AFRIKA UND ÜBERSEE
Trilingual Journal of African Languages and Cultures
Revue trilingue des langues et cultures africaines
Dreisprachige Zeitschrift für afrikanische Sprachen und Kulturen
Volume 93
The expression of diminutivity in Central Ring Grassfields
Bantu
Pius W. Akumbua
& Roland Kießlingb
Universität Hamburgab
[email protected]
[email protected]
DOI: 10.15460/auue.2021.93.1.203
Peer-reviewed article
Submitted: 28.1.2020
Accepted: 26.3.2020
Published: 31.12.2020
Recommended citation:
Akumbu, Pius W. & Roland Kießling. 2020. The expression
of diminutivity in Central Ring Grassfields Bantu. Afrika und
Übersee 93. 257–280.
Licence: © Pius W. Akumbu and Roland Kießling. This article is licensed under the
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Published by Hamburg University Press
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
The expression of diminutivity in
Central Ring Grassfields Bantu 1
Pius W. Akumbua & Roland Kießlingb
Universität Hamburgab
[email protected]
[email protected]
Abstract:
Studies on the expression of diminutivity in Bantoid languages of the
Cameroonian Grassfields have tended to focus on the role that noun class
derivation plays within the familiar Bantu paradigm. A closer look at
individual branches of Bantoid, however, reveals a more complex picture, which rather suggests a division of labour between derivational
strategies and compounding and/or periphrasis. This contribution zooms
in on the languages of the Central Ring (CR) branch of Grassfields Bantu,
presenting an overview of diminutivisation strategies found here: the notorious transfer to gender 19/6a, which is at times, accompanied by the
addition of a semantically bleached suffix –CV, and periphrasis in associative constructions headed by nouns with inherent diminutive meanings
such as ‘child’.
Keywords: Central Ring Grassfields Bantu languages, diminutivity,
diminutives, noun classes, gender
1 Introduction
While diminutives have been studied extensively for their forms and
meanings both from a universal perspective (Jurafsky 1996, Bakema
& Geeraerts 2000, Grandi & Körtvélyessy 2015) and in Bantu specifically (Gibson, Guérois & Marten 2017), they have been largely
neglected in studies on Grassfields Bantu languages, beyond the
general statement that gender 19/6a is employed for this purpose
(Hyman 1979: 24, Hyman 1980: 234, Tamanji 2009: 31, Akumbu
1 We gratefully acknowledge the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation for a
Georg Forster Research Fellowship for Experienced Researchers granted to the first
author (2019–2021) and which has allowed for greater collaboration and research
on this paper.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
257
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
& Chibaka 2012: 54, Möller 2012: 12, Asohsi 2015: 68, Voll 2017:
90). Diminutives are grammatical “elements which make a semantic
contribution pertaining to size” (Gibson et al. 2017: 344) in that they
primarily express ‘physical smallness’ (Schneider 2003: 10). Other
– derived – semantic functions include young age, insignificance or
incompleteness, as well as relation or descent (Jurafsky 1996). Moreover, “diminutives can also be used to convey perspectives and subjective viewpoints, as well as to encode pejorative meanings along
the lines of disdain or contempt, or ameliorative meanings encoding
affection and admiration” (Gibson et al. 2017: 344). This study investigates the forms and functions of diminutives in the Central Ring
(CR) branch of Grassfields Bantu, drawing primarily on data from
six of the seven CR languages, i.e. Babanki, Kom, Kung, Kuk, Men
and Oku.2 After a brief overview of the expression of diminutives in
Bantu in section 2, section 3 discusses the morphological strategies
of diminutivisation in CR. Section 4 sketches a prominent alternative strategy of diminutivisation attested in CR, i.e. periphrasis by an
associative construction headed by the noun ‘child’ or ‘tiny item’. A
conclusion is provided in section 5.
2 Diminutives in Bantu
Diminutives in Bantu are “thought to have been historically expressed
as part of the noun class system, and several noun classes have been
2 Although closely related to Narrow Bantu, Grassfields Bantu languages show
remarkable differences in all parts of their grammar. CR languages themselves have
many features in common and a comparison of vocabularies between immediate
neighbours, such as Babanki and Kom (Brye 2001) show that they share at least 70%
of their vocabulary. Until recently, only five languages (Kom, Oku, Babanki, Men
and Bum) have been listed as CR (Dieu & Renaud 1983, Breton & Fohtung 1991,
Watters 2003). However, it has now been proposed that Kuk and Kung also belong
to this subgroup (Tatang 2016, Eberhard, Simons & Fennig 2019). There are nearly
400,000 people who speak these languages, as follows: Kom 233,000, Oku 40,000,
Babanki 39,000, Men 35,000, Bum 21,000, Kuk 3,000, and Kung 1,750 (Eberhard et
al. 2019). Bum has been left out of this study because we were unable to obtain substantial data from the limited existing literature on the language and also because
we did not have access to any Bum speaker at the time we could have collected
the necessary data (2018–2019). This was due, in part, to the political crisis in the
English-speaking regions of Cameroon that started in October 2016 and significantly
prevented movement to the area, as well as led to the displacement of speakers into
more remote areas for their safety.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
258
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
reconstructed as including diminutive meaning” (Gibson et al. 2017).
In this regard, the Proto-Bantu class 12 prefix *ka- is most often associated with diminutives while the class 13 prefix *tu- acts as the corresponding diminutive plural class marker (Bleek 1862/9, Meinhof
1910[1899], Meeussen 1967, Maho 1999, Demuth 2000). Synchronic
use of gender 12/13 for diminutives in Bantu is seen in Chindamba
(1) and Kimbundu (2).
(1)
(2)
Chindamba (G52, Edelsten & Lijongwa 2010: 36–38, Gibson et
al. 2017: 348)
li-piki
‘tree’
(class 5)
ma-piki
‘trees’
(class 6)
ka-piki
‘small tree’
(class 12)
tu-piki
‘small trees’
(class 13)
Kimbundu (H21, Quintão 1934: 18, Gibson et al. 2017: 348)
di-tadi
‘stone’
(class 5)
ma-tadi
‘stones’
(class 6)
ka-di-tadi
‘small stone’
(class 12+class 5)
tu-ma-tadi
‘small stones’
(class 13+class 6)
Nevertheless, as noted by Maho (1999: 252, 262), class 12 is not
evenly distributed in the whole Bantu area, as class 20 *ɣù- (3),
gender 7/8 *kì-/*βì- (4) and class 19 *pì- (5) compete for diminutive
meaning.
(3) Venda (S53, Poulos 1986: 289, 1990: 38, Gibson et al. 2017:
375)
ku-thavha
‘small mountain’
(class 20)
ku-thavh-ana
‘very small mountain’ (class 20 + -ana)
(4) Tsonga (S53, Poulos 1999: 206, Gibson et al. 2017: 375)
muti
‘village’
(class 3)
xi-mut-ana
‘small village’
(class 7 + -ana)
swi-mut-ana
‘small villages’
(class 8 + -ana)
(5)
Nomaánde (A46, Wilkendorf 2001: 15, Gibson et al. 2017: 351)
o-túmbe
‘walking cane’
(class 3)
hi-túmbétumbe ‘small cane’
(class 19)
In fact, Proto-Bantu *pi- might have a Proto-Benue-Congo ancestor
in *pi- (de Wolf 1971: 170–1) which is assumed to have been grammaticalised from a prior independent noun pi or bi ‘child’, reflexes of
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
259
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
which are attested in other branches of Niger Congo (Kähler-Meyer
1971: 347–348). The diminutive prefix either replaces the ‘original’
noun class prefix in most languages or it is added on top of it in a few
cases (Maho 1999). Diminutive classes in Bantu are typically used
for secondary classification (Meeussen 1967, Maho 1999). For this
purpose, “a noun typically found in a different class is used in the
diminutive class for a specific semantic effect” (Gibson et al. 2017:
359). However, there are also instances where nouns are primarily
members of the diminutive class without necessarily being physically
small. While Bantu languages predominantly employ their noun class
system for the expression of diminutivity, other strategies such as
derivational suffixes and compounding processes are also used (Maho
1999, Gibson et al. 2017: 348).
