Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal – Vol. 9, No. 9
Publication Date: September 25, 2022
DOI:10.14738/assrj.99.13088.
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
Beauty and the Golden Ratio
Luis F. Copertari
Computer Engineering Program
Autonomous University of Zacatecas (UAZ), Zacatecas, México
Gloria V. Reyna
Psychology Department
Autonomous University of Zacatecas (UAZ), Zacatecas, México
ABSTRACT
The ideas of beauty and, specifically, female facial beauty, are developed in depth in
this paper, in light of the adjustment of facial beauty to the golden ratio according
to the parameters defined for such purpose. An experimental design of our own is
made, software for carrying out the experiments is developed and the experimental
results are analyzed for both male and female population from the Computer
Engineering Program of the Autonomous University of Zacatecas (UAZ) and the
Psychology Department, also at UAZ. It is concluded that women are more sensitive
than men to the aesthetics dictated by the conformance to the golden ratio. Also, we
conclude that the golden ratio does play an important role in female facial beauty
perception.
Keywords: Beauty, golden ratio, facial, gender, aesthetics, interface.
INTRODUCTION
This research originated from the curiosity derived from a video by Alexs Syntek published in
YouTube entitled “ASOMBROSA-MENTE. Las Reglas de la Atracción” (Syntek, 2020). In this
video, the idea of the golden ratio as a way to measure the attractiveness of the faces of female
models, as well as other issues related to female and male attractiveness are discussed.
These ideas are developed in depth in this research, our own experiments are carried out and
a thorough theoretical revision is made both concerning the golden ratio and issues related to
psychology as well as probability and statistics issues in order to analyze the experimental
results obtained from students from the Computer Engineering Program of the Autonomous
University of Zacatecas (UAZ) and the Psychology Department also at UAZ.
In order to carry out the experiments, a friendly user interface was designed offering the
experimental subject the possibility of ordering the female model according to their beauty by
interacting with the software while the software counts automatically the time the user takes
in doing the test. At the moment of finishing the experiment, the software adds the new
statistics in the database being created in the same folder where the software is located. The
software records the time of the experiment (in seconds), as well as a numerical score
indicating how close to the golden ratio metric the student is.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
Before carrying out the test, the same instructions are read to the students and the same
indications are given so that the tests are consistent. The work carried out is part of the
activities of the Advanced Research Laboratory in Artificial Intelligence and Human-Machine
Interaction (LIAHM). LIAHM’s logo is shown in Figure 1.
Figure 1: LIAHM’s logo.
In this research paper, all theoretical background related are presented, as well as the system
of equations used for the analysis, the results obtained and the discussion of such results.
MALE AND FEMALE PERCEPTION OF BEAUTY
Syntek (2020) explains in his video several issues related to male and female perception of
beauty about men and women.
Some initial considerations about beauty perception
Symmetry in the perception of male and female beauty
If you are a man, carefully observe the two faces of the twins in Figure 2a and indicate which
one is the most beautiful. If you are a woman, observe the two faces of the twins in Figure 2b
indicating which one is more attractive to you. Stop reading and take some time to do your
choice. Which face is more beautiful? A or B? People always choose face A. When they are
questioned as to why they chose that particular face, they answer that it is due to the eyebrows,
lips or some other characteristic of the face.
However, both faces in Figure 2a correspond to the same person as well as both faces in Figure
2b. Each pair of faces are not of twins. Why then the difference in the perception of beauty?
These differences are due to the fact that each pair of faces were graphically manipulated in
order to increase symmetry in face A and increase asymmetry in face B.
Human beings have evolved to prefer symmetry in faces, which tends to allow them to choose
healthier people. This is because of millions of years of human evolution. Apparently, the human
brain has evolved to prefer symmetry and reject asymmetry.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
260
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
a. Two female twins
b. Two male twins
Figure 2: Symmetry in the perception of male and female beauty.
Time and fashion in the perception of female beauty
The perception of beauty not only depends on biological issues, but also cultural ones. For
example, if people from different times are asked from a varying set of models which one they
think is more beautiful, they are going to choose the one as close as possible to the clothing
parameters of the time.
Consider Figure 3, where three women in bikini are shown. The question is: which of the three
models is more beautiful? This question is biased, because it depends on the time the person
answering the question belongs to. Different people from different times will have different
answers due to different cultural differences.
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
261
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
Figure 3: Beauty perception across time for different cultural backgrounds
A person in 1920 would have chosen model A, one in 1950 would have chosen model B and one
in 1970 would have chosen model C.
Body proportions in the perception of female beauty
For the male brain, the ideal proportion between the waist and the heap is 70%, that is, the
waist should have 70% of the size of the heap. This proportion is maintained from race to race
and for different waist sizes, that is, consider more or less fatty women.
From that comes the famous 90-60-90 proportion for the breast, waist and heap sizes. Notice
that 60/90 ≈ 0.70. Apparently, this proportion indicates the male brain that the women being
considered would be better at giving birth without complications (size of the heap) and possibly
also holding babies easier.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
262
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
Figure 4: The ideal female proportion for the male brain
The golden ration in the perception of female facial beauty
Even more incredible that the previous issues related to the metrics of the human body, is the
golden ratio, which has been considered a relation associated to beauty throughout human
history. Apparently, the human brain seeks the golden ratio in the faces of women. Syntek
(2020) and his team carried out experiments of their own with the faces of three models
analyzing several attributes. As an example, consider two attributes: height and width of a face.
It is assumed that having a face height divided by its width closer to the golden ratio is indicative
of beauty.
Voice in the perception of male and female beauty
Syntek’s team (2020) also did a study in the perception of voice in groups of mean and women.
They had the voice recorded of about three women and each individual in a group of men was
asked to indicate which voice seemed more attractive. The same was done with men’s voices in
a group of women. The results indicate that men prefer a higher pitch in women’s voices,
whereas women prefer a lower pitch in men’s voices. This is probably due to hormonal changes
affecting voice in women and men. A higher pitch in a female voice seems indicative of more
sexual appealing due to the action of hormones in women’s voices. The same, but having a lower
pitch in men’s voices seems to apply to women.
Beauty approaches
Historically, beauty has been the object of interest of philosophy, art, theology and science.
