Eastern cultural differences in parenting practices, as well as the relative paucity of research
on the use of mindfulness-based programs by Chinese parents, we replicated a recently proposed Western model of
mindfulness. The purpose of this study was to test the direct and indirect relations between parents’ dispositional mindfulness,
mindful parenting, parenting practices, and child internalizing and externalizing behaviors.
Method A total of 2237 Chinses parents (M = 38.46, SD = 4.43) of 6- to 12-year-old children participated in the current study.
Results The results showed that parents’ dispositional mindfulness was indirectly associated with child internalizing and externalizing behaviors through mindful parenting and positive parenting practices, whereas this pathway was not significant through
negative parenting practices. In addition, mothers and fathers demonstrated almost equal effects on direct and indirect pathways
except that mothers showed stronger effects on the relationships between dispositional mindfulness and mindful parenting, as
well as on the link between negative parenting practices and child externalizing behaviors.
Conclusions These findings contribute to a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying how mindfulness and parenting
associated with child internalizing and externalizing behaviors, and have important implications for research on interventions
aimed at promoting children’s psychological well-being.
Keywords Mindfulness . Mindful parenting . Internalizing problems . Externalizing problems
Mindfulness refers to Bthe awareness that emerges through
paying attention, on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally to the unfolding of experience moment by
moment^ (Kabat-Zinn 2003, p. 145). Research has highlighted a number of benefits associated with increased mindfulness, including reduced depression and anxiety symptoms
* Xiaoyi Hu
[email protected]
1
Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology,
National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology
Education, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University,
Beijing, China
2
Department of Human Sciences, The Ohio State University,
Columbus, OH, USA
3
Department of Special Education, Faculty of Education, Beijing
Normal University, Rm 408, YingDong Building, Xin Jie Kou Wai
Da Jie, Beijing #19, China
4
Center for Children and Families, Florida International University,
Miami, FL, USA
5
Medical College of Georgia, Augusta University, Augusta, GA, USA
(Keng et al. 2011; Moreira and Canavarro 2018). In addition,
comparable effectiveness has been observed between
mindfulness-based programs and other evidence-based treatments (Goldberg et al. 2017) with promising results demonstrated across various settings and age groups. In recent years,
increased attention has focused on the study of dispositional
(e.g., Wang et al. 2017) or trait mindfulness (Brown et al.
2007) and mindful parenting (e.g., Parent et al. 2016).
Dispositional mindfulness is an individual’s tendency or
inner capacity to pay nonjudgmental attention to experiences
and events occurring in the present moment (Brown and Ryan
2003). Research suggests that higher levels of dispositional
mindfulness are associated with favorable outcomes, such as
better emotion regulation (Baer et al. 2004; Pepping et al.
2013) and more effective coping strategies (Brown and
Ryan 2003). In addition, a recent study reported that parents
with higher levels of dispositional mindfulness are more likely
to engage in mindful parenting with children (Parent et al.
2016).
Mindful parenting consists of nonjudgmental and presentcentered awareness during parent-child interactions (Bögels
and Restifo 2014; Kabat-Zinn and Kabat-Zinn 1997). It
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
involves listening to the child with full attention, maintaining
an awareness of the child’s emotional experience, regulating
one’s own emotions during the parenting process, maintaining
a non-judgmental acceptance of parental functioning, and being compassionate both towards the child, as well as oneself
(Duncan et al. 2009). Research has suggested that higher
levels of dispositional mindfulness can increase the likelihood
of parents’ engagement in mindful parenting behaviors (de
Bruin et al. 2014; Parent et al. 2016). This may be due to
mindful parents being better able to distinguish cognitive, affective, and behavioral experiences compared to their less
mindful counterparts (Bishop et al. 2004), thus decreasing
the likelihood of engaging in maladaptive interactions with
children.
Moreover, mindful parenting has been associated with more
positive parenting practices (Bazzano et al. 2015; Bögels et al.
2013; Haydicky et al. 2015). During parent-child interactions, a
mindful parent is more likely to be consistent with his/her
values and goals (Duncan et al. 2009), non-judgmental, demonstrates present-moment awareness, and sensitive to the children’s needs. Thus, mindful parenting has been shown to be
associated with lower levels of dysfunctional parenting styles
(de Bruin et al. 2014). Parent et al. (2016) examined parents of
children at varying developmental stages and reported similar
results, suggesting that mindful parenting is directly related to
higher levels of positive parenting practices (e.g., warmth and
reinforcement) and lower levels of negative parenting practices
(e.g., coercion or hostility).
There is a substantial amount of literature establishing the
relationship between parenting practices and child psychopathology in Western cultural contexts (Harold et al. 2012;
Kawabata et al. 2011; Lindblom et al. 2017), with similar results in Eastern contexts (Baharudin et al. 2011; Lin et al. 2016).
For instance, Chinese parents’ support for autonomy was associated with fewer depressive symptoms as reported by their
middle-school-aged children (Yan et al. 2017). In addition,
harsh parenting from fathers and mothers negatively contributed to children’s emotion regulation and peer aggression (Wang
et al. 2017). However, little research has been conducted to
delineate the processes through which dispositional mindfulness, mindful parenting, and parenting practices affect child
psychopathology outside of Western cultures. This is an important area of exploration given the significant implications of
early psychopathology for children’s long-term mental health,
academic performance, and overall quality of life (e.g., Barkley
et al. 2006; Yap and Jorm 2015).