3 Diminutives in Central Ring Grassfields Bantu
Diminutives in Central Ring (CR) primarily express physical smallness, but can also encode an offspring relationship, young age, inferiority and/or deficiency. As in other Bantu languages, a shift from one
class to the diminutive class in CR also results in an interpretation
of the noun concerned as physically small, that is, as falling short
of the prototypical size of the referent class member. Therefore, the
entities referred to by diminutives are regarded as smaller members
of the category. Thus, fə̄tsôtə̀ (19/6a) in Men refers to a brook, creek
or rivulet which is smaller in size than what is perceived as standard
for its derivative source noun, i.e. tsò (9/10) ‘river’.3 This is not to
say that the referents of diminutive forms are small by an absolute
standard. Speakers represent a referent as small for a particular communicative purpose, i.e. “smallness is not necessarily perceived, but
in fact ascribed” (Schneider 2003: 11).
Derived diminutives may undergo semantic specialisations, e.g.
Men ēkyû (3/6a) ‘bed’ derives the diminutive fə̄kîə (19/6a) ‘stool’ and
Babanki kə̀tíʔə́tíʔə́ ‘ear’ derives the diminutive fə̀tíʔə́tíʔə́ ‘mushroom
(sp.)’, as motivated by similarity in form. As seen in (6) diminutive
derivation of the concept vein from the concept root is recurrently
observed in CR, e.g. in Men, Kom and Babanki.
3 The morphological derivation of diminutives such as this will be dealt with in
sections 3.1 and 3.2 in more detail.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
260
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
(6) CR diminutive derivation of vein from root
PR *-ɣàŋ ́ Babanki Kom
Men
gloss
*5/6
ə̀-ɣáŋ
ī-ɣâŋ
ē-ɣâŋ (5/13) ‘root’
*19/6a
fə̀-ɣáŋ
fɨ ̄-ɣâŋ
fə̄-ɣâŋ
‘vein, artery’
Diminutives may also entail pejorative or derogatory meanings. Thus,
fə̀ŋgə̀ŋtə̂ ‘small house’ (< ə̀ŋgə̀ŋ (3~5/6) ‘house’), to a Babanki speaker,
can express a negative attitude toward the referent, reflecting the
view that the owner could have built a much bigger house. It could
also mean that someone else will eventually build a bigger house
than the diminutivised one. Linked to pejorative connotations is the
notion of inability or incapability of a person or group of people and
things. For example, Babanki fə̀wìʔtə̂ (< wìʔ (1/2) ‘person’) normally
means ‘small person’, but can also be used to designate someone who
has not achieved much, be they physically small or big. Such pejorative connotations can be adduced for the rest of CR, e.g. in Kung
fə̄fûɔ ‘small and feeble thing’ (< kə̄fúɔ (7/8) ‘thing’) (Kießling 2019:
149). When used on body parts, diminutives can also be a form of
insult, e.g. Babanki fə̀ʃɨ ̀lə̀ ‘small eye’ (< ə̀ʃɨ ́ (5/6) ‘eye’) does not really
mean that the eye is physically small but could be a way to simply
humiliate the person concerned. In the next two subsections we focus
on the morphological strategies of diminutivisation in CR, i.e. the
transfer to gender 19/6a (3.1), and concomitant suffixation (3.2).
3.1 Shift to gender 19/6a
The most common strategy of diminutive formation in CR is derivation by which a noun is shifted to gender 19/6a, as described for
Babanki (Akumbu & Chibaka 2012), Oku (Yensi 1996), Men (Möller
2012), Kuk (Kießling 2016) and Kung (Kießling 2019: 149).4 Class
prefixes of gender 19/6a which are used for diminutivisation might
4 The data used in this study have mostly been taken from the following sources: Babanki (Akumbu & Chibaka 2012), Kom (Jones 2001), Oku (Yensi 1996, Blood
& Davis 1999), Men (Chiatoh 1993, Mua 2015, Möller 2012, Björkestedt 2011, Bangha 2003), Kuk (Kießling 2016, Pleus 2015) and Kung (Kießling 2019: 149; Schlenker 2012). Babanki data have been supplemented by the first author. Men, Kuk
and Kung data have been supplemented based on fieldnotes by the second author.
Surprisingly, published sources on Kom and Oku do not seem to provide any information on diminutives and we had to collect supplementary data to fill the gap.
Proto-Ring reconstructions are taken from Hyman (2007).
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
261
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
attach to nouns of class 1 or 9 which come without a class prefix
as shown in (7) or they replace a pre-existent class prefix as in (8).5
Remarkably, stem alternations which characterize the base forms of
gender 1/2 and 9/10, e.g. consonant alternations such as v ~ɣ and w
~ ɣ, are absent in their diminutive counterparts.
(7)
CR diminutive derivation in 19/6a of nouns without a class
prefix
Babanki
Kom
Kuk
Base
diminutive 19/6a
wáin (1/2) ‘child’,
pl. wɔ́indā
ŋgvɨ ̄ (9/10) ‘chicken’,
pl. ŋgvɨ ̄sə̄
wāe (1/2) ‘child’,
pl. ʌ̄wáe
byī (9/10) ‘goat’, pl. sə̄byí
fɨ ̄wáintɨ ̀ ‘little child’,
pl. mɨ ̄wáintɨ ̀
fɨ ̄ŋgvɨ ̄tɨ ̂ ‘small chicken’,
pl. mɨ ̄ŋgvɨ ̄tɨ ̂
fə̄wáe ‘tiny feeble child’,
pl. m̄ wáe
fə̄byí ‘smallish goat’, pl. m̄ byí
wàn (1/2) ‘child’,
pl. vúnə́
gɨ ̀ (9/10) ‘voice’, pl. gɨ ́ꜜsə́
Kung
wāe (1/2) ‘child’,
pl. ʌ̄(ɣ)wáe
ʙə̀° (9/10) ‘goat’, pl. sə̄ʙə᷇
Men
váin (1/2) ‘child’,
pl. ʌ̄ɣɔ́in
tsò (9/10) ‘river’, pl. sētsò
Oku
wîl (1/2) ‘person’, pl. ɣɨ ́lɨ ̄
ŋgvə́ə (9/10) ‘chicken’,
pl. ŋgvə́əsē
(8)
fə̀wàntə̂ ‘little child’,
pl. mə̀wàntə̂
fə̀gɨ ̀tə̂ ‘tiny voice’, pl. mə̀gɨ ̀tə̂
fə̄wáe ‘tiny feeble child’,
pl. m̄ wáe
fə̄ʙə̂lə̀ ‘small feeble goat’,
pl. m̄ ʙə̂lə̀
fēɣɔ́intə̂ ‘little child’,
pl. m̄ ɣɔ́intə̂
fə̄tsôtə̀ ‘brook, small river’,
pl. m̄ tsôtə̀
fə̀wîl ‘small person’, pl. mə̀wîl
fēŋgvə́ətè ‘small chicken’,
pl. mēŋgvə́ətè
CR Diminutive derivation in 19/6a of nouns with class prefixes
Babanki
Base
ə̀tó (5/13) ‘hut’, pl. tə̀tó
kə̀wʉ́ (7/6) ‘foot’, pl. àwʉ́
diminutive 19/6a
fə̀tótə̀ ‘tiny hut’, pl. mə̀tótə̀
fə̀wʉ́tə̀ ‘small foot’, pl. mə̀wʉ́tə̀
5 All CR languages employ a noun class system of the Bantu type with either
12 (Babanki, Bum, Kuk, Kung) or 13 (Kom, Men, Oku) agreement classes (Akumbu
2019: 2). All of them distinguish class 19 marked by fV- and its corresponding plural
class 6a (also the class for liquids) marked by m(V)-.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
262
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Kom
Kuk
Kung
Men
Oku
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
ātú (7/8) ‘head’, pl. ɨ ̄tú
īsáŋ (5/6) ‘corn’, pl. āsáŋ
īsʌ̂b (5/6) ‘maize cob’,
pl. ʌ̄sʌ̂b
kə̄kɔ́i (7/8) ‘chair’, pl. ūkɔ́i
kə̄bê (7/4) ‘thigh’,
pl. ībê
kə̄fúɔ (7/8) ‘thing’, pl. ūfúɔ
ētʃí (5/6~13) ‘stone’,
pl. ə̄tʃí~tētʃí
āfɨá (7/8) ‘thing’, pl. ēfɨá
ə̄bkún (3/6a) ‘bed’,
pl. ə̄mkún
ɛ̄ʃáŋ (5/6) ‘corn’, pl. ɛ̄yʃáŋ
fɨ ̄túnɨ ̀ ‘small head’, pl. mɨ ̄túnɨ ̀
fɨ ̄sáŋlɨ ̂ ‘small corn’, pl. mɨ ̄sáŋlɨ ̂
fə̄sʌ̂b(lə̀) ‘smallish maize cob’,
pl. m̄ sʌ̂blə̀
fə̄kɔ́i ‘small chair’, pl. m̄ kɔ́i
fə̄bê(lə̀) ‘tiny feeble thigh’,
pl. m̄ bê(lə̀)
fə̄fûɔ ‘tiny thing’, pl. m̄ fûɔ
fə̄tʃílə̂ ‘small stone’, pl. m̄ tʃílə̂
fə̄fɨâ ‘small thing’, pl. m̄ fɨâ
fēkúntè ‘small bed’,
pl. mēkúntè
fēʃáŋtè ‘small corn’, pl. mēʃáŋtè
The addition of noun class prefixes of 19/6a in diminutive function
on top of the original ones, establishing a secondary layer of class
prefixes, seems to be restricted to instances where the original noun
class prefix has started to merge with the root.