However, it corresponds to aesthetics the study of the essence and perception of beauty
(Serracanta, 2020), as well as its rules and methods to study it (Rodríguez et al., 2000). One of
the fundamental problems of aesthetics is to define what constitutes beauty. However, defining
the concept of beauty results complex and it leads to interminable discussions. It could be said
that beauty definitions have been developed within two points of view: the objectivist point of
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
263
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
view dominating from the classic Greek period to the renaissance and the relativistic-subjective
perspective extending from modern times to the present.
The objectivist point of view
Pythagorean philosophers are the main representatives of the objectivist point of view of
beauty. This approach is based on the physical properties of the object: symmetry, balance and
mathematical proportion of its parts. These properties or qualities, considered to be perfect,
express the beauty of the object and generate emotions for everybody contemplating it. From
this point of view, a face with perfect symmetry in its facial characteristics is considered to be
beautiful. Realistic and formalistic aesthetic theories are based on the objectivistic point of view
since they state that aesthetic judgements are universal.
Relativistic-subjectivist approach to beauty
From a subjectivist approach, the concept of beauty varies and it depends on the subject
observing and judging. This approach manifests itself, above all, in the British culture, thanks
to Locke’s conception concerning knowledge. To Hume, beauty “exists only in the mind of the
one contemplating it and each mind perceives different beauty” (as quoted in De Bartolomeo &
Magni, 2012, p. 2-3). The relativistic approach to beauty lies in the consideration that beautiful
depends on the culture and historical period. Generally, for the subjective-relativistic approach,
beauty is a subjective perception depending on multiple factors both individual and
sociocultural. Thus, from this point of view it would be impossible to formulate an objective and
universal concept of beauty, since it is denied that beauty is a quality of the object. Besides,
Hume believes it is impossible to define a rule to apply to all countries and to all historical
periods (De Bartolomeo & Magni, 2012).
Biological basis of beauty perception
The regions of the brain sensitive to the perception of beauty have been studied in human
beings. The scientific evidence suggests that a network of brain regions including the
accumbens nucleus, the fore cingular cortex, the medium prefrontal cortex and the
orbitofrontal cortex are involved in the processing of attractive faces (Clouthier, Heatherton,
Whalen, & Kelley, 2008). The findings of O’Doherty et at. (2003) using functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) indicated that people activated the orbitofrontal medium cortex
when perceiving an attractive face. It is a region involved in representing the stimulus-reward
value. This indicates that when perceiving an attractive face, the rewards areas of the brain are
activated. The same study reported that the response of this region of the brain improved with
a smiling facial expression.
On the other hand, Ishizu and Zeki (2011) analyzed the regions of the brain activated during
auditive and visual perception. The results of their study indicated it is simply a cortical area
located in the middle orbitofrontal cortex that is activated during the perception of beauty. Also,
they observed that the activation force of that area to be proportional to the declared intensity
of the experience of beauty.
According to the sex of the person perceiving facial beauty, studies have revealed that
regardless of the sexual orientation, both sexes perceive female or male facial attractiveness in
a similar way (Kranz & Ishai, 2006).
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
264
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
Concerning age, experimental studies have been carried out reporting that newborns prefer
attractive faces (Slater, Quinn, Hayes, & Brown, 2000). Another study pointed out that young
and middle age people evaluated the faces of all genders in the same way. However, the worst
graded faces corresponded to old women (Foos & Clark, 2011).
Facial beauty
The different ideas about facial beauty have not only varied throughout history, but have
frequently required the use of tricks for faces in order to achieve the ideal of beauty according
to the respective epoch.
Beauty parameter have been pointed out to be arbitrary cultural agreements. However, some
authors such as Eco (2017, p. 14) consider that “it is possible that, beyond different beauty
conceptions, there may be some unique rules for all peoples and all times”. It has been
demonstrated that members of different ethnic groups share common attractiveness standards
(Cunningham, Roberts, Barbee, Druen, & Wu, 1995). Other studies indicate that the opinions
about facial beauty are consistent regardless of race, nationality or age (Fink & Neave, 2005).
For Baudouin (2016) there are common criteria of beauty shared by all human beings. Although
some aspects of judgment may reflect cultural conventions, the geometric characteristics of the
human face originating beauty perceptions reflect universal beauty adaptations (Thornhill &
Grammer, 1999). Some authors suggest that some facial preferences may be part of our
biological rather than cultural inheritance (Rhodes, 2006). Some human facial characteristics
considered to be attractive are mostly symmetry and the average shapes (Germine et al., 2015).
In many species an asymmetry may be linked to a genetic anomaly or be shown in the
individuals exposed to environmental problems (contamination, parasites or diseases). On this
regard symmetry is an indicator of health and reproductive value of competitors (Baudouin,
2016). There is scientific evidence indicating that the preference for symmetrical faces may
have an adaptive value (Scheib, Gangestad, & Thornhill, 1999, as quoted in Fink & Neave, 2005,
p. 320).
Concerning average characteristics, they refer to faces having common facial features or
prototypes (Fink & Penton-Voak, 2016).
The basic proportion of the human face is sexually dimorphic, that is, among men and women
there are differences in their facial appearance depending on sexual hormones. Thus, for
example, male features develop under the influence of testosterone, whereas female features
develop under the influence of estrogens (Fink & Neave, 2005). Therefore, there are facial
features typically male or typically female. Baudoin (2016) points out that for women in
particular these features imply: big eyes, high and thin eyebrows, small nose and narrow jaw.
On the other hand, for men, the characteristics are marked eyebrows closer to the eyes and
smaller eyes. The same author considers people characterized as undefined are in the middle
point of the male-female continuum.
Another characteristic of facial beauty is related to the healthy look of skin. There is scientific
evidence that the facial attractiveness of women is an indicator of hormonal health (Thornhill
& Grammer, 1999). Dermatological studies show that dermatitis is associated to high levels of
sexual hormones (Ghosh, Chaudhuri, Jain, & Aggarwal, 2014). Thus, for example, the polyquistic
ovaric syndrome generates an overproduction of androgens clinically manifested as dermatitis
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
265
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
in women (Schiavone, Rietschel, Sgoutas, & Harris, 1983 as quoted in Fink & Neave, 2005, p.
