VO LU M E 5 0 NU M B E R 1 M A R C H 2 0 0 9
Psychosocial and Biological Markers of Daily Lives of Midlife Parents of
Children with Disabilities
Marsha Mailick Seltzer, David M. Almeida, Jan S. Greenberg, Jyoti Savla, Robert S. Stawski, Jinkuk Hong,
and Julie Lounds Taylor
SOCIAL CONNECTIONS, COMMUNITY AND HEALTH
Mothers’ Community Participation and Child Health
Jenna Nobles and Elizabeth Frankenberg
Social Disconnectedness, Perceived Isolation, and Health among Older
Adults
Erin York Cornwell and Linda J. Waite
Neighborhood Disorder, Subjective Alienation, and Distress
Catherine E. Ross and John Mirowsky
PATHWAYS OF HEALTH PROTECTION AND RISK
Pathways to Adult Marijuana and Cocaine Use: A Prospective Study of
African Americans from Age 6 to 42
Kate E. Fothergill, Margaret E. Ensminger, Kerry M. Green, Judith A. Robertson, and Hee Soon Juon
Ethnic Differences in Trajectories of Depressive Symptoms:
Disadvantage in Family Background, High School Experiences, and
Adult Characteristics
Katrina M. Walsemann, Gilbert C. Gee, and Arline T. Geronimus
Development of Mastery during Adolescence: The Role of Family
Problem-Solving
Katherine Jewsbury Conger, Shannon Tierney Williams, Wendy M. Little, Katherine E. Masyn, and
Barbara Shebloski
Editor
ELIZA K. PAVALKO
Indiana University
Deputy Editors
PAMELA BRABOY JACKSON
Indiana University
BERNICE PESCOSOLIDO
Indiana University
JILL QUADAGNO
Florida State University
SCOTT SCHIEMAN
University of Toronto
Associate Editors
RONALD J. ANGEL
University of Texas-Austin
CLIFFORD L. BROMAN
Michigan State University
CHRISTOPHER BROWNING
Ohio State University
RALPH CATALANO
University of California, Berkeley
BRIAN K. FINCH
San Diego State University
GILBERT C. GEE
University of California, Los Angeles
SUSAN GORE
University of Massachusetts, Boston
BRIDGET K. GORMAN
Rice University
JOSEPH GRZYWACZ
Wake Forest University
STEVEN HASS
Arizona State University
PAMELA HERD
University of Wisconsin–Madison
KARA JOYNER
Bowling Green State University
DONALD A. LLOYD
Florida State University
KAREN LUTFEY
New England Research Institutes
SCOTT LYNCH
Princeton University
FRED E. MARKOWITZ
Northern Illinois University
PEGGY MCDONOUGH
University of Toronto
ELIZABETH MENAGHAN
Ohio State University
Managing Editors
INDERMOHAN VIRK
Indiana University
JOSEPH D. WOLFE
Indiana University
Copy Editor
ANDREW J. COGNARD-BLACK
St. Mary’s College of Maryland
American Sociological Association Executive Officer
SALLY T. HILLSMAN
ILAN H. MEYER
Columbia University
RICHARD ALLEN MIECH
University of Colorado Denver
MELISSA A. MILKIE
University of Maryland
SAMUEL NOH
University of Toronto
DEBORA K. PADGETT
New York University
FRED C. PAMPEL
University of Colorado at Boulder
JO C. PHELAN
Columbia University
JOHN R. REYNOLDS
Florida State University
CHRISTIAN RITTER
Northeast Ohio Universities Colleges of Medicine and
Pharmacy
STEPHANIE A. ROBERT
University of Wisconsin–Madison
SUSAN ROXBURGH
Kent State University
TERESA L. SCHEID
University of North Carolina at Charlotte
MICHAEL J. SHANAHAN
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
MARK TAUSIG
University of Akron
STEFAN TIMMERMANS
University of California, Los Angeles
KRISTI L. WILLIAMS
Ohio State University
ANDREA E. WILLSON
University of Western Ontario
Volume 50
Number 1
March 2009
Psychosocial and Biological Markers of Daily Lives of Midlife Parents of Children
with Disabilities
Marsha Mailick Seltzer, David M. Almeida, Jan S. Greenberg, Jyoti Savla, Robert S. Stawski,
Jinkuk Hong, and Julie Lounds Taylor
SOCIAL CONNECTIONS, COMMUNITY AND HEALTH
Mothers’ Community Participation and Child Health
Jenna Nobles and Elizabeth Frankenberg
1
16
Social Disconnectedness, Perceived Isolation, and Health among Older Adults
Erin York Cornwell and Linda J. Waite
31
Neighborhood Disorder, Subjective Alienation, and Distress
Catherine E. Ross and John Mirowsky
49
PATHWAYS OF HEALTH PROTECTION AND RISK
Pathways to Adult Marijuana and Cocaine Use: A Prospective Study of African
Americans from Age 6 to 42
Kate E. Fothergill, Margaret E. Ensminger, Kerry M. Green, Judith A. Robertson, and
Hee Soon Juon
Ethnic Differences in Trajectories of Depressive Symptoms: Disadvantage in Family
Background, High School Experiences, and Adult Characteristics
Katrina M. Walsemann, Gilbert C. Gee, and Arline T. Geronimus
Development of Mastery during Adolescence: The Role of Family Problem-Solving
Katherine Jewsbury Conger, Shannon Tierney Williams, Wendy M. Little,
Katherine E. Masyn, and Barbara Shebloski
65
82
99
NOTICE TO CONTRIBUTORS
Revised March 2008
Manuscript Submission
Package your manuscript securely and include the following:
• A cover letter providing complete contact information for the corresponding author (name, address, phone,
fax, email), the complete manuscript title, and any other important and relevant information.