A possible candidate is Men fēɣɔ́intə̂ ‘small children’ where the
diminutive prefix is added onto an alleged class 2 prefix ɣ- (Möller
2012: 12).6
Nouns borrowed into CR can also be diminutivised by being shifted
to gender 19/6a, as shown in (9), which proves that this strategy is
indeed very productive in CR.
6 Even more remarkable is the fact that this diminutive is derived from the
plural form ʌ̄ɣɔ́in, not the singular váin. From this perspective, however, it is dubious
whether the segment ɣ is actually to be analysed as plural prefix, since the prefix in
the plural form ʌ̄-ɣɔ́in ‘children’ is ʌ̄-, while the segment ɣ seems to belong to the root.
This is probably due to the special nature of the noun ‘child’ – not only in Men, but
in a number of Ring languages – in that number distinction is not only expressed by
change in NPx but also by suppletion of roots or rather by two distinct forms of one
and the same root whose allomorphic relation has become intransparent by idiosyncratic fusions.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
263
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
(9)
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
CR Diminutive derivation in 19/6a of borrowed words
Babanki
Kom
Kuk
Kung
Oku
Base
bwɔ́tə̀ (1/2) ‘bottle’,
pl. və̀bwɔ́tə̀
bùʃí (1/2) ‘cat’, pl. və̀bùʃí
tʃɔ̂s (1/2) ‘church’, pl. və̀tʃɔ̂s
lámbās (9/10) ‘orange’,
pl. lámbāysɨ ̄
bés (9/10) ‘cat’, pl. béysɨ ̄
tʃɔ̄s (9/10) ‘church’, pl. tʃɔ̄ysɨ ̄
lámâs (9/10) ‘orange’,
pl. sə̀lámâs
bùsí (9/10) ‘cat’, pl. sə̀bùsí
lámâs (9/10) ‘orange’,
pl. sə̀lámâs
bùsí (9/10) ‘cat’, pl. sə̀bùsí
tsɔ̄s (9/10) ‘church’, pl. sə̄tsɔ̄s
bwɔ́tè (1/10) ‘bottle’,
pl. bwɔ́tèsè
bùsé (1/10) ‘cat’, pl. bùsésé
tsɔ́s (1/10) ‘church’, pl. tsɔ́sé
diminutive 19/6a
fə̀bwɔ́tə̀ ‘small bottle’,
pl. mə̀bwɔ́tə̀
fə̀bùʃí ‘small cat’, pl. mə̀bùʃí
fə̀tʃɔ̂ytə̀ ‘tiny church’,
pl. mə̀tʃɔ̂ytə̀
fɨ ̄lámbās ‘small orange’,
pl. mɨ ̄lámbās
fɨ ̄bés ‘small cats’, pl. mɨ ̄bés
fɨ ̄tʃɔ̄s ‘tiny church’, pl. mɨ ̄tʃɔ̄s
fə̄lámâs ‘small orange’,
pl. m̄ lámâs
fə̄bùsí ‘small cats’, pl. m̄ bùsí
fə̄lámâs ‘small orange’,
pl. m̄ lámâs
fə̄bùsí ‘small cat’, pl. m̄ bùsí
fə̄tsɔ̄s ‘tiny church’, pl. m̄ tsɔ̄
fēbwɔ́tè ‘small bottle’,
pl. mēbwɔ́tè
fēbùsé ‘small cats’, pl. mēbùsé
fētsɔ́stè ‘small church’, pl.
mētsɔ́stè
While 19/6a is employed for secondary diminutive classification,
there are nouns that are primarily members of this gender in CR,
without necessarily being physically small. Even those 19/6a nouns
whose referents might be regarded as physically small in comparison
to some absolute standard do not appear to be derived from any
other non-diminutive class, as pointed out for other Bantu languages
(Gibson et al. 2017: 359).
(10) CR nouns primarily affiliated to gender 19/6a
Babanki
Kom
Kuk
Kung
Men
Oku
fə̀kɔ̀ʔ
fɨ ̄kâʔ
fə̄kâʔ
fə̄kâʔ
fēkâʔ
fēkâʔ
fə̀ɲín
fə̀ɲì
fə̀kù
fɨ ̄ɲúin
fɨ ̀ɲù
fɨ ̄kû
fə̄ɲɨ ᷇n
fə̄ɲí
fə̄kûɔ
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
fə̄ɲɨ ᷇m
fə̄ɲi ᷇ə
fə̄kô
gloss
fēnʌ᷇ŋ
fēnə́n
‘bird’
fēɲí
fèfìak
‘knife’
fēkû
fēkóo
‘tree’
‘belt’
264
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
Babanki
Kom
Kuk
Kung
Men
Oku
fə̀lɨ ́ʔ
fɨ ̄léʔ
[kə̄ɲɨ ́ʔ]
[kə̄ɲɨ ́ʔ]
fēndə́ʔ
fēlík
fə̀mbváŋ
fə̀làm
fɨ ̄ŋgwáŋ
fɨ ̀làm
fə̄ŋgbáŋ
?
fə̄mgbáŋ
fə̄wô
gloss
fēŋgwáŋ
fēŋgwáŋ
‘salt’
fèlȁm
fèlàm
‘net’
‘smoke’
The nouns in (10) are primarily members of gender 19/6a since there
is no evidence of their derivational shift from another source gender
on the synchronic level. Note that the absolute size of referents of primary 19/6a nouns such as ‘bird’, ‘tree’, or ‘knife’ is, in principle, no
argument against their potential historical origin in derived diminutives, since the contemporarily productive derivational process is
also not guided by orientation towards any absolute standard of size,
but rather depends on the size which is perceived as prototypical of
a given class member. Therefore, it might be that these nouns which
are today – and most probably already in proto-(C)R times – primarily affiliated to gender 19/6a actually represent historical diminutives derived at pre-Ring times from a non-diminutive root which
has disappeared from (C)R.
3.2 Suffixation
Shifting nouns to gender 19/6a for diminutivisation is, at times,
accompanied by the addition of a -CV suffix. CR languages differ
with respect to the scope of application of the -CV suffix across the
lexicon (lexical coverage), the optionality of its presence in diminutives, the available forms of its (lexically conditioned) allomorphs (-t,
-l, or -n) and the degree to which they undergo phonological reduction, as briefly summarized in table (11).
(11) Overview of CR suffixation
lexical
coverage
presence of
suffix:
number of
allomorphs:
full vs.
reduced
shape:
Babanki Kom
total
total
Oku
total
Men
partial
Kuk
partial
Kung
partial
n.a.
n.a.
oblig.
option. option. n.a.
3
3
3
2 (tə, lə)
1 (lə)
1 (lə)
full
full
full
reduced
reduced
reduced
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
265
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
The suffix is obligatorily present in derived diminutives in Babanki
but lexically conditioned in Kom, Kung, Kuk, Men, and Oku such that
some nouns take a suffix in forming the diminutive while others do
not, e.g. Kuk fə̄byí ‘smallish goat’, Oku fə̀wîl ‘small person’. In Kung
there is also variation with respect to application of the suffix, e.g.
kə̄bê ‘thigh’ derives the diminutive singular fə̄bê-lə̀ which varies freely
with fə̄bê, whereas the diminutive plural m̄ bê-lə̀ does not allow for
omission of the suffix in *m̄ bê.7 Concomitant suffixation for diminutive derivation in CR is exemplified in (12).