319). Within this context the skin indicates, more or less reliably, the value of the female
partner. In humans it is expected for men to be more sexually attracted towards women
showing a skin free of lesions, eruptions, lunars, quits, tumors, acne and hirsutism (Fink &
Penton-Voak, 2016).
Concerning eye beauty, Baudouin and Tiberghien (2004) applied a metrical facial approach to
the study of beauty and discovered that female facial attractiveness is higher when the face
shows big eyes and thin eyebrows.
Related to lips beauty, Etcoff (1999) holds that when women are young, their lips are red and
burly. Such characteristics are indicators of youth and reproductive capacity. In puberty
estrogens make the lips to thicken and redden whereas thin and flat lips indicate fragility and
senility.
THE GOLDEN RATIO (j)
The golden ratio is a very intriguing number related to perceived beauty in nature. It is denoted
using the Greek letter Phi (F) or the Greek letter phi (f), being these the uppercase and
lowercase letters, respectively. We will simply use fi or phi (j).
Definition of phi (j)
Phi (j) is simply a ratio or proportion between two distances (Dobre, 2013). Figure 5 shows a
#### must be equal to line AB
#### divided by
straight line and three points. Line ####
AC divided by line AB
####, which equals j, as indicated in equation (1).
line BC
B
A
C
Figure 5: Basic definition of j
####
!"
####
!$
=
####
!$
####
$"
=φ
(1)
Specifying phi (j)
In order to specify the value of j it is necessary to use a bidimensional graphical context.
Actually, j equals the longer distance of a specific rectangle divided by its shorter distance. Such
rectangle is further divided in a square and another rectangle. This last rectangle has the same
proportions of j as to the division of its longer distance by its shorter distance. And we can
proceed further endlessly. This is illustrated in Figure 6.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
266
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
a
a
b
Figure 6: Illustrating the Golden ratio in a series of rectangles
We have that the horizontal distance of the larger rectangle (a+b) divided by the distance of the
vertical side of the yellow square given by a, equals the vertical distance of the red rectangle
given by a divided by its width given by b. This is mathematically expressed in equation (2).
%&'
%
='=φ
(2)
%
Algebraically working out equation (2) yields equation (2a).
%
'
'
%
+% =1+% ='=φ
%
Taking the right side of equation (2a) yields equation (3).
%
=φ
'
Algebraically manipulating equation (3) yields equation (3a).
(
'
=
)
%
(2a)
(3)
(3a)
Notice that equation (3a), which is b/a, equals 1/j, so that substituting b/a for 1/j in equation
(2a) yields equation (4).
(
φ=1+)
(4)
Equation (4) is the fundamental equation for j.
Calculating phi (j)
But, what is the exact value of j? For this we consider equation (4) again. Both sides of such
equation are multiplied by j, as indicated in equation (5).
(
φ )φ = 1 + )* = φ* = φ + 1
(5)
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
267
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
Equation (5) is equated to zero in equation (5a).
φ* − φ − 1 = 0
(5a)
Being j the unknown in equation (5a), it is possible to calculate the value of j using the
quadratic formula, which is applied in equation (6).
φ=
(±√(&*
=
(±√.
*
(6)
The negative part of the square root is discarded, so that j is defined in equation (7). Using the
high precision scientific calculator of an iPhone yields the first digits of j as shown in equation
(7).
φ=
(&√.
*
≈ 1.618033988749895
(7)
Figure 7: The infinite spiral of j
The beauty of phi (j)
The number j is related to a spiral infinite in nature, which can be expanded outwards and
inwards. In order to appreciate it we have to work with the rectangle of Figure 6. Notice such
rectangle is horizontal and that the square is on the left side. The vertical rectangle remaining
on the right side can be divided into another rectangle in the superior side and then for the
horizontal rectangle resulting in the inferior side, a square can be extracted on the right side,
then from the vertical rectangle remaining we can extract another square on the inferior side,
finally remaining another rectangle in the middle that is once again horizontal. And so on, it is
possible to further make cuts in the rectangles, first on the left side, then the up side, then the
right side, then on the down side to go back to the same sequence, left, up, right, down, and so
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
268
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
on, which allows us to draw a spiral that reduces in size (and as extrapolation follow an inverse
process for the spirals outside the page). This is illustrated in Figure 7.
Figure 8: The eye of God
What is important to notice is that the spiral touches the extreme points of the sequences of
squares, which are highlighted in Figure 7. Observe Figure 8. The diagonals drawn on the
rectangles are the golden ratio (gray line) marking what is known as the eye of God, which is
where the spiral converges to in an infinitely small point. These lines cross the vertices of all
golden ratio rectangles than can be drawn. The point where the lines intersect is known as the
eye of God. These spirals are seen in nature and the ratios indicated by j are shown in all kinds
of aesthetic situations, being those related to the beauty of the human body, to art, to nature,
among a limitless number of applications.
FACE PROPORTIONS TO CONSIDER AND THEIR MEASUREMENT
The golden ratio is compared to a series of proportions of the face of a woman. A numerical
score between zero and one is obtained which, when multiplied by ten, becomes a grade
between zero and ten. Concepts of fuzzy logic are used for this purpose (Zadeh, 1965, 1997,
1999, 2002, 2005, 2008). Fuzzy logic is an area of study of artificial intelligence. A series of
measurements of the face of each participant are made and then a grade between zero and ten
is obtained in order to assess how close to the golden ratio such facial feature is. All grades are
averaged using a weighted score in order to get a final grade for each participant.
The literature was reviewed and considerations concerning facial beauty according to the
golden ratio have been made (Packiriswamy, Kumar, & Rao, 2012; Alam, Mohd Noor, Basri,
Yew, & Wen, 2015; Companioni Bachá, Torralbas Velázquez, & Sánchez Meza, 2010), as well as
measurements of the skull considering as a reference the golden ratio (Bakirsi, Kafa, Coskun,
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
269
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
Buyukuysal, & Barut, 2016a, 2016b; Suazo Galdames, Trujillo Hernández, Cantín López, &
Zavando Matamala, 2008).