• One (1) original manuscript typed double-spaced with 1-inch margins (see below), including title page.
• One (1) blinded copy that starts with the abstract page headed by the full article title. Do not include the
title page or any other self-identifying information in this copy. Copy may be printed or photocopied.
• One (1) electronic copy of the original manuscript on CD or via e-mail. Electronic files (excluding artwork) must be in Microsoft Word or Excel format. Do not send pdf files.
• One (1) electronic copy of the blinded manuscript on CD or via e-mail.
• A $25 manuscript processing fee in the form of a check or money order payable to the American
Sociological Association. No fee is required for submissions by ASA student members.
Mail manuscripts to the Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Indiana University, Karl F. Schuessler Institute
for Social Research, 1022 East Third St., Bloomington, IN 47405-7103; phone (812) 856-6979; e-mail
[email protected]. The JHSB editorial office will acknowledge the receipt of your manuscript via e-mail.
Manuscript Preparation
Articles published in the Journal of Health and Social Behavior are seldom longer than 35 manuscript pages,
including (1) title page, (2) abstract, (3) text, (4) notes, (5) references, (6) tables, (7) figures, and (8) appendices.
All pages must be typed double-spaced (including notes and references), on 8-1/2-by-11-inch white paper.
Margins must be at least 1 inch (i.e., line length must not exceed 6-1/2 inches). Please use Times New Roman
font, 12-point type size (roughly equivalent to 10-pitch type size). The object is to provide reviewers and editors
with easy-to-read text and space for notes. It is the responsibility of authors to submit manuscripts in the proper
JHSB format (see below). Manuscripts not submitted in JHSB format may be returned for revision. Additional
details on preparing and submitting manuscripts to JHSB are published in the American Sociological Association
Style Guide (ISBN 0-912764-29-5), available from the ASA Publications Department; phone: (202) 383-9005;
email:
[email protected].
1. The title page should include the full title of the article, each author’s complete name and institutional affiliation, total word count (include all text, notes, and references; do not include word counts for tables or figures),
number of tables, number of figures, and running head (short title, fewer than 55 characters with spaces). Use
an asterisk (*) to add a note to the title giving the corresponding author (name, address, phone, fax, and email).
In the same note, cite acknowledgments, credits, or grants.
2. Print the abstract (fewer than 150 words) on a separate page headed by the title. Omit author identification.
3. The text of the manuscript should begin on a new page headed by the full title. Notes, references, tables, figures, and appendices appear in separate sections following the text, in that order. Since manuscripts are evaluated through an anonymous peer review process, authors should make every effort to remove identifying references or material. When citing your own work, please write “Smith (1992) concluded .|.|. ,” but do not write
“I concluded (Smith 1992) .|.|. ”
a. Headings and subheadings in the text indicate the organization of content. Generally, three heading levels
are sufficient. See recent issues for examples.
b. Citations in the text should provide the last name of the author(s) and the year of publication. Include page
numbers for direct quotes or specific passages. Cite only those works needed to provide evidence for your
assertions and to refer to important sources on the topic. In the following examples of text citations,
ellipses (.|.|.) indicate manuscript text:
• If author’s name is in the text, follow it with the year in parentheses: “Duncan (1959) .|.|.”
• If author’s name is not in the text, enclose the last name and year in parentheses: “.|.|. (Gouldner 1963).”
• Pages cited follow the year of publication after a colon: “.|.|. (Ramirez and Weiss 1979:239–40).”
• Provide last names for joint authors: “.|.|. (Martin and Bailey 1988).”
• For three authors, list all three last names in the first citation in the text: “.|.|. (Carr, Smith, and Jones
1962).” For all subsequent citations use “et al.” throughout: “.|.|. (Carr et al. 1962).” For works with four
or more authors, use “et al.” throughout.
• For institutional authorship, supply minimum identification from the complete citation: “.|.|. (U.S.
Bureau of the Census 1963:117).”
• List a series of citations in alphabetical order or date order separated by semicolons: “.|.|. (Burgess
1968; Marwell et al. 1971).” Use consistent ordering throughout the manuscript.
• Use “forthcoming” to cite sources scheduled for publication. For dissertations and unpublished papers,
cite the date. If no date, use “n.d.” in place of the date: “.|.|. Smith (forthcoming) and Oropesa (n.d.).”
• For machine-readable data files, cite authorship and date: “.|.|. (Institute for Survey Research 1976).”
c. Notes should be numbered in the text consecutively using superscript Arabic numerals. If referring to a
note earlier or later in the text, use a parenthetical note: “.|.|. (see note 3).”
d. Equations in text must be typed. Use consecutive Arabic numerals in parentheses at the right margin to
identify important equations.