(12) CR diminutive derivation in 19/6a and suffixation8
Babanki
Kom
Kung
Base
diminutive 19/6a
wàn (1/2) ‘child’, pl. vúnə́ fə̀wàntə̂ ‘little child’, pl. mə̀wàntə̂
ə̀fwín (5/6) ‘leg’, pl. àfwín fə̀fwíntə̀ ‘small leg’, pl. mə̀fwíntə̀
kə̀ʃí (7/8) ‘piece’, pl. ə̀ʃí
fə̀ʃílə̀~fə̀ʃínə̀ ‘tiny piece’,
pl. mə̀ʃílə̀~mə̀ʃínə̀
wáin (1/2) ‘child’, pl.
wɔ́indā
īsáŋ (5/6) ‘corn’, pl. āsáŋ
ātú (7/8) ‘head’, pl. ə̄tú
īɣa᷇ŋ (5/10) ‘root’, pl.
sə̄ɣa᷇ŋ
kə̄bê (7/4) ‘thigh’, pl. ībê
sʌ̀f (9/10) ‘maize’,
pl. sə̀sʌ̀f
fɨ ̄wáin(tɨ ̀) ‘little child’,
pl. mɨ ̄wáin(tɨ ̀)
fɨ ̄sáŋ(lɨ ̂) ‘small corn’, pl. mɨ ̄sáŋ(lɨ ̂)
fɨ ̄tú(nɨ ̀) ‘small head’,
pl. mɨ ̄tú(nɨ ̀)
fə̄ɣa᷇ŋə̂ ‘small root’, pl. m̄ ɣa᷇ŋə̂
fə̄bê(lə̀) ‘tiny feeble thigh’,
pl. m̄ bêlə̀
fə̄sʌ̂blə̀ ‘tiny feeble maize plant’,
pl. m̄ sʌ̂blə̀
7 The datasets on which these claims are based vary with respect to individual
languages. The datasets for Kuk and Kung are quite limited comprising some 20
diminutives which have been checked with two consultants, respectively. Regarding
Kom and Oku, 25 items were taken from secondary sources and checked systematically with two consultants each. The Men dataset is a bit larger including some 40
items from various sources. While the Kuk dataset stems from elicitation exclusively,
Kung and Men data are based on elicitation and narrative discourse. 80 Babanki
items were provided by the first author and checked by two other native speakers.
In all cases diminutives have not been checked for their potential range of variation
across different individuals.
8
Bracketing of the suffix indicates its optional presence.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
266
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Kuk
Men
Oku
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
zùɣù (9/10) ‘snake’, pl.
sə́zûɣù
ɲȁm (9/10) ‘animal’,
pl. sə̄ɲâm
īsʌ̂b (5/6) ‘maize cob’,
pl. ʌ̄sʌ̂b
āfɨá (7/8) ‘thing’, pl. ēfɨá
váin (1/2) ‘child’,
pl. ʌ̄ɣɔ́in
tsə̀m (9/10) ‘dream’,
pl. sētsə̀m
ə̄bkún (3/6a) ‘bed’,
pl. ə̄mkún
kētíɛ (7/8) ‘chair’,
pl. ə̄btíɛ
ntɔ̀n (9/10) ‘pot’,
pl. ntɔ̀nsè
fə̄ʒúglə̀ ‘smallish snake’, pl. m̄ zúglə̀
fə̀ɲàmə̏ ‘small animal’, pl. m̀ ɲàmə̏
fə̄sʌ̂b(lə̀) ‘smallish maize cob’,
pl. m̄ sʌ̂blə̀
fēfɨâ ‘little thing’, pl. m̄ fɨâ
fēɣɔ́intə̂ ‘little child’, pl. m̄ ɣɔ́intə̂
fə̄tsə́mtə̀ ‘small dream’, pl. m̄ tsə́mtə̀
fēkún(tè) ‘small bed’, pl. mēkún(tè)
fētíɛ(lé) ‘small chair’, pl. mētíɛ(lé)
fèntɔ̀n(nè) ‘small pot’,
pl. mèntɔ̀n(nè)
In Babanki, Kom, and Oku, all three suffix allomorphs can occur
in both the singular and plural and it is possible for some words
to take -lə̀ or -nə̀ in Babanki without any semantic difference. In
Kom and Oku, it is possible to leave out a suffix and still obtain the
diminutive meaning only by transfer to gender 19/6a. In Kung and
Kuk suffixation for diminutivity is more restrictive than in Babanki,
Kom and Oku in two respects. First, only one suffix allomorph, i.e.
-(l)ə̀ (along with various types of reduction) has been observed so far.
Second, the distribution of this suffix is constrained by semantically
intransparent lexical criteria, i.e. some diminutives require the suffix
obligatorily, others apply it optionally, while some lack it altogether.
Of all CR languages Kuk is the one with the most limited use of the
suffix. In all 10 examples given in Kießling (2016), only one, fə̄ʒúglə̀
‘smallish snake’, requires the suffix -lə. Another one, fə̀ɲàmə̏ ‘small
animal’, contains the reduced form of the suffix, i.e. -ə, and in a third
one, fə̄sʌ̂b(lə̀) ‘smallish maize cob’, it is optionally present.
In Kung the suffix -lə seems to be reduced to schwa, as soon as it
appears with a velar nasal, e.g. in fēɣa᷇ŋ-ə̀ ‘small root’ (< *fēɣa᷇ŋ-lə̀)
and fə̀pfə̀ndʊ́ ŋ-ə̀ ‘small pig, piglet’ (< *fə̀pfə̀ndʊ́ ŋ-lə̀). Occasional alternations in root final consonants, as in fə̄-sʌ̂b-lə̀ ‘small maize plant’ (<
sʌ̀f ‘maize cob’), suggest that suffixation of -lə must have protected
a prior root final plosive *b from lenition to f (Kießling 2019: 149).
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
267
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
The surface tones of some nouns are also realized differently as compared to their derivational base, e.g. fə̄ɣɨ ̂ə ‘tiny feeble person’ (<ɣùɔ
(1/2) ‘person’, pl. ʌ̄ɣɨ ᷇(ə)). Some nouns, e.g. īɣa᷇ŋ (5/10) ‘root’ derive
two distinct forms in gender 19/6a in which the presence vs. absence
of the suffix produces a difference in meaning, i.e. fə̄ɣa᷇ŋ-ə̂ ‘small root’
(< *fə̄ɣa᷇ŋ-lə̂) vs. fə̄ɣáŋ ‘edible root’.
In Men there are instances where it looks as if the diminutive is
derived exclusively by suffixation. This is the case for a few nouns
which are primarily affiliated to 19/6a, e.g. fēnɨ ᷇ŋ-tə̂ ‘small bird’ (<
fēnɨ ᷇ŋ ‘bird’), fēsɨ ́y-lə́ ~ fēsɨ ́y-tə̀ ‘small pepper’ (< fə̄sɨ ́s ‘pepper’), fə̄ɲí-tə̂
‘small knife’ (< fə̄ɲí ‘knife’). Derived diminutives in this language
may retain only tonal traces of prior suffixation, e.g. the final L component in fēfɨâ ‘small thing’ (< āfɨá (7/8) ‘thing’).
As mentioned above, suffixation in the course of diminutive derivation triggers morphophonological effects in some root terminal
consonants, though it is difficult to generalize on these, since in
some cases suffixation seems to block lenition which otherwise
affects terminal consonants in non-suffixed forms, e.g. Kung fēsʌ̂b-lə̀
‘small maize plant’ (< sʌ̀f (9/10) ‘maize cob’), Kuk fə̄ʒúg-lə̀ ‘smallish
snake’(< zùɣù (9/10) ‘snake’), while in other cases it is just the other
way round, i.e. suffixation causing lenition, e.g. Men fēsɨ ́y-lə́ ~ fēsɨ ́y-tə́
‘small pepper’ (< fə̄sɨ ́s (19/6a) ‘pepper’), Babanki fə̀sáy-lə̀ ~ fə̀sáy-tə̀
‘small buttock’ (< ə̀sás (5/6) ‘buttock’).