Calculating a score
Suppose we have a female face such as the one in Figure 9. This face is framed by a rectangle
spanning it and two measurements are considered, the height of the rectangle, denoted by “y”
as well as the width of the rectangle, denoted by “x”.
y
x
Figure 9: A possible measurement of the face of a woman
How close is the proportion y/x to the golden ratio j? If the ratio y/x were a perfect match to
the golden ratio, then equation (8) would hold.
/
=φ
(8)
0
Thus, in this case, the ratio y/x divided by j would be equal to one. Equation (9) shows the
division between the ratio y/x and j. This division is denoted as “z”. It is possible that the ratio
y/x to be approximately equal to j, in which case z ≈ 1, to be greater than j, in which case z > 1
or to be less than j, having z < 1. It is not possible for z to be equal to zero, since that would
require a value for “y” to be zero, that is, not to have such measurement, which is not possible.
//0
z= )
(9)
Additionally, we would not expect z to be greater than two, since that would require y/x to be
twice the value of j, which would be the measurement of a characteristic completely out of
what is reasonable to expect. Consequently, we can calculate the value of f according to equation
(10).
2 − z, 1 ≤ z ≤ 2
0≤z<1
f = 8 z,
(10)
0,
z>2
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
270
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
What is the fuzzy logic reasoning used in equation (10)? If z has a value of one, f would be equal
to 2-1 = 1, that is, a perfect score of closeness to the golden ratio. If z is between one and two
we would get a score between zero and one, depending on how far z is to the golden ratio. On
the other hand, if z is between zero and one, the score is simply equal to z, since this yields a
value between zero and one. Finally, if the proportion z = (y/x)/j is greater than two we have
a case very far from the golden ratio, so that the score associated to such measurement would
be equal to zero.
The numerical grade of the proportion for the face, denoted as “c”, equals ten time the value of
f, as indicated in equation (11).
c = 10f
(11)
Weighting several scores corresponding to several measurements
Suppose there are different ratios for the face of some woman. In this case, there are m values
of y, that is, yk, k = 1, 2, …, m, and m values for xk, k = 1, 2, …, m, where k is the kth measurement.
Notice that in all measurements the value of yk must be associated to the measurement
expected to be the highest and the value of xk to that expected to be lowest. In this way, we get
scores zk, k = 1, 2, …, m, according to equation (12).
/ /0
z2 = !) ! , k = 1, 2, …, m
(12)
The fuzzy logic grade for zk is calculated according to equation (13), which follows the same
logic as equation (10).
2 − z2 , 1 ≤ z2 ≤ 2
0 ≤ z2 < 1, k = 1, 2, …, m
f2 = 8 z2 ,
(13)
0,
z2 > 2
The zero to ten grade, denoted by ck, for the fuzzy logic score fk, equals ten times the value of fk,
as indicated in equation (14).
ck = 10fk, k = 1, 2, …, m
(14)
Finally, and assuming the weight assigned to the kth measurement is given by wk, the final score
is given by equation (15). Equation (16) must hold, that is, the sum of the weights must be 100%
of the final score or grade.
c = ∑3
(15)
24( w2 c2
∑3
24( w2 = 1
(16)
Measurements to do
What ratios of the face of a woman should be considered? We seek those proportions that tend
to approach the golden ratio of a beautiful face. The first measurement to do is the height of the
face (y1) with respect to the width of the face (x1), which has already been indicated in Figure
9. The second measurement to do is the distance between the forehead and the tip of the nose
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
271
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
(y2) and the distance between the tip of the nose and the chin (x2), as indicated in Figure 10.
The third measurement is the distance between the eyes (y3) and the width of the mouth (x3)
as indicated in Figure 11. Finally, the fourth measurement to do is the distance between the top
of the nose to the mouth (y4) and the distance between the mouth and the chin (x4), illustrated
in Figure 12. Notice that, for having a total of four measurements for each woman, m = 4.
y2
x2
Figure 10: Second measurement to do per woman
y3
x3
Figure 11: Third measurement to do per woman.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
272
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
y4
x4
Figure 12: Fourth measurement to do per woman
We are going to work with three women. The images of the three women are shown in Figure
13. Figure 13a shows the first such woman, Figure 13b shows the second such woman and
Figure 13c shows the third such woman.
a. Woman 1. b. Woman 2. c. Woman 3.
Figure 13: The three women to consider.
Based on these images the measurements to calculate the adjustment to the golden ratio for
each woman are taken. We assume the four measurements have the same weight, that is, w1 =
w2 = w3 = w4 = 0.25. Table 1 summarizes all relevant information.
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
273
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
Table 1: Calculating the adjustment to the golden ratio for the three women.
Concept
Woman 1
Woman 2
Woman 3
y1
4.0
4.5
4.3
x1
3.3
3.5
3.1
y1/x1
1.2121
1.2857
1.3871
z1
0.7491
0.7946
0.8573
f1
0.7491
0.7946
0.8573
c1
7.5
7.9
8.6
y2
2.5
2.8
2.8
x2
1.7
1.6
1.5
y2/x2
1.4706
1.7500
1.8667
z2
0.9089
1.0816
1.1537
f2
0.9089
0.9184
0.8463
c2
9.1
9.2
8.5
y3
1.7
1.6
1.7
x3
1.1
1.2
1.1
y3/x3
1.5455
1.3333
1.5455
z3
0.9551
0.8240
0.9551
f3
0.9551
0.8240
0.9551
c3
9.6
8.2
9.6
y4
1.6
1.7
1.7
x4
1.0
1.0
0.8
y4/x4
1.6000
1.7000
2.1250
z4
0.9889
1.0507
1.3133
f4
0.9889
0.9493
0.6867
c4
C
j=
9.9
9.0
1.618034
9.5
8.7
6.9
8.4
The best grade is for the first woman (C = 9.0), the second-best grade is for the second woman
(C = 8.7) and the worse grade is for the third woman (C = 8.4). With these grades or scores, it is
possible to compare the order in the degree of beauty of the three women according to how
closely they match the golden ratio with respect to what two groups of test subjects consider
how beautiful each woman is (the first group of test subjects for men, the second one for
women). Certainly, in order to measure how close to the score obtained in Table 1 for each
woman the test subjects consider these women to be, requires a quantitative and statistical way
of measuring the degree of closeness between what the golden ratio suggests and what is
obtained in the experiments carried out.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
274
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
In order to prove (or deny) the theory that a female face is beautiful if it adjusts, statistically
speaking, to the parameters indicated by the golden ratio, a series of statistical tools and
considerations are required. First, there is a general score calculated from the m measurements
carried out for each of the three faces of the female models (women) selected. Let C1 be the
highest score obtained by one of the three contestants, C2 the score in between and C3 the lowest
score obtained. A series of experiments are carried out using a graphic interface specifically
developed in order to do the statistical measurements. In this case, we consider two groups of
individuals separately: men and women. Let n be the total number of experimental data
obtained by having men (or women) judging the facial beauty of the contestants. We have two
sets of experimental data of n pairs of measurements each. Notice that the number of n men
doing the subjective evaluations does not necessarily have to be the same number of n women
doing the same subjective evaluations. However, for simplicity, we speak of n measurements in
each series of experiments.