4. Notes should be typed or printed, double-spaced, in a separate “NOTES” section and should appear after the
text but before the references. Begin each note with the Arabic numeral to which it is keyed in the text (e.g.,
“1. After 1981, .|.|.”). Notes can (a) explain or amplify text, (b) cite materials of limited availability, or (c)
append information presented in a table. Avoid long notes. Consider (a) stating in the text that information is
ii
available from the author, (b) depositing the information in a national retrieval center and inserting an appropriate note, or (c) adding an appendix.
5. References follow the text in a separate section headed “REFERENCES.” All references cited in the text must
be listed in the reference section, and vice versa. Publication information for each must be complete and correct. It is authors’ responsibility to make sure that all information provided in the reference section is complete
and correct. List the references in alphabetical order by authors’ last names; include first names and middle initials for all authors. If there are two or more items by the same author(s), list them in order of year of publication. If the cited material is unpublished but has been accepted for publication, use “Forthcoming” in place of
the date, and give the name of the journal or publishing house. For dissertations and unpublished papers, cite
the date and place the paper was presented and/or where it is available. If no date is available, use “N.d.” in
place of the date. If two or more works are by the same author(s) within the same year, list them in alphabetical order by title and distinguish them by adding the letters a, b, c, and so on, to the year (or to “Forthcoming”
or “N.d.”). For works with multiple authors, only the name of the first author is inverted (e.g., “Jones, Arthur
B., Colin D. Smith, and James Petersen.”). List all authors; using “et al.” in the reference section is not acceptable. A few examples follow. Refer to the American Sociological Association Style Guide and recent issues of
JHSB for additional examples:
• Books:
Bernard, Claude. [1865] 1957. An Introduction to the Study of Experimental Medicine. Translated by
Henry C. Greene. New York: Dover.
House, James S. 1981. Work Stress and Social Support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
U.S. Bureau of the Census. 1960. Characteristics of the Population. Vol. 1. Washington, DC: U.S.
Government Printing Office.
• Periodicals:
Conger, Rand D. Forthcoming. “The Effects of Positive Feedback on Direction and Amount of
Verbalization in a Social Setting.” Sociological Perspectives.
Goodman, Leo A. 1947a. “The Analysis of Systems of Qualitative Variables When Some of the Variables
Are Unobservable. Part I—A Modified Latent Structure Approach.” American Journal of Sociology
79:1179–1259.
———.–1947b. “Exploratory Latent Structure Analysis Using both Identifiable and Unidentifiable
Models.” Biometrika 61:215–31.
• Collections:
Clausen, John A. 1972. “The Life Course of Individuals.” Pp. 457–514 in Aging and Society, vol. 3, A
Sociology of Age Stratification, edited by M. W. Riley, M. Johnson, and A. Foner. New York: Russell
Sage.
• Dissertations:
Charles, Maria. 1990. “Occupational Sex Segregation: A Log-Linear Analysis of Patterns in 25 Industrial
Countries.” Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Sociology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
• Machine-Readable Data Files:
American Institute of Public Opinion. 1976. Gallup Public Opinion Poll #965 [MRDF]. Princeton, NJ:
American Institute of Public Opinion [producer]. New Haven, CT: Roper Public Opinion Research
Center, Yale University [distributor].
Miller, Warren, Arthur Miller, and Gerald Klein. 1975. The CPS 1974 American National Election Study
[MRDF]. Ann Arbor, MI: Center for Political Studies, University of Michigan [producer]. Ann Arbor,
MI: Interuniversity Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor].
• Electronic Sources:
American Sociological Association. 1997. “Call for Help: Social Science Knowledge on Race, Racism,
and Race Relations” (ASA Action Alert, October 15). Washington, DC: American Sociological
Association. Retrieved October 15, 1997 (http://www.asanet.org/racecall.htm).
Kao, Grace and Jennifer Thompson. 2003. “Racial and Ethnic Stratification in Educational Achievement
and Attainment.” Annual Review of Sociology 29:417–42. Retrieved October 20, 2003 (http://arjournals.annualreviews.org/doi/abs/10.1146/annurev.soc.29.010202.100019).
6. Number tables consecutively throughout the text. Insert a note in the text to indicate the placement (e.g., “Table
1 about here”). Type each table on a separate page. Each table must include a descriptive title and headings for
columns and rows. Do not use abbreviations for variable names or column and row headings within tables.
Align numbers in columns by decimal. Gather general notes to tables as “Note:”; use a, b, c, and so on, for
table footnotes. Use asterisks *, **, and *** to indicate significance at the p < .05, p < .01, and the p < .001
levels, respectively, and specify one-tailed or two-tailed tests. Do not photo-reduce tables.
7. Number figures consecutively throughout the text. Insert a note in the text to indicate placement (e.g., “Figure
1 about here”). Each figure should include a title or caption. Do not use abbreviations within figures. All artwork must be submitted on diskette or as camera-ready art. Figures must be executed by computer or by
graphic artist in black ink on white paper; lettering must be done in pen and ink or typeset; photographs must
be black-and-white on glossy paper. Contact the JHSB office to discuss preferred file formats for computergenerated files.
IMPORTANT: All figures (including all type) must be legible when reduced or enlarged to widths of 2-9/16
inches (one column width) or 5-5/16 inches (full page width).