It has been suggested that over time all cultures derive
unique concepts and values on effective parenting, and, therefore, the support for mindful parenting is likely to vary depending on cultural context (Smith and Dishion 2013). In
addition, similar parenting practices may have varying effects
on children of different cultures (Leung et al. 1998). For example, many researchers have argued that culture shapes how
335
children’s emotional competence is defined and thus influences parenting behaviors, as well as child mental health outcomes (e.g., Friedlmeier et al. 2011). As such, it is of great
importance to explore mindful parenting beyond Western cultures and to elucidate the process through which mindful parenting is associated with child outcomes in comparatively
understudied cultural contexts (e.g., Chinese culture).
Hofstede (1980) initially proposed the individualismcollectivism dimension to help describe the primary distinction between cultures, and this principle can be used to better
understand parenting and the role of parenting in child
adjustment beyond Western culture. In this context, Hofstede
(1980) proposed that individualistic societies (e.g., the USA)
value independence and tend to focus on the self, thus encouraging parenting practices (e.g., warmth) that foster children’s
self-reliance. In contrast, collectivistic societies (e.g., China
and India) emphasize interdependence and group harmony,
thus encouraging parenting practices (e.g., training) that promote children’s obedience to group rules. Parents from
individualistic and collectivistic cultures might adopt
different parenting behaviors given the various and possibly
divergent cultural norms and values related to these behaviors
and the associated child outcomes. Little research has
extended beyond the Western model of mindful parenting to
explicitly discuss cultural issues pertaining to mindful
parenting and related child outcomes; however, an emerging
line of research has been conducted with Chinese parents that
can help provide a foundation for this work. For example, Siu
et al. (2016) found parental mindfulness had a negative indirect association on children’s emotional and behavioral problems through a series of positive factors related to the parentchild relationship. These emerging findings have been consistent across Western societies and suggest parents who mindfully interact with their children have higher quality relationships with their children than those who have less mindful
interactions (Duncan et al. 2009). This, in turn, is related to
greater psychological adjustment and fewer problem behaviors in children (Geurtzen et al. 2015; Parent et al. 2010;
Williams and Wahler 2010).
These findings across cultures might be explained by the
recent proposition that while Chinese parenting is still largely
influenced by traditional cultural values, a Western and childcentered approach has gradually been incorporated into contemporary Chinese parenting, particularly among more highly
educated parents (Xu et al. 2014). Despite this preliminary
evidence, studies have not delineated the processes through
which parental mindfulness and parenting practices are associated with child outcomes using a large Chinese sample.
Elucidating these processes with a Chinese sample will enable
more nuanced research on the development and implementation of mindfulness-based program with Chinese families.
Parent gender might also shape the processes by which
parenting influences child psychopathology (Friedlmeier
336
et al. 2011; Klimes-Dougan et al. 2010). For example, when
compared to Western parents (both mothers and fathers) and
Chinese mothers, Chinese fathers’ parenting practices (e.g.,
harsh parenting and physical control) are viewed as strong
behavioral modeling for children, especially sons (Chen
et al. 2000). Thus, it is important to consider the cultural expectations of mothers and fathers when examining the influence of parenting on their children. Furthermore, gender differences have only been evaluated in the Western context of
mindful parenting (Gouveia et al. 2016; Medeiros et al. 2016).
In these studies, mothers generally have shown higher level of
mindful parenting compared to fathers (e.g., Gouveia et al.
2016), whereas fathers have displayed more supportive emotion socialization with children than mothers when they both
participate in mindfulness programs (Coatsworth et al. 2015).
The current study aimed to test the Parent et al. (2016)
model of mindful parenting practices and child psychopathology with a large group of parents from Mainland China. The
hypotheses of the current study take into consideration the
potential impact of cultural context and parent gender (see
Fig. 1). First, we hypothesized that parent dispositional mindfulness would be positively associated with mindful parenting
and that mindful parenting would be associated with greater
positive parenting practices and fewer negative parenting
practices. Second, we hypothesized that positive parenting
practices would be negatively associated with children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior, whereas negative parenting practices would be positively associated with children’s
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
internalizing and externalizing behavior. Third, regarding the
overall theoretical model, we hypothesized that parent dispositional mindfulness would have a negative indirect association with children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior
through an increase in mindful parenting, resulting in positive
parenting practices, as well as decreased negative parenting
practices.
Method
Participants
Participants were recruited through flyers that were distributed
throughout the community and electronically through communication websites among parents in Mainland China. A
total of 2237 Chinese parents (M = 38.46, SD = 4.43) of children aged 6 to 12 (M = 9.40 years, SD = 1.78) participated in
the study. Of these, 23% of the parents were fathers, and
approximately half (51.9%) of the children were male. Most
parents were of the Han nationality (93.8%), held a college
degree or above (56.4%), and were employed either full(67.2%) or part-time (13.7%). Regarding family socioeconomic status, 70.3% of the families reported living in households with an income at or above average for urban Chinese
families (around $17,316 annually; National Bureau of
Statistics of the People’s Republic of China 2017). Parents
completed a survey designed to collect information regarding
Fig. 1 Standardized path estimates for fathers and mothers in the final model. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
demographic variables, dispositional mindfulness, mindful
parenting, broadband positive/negative parenting, and
externalizing/internalizing behaviors of their children.