We consider two pieces of information: the number of seconds the person took to reach its
subjective evaluation of the beauty of the female models (y), as well as the score obtained in
such subjective evaluation with respect to how close was the evaluation compared to the
suggested order given by the pattern indicated by the adjustment to the golden ratio of the
facial beauty of the three female models (x). The time calculation (in seconds) for the amount
of time each experiment lasts for each test subject is trivial to obtain. However, the calculation
of the score of adjustment to the facial beauty as indicated by the golden ratio is not.
Calculating the experimental adjustment score to the pattern suggested by the golden
ratio criteria
According to the score obtained considering the adjustment of each of the three faces of the
female models to the golden ratio, it is possible to sort the three faces, from left to right, going
from highest to lowest score, respectively. Thus, the female face with the highest score is
denoted as 1, the one with the middle score is 2 and the one with the worse score is 3. Then, the
“optimal” sequence according to the subjective grade of the faces of the three female models is
1-2-3. The test subject is presented with the three faces not sorted from left to right as best to
worst score. How many possible sortings can we have? We have to find the number of
permutations of n faces taken by r per r. Such number of permutations (Walpole & Myers,
1989), denoted as nPr, is indicated according to equation (17). Notice that this n has nothing to
do with the n pair of experimental results (one for men and the other for women).
5!
(17)
n Pr =
(589)!
In this case, we have 3 permutations taken by 3 per 3, that is, n = 3 and r = 3, so that 3P3 = 3!/(33)! = 3!/0! = 6/1 = 6. Nevertheless, which ones are these six permutations? The experimental
subject can choose the faces in the following ways: 1-2-3, 1-3-2, 2-1-3, 2-3-1, 3-1-2 and 3-2-1.
Let T1 be the priority assigned to the face considered by the test subject to be the most beautiful
(where T1 = {1, 2, 3}), T2 the priority assigned by the test subject to be the medium best-looking
face (where T2 = {1, 2, 3}), and T3 the priority assigned by the test subject to be the worst looking
face (where T3 = {1, 2, 3}). The formula used to calculate the score (Si, i = 1, 2, …, n) for the
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
275
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
selection of each test subject with respect to what is suggested by the fitness to the golden ratio
is indicated in equation (18), where n is the total number of measurements carried out (in one
case for men test subjects and in the other for women test subjects).
Si = (T1-1)2+(T2-2)2+(T3-3)2, i = 1, 2, …, n
(18)
Does the formula for equation (18) make sense? Let us see. If test subject i grades the models
as 1-2-3 (that is, exactly according to what the golden ratio suggests), it would have a score of
Si = (1-1)2+(2-2)2+(3-3)2 = 0. This is the best score than can be obtained. If, on the other hand,
the test subject i grades the models as 3-2-1 (that is, in a completely reversed order, because
the worst looking model goes first and the best-looking model goes last), the score obtained
would be Si = (3-1)2+(2-2)2+(1-3)2 = 8. A modestly bad score would be, for example, 3-1-2, that
is, Si = (3-1)2+(1-2)2+(2-3)2 = 6. Consequently, the best score possible is zero and the worst
score possible is eight, having as intermediate scores two and six. In order to get a numerical
score between zero and one, Pi, we use equation (19).
(<8= )
P; = < " , i = 1, 2, …, n
(19)
This score (Pi) is the one used as first variable (xi, i = 1, 2, …, n). We can have values for xi, i = 1,
2, …, n, of (8-0)/8 = 1, (8-2)/8 = 6/8 = 3/4 = 0.75, (8-6)/8 = 2/8 = 1/4 = 0.25 and finally (8-8)/8
= 0. The second variable (yi, i = 1, 2, …, n) is the number of seconds it took test subject i to sort
the models.
Therefore, for each set of n data (one for male experimental subjects and the other for female
experimental subjects, where the values for n are not necessarily the same), we have two sets
of results, (xi,yi), i = 1, 2, …, n.
y
(x4,y4)
(x1,y1)
(x2,y2)
(x3,y3)
(x5,y5)
x
Figure 14: Perfect linear adjustment with positive slope (r = 1) and n = 5 data points.
Linear adjustment and the linear correlation coefficient
Generally speaking, if the we use scatterplots for two variables (x,y), we try to fit the results
using a straight line as well as to evaluate how good is such adjustment using the linear
correlation coefficient (r). If r = 1, the data fit perfectly a straight line with a positive slope (see
Figure 14), if r = -1, it means the data fit perfectly a negative slope (see Figure 15) and if r = 0,
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
276
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
there is no adjustment, meaning all the data is completely scattered for the entire (x,y)
scatterplot.
y
(x3,y3)
(x1,y1)
(x4,y4)
(x5,y5)
(x2,y2)
x
Figure 15: Perfect linear adjustment with negative slope (r = -1) and n = 5 data points.
Nevertheless, what happens in realistic cases where the data does not perfectly fit a straight
line? Such situation is illustrated in Figure 16. In this figure, we try for the straight line to fit in
such way so as to minimize the errors (distance) between the points and the straight line
adjusting them.
y
(x4,y4)
(x2,y2)
(x5,y5)
(x1,y1)
(x3,y3)
x
Figure 16: Optimal adjustment for a straight line having five (n = 5) realistic experimental
results.