The tone in the suffix seems to be lexically determined in that
some diminutives apply a low tone and others a high tone e.g. Oku
fē-tú-lè ‘small head’ (< kē-tú (7/8) ‘head’) vs. fē-tíɛ-lé ‘small chair’ (<
kē-tíɛ (7/8) ‘chair’).9 Falling contour tones might be a result of two
tones merging to one in a single tone bearing unit, either a high root
tone spreading on a low suffix tone, e.g. Kom fɨ ̄ŋgvɨ ́-tɨ ̂ ‘small chicken’
(<ŋgvɨ ́ (9/10) ‘chicken’) and Men fə̄ɲí-tə̂ ‘small knife’ (< fə̄ɲí (19/6a)
‘knife’), or a low suffix tone docking to a high root tone, e.g. Kung
fə̄fûɔ ‘tiny thing’ (< kə̄fúɔ (7/8) ‘thing’). In Babanki, however, the
tendency is for H tone roots to take a L tone suffix, e.g. fə̀tó-tə̀ ‘small
9 These tonal variations in the suffix might reflect a contrast of different final
floating tones associated to the root as reconstructed for Proto-Grassfields (Hyman
2007). Thus, the low tone suffixes in Oku fē-tú-lè, Men fə̄-tú-ə̀, and Babanki fē-tʉ́-lə̀
‘small head’ might reflect the final floating low tone of the Proto-Grassfields root *tʊ́ ̀
‘head’, whereas the high tone suffix in Men fə̄-fó-lə́ ‘very small rat’ (< āfól (7/8) ‘rat’)
rather reflects the terminal floating high tone of the Proto-Grassfields root *fʊ́ ĺ ‘rat’.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
268
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
hut’ (< ə̀tó (5/13) ‘hut’), fə̀mbvʉ́-lə̀ ‘small chicken’ (< mbvʉ́ (9/10)
‘chicken’), while L tone roots take a falling tone suffix, e.g. fə̀wìʔ-tə̂
‘small person’ (< wìʔ (1/2) ‘person’), fə̀ɲàm-tə̂ ‘small animal’ (< ɲàm
(9/10) ‘animal’). This suggests that the diminutive suffix in Babanki
must be low-toned with a preceding floating high tone which is
absorbed by final high tones in the nominal root, but creates a contour tone in the suffix when preceded by a low tone in the nominal
root.
Borrowed nouns do not accept suffixation of -CV along with transfer
to gender 19/6a for diminutive formation, as seen in (4) above. The
exceptions found so far are in Babanki and Oku where monosyllabic
bases can also receive a suffix, e.g. Babanki fə̀tʃɔ̂ytə̀ ‘tiny church’ (<
tʃɔ̂s ‘church’), and Oku fētsɔ́stè ‘tiny church’ (< tsɔ́s ‘church’).
Remarkably, two of these suffixes, namely -tV and -lV, which
accompany diminutive derivation of nouns in CR resemble the verbal
extensions -tV, and -lV commonly found in Bantoid and in Grassfields
(Watters 2003: 245, Hyman 2018: 180) to derive attenuative meanings in verbs (Akumbu & Chibaka 2012: 137, Tamanji & Mba 2003,
Mba & Chiatoh 2003, Harro 1989, Mba 1997), i.e. a reduced degree
of quality in states and intensity in actions and events. Semantically,
the effect is parallel to diminutivisation with nouns. While the suffix
-nV serves diminutive function, it is not attested in the attenuative.
On the other hand, -kV is attested in the attenuative but has not been
found in diminutives. In Babanki, for example, -tə and -kə function as
attenuative suffixes often combined with a frequentative or iterative
function, as shown in (13).
(13) Babanki verbal diminutive suffixes -tə and -kə
Base
diminutive in -tə
Base
wyé ‘pour’
wyétə́ ‘pour a little’
sáʔ ‘scatter’ sáʔkə́ ‘scatter in bits’
dìtə̀ ‘cry a little’
bàs ‘tear’
ɲʉ́ ‘drink’
ló ‘lick’
dì ‘cry’
tʃò ‘pass’
ɲʉ́tə́ ‘drink a bit’
lótə́ ‘lick a bit’
tʃòtə̀ ‘pass a little’
pfɨ ́ ‘die’
diminutive in -kə
pfɨ ́kə́ ‘die bit by bit’
káʔ ‘turn’
káʔkə́ ‘turn a bit’
fwɔ̀s ‘fart’
fwɔ̀ykə̀ ‘fart little by little’
bvù ‘grind’ bvùtə̀ ‘grind a little’ fwè ‘rot’
bàykə̀ ‘tear into pieces’
fwèkə̀ ‘rot bit by bit’
Mba and Chiatoh (2003: 94, 98) demonstrate that -tɨ and -lɨ function
as diminutive suffixes in Kom (14).
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
269
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
(14) Kom verbal diminutive suffixes -tɨ and -lɨ
Base
diminutive in -tɨ
Base
diminutive in -lɨ
séf ‘carry’
sébtɨ ́ ‘carry a bit’
tás ‘sharpen’
táylɨ ́ ‘sharpen a bit’
káŋ ‘fry’
káŋtɨ ́ ‘fry a bit’
káŋ ‘fry’
káŋlɨ ́ ‘fry a bit’
kàf ‘scratch’ kàbtɨ ́ ‘scratch a bit’ tàs ‘push down’ tàylɨ ́ ‘push down a bit’
nyíŋ ‘run’
nyíŋtɨ ́ ‘run a bit’
tʃéʔ ‘rob’
tʃéʔlɨ ́ ‘rob a bit’
Depending on the situation in individual CR languages, attenuation
is often also linked to repetitive and frequentative notions, due to
the common experience that distributive repetition and parcellation
tends to entail a diminution of intensity. Thus, in Men, the cognate
suffix -te has attenuative function which is often combined with a
frequentative, iterative, distributive or pluractional notion, as shown
in (15).
(15) Men verbal diminutive suffix -te
Base
diminutive
ḿ[ə́] ‘drink’
mə́té ‘take sips’
kɔ́ʔ ‘climb, go up’
kɔ́ʔté ‘climb up a little’
ŋgɔ́ʔ ‘give one knock’
ŋgɔ́ʔté ‘knock slightly several times’
tɨ ́m ‘shoot; dig’
tɨ ́mté ‘dig in a disorderly fashion
(flinging up earth here and there)’
ndʒì ‘take’
ndʒìtè ‘take a little, take bit by bit’
In Kom some lexemes seem to distinguish frequentative and attenuative, e.g. tʃʲá ‘kick’ allows for a contrast of the frequentative tʃʲá-lɨ́
‘kick repeatedly’ vs. the attenuative tʃʲá-tɨ ́ ‘kick a little’, whereas
others derive polysemous stems with -tɨ ̀, e.g. mzì-tɨ ̀ (< mzì ‘swallow’)
‘swallow a bit; swallow repeatedly’ (Jones 2001).
In Kung, the suffixes -nə and -lə are marginally attested in attenuative function: mwàe-nə̀ ‘twinkle’ (< mwàe ‘shine’), ɲɔ̀ʔ-lə̀ ‘roast a bit’
(< ɲɔ̀ʔ ‘roast’), zʊ́ ʔ-lə́ ‘make warm’ (< zʊ́ ʔ ‘heat’). In Kuk, the widespread pluractional suffix -kə is marginally attested in lɨ ́m-kə́ ‘wait for
a long time’ (< lɨ ́mə́ ‘wait for’).
The application of these verbal extensions to nominal bases, obviously motivated by the functional parallelism of diminution and
attenuation, thus represents an instance of morphological strategies
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
270
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
crossing word class boundaries. In a diachronic perspective, this
might be analysed as a spillover of verbal derivational morphology
into the nominal domain which could have been triggered or promoted by nominalisation of verbs extended by the attenuative, as is
suggested by examples such as the Men diminutive fə̄tsə́mtə̀ ‘small
dream’ (< tsə̀m (9/10) ‘dream’) which coexists with a verb tsə̀mtè
‘dream’ obviously including the attenuative suffix -tə.
Kuk presents another strand of derivational morphology where
the suffix -lə̀ which occasionally accompanies diminutive derivation
is also involved in other types of denominal noun derivation, e.g. in
deriving īɣâʔlə̀ (5/10) ‘wing’ (pl. sə̄ɣâʔlə̀) from īɣâʔà (5/10) ‘upper
arm’ (pl. sə̄ɣâʔà).