Procedure
The Institutional Review Board of sponsoring university approved all materials and procedures. Parents were provided
with a brief description of the study prior to obtaining their
consent electronically. Following parental consent, they were
asked to complete a series of questionnaires through an online
questionnaire system (Qualtrics). For families with more than
one child, parents were asked to only report on one child who
was within the study’s age range (i.e., 6 to 12 years). The
families received feedback on parenting and children’s develo pm en t a l ou t c om es b as e d on t he i r r e s po n s e s t o
questionnaires.
Measures
Dispositional Mindfulness The 39-item Five Facet
Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al. 2008) was
utilized to assess parental dispositional mindfulness. The current study used the Chinese version of FFMQ (Deng and Xia
2011). These items captured five aspects of individuals’ mindful dispositions: (1) observing, (2) describing, (3) acting with
awareness, (4) non-judging of inner experience, and (5) nonreactivity to inner experience. The parents indicated to what
extent each statement (e.g., I criticize myself for having irrational or inappropriate emotions) was true using a 5-point
scale ranging from 1 (never or very rarely true) to 5 (very
often or always true). A total dispositional mindfulness score
was derived by computing the sum of all items. The FFMQ
has been found to have good reliability and has been used
widely with Chinese samples (Deng and Xia 2011). For the
current study, the FFMQ demonstrated good reliability (α =
0.89).
Interpersonal Mindfulness Parenting Mindful parenting was
measured using the composite score from the Interpersonal
Mindfulness in Parenting (IM-P; Duncan et al. 2009). The
Chinese version of the IM-P, which has been good psychometric properties, was used (Lo et al. 2018). The 31-item
version is comprised of five subscales corresponding to the
different dimensions of mindful parenting: (1) listening with
full attention (e.g., I find myself listening to my child with one
ear because I am busy doing or thinking about something else
at the same time); (2) emotional awareness of self and child
(e.g., I notice how changes in my child’s mood affect my
mood); (3) self-regulation in the parenting relationship (e.g.,
When I am upset with my child, I notice how I am feeling
before I take action); (4) non-judgmental acceptance of self
and child (e.g., I try to understand my child’s point of view,
337
even when his/her opinions do not make sense to me); and (5)
compassion for self and child (e.g., I tend to be hard on myself
when I make mistakes as a parent). A 5-point Likert scale
ranging from 1 (never true) to 5 (always true) was used, with
higher scores indicating higher levels of mindful parenting.
Internal consistency in the current sample was good (α =
0.84).
Parenting Broadband positive and negative parenting practices were measured via the Multidimensional Assessment
of Parenting Scale (MAPS; Parent and Forehand 2017). The
16-item positive parenting subscale included items
representing proactive parenting, positive reinforcement,
warmth, and supportiveness (e.g., I express affection by hugging, kissing, and holding my child). The 18-item negative
parenting subscale included items representing hostility, lax
control, and physical control (e.g., I spank my child with my
hand when he/she has done something wrong). Parents
responded to each item using a 5-point Likert rating scale from
1 (never) to 5 (always). Each item was forward- and backtranslated by three associate professors or doctoral students
who were fluent in both Chinese and English. The back translation was sent to the original author to ensure that all items
retained the original meanings. A pilot study was conducted
among adults to determine the readability of the Chinese version, and the lax control scale was removed due to its relatively low factor loadings and reliability. Then two subscales were
scored separately, and the reliability of the positive (α = 0.89)
and negative subscales (α = 0.89) was excellent.
Child Psychopathology Children’s externalizing and internalizing behaviors were assessed by the Brief Problem MonitorParent Form (BPM-P; Achenbach et al. 2011). Nineteen items
of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach 1991)
made up the BPM-P for children ages 6–18: six items for
attention problems (e.g., inattentive or easily distracted), seven
for externalizing problems (e.g., threatens people), and six for
internalizing problems (e.g., unhappy, sad, or depressed).
Items were rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (not true)
to 3 (very true). For the purpose of the present study, only the
internalizing and externalizing subscales were scored. The
Chinese measure of the BPM-P was translated and backtranslated by three associate professors of Psychology. The
back translation was sent to the original author to ensure that
all items retained the original meanings. The internal consistency of the BPM-P in the current study was α = 0.76 for
externalizing problems and α = 0.81 for internalizing
problems.
Data Analyses
Preliminary Analysis The associations between primary
study variables with categorical (e.g., parent gender)
338
and continuous demographic variables (e.g., child age)
were examined using analysis of variance (ANOVA)
and zero-order correlations, respectively. We then examined means (M), standard deviations (SD), missing rates,
and zero-order correlations of all study variables.
Missing Data Missing data mechanisms were examined
using R packages BaylorEdPsych (Beaujean 2012).
Results of Little’s MCAR test showed that the
missingness was not completely at random, χ2 (62) =
116.58, p < .001. The associations between missingness
in study variables and demographic characteristics were
examined using an unpooled t test for continuous demographic variables (i.e., age, education, income) and a
chi-square test of independence for dichotomous demographic variables (i.e., parent and child gender). Full
information maximum likelihood estimation techniques
were used to include all available data.