How can we get the parameters of such straight line for optimal adjustment? The general
formula for all straight lines is given in equation (20), where a is the point at which the line cuts
the vertical axis and b is the slope of such straight line.
y = a + bx
(20)
Following some mathematical work that needs no demonstration here, we get the value of a
and b as indicated in equations (21) and (22), respectively.
a=
∑ 0# ∑ /8∑ 0 ∑ 0/
5 ∑ 0# 8(∑ 0)#
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
(21)
277
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
b=
? ∑ 0/8∑ 0 ∑ /
5 ∑ 0# 8(∑ 0)#
(22)
How well these two parameters (a and b) fit the experimental data? Walpole and Myers (1989)
indicate that the straight line described by equation (20) fits the parameters from equations
(21) and (22) having a linear correlation coefficient, r, where -1≤r≤1, given by equation (23).
r=
∑$
"%& @" A"
# $
#
B∑$
"%& @" ∑"%& A"
(23)
We have for the case of equation (23) that Xi and Yi are given by equations (24) and (25),
respectively, having x# and y# given by equations (26) and (27), respectively. Notice that x# and y#
are the mean (average) values of the data.
(24)
Xi = xi-x#, i = 1, 2, …, n
Yi = yi-y#, i = 1, 2, …, n
x# =
y# =
(25)
∑$
"%& 0"
5
(26)
∑$
"%& /"
5
(27)
Also, we have that the variances of the variables x and y, sx2 and sy2, respectively, are given by
equations (28) and (29), respectively. Notice that if we consider the population mean for both
variables (µx and µy) to be properly represented by the means of equations (26) and (27), and
if the data is normally distributed, 95% of all the data would be between µx-2sx and µx+2sx for
x and between µy-2sy and µy+2sy for y. Realize that the set of data “y” is given by the time it
takes the experimental subject to solve the problem, whereas the set of data “x” is given by the
score (Pi) each individual obtains in his/her subjective evaluation of the facial beauty of the
three female models.
σ*0 =
σ*/ =
∑$
# )#
"%&(0" 80
58(
(28)
∑$
C )#
"%&(/" 8/
58(
(29)
Designing the user interface (software) in order to carry out the experiments
A specially designed software was created in order to carry out the experiments with men and
women. In Figure 17 we show the interface for experiments with men (it says “Prueba para
Hombres” meaning “Test for Men”). In the case of the interface for women, it is the same, but it
says “Prueba para Mujeres”, meaning “Test for Women”.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
278
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
Figure 17: Experimental interface.
RESULTS
We had a total of 88 men and 109 women doing the golden ratio experiment. In both cases, the
independent variable (x) was the grade or score obtained according to equation (19), which is
a number going from zero to one, but can only take specific values of 0, 0.25, 0.75 and 1. The
dependent variable (y) was the time each individual took to do the test, measured in seconds.
A Pearson correlation between the two variables (x,y) was carried out. Also, a linear regression
was conducted. Coefficients a and b for the linear regression (linear correlation) are given by
equations (21) and (22), respectively. The Pearson correlation coefficient is given by equation
(23), where Xi, Yi, x# and y# are given according to equations (24), (25), (26) and (27),
respectively.
Table 2 summarizes the results obtained for men. Table 3 indicates the results obtained for
women.
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
279
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
Table 2: Results of the experiments carried out for men.
n=
Sx =
Sy =
88
29.25
2324
Sx2 =
Sxy =
x" =
y" =
19.3125
818
0.33
26.4091
SX2 =
9.59
SY2 =
SXY =
a=
b=
r=
15387.27
45.53
24.831
4.748
0.1185
R2 = r2 =
0.0141
Tabla 3: Results of the experiments carried out for women.
n=
Sx =
Sy =
109
55.25
2955
Sx2 =
Sxy =
x" =
y" =
43.8125
1646.75
0.51
27.1101
SX2 =
15.81
SY2 =
SXY =
a=
b=
r=
26672.68
148.92
22.335
9.4208
0.2293
R2 = r2 =
0.0526
In order to deepen the analysis and to clarify what is happening, descriptive statistics are
included. Table 4 shows the calculation for minimum and maximum, mean or average, T.D. or
typical deviation (also known as standard deviation, which equals the square root of the
variance), mode and median.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
280
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
Variable
x Men
y Men
x Women
y Women
Minimum
0
2
0
1
Table 4: Descriptive statistics
Maximum Mean
T.D.
1
0.3324
0.3320
77
26.4091
13.2991
1
0.5069
0.3826
97
27.1101
15.7153
Mode
0
17
0.25
16
Median
0.25
24
0.75
23
The descriptive statistics for the values of x are analyzed considering the number of times each
one of the possible values (0, 0.25, 0.75 and 1) occur, which is shown in Table 5 and Table 6 for
men and women, respectively.
Table 5: Score distribution for men.
x Men
Totals
Percentage
0
31
35.23%
0.25
29
32.95%
0.75
24
27.27%
1
4
4.55%
0.3324
88
100.00%
Table 6: Score distribution for women.
x Women Totals
Percentage
0
23
21.10%
0.25
31
28.44%
0.75
30
27.52%
1
25
22.94%
0.5069
109
100.00%
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
The data for Table 2 indicates than there is a positive relationship between the score (x) and
the time used for the test (y). In the case of men, such relationship is given by equation (30),
whereas in the case of women the relationship is given by equation (31).
y = 4.748x + 24.831
(30)
y = 9.4208x + 22.335
(31)
However, actually, the score should be the dependent variable (it is the independent variable
or x) and the time used should be the independent variable (it is the dependent variable or y).
Thus, solving for x in both equation (30) and equation (31) yields equations (32) and (33) for
the cases of men and women, respectively.
x = 0.2106y – 5.2298
(32)
x = 0.1061y – 2.3708
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
(33)
281
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
Both in the case of equations (30) and (31) as in the case of equations (32) and (33) the Pearson
correlation coefficient (r) is the same. In this case, there is a positive correlation for both
variables of r = 0.1185 for men and r = 0.2293 for women (the correlation does not change
regardless of what variables are considered independent or dependent). The slope for both
equations is always positive. This simply means that the more time it takes to reach a decision,
the better the decision is.