4 Associative construction for diminutive formation
Diminution is also achieved in CR by periphrasis in associative constructions headed by various nouns of gender 19/6a which encode
a diminutive notion either in their lexical meaning as with nouns
meaning ‘tiny item’ or by a combination of their lexical meaning
with a diminutive derivation as in the case of fə̀wán (Babanki), fə̄wáe
(Kung) and fēwán (Oku), all meaning ‘little child’. Other nominals
that can be used in head position are fə̀ndɛ́ʔ (Babanki) and fɨ ̄fúin
(Kom) both meaning ‘tiny item’. In Babanki both fə̀wán ‘little child’
and fə̀ndɛ́ʔ ‘tiny item’ can be used interchangeably for the same
diminutive function. While Kuk and Men also have distinct lexical
items for this meaning, i.e. fə̄fwâtə̀ (Kuk) and fə̄ɲâŋ (Men), it is not
clear to which extent they are also used in constructions such as the
ones in (16) and which type of division of semantic labour pertains
with respect to the usage of ‘child’.
(16) CR diminutive derivation using associative constructions
Babanki
Base
ə̀ɣàm (5/13) ‘mat’, pl. tə̀ɣàm
kə̀ɲù (7/8) ‘thing’, pl. ə̀ɲù
ʃʉ̀ (9/10) ‘fish’, pl. ʃʉ́ꜜsə́
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
diminutive 19/6a
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ ɣàm ‘small
mat’
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ kə́ɲù ‘small
thing’
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ ʃʉ̂ ‘tiny fish’
271
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Kom
Kung
Oku
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
ŋgvɨ ̄ (9/10) ‘chicken’, pl. ŋgvɨ ̄sə̄ fɨ ̄fúin fɨ ̄ ŋgvɨ ̄ ‘tiny chicken’
īsáŋ (5/6) ‘corn’, pl. āsáŋ
fɨ ̄fúin fī sáŋ ‘small corn’
ātú (7/8) ‘head’, pl. ɨ ̄tú
fɨ ̄fúin fā tú ‘small head’
mbvə̄ (9/10) ‘chicken’, pl.
sə̄mbvə̄
tsàʔ° (9/10) ‘trap’, pl. sə̀tsàʔ°
kə̄pfə̀ndʊ́ ŋ (7/8) ‘pig’ pl.
ūpfə̀ndʊ́ ŋ
ə̄bkún (3/6a) ‘bed’, pl. ə̄mkún
kētíɛ (7/8) ‘chair’, pl. ə̄btíɛ
ntɔ̀n (9/10) ‘pot’, pl. ntɔ̀nsè
wāe fə̄ mbvə̄ fə̄ ‘small feeble
chicken’
wāe fə̄ tsàʔ fə̀ ‘small trap’
wāe fə̄ fə̀pfə̀ndʊ́ ŋ fə̄ ‘small
feeble pig’
fēwán é ə̄bkún ‘small bed’
fēwán é kētíɛ ‘small chair’
fēwán é ntɔ̀n ‘small pot’
The syntax of the examples presented above follows the pattern of
CR associative constructions, i.e. the preceding head noun (N1) is
linked to the following modifier noun (N2) by an associative marker
(AM) which agrees with the class of the head noun according to the
formula given in (17) and illustrated by the Kung example in (18).
(17) CR Formula of associative constructions
[NPx – R]N1 AMN1 [NPx – R]N2 ENCLN1
(18) Kung associative construction
wāe
fə̄
mbvə̄
19.little.child
19
9.chicken
‘small feeble chicken’
fə̄
19
In some CR languages such as Kung and Kuk, the prefix of the head
noun (NPx) is dropped, as soon as a modifier follows (Kießling 2016,
2019). Thus in (18), the head noun fə̄wāe ‘little child’ loses its noun
class prefix fə- due to the fact that it is modified by the noun mbvə̄
‘chicken’ which is linked to the head noun by the associative concord
of class 19 fə̄. Under certain conditions, some CR languages such
as Kung require an additional noun phrase terminal enclitic (ENCL)
which indexes the class of the head noun and which is reminiscent of
the determiner enclitic in the West Ring languages Aghem (Hyman
2010) and Isu (Kießling 2010). While the West Ring determiner
enclitic largely serves to mark the non-focalised status of nouns, the
morphosynactic and pragmatic conditions of its distribution in Central Ring, however, remain completely unclear so far.
From a wider comparative perspective, lexical items meaning
‘child’ are quite commonly employed for diminutive functions, evenPublished by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
272
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
tually following a universal path of grammaticalisation (Heine &
Kuteva 2002: 65–7). In various Bantu languages such as Cuwabo,
Nzadi, Eton and Bafia (Gibson et al. 2017: 358–359), in Kikongo
(Huth 1992) and in Sotho-Tswana and Nguni (Güldemann 1999),
reflexes of Proto-Bantu *jánà ‘child’ seem to have been developed to
diminutive markers, probably independently of each other. Beyond
Bantu, the same process operates in various branches of Niger-Congo,
e.g. on Susu díi ‘child’ (Anderson, Green & Obeng 2018) and on Ewe
ví ‘child’ (Heine & Hünnemeyer 1988, Heine, Claudi & Hünnemeyer
1991: 79–89), reflex of a Niger-Congo root *bi ‘child’ (Kähler-Meyer
1971: 347–348) which is assumed to be the ultimate source of Proto-Benue-Congo *pi- (de Wolf 1971: 170–1), the ancestor of Proto-Bantu class 19 *pi-. A remarkable detail about the CR situation is
that in none of the CR languages it is simply the noun ‘child’ which
is employed for periphrastic diminution purposes in associative constructions, but rather its diminutive stem in 19/6a.
Borrowed words can also be diminutivised in CR by means of the
associative construction (19).
(19) CR diminutive derivation of borrowed words using associative
constructions
Babanki
Kom
Kung
Oku
Base
bwɔ́tə̀ (1/2) ‘bottle’,
pl. və̀bwɔ́tə̀
bùʃí (1/2) ‘cat’, pl. və̀bùʃí
diminutive 19/6a
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ bwɔ́tə̀ ‘small
bottle’
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ bùʃí ‘small cat’
lámâs (9/10) ‘orange’,
pl. sə̀lámâs
bùsí (9/10) ‘cat’, pl. sə̀bùsí
wāe fə̄ lámâs fə̄ ‘small orange’
wāe fə̄ bùsí fə̄ ‘small cat’
bés (9/10) ‘cat’, pl. béysɨ ̄
fɨ ̄fúin fɨ ̄ bés ‘small cat’
tʃɔ̄s (9/10) ‘church’, pl. tʃɔ̄ysɨ ̄ fɨ ̄fúin fɨ ̄ tʃɔ̄s ‘tiny church’
tsɔ́s (1/10) ‘bed’, pl. tsɔ́sē
bùsé (1/10) ‘cat’, pl. bùsésē
fēwán é tsɔ́s ‘tiny church’
fēwán é bùsé ‘small cat’
Nouns primarily assigned to gender 19/6a form diminutives preferably by means of such associative constructions headed by nouns
which include the diminutive notion in their lexical meaning, as
illustrated in (20).
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
273
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
(20) CR diminutive derivation of 19/6a nouns using associative
constructions
Babanki
Kom
Kung
Base
fə̀kɔ̀ʔ (19/6a) ‘tree’, pl. ŋkɔ̀ʔ
fə̀ɲín (19/6a) ‘bird’, pl. mə̀ɲín
fə̀sɛ́s (19/6a) ‘pepper’, pl.
mə̀sɛ́s
fɨ ̄ɲúin (19/6a) ‘bird’, pl.
mɨ ̄ɲúin
fɨ ̄kâʔ (19/6a) ‘tree’, pl. mɨ ̄kâʔ
fɨ ̀ɲù (19/6a) ‘knife’, pl. mɨ ̀ɲù
fə̄ɲɨ ᷇m (19/6a) ‘bird’, pl. m̄ ɲɨ ᷇m
fēkâʔ (19/6a) ‘tree’, pl. m̄ kâʔ
fə̄ɲi ᷇ə (19/6a) ‘knife’, pl. m̄ ɲi ᷇ə
diminutive 19/6a
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ fə́kɔ̀ʔ ‘small
tree’
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ fə̀ɲìn ‘small
bird’
fə̀wán ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ fə́sɛ́s ‘small
pepper’
fɨ ̄fúin fɨ ̄ fɨ ̄ɲùin ‘small bird’
fɨ ̄fúin fɨ ̄ fɨ ̄kàʔ ‘small tree’
fɨ ̄fúin fɨ ̄ fɨ ̀ɲù ‘small knife’
wāe fə̄ ɲɨ ́m fə̄ ‘small bird’
wāe fə̄ káʔ fə̀ ‘small tree’
wāe fə̄ ɲíə fə̄ ‘small knife’
This preference is probably due to the fact that the ordinary diminutivisation strategy by transfer to gender 19/6a would create no visible
effect in contrast to the base form which is already assigned to 19/6a.