Structural Equation Models Model fit and path coefficients
for the proposed structural equation models were estimated with R package lavaan (Rosseel 2012). Maximum
likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR)
was used because of the skewness in the outcome variables. For model fit indices, the model chi-square with its
degrees of freedom and p value, Steiger-Lind root mean
square error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger 1990)
and its 90% CI, Bentler comparative fit index (CFI;
Bentler 1990), and standardized root mean square residual
(SRMR) of each model were reported as recommended by
Kline (2015). The null hypothesis of the chi-square test
was that the proposed variance-covariance matrices and
sample matrices are the same. RMSEA values below
0.05, 0.08, and 0.10 were indicative of close, reasonable,
and mediocre fit, respectively (Browne and Cudeck
1992). For the 90% confidence intervals, lower values
less than 0.05 and upper values less than 0.08 were considered acceptable. CFI values greater than 0.95 and
SRMR values less than 0.08 suggested good fit (Hu and
Bentler 1999).
Model Comparisons and Model Building A series of nested
model comparisons were conducted to build the most parsimonious model. The scaled chi-square difference test (Satorra
2000) was conducted to make comparisons among nested
models.
Indirect Effects Indirect effect sizes were calculated using
lavaan, and the standard errors and confidence intervals for
the indirect effects were computed with 5000 bootstrap samples. All data analyses were completed using RStudio
(RStudio Team 2015).
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
Results
Preliminary Analysis
All means, standard deviations, missing rates, and bivariate
correlations among study variables and covariates are
displayed in Table 1. In particular, boys were reported to display higher externalizing [t (2093.44) = 3.36, p < 0.001, d =
0.15] and internalizing behaviors [t (2084.60) = 2.31, p =
0.021, d = 0.10]. Parents of boys reported lower dispositional
mindfulness [t (1933.62) = − 2.23, p = 0.026, d = −0.10] and
higher negative parenting [t (2140.82) = 3.91, p < 0.001, d =
0.17]. No study variables differed across parent gender.
We then examined whether missingness in study variables
was associated with demographic characteristics. The families
who did not fill out the CBCL (resulting in missingness for
internalizing and externalizing behaviors) tended to also report lower income [t (158.42) = − 2.70, p = 0.008, d = − 0.24]
and parent education [t (157.80) = − 3.98, p < 0.001, d = −
0.36]. Similarly, the parents with missingness in dispositional
mindfulness tended to report low education [t (331.68) = −
6.91, p < 0.001, d = − 0.47] and income [t (337.14) = −4.87,
p < .001, d = −0.32]. The parents who did not fill out the
MAPS measure (resulting in missingness in positive and negative parenting) had fewer years of education than those who
completed the measure [t (73.82) = −2.63, p = 0.010, d = −
0.31].
Model Building and Direct Effects
The proposed model fit well to the data (χ2 (4) = 7.99,
p = .092; RMSEA = 0.03 (90% CI = 0.00, 0.06); CFI =
0.999; SRMR = 0.007). A series of nested model comparisons
were conducted to build the most parsimonious model possible. First, we removed the direct paths from parent dispositional mindfulness to internalizing and externalizing behaviors. The model fit was not worse after this constraint (Δ χ2
(4) = 3.34, p = 0.50). Next, we constrained the direct paths
from mindful parenting to internalizing and externalizing behaviors to be 0. Such constraints did not change the model fit
(Δ χ2 (4) = 7.15, p = 0.12). Therefore, these paths were
trimmed from the final model. The following model comparison showed that the paths from dispositional mindfulness to
positive and negative parenting practices were different from 0
and needed to be kept in the final model (Δ χ2 (4) = 185.25,
p < 0.001). Similarly, the paths from dispositional mindfulness
to parenting (Δ χ2 (2) = 65.12, p < 0.001) remained in the
final model.