First, in Table 4, there is a summary of basic descriptive statistics. In this case, we have the
values for the minimum, maximum, mean, typical deviation or standard deviation, mode and
median. Notice that in the case of the score calculated (which is different from the golden ration
score for each model shown in Table 2), there are discrete instances for x, that is, x can be equal
to 0, 0.25, 0.75 or 1. It is not a surprise to see that the mode for the values of x in Table 4
corresponds to the most common value (0 in the case of men, 0.25 in the case of women). The
median is simply the value in the middle or the sorted list of the values obtained. In the case of
men such value is 0.25, whereas in the case of women such value is 0.75. The mean for men is
0.3324 whereas for women it is 0.5069. It seems that in the case of men the selections were
contrary to what the golden ratio suggests. Apparently, men got carried away by stereotypes in
their decision. Notice that according to our golden ratio measurements, the most beautiful
model is number 1, followed by model number 2 and finally model number 3, as shown in
Figure 13. However, in the interface shown in Figure 17 the most beautiful woman is shown
last, the least beautiful model is in the middle, whereas the model in between with respect to
its fit to the golden ration appears first in the interface. Consequently, since the model that
appears first in the interface is blond and the model that appears last in the interface (being the
most beautiful according to the fit to the golden ratio measurements) has big ears, it seems men
acted impulsively without concentrating on the face but rather following stereotypes and
following the impulse to click on the finalize button immediately without giving enough
consideration to the faces instead of the overall look of the model. All test subjects were given
specific instructions to focus on the beauty of the face and not to follow stereotypes. On the
other hand, women did a much better job and considered the instructions carefully. All four
possible scores in the case of women are approximately equally likely and, as a consequence,
the mean approximately equals 0.50. It seems men perceive beauty in a more visual way,
whereas women perceive beauty more verbally. Men seem to consider beauty judging by looks
and visual stereotypes, whereas women consider beauty based on what they are told is
beautiful. In order to get a score of 1 the minimum effort requires doing two face swapes using
the interface, which requires considerable effort and time dedicated to the experiment. Thus,
the time taken to do the experiment is relevant and it makes sense to see that the greater the
amount of time taken to do the experiment, the better the decision reached.
As such, what about the analysis for the time to respond? Although the response time is a
discrete variable (seconds), it has considerable variation. In the case of men, according to Table
4, it varies between 2 seconds and 77 seconds, whereas in the case of women it varies between
1 second and 97 seconds. Notice that women on average took 27.1101 seconds, whereas men
took less time, 26.4091 seconds. Thus, women tend to take more time to decide which should
be the order of the models and tend to reach a better result when doing the experiment, which
coincides with the theory of the golden ratio.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
282
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
It is concluded that women tend to make a better decision than men, possibly due precisely to
the instructions given consisting in only considering the interior of the face, without
considering ears or hair color, and to avoid prejudice such as “if it is blond is more beautiful”.
For men, it seems the first impression, leading to avoid making changes in the order of the faces
given by the interface and tending to use little time to do the experiment is what prevails.
Apparently, men tend to decide female facial beauty more based on first impression visual cues,
tending to prejudice, while women tend to decide female facial beauty in a more verbal fashion,
trying to go deeper in their decisions. The parameters of beauty suggested by the golden ratio
are more compatible to the perception of female facial beauty in the case of women than in men.
The results should also be considered from another point of view. There is a total of 6 possible
outcomes for the score obtained, both in men as in women, as indicated in equation (17). Of
these six possibilities, only one, the 1/6 ≈ 0.1667 ≈ 16.67% would be the case for a perfect score
of one by ordering the faces correctly as 1-2-3 (first model 1 as the most beautiful, followed by
model 2 and then 3). That is, a perfect score of one corresponds to the order indicated if the
golden ratio is a perfect benchmark for the facial beauty of the models. All other scores do not
follow the score obtained if the golden ratio is an accurate indicator of facial beauty. All other
possible scores yield a value less than one. Thus, the perfect score would occur only in one out
of six possible outcomes, that is, the perfect score would be 1/6 ≈ 0.1667 ≈ 16.67% of the cases.
In all other cases the golden ratio would not be followed. Consequently, having an average score
above 0.1667 is already an indicator that the golden ratio does follow an important role, since
in all the remaining 5 possibilities the golden ratio is not considered to be the indicator with
the highest score. As a consequence, the fact that men have obtained an average score of 0.33
and women an average of 0.51 is already an indicator that the golden ratio does follow an
important role in determining female facial beauty. To see things from this other point of view,
consider throwing a dice. A dice can have as outcomes: 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6. A truly random
behavior would be to get probabilities for each of these possible outcomes of 1/6 ≈ 0.1667. Any
other result above this probability for any of the outcomes would indicate that the sides of the
dice are loaded, that is, that certain possible result is more relevant. As such, an outcome above
0.1667 for the value obtained in the experiments carried out would be indicative that the golden
ratio is affecting the outcomes.
From this alternative point of view, we could consider that the golden ratio plays a fundamental
role in the perception of beauty of the test subjects. Thus, we would conclude that the golden
ratio is a relevant factor indicative of female facial beauty both in men as in women. More
specifically, it indicates that women are more sensitive than men to female beauty and that the
golden ratio is important in such perception. Why are women more sensitive than men to the
parameters of the golden ratio? Maybe it is because women are more responsive to issues
related to facial beauty. It could also be argued that women are evolutionary more advanced
than men. Nevertheless, all of the above would be highly speculative. What can indeed be stated
is that the golden ratio substantially affects the perception of female facial beauty both in men
as in women, but more for women than men.
As to the debate about whether beauty is objective and has to do with issues related to
proportions, which is the idea of this paper, or if it is completely relative and depends entirely
on environmental issues, it is better to leave that to the judgment of the reader. Keep in mind,
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
283
Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ)
Vol. 9, Issue 9, September-2022
however, that according to the literature review of section 2, an innate biological component in
the perception of beauty and even to regions of the brain doing such perception cannot be
denied.
References
Alam, M. K., Mohd Noor, N. F., Basri, R., Yew, T. F., & Wen, T. H. (2015). Multiracial Facial Golden Ratio and
Evaluation of Facial Appearance. PLOS One, 10: 1-22.