PERMISSION: The author(s) are responsible for securing permission to reproduce all copyrighted figures or
materials before they are published by JHSB. A copy of the written permission must be included with the manuscript submission.
8. Appendices should be lettered to distinguish them from numbered tables and figures. Include a descriptive title
for each appendix (e.g., “APPENDIX A. Variable Names and Definitions”).
iii
The JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR (ISSN 0022-1465, formerly the Journal of Health
and Human Behavior) is published quarterly in March, June, September, and December by the American
Sociological Association, 1430 K Street NW, Suite 600, Washington, DC 20005. JHSB is typeset by
Marczak Business Services, Inc., Albany, New York and is printed by Boyd Printing Company, Albany,
New York. Periodicals postage paid at Washington, DC, and additional mailing offices. Postmaster: Send
address changes to the Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 1430 K Street NW, Suite 600, Washington,
DC 20005.
Editorial Scope
The Journal of Health and Social Behavior publishes articles that apply sociological concepts and methods to the understanding of health and illness and to the organization of medicine and health care. Its editorial policy favors those manuscripts that build and test knowledge in medical sociology, that show stimulating scholarship and clarity of expression, and that, taken together, reflect the breadth of interests of its
readership.
Manuscript Submission
Manuscript format: Manuscripts should meet the format guidelines specified in the Notice to
Contributors, which is published in the March and September issue of each volume. Electronic files
(excluding artwork) must be in Microsoft Word or Excel format. All text must be printed double-spaced
on 8-1/2-by-11-inch white paper. Use Times New Roman font, 12-point type size. Margins must be at least
1 inch on all four sides. On the title page, note the manuscript’s total word count (include all text, references, and notes; do not include word counts for tables or figures). If you cite your own work, do not use
wording that identifies you as the author.
Submission requirements: Submit one (1) print copy of the original manuscript and one (1) print copy of
a blinded manuscript from which all identifying information has been removed. Also submit one (1) electronic copy of the original manuscript and one (1) electronic copy of the blinded manuscript on CD or via
e-mail. Enclose a $25 manuscript processing fee in the form of a check or money order payable to the
American Sociological Association. The fee must be paid in order for the review process to begin. The fee
is waived for student members of the ASA. This reflects a policy of the ASA Council and Committee on
Publications affecting all ASA journals. Provide e-mail address, and JHSB will acknowledge the receipt of
your manuscript. Manuscripts are not returned after review.
Address for manuscript submission: Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Indiana University, Karl F.
Schuessler Institute for Social Research, 1022 East Third St., Bloomington, IN 47405-7103; phone (812)
856-6979; e-mail
[email protected].
Editorial decisions: Median time between submission and decision is approximately 12 weeks.
Advertisements, Subscriptions, and Address Changes
Advertisements: Submit to Publications Department, American Sociological Association, 1430 K Street
NW, Suite 600, Washington, DC 20005; phone: (202) 383-9005 ext. 303; e-mail:
[email protected].
Subscriptions: ASA members, $35; ASA student members, $25; institutions, $155. Add $20 for postage
outside the United States and Canada. New subscriptions will be entered on a calendar year basis only. To
subscribe to JHSB or to request single issues, contact the ASA Customer Service Department; phone: (202)
383-9005 ext. 389; e-mail:
[email protected].
Address Changes: Subscribers must notify the ASA Executive Office (e-mail:
[email protected]) six
weeks in advance of an address change. Include both old and new addresses. Claims for undelivered copies
must be made within the month following the regular month of publication. When the reserve stock permits, the ASA will replace copies of JHSB that are lost because of an address change.
Copyright © 2009, American Sociological Association. Copying beyond fair use: Copies of articles in this
journal may be made for teaching and research purposes free of charge and without securing permission,
as permitted by Sections 107 and 108 of the United States Copyright Law. For all other purposes, permission must be obtained from the publisher.
The American Sociological Association acknowledges with appreciation the facilities and assistance provided by Indiana University.
Cover design by Michael Mabry Design.
ERRATUM
In the December 2008 issue of JHSB, the publication date is erroneously listed on the
spine as September 2008. The volume number, however, is correctly stated. If you would
like a label with the correct publication date, please write to
[email protected].
Mothers’ Community Participation
and Child Health*
JENNA NOBLES
University of California–Berkeley
University of California–San Francisco
ELIZABETH FRANKENBERG
Duke University
Journal of Health and Social Behavior 2009, Vol 50 (March): 16–30
We use rich data from the Indonesia Family Life Survey to assess the relationship between mothers’ access to social capital via participation in community
activities and their children’s health. We exploit the advantages of longitudinal
data and community fixed effects to mitigate some of the concerns about spuriousness and reverse causality that predominate in this literature. We find that
children from families with relatively low levels of human and financial capital
fare better with respect to health status when their mothers are more active participants in community organizations. In fact, the association between maternal
participation and child health is strong and positive only for children from relatively disadvantaged backgrounds, as measured by their mothers’ educational
and household economic resources. The results suggest that in poorer settings
community involvement may benefit disadvantaged families, possibly by providing resources and information that would otherwise be inaccessible.
The costs and benefits of social engagement
have long been of interest to social scientists.