The final model fits well to the data (χ2 (12) = 18.51,
p = 0.101; RMSEA = 0.02 (90% CI = 0.00, 0.04); CFI =
0.997; SRMR = 0.012). Standardized direct path estimates for mothers and fathers in the final model are
339
− 0.15**
− 0.26**
0.42**
− 0.05*
− 0.02
0.10**
0.00
− 0.01
− 0.07**
0.24**
0.21**
− 0.04
**p < 0.01
*p < 0.05
0.93
1.60
1.27
9.40
11.31
5.02
10. Dispo mindfulness
11. Positive parenting
12. Negative parenting
Dispo mindfulness, dispositional mindfulness
− 0.03
0.11**
0.07**
− 0.07**
Indirect Effects
11.89
3.13
3.13
− 0.06*
− 0.11**
− 0.01
0.22**
0.20**
− 0.05*
0.60**
− 0.06**
0.07**
0.05*
0.03
− 0.01
− 0.01
− 0.09**
0.11**
− 0.04
0.02
8.09
9.83
8. Internalizing
9. Mindful parenting
2.34
0.96
6.30
0.00
0.04
− 0.02
− 0.09**
0.13**
0.04
0.00
0.07**
01.00
− 0.09**
− 0.14**
0.12**
0.20**
− 0.02
− 0.05*
9.40
38.46
2.58
5.79
0.23
0.52
10.08
1. Child age
2. Parent age
3. Parent education
4. Income
5. Parent gender
6. Child gender
7. Externalizing
1.77
4.43
1.03
2.94
0.42
0.50
2.59
0.00
3.98
0.00
0.00
0.00
0.00
6.30
0.28**
0.19**
− 0.11**
0.00
0.03
− 0.01
− 0.55**
− 0.11**
− 0.02
− 0.09**
− 0.15**
− 0.22**
0.30**
0.23**
0.51**
− 0.01
10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
Missing (%)
SD
M
Variable
Means, standard deviations (SD), missing rates, and zero-order correlations of the study variables
Table 1
shown in Fig. 1. As expected, parent dispositional
mindfulness was positively associated with mindful parenting for both mothers and fathers. Results of the chisquare difference test showed that constraining this regression coefficient being equal across mothers and fathers did not worsen the model fit (Δ χ2 (1) = 1.76, p =
0.18). The association between dispositional mindfulness
and mindful parenting tended to be stronger for
mothers. Mothers’ and fathers’ dispositional mindfulness
was positively associated with positive parenting practices and negatively associated negative parenting practices (Δ χ2 (1) = 0.52, p = 0.47 for positive parenting; Δ
χ2 (1) = 0.51, p = 0.47 for negative parenting). Mothers’
and fathers’ mindful parenting was positively associated
only with positive parenting practices. Mindful parenting was not significantly associated with negative parenting practices for either mothers or fathers. Mindful
parenting was equally predictive of parenting practices
for mothers and fathers (Δ χ2 (1) = 0.01, p = 0.91 for
positive parenting; Δ χ2 (1) = 0.04, p = 0.83 for negative
parenting). Higher levels of mothers’ and fathers’ positive parenting practices were predictive of lower levels
of child internalizing and externalizing behaviors,
whereas higher levels of mothers’ and fathers’ negative
parenting practices were predictive of higher levels of
child internalizing and externalizing behaviors. The association between negative parenting practices and externalizing behaviors was stronger for mothers (Δ χ2
(1) = 5.71, p = 0.017), whereas the other links were not
different across parent gender (Δ χ2 (1) = 0.03, p = 0.86)
for positive parenting to externalizing behaviors; Δ χ2
(1) = 0.01, p = 0.93 for negative parenting to internalizing behavior; Δ χ2 (1) = 0.02, p = 0.36 for positive parenting to internalizing behaviors).
0.41**
− 0.20**
11
− 0.30**
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
As shown in Table 2, higher levels of parent dispositional
mindfulness were indirectly associated with higher levels
of positive parenting through a higher level of mindful
parenting. The indirect effect between dispositional mindfulness and negative parenting practices through mindful
parenting was not significant. Mindful parenting was negatively associated with internalizing and externalizing behaviors through increased levels of positive parenting practices, but not through negative parenting practices. As expected, the indirect effect of dispositional mindfulness on
child internalizing and externalizing behaviors through
mindful parenting and subsequent positive parenting was
significantly different from 0 (b = − 0.04, SE = 0.01, 95%
CI = [− 0.06, − 0.02] for child internalizing behaviors; b =
− 0.04, SE = 0.01, 95% CI = [− 0.06, − 0.02] for child externalizing behaviors).
340
Table 2
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
Indirect effects from the final path model and corresponding standard errors (SE)
Estimate
SE
z-value
p value
7.884
< 0.001
Dispo mindful–mindful parenting–pos parenting
0.162
0.021
Dispo mindful–mindful parenting–neg parenting
0.010
0.009
1.092
0.275
Mindful parenting–pos parenting–int
Mindful parenting–pos parenting–ext
− 0.177
− 0.175
0.035
0.035
− 5.073
− 4.960
< 0.001
< 0.001
Mindful parenting–neg parenting–int
Mindful parenting–neg parenting–ext
− 0.010
− 0.010
0.010
0.010
− 1.061
− 1.060
0.289
0.289
Dispo mindful–mindful parenting–pos parenting–int
Dispo mindful–mindful parenting–pos parenting–ext
− 0.039
− 0.039
− 0.002
0.009
0.009
0.002
0.002
− 4.445
− 4.339
− 1.042
− 1.041
< 0.001
< 0.001
0.297
0.298
Dispo mindful–mindful parenting–neg parenting–int
Dispo mindful–mindful parenting–neg parenting–ext
− 0.002
Confidence intervals
(0.123, 0.204)
(− 0.007, 0.009)
(− 0.249, − 0.111)
(− 0.246, − 0.107)
(− 0.030, 0.008)
(− 0.030, 0.008)
(− 0.058, − 0.024)
(− 0.057, − 0.023)
(− 0.007, 0.002)
(− 0.007, 0.002)
Dispo mindful, dispositional mindfulness; Pos, positive; Neg, negative; Int, internalizing problems; Ext, externalizing problems
Discussion
Given the possible differences in parenting processing (e.g.,
mindful parenting and parenting practices) between Western
and Eastern cultures, the current study aimed to replicate the
model proposed by Parent et al. (2016) using a sample of
Mainland Chinese families to determine if similar effects
would be observed in a Chinese cultural context.