Alternatively, the simple addition of one of the diminutive suffixes
-tV, -lV or -nV is not sufficient in most cases. Thus, diminutives such as
*fə̀ɲín-tə̀ ‘small bird’ or *fə̀kɔ̀ʔ-tə̀ ‘small tree’ which are simply formed
by adding the suffix to the basic 19/6a noun forms (as elaborated in
section 3.1), are not acceptable in Babanki. However, precisely this
case is attested in Men where nouns primarily affiliated to gender
19/6a such as fēnɨ ᷇ŋ ‘bird’, fə̄sɨ ́s ‘pepper’ and fə̄ɲí ‘knife’ derive their
diminutives, i.e. fēnɨ ᷇ŋ-tə̂ ‘small bird’, fēsɨ ́y-lə́ ~ fēsɨ ́y-tə́ ‘small pepper’
and fə̄ɲí-tə̂ ‘small knife’, respectively, only by additional suffixation.
So far, it has been assumed that every noun can be diminutivised
through nominal affixation, but this is not always the case. In Babanki,
for instance, the nouns in (21) can only be diminutivised by means
of the associative construction with fə̀wán ‘little child’ or fə̀ndɛ́ʔ ‘tiny
item’ as head noun, since a morphologically derived diminutive in
gender 19/6a is not available for them. So, the associative construction appears as a compensatory strategy here.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
274
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
(21) Babanki associative constructions as compensation for absence
of morphologically derived diminutives
Base
diminutive
kə̀ntʃíʔ (7/8) ‘lid’
fə̀wán~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ kə́ntʃíʔ ‘small lid’
*fə̀ntʃíʔ
kə̀tsɔ́ʔ (7/8) ‘mud’
fə̀ndɛ́ʔ~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ kə́ꜜtsɔ́ʔ ‘small mud’
*fə̀tsɔ́ʔ
kə̀tíʔə́tíʔə́ (7/8) ‘ear’
fə̀wán~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ kə́ꜜtíʔə́tíʔə́ ‘small ear’ *fə̀tíʔə́tíʔə́
kə̀ntsì (7/8) ‘cricket’
kə̀ʃí (7/8) ‘place’
ə̀kwɛ́n (5/6) ‘bean’
fə̀wán~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ kə̀ntsì ‘small cricket’ *fə̀ntsì
fə̀wán~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ kə́ꜜʃí ‘small place’
fə̀wán~ fə̀ndɛ́ʔ fə́ ꜜkwɛ́n ‘small bean’
*fə̀ʃí
*fə̀kwɛ́n
The range of nouns that do not lend themselves to morphological
diminutivisation by transfer to gender 19/6a in Babanki is varied,
including, but not limited to insects, household items and body parts.
While the motivations for these restrictions are still unclear, instances
such as kə̀tíʔə́tíʔə́ ‘ear’ suggest that morphological diminu-tivisation
might be blocked by the presence of semantic specialisations of parallel forms in gender 19/6a such as fə̀tíʔə́tíʔə́ ‘mushroom (sp.)’.
While diminutives are formed by the morphological and morphosyntactic operations outlined above, it appears that augmentatives do
not receive a similar treatment in CR. In Babanki, for example, augmentatives are expressed by an attributive usage of inchoative-stative verbs such as ɣɔ́ʔ ‘be(come) big’ (ɣɔ́ʔkə́ pl.) illustrated in (22).
(22) Babanki augmentative periphrasis with ɣɔ́ʔ ‘be(come) big’
(ɣɔ́ʔkə́ pl.)
Base
Augmentative
kə̀kɨ ́ (7/8) ‘chair’, pl. ə̀kɨ ́
kə̀kɨ ́ kə̄ɣɔ́ʔɔ́ kə́ ‘big chair’, pl. ə̀kɨ ́ ə̄ɣɔ́ʔkə́ və́
ʙʉ́ (9/10) ‘dog’, pl. ʙʉ́ꜜsə́
ʙʉ́ ə̄ɣɔ́ʔɔ́ ‘big dog’, pl. ʙʉ́ꜜsə́ ɣɔ́ʔkə́ sə́
ə̀lɛ́m (5/6) ‘yam’, pl. àlɛ́m
ə̀lɛ́m ə̄ɣɔ́ʔɔ́ ɣə́ ‘big yam’, pl. àlɛ́m āɣɔ́ʔkə́ ɣə́
Augmentation is also achieved in Babanki by the alternative strategy
of periphrasis in associative constructions headed by nouns with
inherent augmentative meanings such as kə̀mpfɨ ́ ‘huge thing’, illustrated in (23) – which is parallel to the periphrastic diminutivization
strategy with fə̀wán ‘little child’ and fə̀ndɛ́ʔ ‘tiny item’, exemplified
above in (19–20).
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
275
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
(23) Babanki augmentative periphrasis with kə̀mpfɨ ́ ‘huge thing’
Base
Augmentative
ʃʉ̀ (9/10) ‘fish’, pl. ʃʉ́ꜜsə́
kə̀mpfɨ ́ kə́ ʃʉ̂ ‘big fish’
ə̀ɣàm (5/13) ‘mat’, pl. tə̀ɣàm
kə̀mpfɨ ́ kə́ ɣàm ‘big mat’
5 Conclusion
Diminutivisation in Central Ring languages is generally achieved
by a widely attested shift of nouns from various genders to gender
19/6a marked by prefixes fV-/m(V)- which replace the original noun
class prefixes. The productivity of this strategy is manifested in its
recurrent application to borrowed nouns. Sometimes, diminutivisation in gender 19/6a is accompanied by the addition of a semantically bleached suffix CV. Remarkably, some of the allomorphs of
this suffix, i.e. -tV, -lV, resemble the verbal extensions -tV, and -lV
commonly used in Bantoid and in Grassfields to derive attenuative
meanings in verbs. From a diachronic perspective, this might be
analysed as a spillover of verbal derivational morphology into the
nominal domain which could have been promoted by nominalisation of verbs extended by the attenuative. Another diminutivisation
strategy discussed is periphrasis in associative constructions headed
by various nouns of gender 19/6a which encode a diminutive notion
either in their lexical meaning as with nouns meaning ‘tiny item’
such as fə̀ndɛ́ʔ (Babanki) and fɨ ̄fúin (Kom) or by a combination of
their lexical meaning with a diminutive derivation as in the case of
fə̀wán (Babanki), fə̄wáe (Kung) and fēwán (Oku), all meaning ‘little
child’. More finegrained generalisations about the limits of morphological diminutivisation in CR and regularities regarding its division
of labour with syntactic strategies will only be possible on the basis
of a much more extensive corpus of diminutives which includes data
from all under-researched CR varieties, especially from Bum for
which diminutive data have not been available at all so far.
Abbreviations
AM associative marker, CR Central Ring, ENCL enclitic, N noun, NPx noun
class prefix, pl. plural, PR Proto-Ring, sg. singular. Numbers refer to noun
classes/genders.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
276
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
References
Akumbu, Pius W. 2019. Noun class 9/10 tone in Central Ring Grassfields
Bantu: An overview. In Pius W. Akumbu & Esther P. Chie (eds.),
Engagement with Africa: Linguistic essays in honor of Ngessimo M. Mutaka.
Köln: Köppe. 1–13.
Akumbu, Pius W. & Evelyn F. Chibaka. 2012. A pedagogical grammar of
Babanki. Köln: Köppe.
Anderson, Jonathan C., Christopher R. Green & Samuel G. Obeng. 2018. On
the expression of diminutivity in Susu. Journal of West African Languages 45(1). 59-77.
Asohsi, Melvice. 2015. Structural and typological approaches to Obang grammar.
Köln: Köppe.
Bakema, Peter & Dirk Geeraerts. 2000. Diminution and augmentation. In
Armin Burkhardt, Hugo Steger & Herbert Ernst Wiegand (eds.), Handbücher zur Sprach- und Kommunikationswissenschaft – Handbook of
linguistics and communication science 17.2: Morphology. Berlin & New
York: de Gruyter. 1045–52.
Bangha, George F. 2003. The Mmen noun phrase. Yaoundé: MA thesis.
Björkestedt, Lena. 2011. Mmen-English lexicon. Yaoundé: CABTAL.
Bleek, Wilhelm Heinrich I. 1862. A comparative grammar of South African
languages. Part 1: phonology. Cape Town & London: J. C. Juta and
Trübner & Co.