Bakirci, S., I., Kafa, M., Coskun, I., Buyukuysal, M. C., & Barut, C. (2016a). A Comparison of Anatomical
Measurements of the Infraorbital Foramen of Skulls of the Modern and Late Byzantine Periods and the Golden
Ratio. International Journal of Morphology, 34: 788-795.
Bakirci, S., I., Kafa, M., Coskun, I., Buyukuysal, M. C., & Barut, C. (2016b). A Comparison of the Relationship
between the Golden Ratio and Anatomical Characteristics of the Supraorbital Foramen in Bare Skulls Belonging
to the Byzantine Era and Modern Era. International Journal of Morphology, 34: 671-678.
Baudouin, J. Y. (2016). “Que révèle le visage?”. Retrieved from: https://www.fichierpdf.fr/2016/05/05/que_revele_le_visage_jean_yves_baudoin/que_revele_le_visage_jean_yves_baudoin.pdf.
Baudouin, J. Y. & Tiberghien, G. (2004). Symmetry, averageness, and feature size in the facial attractiveness of
women. Acta Psychol. (Amst.), 117: 313-332.
Clouthier, J., Heatherton, T. F., Whalen, P. J., & Kelley, W. M. (2008). Are attractive people rewarding? Sex
differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. J. Cog Neurosci, 20: 1-18.
Companioni Bachá, A. E., Torralbas Velazquez, A., & Sánchez Mesa, C. (2010). Relación entre la proporción áurea
y el índice facial en estudiantes de Estomatología de La Habana. Revista Cubana de Estomatología, 47: 50-61.
Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee, A. P., Druen, P. B., & Wu, C. H. (1995). Their ideas of beauty are, on the
whole, the same as ours: Consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical
attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68: 261-279.
De Bartolomeo, M. & Magni, V. (2012). La fondazione dell´estetica. Istituto Italiano Edizioni Atlas.
Dobre, D. (2013). Mathematical Properties of the Golden Ratio – a Fascinating number. Journal of Industrial
Design and Engineering Graphics, 8: 11-16.
Eco, U. (2017). Historia de la belleza. Debolsillo.
Etcoff, N. (1999). Survival of the prettiest. Doubleday.
Fink, B. & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2016). Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness, Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci, 11:
154-158.
Fink, B. & Neave, N. (2005). The biology of facial beauty. International Journal of Society of Cosmetic Science, 27:
317-325.
Foos, P. W. & Clark, M. C. (2011). Adult age and gender differences in perceptions of facial attractiveness: Beauty
is in the eye of the older beholder. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172: 162-175.
Germine, L., Russell, R. , Bronstad, M. , Blokland, G. A. M., Smoller, J. W., Kwok, H., Anthony, S. E., Nakayama, K.,
Rhodes, G., & Wilmer, J. B. (2015). Individual aesthetic preferences for faces are shaped mostly by environments,
not genes. Current Biology, 25: 2684-2689.
Ghosh, S., Chaudhuri, S., Jain, V. K., & Aggarwal, K. (2014). Profiling and hormonal therapy for acne in women.
Indian Journal of Dermatology, 59: 107-115.
Ishizu, T. & Zeki, S. (2011). Toward a brain–based theory of beauty. PLoS ONE, 6: e21852.
Kranz, F. & Ishai, A. (2006). Face perception is modulated by sexual preference. Curr Biol. 16: 63-68.
O´Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perret, D., Burt, D. M., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: the role of
medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia, 41: 147-155.
Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom
284
Copertari, L. F., & Reyna, G. V. (2022). Beauty and the Golden Ratio. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 9(9). 259-285.
Packiriswamy, V., Kumar, P., & Rao, M. (2012). Identification of Facial Shape by Applying Golden Ratio to the
Facial Measurements: An Interratial Study in Malaysian Population. North American Journal of Medical Sciences,
4: 624-629.
Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annu. Rev. Psychol, 57: 199-226.
Rodríguez, M., Rodríguez, M. E., Barbería, E., Durán, J., Muñoz, M., & Vera, V. (2000). Evolución histórica de los
conceptos de belleza facial. Ortodoncia Clínica, 3: 156-163.
Serracanta, F. (2020). “Concepto de belleza – Historia de la sinfonía”. Retrieved from:
https://www.historiadelasinfonia.es/monografias/las_sinfonias_de_khrennikov/concepto_de_belleza/.
Slater, A., Quinn, P. C., Hayes, R. & Brown, E. (2000). The role of facial orientation in newborn infants´ preference
for attractive faces. Developmental Science, 3: 181-185.
Suazo Galdames, I., Trujillo Hernández, E. G., Cantín López, M., & Zavando Matamala, D. (2008). Determinación de
Proporciones Áurea Cráneofaciales para la Reconstrucción con Fines de Identificación Médicolegal. International
Journal of Morphology, 26: 331-335.
Syntek, A. (2020). “ASOMBROSA-MENTE. Las Reglas de la Atracción”. Retrieved from:
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zMm6QXEgpxU.
Thornhill, R. & Grammer, K. (1999). The body and the face of woman: One ornament that signals quality?
Evolution and Human Behavior, 20: 105-120.
Walpole, R. E. & Myers, R. H. (1989). Probabilidad y Estadística para Ingenieros. McGraw-Hill.
Zadeh, L. A. (1965). Fuzzy sets. Information and Control, 8: 338-353.
Zadeh, L. A. (1997). Towards a theory of fuzzy information granulation and its centrality in human reasoning and
fuzzy logic. Fuzzy Sets and Systems, 90: 111-127.
Zadeh, L. A. (1999). Fuzzy sets as a basis for a theory of possibility. Fuzzy Sets and Systems, 100: 9-34.
Zadeh, L. A. (2002). Toward a perception based theory of probabilistic reasoning with imprecise probabilities.
Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference, 105: 233-264.
Zadeh, L. A. (2005). Toward a generalized theory of uncertainty (GTU) – an outline. Information Sciences, 172: 140.
Zadeh, L. A. (2008). Is there a need for fuzzy logic? Information Sciences, 178: 2751-2779.
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.99.13088
285