The present model suggests that the increases in parents’
dispositional mindfulness are negatively associated with children’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms through
mindful parenting and subsequent parenting practices. In addition, this study aimed to determine if parents’ gender influenced child outcomes. We hypothesized that parents’ dispositional mindfulness would be positively associated with mindful parenting which, in turn, would be associated with greater
positive parenting practices and fewer negative parenting
practices. We also hypothesized that positive parenting practices would be associated with decreases in child internalizing
and externalizing behaviors, while negative parenting practices would be associated with increases in child internalizing
and externalizing behaviors. Finally, we hypothesized that our
findings would replicate those found by Parent et al. (2016),
such that parent dispositional mindfulness would have a negative indirect effect on children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors through an increase in mindful parenting and
positive parenting practices, as well as a decrease in negative
parenting practices.
The current study produced mixed results in replicating the
findings of the original study conducted in a Western cultural
context. Our findings were consistent in that parents’ dispositional mindfulness was associated with mindful parenting, and
negative parenting practices were related to child internalizing
and externalizing symptoms. In the Chinese sample, parents’
dispositional mindfulness was also associated with positive
parenting practices, such that parents with higher levels of
dispositional mindfulness reported greater use of positive parenting practices. In addition, higher levels of positive parenting were also associated with decreases in child internalizing
and externalizing behaviors. While all pathways were significant in the original Western sample, the relationship between
parents’ mindful parenting and negative parenting practices in
the current study was not significant. These results indicate
that for Chinese parents, mindful parenting was more effective
on parents’ positive behaviors such as positive reinforcement,
warmth, and supportiveness for children rather than negative
parenting practices.
In the current study, we created separate models for
mothers and fathers to explore gender differences in mindful
parenting. The results indicate that parents’ dispositional
mindfulness was associated with mindful parenting for both
mothers and fathers, but this link was stronger for mothers.
This association was, in turn, linked to only positive parenting
practices in both mothers and fathers. In the final component
of the model, positive and negative parenting practices were
associated with child internalizing and externalizing behaviors
regardless of parent’s gender. Specifically, positive parenting
was directly related to decreases in children’s internalizing and
externalizing behaviors, whereas negative parenting was directly related to increases in children’s behaviors. These results were consistent with Western findings (Jones et al. 2008;
McKee et al. 2018). In addition, the association between negative parenting practices and increased child behaviors suggests that hostile (i.e., overcontrolling), harsh (i.e., yelling and
threatening), and the use of physical control (i.e., physical
discipline) may have more implications for externalizing behaviors. This relationship was accentuated in a mother-child
relationship, such that the above traits were more strongly
associated with increases in child behaviors when observed
in the sample of mothers as compared to fathers.
Our findings provide initial evidence for the importance of
mindfulness in parent-child relationship, as there are clear
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
benefits to being present-focused and attentive ar the moment
when interacting with children. The integration of mindfulness skills and parenting practices can increase parents’ positive interactions with children, as modeling these positive behaviors (i.e., self-regulation and providing direct attention)
and exhibiting fewer negative behaviors (i.e., corporal punishment and dysregulation while angry) can likely lead to improvements in children’s own behaviors. Consequently, the
reduction of negative parenting behaviors may, in turn, be
associated with reductions in internalizing and externalizing
behaviors among children.
Limitations
There are several limitations of our study that should be noted.
First, a clear limitation is that temporal order cannot definitively be established based on cross-sectional analyses as used
in this study. The use of a cross-sectional design to test mediation seriously limits the reliability of the findings because
cross-sectional data often lead to biased estimates when compared to longitudinal data (Maxwell and Cole 2007). Future
research should use longitudinal data, preferably with at least
three separate measurements across time to assess not only the
amount of change (which can be assessed with two measurements) but also the rate of change (i.e., developmental trajectory) which can be assessed only with three or more wellspaced measurements. Second, our sample was restricted to
parents of children ages 6 to 12; future research should incorporate additional developmental time points to determine the
extent to which these findings are consistent across different
age groups. Moreover, clinical samples should be considered
for more targeted intervention programs. Third, our study may
be subject to common method bias, which is a welldocumented phenomenon observed in research based on
self-reported measures (Lindell and Whitney 2001).
Measuring multiple constructs using common methods (e.g.,
multiple-item scales presented within the same survey) often
leads to spurious effects due to the measurement instruments
than to the constructs being measured (Podsakoff et al. 2003).
For example, spurious rather than true correlations among the
constructs being measured may result from response styles,
social desirability, and priming effects. Future research can
avoid common methods bias using multiple methods or instruments. Despite these limitations, the current study extends
our understanding of potential mechanisms that could account
for the relationship between parent dispositional mindfulness
and child internalizing and externalizing behaviors in one
Eastern population.
Author Contributions ZRH: designed and executed the study, and
drafted the manuscript. NA: collected data and drafted the manuscript.
JY: analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. XH: assisted with the
writing and editing of the manuscript. JP: designed the study, helped with
341
data analysis, and edited the manuscript.CD and KD: drafted the manuscript. NNS: designed the study, helped with data analysis, and edited the
manuscript.
Funding This research was supported by the 2019 Comprehensive
Discipline Construction Fund of Faculty of Education, Beijing Normal
University, and the Funding of International Center for Educational
Research, ICER, Faculty of Education, Beijing Normal University.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human
participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Beijing
University Institutional Research Board (IRB) and with the 1964 Helsinki
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use,
distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18,
YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont,
Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.