Bleek, Wilhelm Heinrich I. 1862. A comparative grammar of South African languages. Part 2: the concord: Section 1: the noun. Cape Town & London:
J. C. Juta and Trübner & Co.
Blood, Cynthia L. & Leslie Davis. 1999. Oku-English provisional lexicon.
Yaoundé: SIL.
Breton, Roland & Bakia Fohtung. 1991. Atlas administratif des langues nationales du Cameroun. Yaounde & Paris: CERDOTOLA, ACCT.
Brye, Ed. 2001. Rapid appraisal sociolinguistic research among the Babanki
ALCAM 824. Yaoundé: SIL International.
Chiatoh, Blasius A. 1993. The noun class system of Mmen. Yaoundé: University of Yaoundé MA thesis.
Wolf, Paul de. 1971. The noun class system of Proto-Benue-Congo. The Hague:
Mouton.
Demuth, Katherine. 2000. Bantu noun class systems: Loan word and acquisition evidence of semantic productivity. In Gunter Senft (ed.), Classification systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 270–292.
Dieu, Michel & Patrick Renaud. 1983. Atlas linguistique du Cameroun
(ALCAM): Inventaire préliminaire. Paris & Yaoundé: ACCT/CERDOTOLA.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
277
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
Eberhard, David M., Gary F. Simons & Charles D. Fennig (eds.). 2019. Ethnologue: Languages of the World. Twenty-second edition. Dallas, Texas:
SIL International. Online version: http://www.ethnologue.com.
Edelsten, Peter. & Chiku Lijongwa. 2010. A grammatical sketch of Chindamba:
A Bantu language (G52) of Tanzania. Köln: Köppe.
Gibson, Hannah, Rozenn Guérois & Lutz Marten. 2017. Patterns and developments in the marking of diminutives in Bantu. Nordic Journal of
African Studies 26(4). 344–383.
Grandi, Nicola & Lívia Körtvélyessy L. (eds.). 2015. Edinburgh handbook of
evaluative morphology. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Güldemann, Tom. 1999. Head-initial meets head-final: nominal suffixes in
eastern and southern Bantu from a historical perspective. Studies in
African Linguistics 28. 49–91.
Harro, Gretchen. 1989. Les extensions verbales en yemba (BamilékéDschang). In Daniel Barreteau & Robert Hedinger (eds.), Description
de langues camerounaises. Paris: ORSTOM. 239–269.
Heine, Bernd & Friederike Hünnemeyer. 1988. On the fate of Ewe ví ‘child’ –
the development of a diminutive marker. Afrikanistische Arbeitspapiere
16. 97–121.
Heine, Bernd, Ulrike Claudi & Friederike Hünnemeyer. 1991. Grammaticalization. A conceptual framework. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Heine, Bernd & Tania Kuteva. 2002. World lexicon of grammaticalization.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Huth, Karin. 1992. Mwa-: a new nominal prefix in Koongo? Paper presented
at the 22nd colloquium on African languages and linguistics, Leiden.
Hyman, Larry M. 1979. Aghem grammatical structure. SCOPIL 7. University
of Southern California.
Hyman, Larry M. 1980. Babanki and the Ring group. In Larry M. Hyman &
Jan Voorhoeve (eds.), Noun classes in Grassfields Bantu. Vol. 1 of Actes
du Colloque ‘Expansion Bantoue’. Paris: C.N.R.S. 225–258.
Hyman, Larry M. 2007. Index of Proto-Grassfields Bantu Roots. Ms. University
of California, Berkeley. (Original draft 1979, retyped 1993, refonted
2007) [http://comparalex.org/index.php?page=query&w1=133].
Hyman, Larry M. 2010. Focus marking in Aghem: Syntax or semantics? In
Fiedler, Ines & Anne Schwarz (eds.), The expression of information
structure. A documentation of its diversity across Africa. Amsterdam &
Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 95–116.
Hyman, Larry M. 2018. Common Bantoid verb extensions. In John R. Watters (ed.), East Benue-Congo: Nouns, pronouns, and verbs. Berlin: Language Science Press. 173–198.
Jones, Randy. 2001. Provisional Kom-English lexicon. Yaounde: SIL.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
278
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
Jurafsky, Daniel. 1996. Universal tendencies in the semantics of the diminutive. Language 72(3). 533–578.
Kähler-Meyer, E. 1971. Bantu noun class 19 pi- in Benue-congo. In Actes du
huitième congrès international de linguistique africaine, Abidjan 24–28
mars 1969, volume 2. Abidjan: Université d’Abidjan. 347–365.
Kießling, Roland. 2010. Focalisation and defocalisation in Isu. In Fiedler,
Ines & Anne Schwarz (eds.), The expression of information structure. A
documentation of its diversity across Africa. Amsterdam & Philadelphia:
John Benjamins. 145–163.
Kießling, Roland. 2016. Kuk. Hamburg: Universität Hamburg Ms.
Kießling, Roland. 2019. Salient features of the noun class system of Kung
in a Ring perspective. In Pius W. Akumbu & Esther P. Chie (eds.),
Engagement with Africa: Linguistic essays in honor of Ngessimo M. Mutaka.
Köln: Köppe. 139–161.
Maho, Jouni F. 1999. A Comparative study of Bantu noun classes. (Orientalia et Africana Gothoburgensia 13). Gothenburg: Acta Universitatis
Gothoburgensis.
Mba, Gabriel. 1997. Les extensions verbales en ghomala’. Journal of West
African Languages 26(1). 77–101.
Mba, Gabriel & Blasius Chiatoh. 2003. Verbal extensions in Kom. In Daniel F.
Idiata & Gabriel Mba (eds.), Studies on voice through verbal extensions
in nine Bantu languages spoken in Cameroon, Gabon, DRC and Rwanda.
München: Lincom. 81–112.
Meeussen, Achiel E. 1967. Bantu grammatical reconstructions. Africana Linguistica 3. 79–121.
Meinhof, Carl. 1899. Grundriss einer Lautlehre der Bantusprachen, Leipzig: F.
A. Brockhaus. Revised edition, 1910. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer.
Möller, Mirjam. 2012. The noun and verb in Mmen, a center Ring Grassfields Bantu language. Yaoundé: SIL.
Mua, Benjamin N. 2015. Mmen language grammar sketch. MELANCO.
Pleus, Martin. 2015. Das Nominalklassensystem im Kuk. Hamburg: Universität Hamburg BA thesis.
Poulos, George. 1986. Instances of semantic bleaching in South-Eastern
Bantu. In Gerrit J. Dimmendaal (ed.), Current approaches to African
linguistics 3. Dordrecht: Foris. 281–296.
Poulos, George. 1990. A linguistic analysis of Venda. Pretoria: Via Afrika Limited.
Poulos, George. 1999. Grammaticalisation in South-Eastern Bantu and the
linguistic ‘dynamics’ underlying this process. South African Journal of
African Languages 19. 204–214.
Schlenker, Rebecca. 2012. Das Nominalklassensystem des Kung (Graslandbantu). Hamburg: Universität Hamburg BA thesis.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
279
A&Ü | 93 / 2020
Akumbu & Kießling | The expression of diminutivity
Schneider, Klaus P. 2003. Diminutives in English. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer
Verlag.
Tamanji, Pius N. 2009. A descriptive grammar of Bafut. Köln: Köppe.
Tamanji, Pius N. & Gabriel Mba. 2003. A morphological study of verbal
extension in Bafut. In Daniel F. Idiata & Gabriel Mba (eds.), Studies on
voice through verbal extensions in nine Bantu languages spoken in Cameroon, Gabon, DRC and Rwanda 15–38. München: LINCOM.
Tatang, Joyce. 2016. Aspects of Kung grammar. Buea: University of Buea
MA thesis.
Voll, Rebecca. 2017. A grammar of Mundabli: A Bantoid (Yemne–Kimbi) language of Cameroon. LOT: Netherlands Graduate School of Linguistics.
Watters, John R. 2003. Grassfields Bantu. In Derek Nurse & Gerard Philippson
(eds.), The Bantu languages. London: Routledge. 225–256.
Wilkendorf, Patricia. 2001. Sketch grammar of Nomaánde. Yaounde: SIL
Cameroon.
Yensi, Agnes M. 1996. The noun class system of Oku. Yaoundé: University
of Yaoundé 1 MA thesis.
Published by Hamburg University Press
DOI 10.15460/auue.2020.93.1.203
280