Whether social connectedness results in the accrual of social capital that in turn produces
positive or negative outcomes for those who
hold it, relative to those who do not, remains
debated. In the sociological literature the ques* The authors gratefully acknowledge support from
the National Science Foundation (to Nobles) and the
National Institute for Child Health and Development
(grant HD040384 to Frankenberg). Earlier versions
of this article were presented at the Population
Association of America and the American Sociology
Association meetings. The authors would like to
thank Duncan Thomas, Megan Sweeney, Bill
Mason, Will Dow, and Anne Pebley for their helpful
comments on earlier versions of this work. Any
opinions expressed or errors found are those of the
authors. Address correspondence to Jenna Nobles,
Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health and
Society Scholar Program, 3333 California Street,
Suite 465, Campus Box 0844, San Francisco, CA
94118 (e-mail:
[email protected]).
tion is often posed in terms of whether, and to
what extent, individuals benefit from ties to
groups. We investigate this question, asking
whether women’s links to others in their community translate into benefits with respect to
children’s health outcomes.
Previous studies link social capital to a number of measures of health and well-being in the
developed world (for reviews, see Macinko and
Starfield 2001; Almedom 2005). Yet, given that
social capital is theorized to substitute for other forms of capital, it may be a particularly relevant concept in environments lacking well-developed infrastructure and access to education,
such as rural areas, poorer communities, and
developing countries (Kunitz 2004). Very little
work has examined the link between social
capital and health in resource-poor settings.
Accordingly, the setting for our study is
Indonesia. By focusing on a developing country, we provide a contrast with the far more extensive work on social capital and health that
draws on data from the United States and
16
MOTHERS’ COMMUNITY PARTICIPATION AND CHILD HEALTH
Western Europe. Indonesia is also an interesting context of study because many regions of
the country boast a long-standing indigenous
tradition of community involvement, which
the government has tried to harness as a means
of promoting its development objectives.
Relatively little research, however, has examined the implications of these dynamics for
well-being.
Following one strand of the literature, we
conceptualize social capital as a collective
property that is embedded in networks, but one
that individual members of the collectivity may
access differentially depending on the extent to
which they participate in community organizations. Intuitively, a positive, causal relationship
between mothers’ community participation and
children’s health could arise through several
mechanisms. By interacting with other parents,
mothers may acquire information that helps
them raise children to be healthier. Mothers
may also develop connections with women
who can help provide care for the child, help
them navigate the process of obtaining formal
medical care, help them interpret a health care
provider’s advice or even the instructions on
medicine when the child is ill.
We use data from the Indonesia Family Life
Survey. This survey has several features that
support a more sophisticated methodological
approach than has typically been feasible in
empirical literature. Because the survey is longitudinal, we are able to consider health outcomes as a function of maternal participation
measured at an earlier point in time, thus establishing a temporal ordering to the relationship. Because the survey includes physical
health assessments, we are able to measure
health status using children’s height, an indicator of nutrition and illness that is widely accepted as meaningful. The data also allow us to
control for potentially confounding factors
such as children’s health at birth. Finally, we
are able to control for a number of features of
the community that could be correlated both
with maternal participation and with child
health outcomes.
SOCIAL CAPITAL AND ITS LINK TO
HEALTH OUTCOMES
The concept of social capital originates in
the work of Durkheim, and in particular the
idea that individuals are embedded in a normative structure that regulates social life.
Coleman (1988) played a central role in devel-
17
oping the theory of social capital, arguing that
norms of reciprocity help establish and maintain connections between individuals who can
be called on for support, but simultaneously
create an obligation to respond to others
(Coleman 1988; Furstenberg 2005). Much of
Coleman’s work centers on whether parents use
the social capital embedded in community
structures for the benefit of their children,
which is the question that we focus upon.
To guide this analysis, we draw on a social
capital framework developed by Lin (2001),
which starts with the proposition that individuals invest in social relations and expect that
these investments will generate returns. The
idea that individuals can use their social connections to enhance status attainment has received widespread attention over the past two
decades. Studies at both the micro and macro
levels have attempted to link social capital and
a diverse array of outcomes. The rapid expansion of empirical work has been accompanied
by well-founded concerns that the concept has
become so broad and all-encompassing as to
lose any utility as a measurable construct
(Portes 1998).
In the interest of laying out a more rigorous
approach to research on social capital, Lin
(2001) formalizes his definition as “investment
in social relations by individuals through
which they gain access to embedded resources
to enhance expected returns of instrumental or
expressive actions” (p. 17). This definition facilitates developing a strategy for empirical
analysis, because it clarifies the processes underlying the basic concept. Specifically, investments in social capital generate network resources that individuals can access in order to
claim returns of some sort.
One of the criticisms of empirical work on
social capital contends that in some analyses
the specified cause and its effect are so similar
that a strong relationship between the two of
them is tautological (for example, see the discussion of Putnam’s work in Portes 1998:
20–21). To be credible, a postulated causal link
between investment in social capital and the return on that investment must involve investments and returns that are separate and distinct
entities rather than narrowly different indicators of the same underlying phenomenon
(Portes 1998; Furstenberg 2005). From an empirical perspective then, it is desirable to relate
individual-level measures of accessing community-level social capital to conceptually dis-
18
JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
tinct individual-level measures of outcomes
that are plausibly linked to the possession of
such capital (Portes 1998).