Achenbach, T. M., Mcconaughy, S. H., Ivanova, M. Y., & Rescorla, L. A.
(2011). Manual for the ASEBA brief problem monitor (bpm/6).
Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children,
Youth, & Families.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills.
Assessment, 11, 191–206.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer,
S., & William, J. M. G. (2008). Construct validity of the five-facet
mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15, 329–342.
Baharudin, R., Krauss, S. E., Yacoob, S. N., & Pei, T. J. (2011). Family
processes as predictors of antisocial behaviors among adolescents
from urban, single-mother Malay families in Malaysia. Journal of
Comparative Family Studies, 42, 509–522.
Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2006). Young
adult outcome of hyperactive children: adaptive functioning in major life activities. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 192–202.
Bazzano, A., Wolfe, C., Zylowska, L., Wang, S., Schuster, E., Barrett, C.,
& Lehrer, D. (2015). Mindfulness based stress reduction (MBSR)
for parents and caregivers of individuals with developmental disabilities: a community-based approach. Journal of Child and Family
Studies, 24, 298–308.
342
Beaujean, A. A. (2012). Latent variable modeling using R. New York:
Routledge.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models.
Psychology Bulletin, 107, 256–259.
Bishop, S., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N., Carmody, J.,
et al. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241.
Bögels, S., & Restifo, K. (2014). Mindful parenting: a guide for mental
health practitioners. New York: Springer.
Bögels, S., Hellemans, J., van Deursen, S., Römer, M., & van der Meulen,
R. (2013). Mindful parenting in mental health care: effects on parental and child psychopathology, parental stress, parenting,
coparenting, and marital functioning. Mindfulness, 5, 536–551.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present:
mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Addressing fundamental questions about mindfulness. Psychological Inquiry,
18(4), 272–281.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing
model fit. Sociological Methods & Research, 21, 230–258.
Chen, X., Liu, M., & Li, D. (2000). Parental warmth, control, and indulgence and their relations to adjustment in Chinese children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 401–419.
Coatsworth, J. D., Duncan, L. G., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Gayles, J.
G., Bamberger, K. T., et al. (2015). Integrating mindfulness with
parent training: effects of the mindfulness-enhanced strengthening
families program. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 26–35.
de Bruin, E. I., Zijlstra, B. J., Geurtzen, N., van Zundert, R. M., Van, d. W.
E., Hartman, E. E., et al. (2014). Mindful parenting assessed further:
psychometric properties of the Dutch version of the interpersonal
mindfulness in parenting scale (IM-P). Mindfulness, 5, 200–212.
Deng, Y. Q., & Xia, C. Y. (2011). The five-facet mindfulness questionnaire: psychometric properties of the Chinese version. Mindfulness,
2, 123–128.
Duncan, L. G., Coatsworth, J. D., & Greenberg, M. T. (2009). A model of
mindful parenting: implications for parent–child relationships and
prevention research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review,
12, 255–270.
Friedlmeier, W., Corapci, F., & Cole, P. M. (2011). Emotion socialization
in cross-cultural perspective. Social and Personality Psychology
Compass, 5, 410–427.
Geurtzen, N., Scholte, R. H. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., Tak, Y. R., &
Zundert, R. M. P. V. (2015). Association between mindful parenting
and adolescents’ internalizing problems: non-judgmental acceptance of parenting as core element. Journal of Child and Family
Studies, 24, 1117–1128.
Goldberg, S. B., Tucker, R. P., Greene, P. A., Davidson, R. J., Wampold,
B. E., Kearney, D. J., & Simpson, T. L. (2017). Mindfulness-based
interventions for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 59, 52–60.
Gouveia, M. J., Carona, C., Canavarro, M. C., & Moreira, H. (2016).
Self-compassion and dispositional mindfulness are associated with
parenting styles and parenting stress: the mediating role of mindful
parenting. Mindfulness, 7, 700–712.
Harold, G. T., Elam, K. K., Lewis, G., Rice, F., & Thapar, A. (2012).
Interparental conflict, parent psychopathology, hostile parenting,
and child antisocial behavior: examining the role of maternal versus
paternal influences using a novel genetically sensitive research design. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 1283–1295.
Haydicky, J., Shecter, C., Wiener, J., & Ducharme, J. M. (2015).
Evaluation of mbct for adolescents with ADHD and their parents:
impact on individual and family functioning. Journal of Child and
Family Studies, 24, 76–94.
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture and organizations. International Studies of
Management and Organization, 10, 15–41.
Jones, D. J., Forehand, R., Rakow, A., Colletti, C., McKee, L., & Zalot,
A. (2008). The specificity of maternal parenting behavior and child
adjustment difficulties: a study of inner-city African American families. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 181–192.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past,
present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10,
144–156.
Kabat-Zinn, M., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (1997). Everyday blessings: the inner
work of mindful parenting. New York: Hyperion.
Kawabata, Y., Alink, L. R. A., Tseng, W. L., Ijzendoorn, M. H. V., &
Crick, N. R. (2011). Maternal and paternal parenting styles associated with relational aggression in children and adolescents: a conceptual analysis and meta-analytic review. Developmental Review,
31, 240–278.