Why might access to social capital affect
health? Several causal mechanisms are thought
to link the two endowments. For example,
many social scientists argue that health is a
form of human capital produced at the individual or household level (Grossman 1972; Bolin
et al. 2003). The theoretical underpinning of
such models is that health is produced in part
by choices, such as those related to nutrition,
physical activity, and the use of medical care.
Social networks present the possibility of access to health-related information and sanctioning health-related behavior, and so potentially influence lifestyle choices. In periods of
difficulty, networks may provide the resources
necessary to seek expensive medical treatments or implement practitioners’ advice.
Beyond influencing one’s own health, social
capital may well influence the health of other
family members, including children. Where
health, and particularly child health, is concerned, the strength of a social tie does not
need to be remarkably strong to result in the
sharing of relevant information. Unlike information related to income generation, wherein
competitors may risk personal loss by sharing,
no substantial incentive exists to protect information related to children’s health. In fact, given the transmission mechanisms of communicable disease, parents may actually benefit
from good health among the children with
whom their own children interact.
The role that social capital is posited to play
in producing better health for children is qualitatively similar to the mechanisms hypothesized to link maternal education and child
health in developing settings. Research suggests that maternal education influences child
health via three pathways: knowledge about
health acquired in school helps parents in raising healthy children; literacy helps parents to
correctly diagnose and treat ailments; and exposure to “modern” society through school encourages parents to embrace medical methods
of treatment for their own families (Glewwe
1999). In a setting such as Indonesia, where the
average adult female has only an elementary
education, social interaction likely provides
mothers with information they have not obtained through schooling. With respect to child
health, this may include information ranging
from the benefits of oral rehydration therapy to
the location of preventive care providers.
Indeed, a large body of network research underscores the importance of these types of social ties as a key mechanism of ideational
change (Behrman, Kohler, and Watkins 2002).
Other research finds evidence of this type of
information exchange among participants in
community programs. For example, Barber et
al. (2002) use rich qualitative data from Nepal
to document how participation in voluntary
community associations provides families with
social support, and with economic and education benefits, while also providing a vehicle for
change in fertility limiting behavior.
The theoretical link between social capital
and health is supported by several decades of
epidemiological research concluding that social connections are of key importance to
health. This research documents the association between the presence of individual social
networks and mortality, the ability to rebound
after serious illness, and mental health status
overall (Seeman 1996; Lindau et al. 2003;
Kunitz 2004). With a few notable exceptions
(Yip et al. 2007), the vast majority of this work
is set in developed, resource-rich settings.
Related studies document a significant association between various neighborhood-level
measures of social capital and health. Perhaps
the most well-known are those using data from
the Project on Human Development in Chicago
Neighborhoods. For example, Morenoff (2003)
uses these data to create neighborhood measures of reciprocated exchange among community members and voluntary participation in
local groups. Results suggest that these measures are significantly and positively associated with birth weight of children in the neighborhoods.
Several studies find evidence of links between individual/familial access to community
social capital and child health. Carter and
Maluccio (2003) use height-for-age data to
measure family coping in South Africa. The
authors find that the presence of community
ties significantly boosts a household’s ability to
manage economic shocks to the extent that adequate nutrition can be provided to children.
Kana’iaupuni and colleagues (2005) assess the
relationship between maternal kin ties and
mothers’ reports of children’s general health
status in a region of northern Mexico. The authors find evidence that the frequency with
which mothers interact with their kin is posi-
MOTHERS’ COMMUNITY PARTICIPATION AND CHILD HEALTH
tively associated with the probability that
mothers report their children are in good
health. Surkan and colleagues (2007) examine
the correlates of very young children’s growth
in a city in Brazil. Children of mothers who report having more friends and family, who engage in leisure activities with others, and who
report having more affectionate support have
higher weight-for-height scores than do children of women who have fewer social ties and
less support.
Much of the previous research on social capital has produced interesting and informative
results, but in only a few cases can one conclude that participation or community social
capital causes better child health. A commonly
cited concern with existing work on social capital is the inability to consider potential alternative explanations because data are either limited to one point in time or do not include
enough information to test competing hypotheses (discussed in Macinko and Starfield 2001).
An inherent difficulty is that an individual’s
time or monetary investments that provide access to social capital represent a choice, just as
many of the behaviors that contribute to health
are choices. The characteristics that influence
investments in activities that promote social
capital are also likely to influence health (or the
health of one’s children), and failure to take
those characteristics into account produces biased estimates of the relationship between social capital and health outcomes.
By using rich longitudinal data that allow us
to address concerns of temporal ordering, to
test a number of competing hypotheses, and to
hold constant all observed and unobserved
time-invariant features of communities that
may affect both mothers’ social engagement
and children’s health, we are able to sidestep
many of these potential threats to causal inference. Because this approach in part reflects aspects of the Indonesia setting, we turn to a discussion of context and then describe our data
and methods in detail.
THE INDONESIAN CONTEXT
Over the last 40 years, Indonesia has experienced formidable economic growth and social
change. From 1965 to 1997, the annual GDP
increased at an average of over 5 percent a year,
while the proportion of women aged 15 to 19
with no formal education fell from one-third to
nearly zero (Central Bureau of Statistics 1998).
Demographic change in the form of falling
19
levels of both fertility and infant mortality has
been equally substantial.
A hallmark of socioeconomic development
in Indonesia has been the involvement of local
organizations that draw on the time and energy
of local community volunteers. In many instances these organizations began as grassroots
initiatives and were subsequently adopted by
higher levels of government as regional or national programs.1 In fact, Indonesia is often cited as a success story by donor organizations
for the development of such programs
(Shiffman 2002; World Bank 2003). The goals
of the various community programs differ, but
include improving health care, education, sanitation, security, and village upkeep (Wibisana,
Trihono, and Nurwati 1999).
The emphasis on community engagement is
often traced to the Javanese concepts of gotong
royong, or “mutual assistance,” and rukun,
“communal harmony.” Anthropologists describe gotong royong and rukun as genuinely
indigenous concepts of moral obligation, generalized reciprocity, and community solidarity,
but they note that the state has harnessed the
concepts as a means of mobilizing village labor (Bowen 1986).
Grootaert (1999) examines community programs in detail in three Indonesian provinces.
The results support the idea that participation
in social organizations in Indonesia constitutes
serious and meaningful involvement. For the
types of groups we consider in this study,
Grootaert finds that meetings are relatively frequent, individuals have an active role in decision-making, and most respondents consider
participation “very important” to the household.
Programs that involve active participation
on the part of community members are found
across the country; indeed, at least one type of
volunteer program existed in every one of the
309 communities in the 1997 Indonesia Family
Life Survey (these data are described in detail
in the subsequent section). In this study, we focus on mothers’ participation in five specific
programs: community meetings, village cooperatives, voluntary labor associations, village
improvement projects, and the women’s association. Table 1 draws on the data and presents
descriptive statistics of mothers’ volunteer participation. Thirty-seven percent of mothers report participating in at least one program in the
year prior to interview. Among those who participated, about one-third were involved in
20
JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
TABLE 1. Community Involvement in 1997 among Mothers of Children Age 0–10 years in 2000
(N = 3,281)
Community Programs
Participation in any program
Number of programs (if > 0)
Participation, by program type1
—Community Meetings
—Cooperatives
—Women’s Association
—Village Improvement
—
1
Not mutually exclusive categories
Source: Indonesian Family Life Survey
multiple programs. With respect to the type of
activity in which one is involved, participation is
highest for community meetings, the women’s
association, and voluntary labor.
It is important to note that none of these five
programs is specifically geared toward improving children’s health; this feature is essential to the interpretation of our results. If the
programs in which mothers participate target
child health, a positive association between
participation and children’s health would likely reveal the effect of the programs, and not
necessarily the social capital generated by participating. Some of the above programs may indirectly improve conditions in the village, such
as sanitation or transportation. Indeed, Miller
and colleagues (2006) find a significant and
positive community-level association between
the number of programs in a village and several measures of adult well-being. Similarly, we
would expect that these improvements would
affect all children in a village (as opposed to
just those of participating parents),2 and, as we
describe in the next section, our methodological approach addresses such potentially confounding across-community heterogeneity.
DATA AND METHODS
The Indonesia Family Life Survey is an ongoing longitudinal survey that began in
Indonesia in 1993. We use data from the 1997
and 2000 waves (waves 2 and 3). The survey
sampling scheme stratifies on province and urban/rural location, selecting a total of 321 enumeration areas from 13 provinces, which represent about 83 percent of Indonesia’s 1993
population. Households, defined as a group of
people who reside together and “eat from the
same cooking pot,” were randomly selected
Percentage Participating
37%
1
2
3 or more
65%
23%
12%
15%
03%
16%
06%
15%
from within the enumeration areas. The first
wave was fielded in 1993 and included interviews with over 7,200 households. Both the
second and third waves, fielded in 1997 and
2000, successfully re-interviewed over 94 percent of households in the original sample
(Frankenberg and Thomas 2000; Strauss et al.
2004).
For this analysis, we restrict the sample to
the children who, as of the 2000 survey, were
age 10 and younger and were living with their
mothers. This study measures children’s nutritional status in 2000 as a function of maternal
community participation and other covariates
in 1997. As such, the sample is restricted to the
5,144 children whose height was measured in
2000 and whose 3,281 mothers provided data
in the 1997 wave.
Interpretation of the literature relating social
capital to health is complicated by the difficulty of establishing a causal relationship between
the two, which arises largely from concerns
that participation is endogenous. With respect
to maternal participation and child health, we
envision three potential sources of bias that
complicate establishing a causal relationship
between maternal participation and child
height-for-age.
One possibility is that mothers with children
who are not thriving may be unable to participate in community programs because of the
time required to care for the children. In this
case, reverse causality is a potential issue: children’s poor health status causes mothers’ participation rates to be relatively low, rather than
the reverse. We help address this concern by
exploiting the advantages of longitudinal data
for establishing temporal order. Specifically,
we measure children’s health outcomes at a lat-
MOTHERS’ COMMUNITY PARTICIPATION AND CHILD HEALTH
er point in time (in 2000) than we measure maternal participation (1997). Furthermore, we
include birth weight as a control measure that
is predictive of chronic illness in children
(Conley, Strully, and Bennett 2003).
A second source of bias arises if there are
characteristics of women that affect both their
participation and their children’s health, and
are unaccounted for in the analysis. For example, some evidence suggests that people who
participate in voluntary community programs
are advantaged with respect to socioeconomic
status (Schady 2001; Thoits and Hewitt 2001).