Keng, S. L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness
on psychological health: a review of empirical studies. Clinical
Psychology Review, 31, 1041–1056.
Klimes-Dougan, B., Brand, A. E., Zahn-Waxler, C., Usher, B., Hastings,
P. D., Kendziora, K., et al. (2010). Parental emotion socialization in
adolescence: differences in sex, age and problem status. Social
Development, 16, 326–342.
Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation
modeling (4th ed.). New York: Guildford.
Leung, K., Lau, S., & Lam, W. L. (1998). Parenting styles and academic
achievement: a cross-cultural study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44,
157–172.
Lin, X., Li, L., Chi, P., Wang, Z., Heath, M. A., Du, H., & Fang, X.
(2016). Child maltreatment and interpersonal relationship among
Chinese children with oppositional defiant disorder. Child Abuse
and Neglect, 51, 192–202.
Lindblom, J., Vänskä, M., Flykt, M., Tolvanen, A., Tiitinen, A., Tulppala,
M., & Punamäki, R.-L. (2017). From early family systems to internalizing symptoms: the role of emotion regulation and peer relations. Journal of Family Psychology, 31, 316–326.
Lindell, M. K., & Whitney, D. J. (2001). Accounting for common method
variance in cross-sectional research designs. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 86(1), 114–121.
Lo, H. H. M., Yeung, J. W. K., Duncan, L. G., Ma, Y., Siu, A. F. Y., Chan,
S. K. C., et al. (2018). Validating of the interpersonal mindfulness in
parenting scale in Hong Kong Chinese. Mindfulness, 5, 1390–1401.
Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of
longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods, 12, 23–44.
McKee, L., Parent, J., Zachery, C. R., & Forehand, R. (2018). Mindful
parenting and emotion socialization practices: concurrent and longitudinal associations. Family Process, 57, 752–766.
Medeiros, C., Gouveia, M. J., Canavarro, M. C., & Moreira, H. (2016).
The indirect effect of the mindful parenting of mothers and fathers
on the child’s perceived well-being through the child’s attachment to
parents. Mindfulness, 7(4), 916–927.
Moreira, H., & Canavarro, M. C. (2018). The association between selfcritical rumination and parenting stress: the mediating role of mindful parenting. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 27, 2265–2275.
National Bureau of Statistics of the People’s Republic of China. (2017).
Household report of urban Chinese families. Retrieved from http://
www.stats.gov.cn/tjsj/zxfb/201801/t20180118_1574931.html.
Accessed 18 Jan 2018.
Parent, J., & Forehand, R. (2017). The multidimensional assessment of
parenting scale (MAPS): development and psychometric properties.
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(8), 2136–2151.
Parent, J., Garai, E., Forehand, R., Roland, E., Champion, J. E., Haker,
K., Hardcastle, E. J., & Compas, B. E. (2010). Parent mindfulness
Mindfulness (2021) 12:334–343
and child outcome: The roles of parent depressive symptoms and
parenting. Mindfulness, 1, 254–264. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s12671-010-0034-1.
Parent, J., McKee, L. G., Rough, J. N., & Forehand, R. (2016). The
association of parent mindfulness with parenting and youth psychopathology across three developmental stages. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 44, 191–202.
Pepping, C., O’Donovan, A., & Davis, P. (2013). The positive effects of
mindfulness on self-esteem. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 8,
376–386.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003).
Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of
the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 88(5), 879–903.
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 1–36.
RStudio Team. (2015). RStudio: integrated development for R. RStudio,
Inc. Boston: Author.
Satorra, A. (2000). Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi-sample
analysis of moment structures. Adv. Stud. Theoret. Appl.
Econometrics, 36, 233–247.
Siu, A. F. Y., Ma, Y., & Chui, F. W. Y. (2016). Maternal mindfulness and
child social behavior: the mediating role of the mother-child relationship. Mindfulness, 7, 577–583.
Smith, J. D., & Dishion, T. J. (2013). Mindful parenting in the development and maintenance of youth psychopathology. In J. T.
343
Ehrenreich-May & B. C. Chu (Eds.), Transdiagnostic mechanisms
and treatment for youth psychopathology (pp. 138–160). New York:
Guilford Press.
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: an
interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25,
173–180.
Wang, G., Liu, L., Tan, X., & Zheng, W. (2017). The moderating effect of
dispositional mindfulness on the relationship between materialism
and mental health. Personality and Individual Differences, 107,
131–136.
Williams, K. L., & Wahler, R. G. (2010). Are mindful parents more
authoritative and less authoritarian? an analysis of clinic-referred
mothers. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 230–235.
Xu, Y., Zhang, L., & Hee, P. (2014). Parenting practices and shyness in
Chinese children. In H. Selin (Ed.), Parenting across cultures: child
rearing, motherhood and fatherhood in non-western cultures (pp.
13–24). New York: Springer.
Yan, J., Han, Z. R., Tang, Y., & Zhang, X. (2017). Parental support for
autonomy and child depressive symptoms in middle childhood: The
mediating role of parent–child attachment. Journal of Child and
Family Studies, 26, 1970–1978.
Yap, M. B., & Jorm, A. F. (2015). Parental factors associated with childhood anxiety, depression, and internalizing problems: a systematic
review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175, 424–
440.