If we fail to control for these factors and they
are also positively related to child health, as is
almost certainly the case, regression results
will misstate the contribution of social capital.
To address this issue, we identify factors related to mothers’ participation and control for
these in the regressions relating participation to
child health. We begin the analysis by modeling the relationship between socioeconomic,
demographic, and health characteristics and
mothers’ participation. In these models, both
covariate and outcome data (participation) are
from the 1997 wave. These models also consider community-level measures of expenditures and urbanicity.
Next, we estimate the relationship between
maternal participation measures in 1997 and
child health outcomes in 2000, while controlling for the characteristics in 1997 that are related to maternal participation. We hypothesize
that, net of other controls for socioeconomic
status, the relationship between maternal participation and child height-for-age will be positive.
The third source of coefficient bias arises if
unmeasured community characteristics are
correlated with both mothers’ participation and
child health. For example, perhaps more advantaged communities simply have more participation opportunities and better population
health. For example, it is possible that some
communities have particularly effective leaders who simultaneously succeed in establishing
hygienic practices with respect to disposal of
sewage and also succeed in promoting
women’s participation in community activities.
From another angle, consider that a high level
of women’s participation at the community level may also result in improvements to community infrastructure. If this were the case, a positive association between mothers’ participation and child health could simply reflect the
21
effects of the programs as opposed to the social
capital generated through participation. To address these issues, we estimate specifications
that include community fixed-effects. These
models, which can be interpreted as including
a dummy variable for each community, provide
an assessment of the relationship between maternal participation and child health outcomes
within communities, or net of time-invariant
features of the community that may affect both
characteristics.
Theorists posit that social capital is a potentially more important resource for those whose
human and financial capital is limited (Kunitz
2004). Coleman (1988), in fact, argues explicitly for an analytical strategy that includes “an
interaction between human capital (parents’
education) and social capital” (1988:S110).
For this reason, we also test for interactions between maternal participation and measures of
household capital: maternal education and
household expenditures.
MEASURES
Table 2 presents summary statistics for the
key measures used in our analysis. We describe
each of these measures in more detail in the
following discussion.
Child Height-for-Age
The Indonesia Family Life Survey collects
data on height measured directly by trained anthropometrists for all household members. For
children under two years of age, height is measured while the child is recumbent. Because
height varies systematically with age and gender, capturing variation in these measures within a population is facilitated by standardizing
against the median values for children of the
same age and sex from a well-nourished population. Thus we assess respondents’ height relative to sex- and age-specific height medians
of children in the United States, using data
available from the National Center for Health
Statistics (2000). For each child, a z score is
computed that expresses the child’s height-forage as the number of standard deviations that
the child is above or below the median for a
child of that sex and age in the United States.3
As most Indonesian children are shorter than
U.S. children, the median z score for Indonesian children is negative. The median z score
is –1.57 for females and –1.73 for males.
22
JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
TABLE 2 Summary Statistics for Key Variables
Mean
S.D.
Children’s indicators
—Height-for-age z score (2000) (Females)
—Height-for-age z score (2000) (Males)
—Age in Years (2000)
—Birth weight in kilograms
—Percentage of children male
—Percentage of children not weighed at birth
–1.60
–1.70
5.7
3.2
51%
33.8%
1.1
1.2
3.1
.58
—Number of children
5,144
Parental and household indicators (measured in 1997)
—Mother’s age in years
—Mother’s educational attainment in years
—Mother’s height in centimeters
—Mother reports being in average or poor health
—Number of children under 15 per mother
—Mother’s number of adult siblings in her village
—Mother interacts with her mother at least once a week
—Monthly per capita household expenditures
—Household in urban location
—Percentage of mothers who moved between 1993 and 1997
—Number of mothers
Community indicators (measured in 1997)
—Mean per capita household expenditures
—Number of community programs
—Number of communities
Source: Indonesian Family Life Survey
Community Activities and Maternal
Participation
Our fundamental question concerns the relationship between children’s health and whether
women choose to participate in community activities; theoretically it is the choice to participate that gives mothers access to the resources
that reside in network ties. Accordingly, we
consider mothers’ participation in key community programs: community meetings, cooperatives, voluntary labor, village improvement activities, and the village women’s association.
The Indonesia Family Life Survey asks women
whether they participated in these programs in
the 12 months prior to the interview. We measure participation with (1) a dichotomous variable indicating participation in any program,
and (2) a continuous variable that takes on a
value between zero and five, measuring the
number of programs in which mothers participate (see Table 1).
Individual and Household Characteristics
We control for individual and household
characteristics, including levels of per capita
monthly household expenditures; maternal ed-
30.2
6.5
150.3
28.2%
2.0
1.3
61%
Rp 141,618
36.0%
9.2%
7.3
4.1
5.2
1.3
1.7
Rp 485,463
3,281
Rp 186,874
6.4
Rp 191,388
2.3
309
ucation, age, and number of children; the
child’s gender and age; and household location
(urban or rural). With the exception of children’s gender and age, these characteristics are
measured in 1997. To measure mothers’ education we construct a dummy variable indicating
completion of six or fewer years of education
(relative to more than six), denoting completed
primary school in the Indonesian schooling
systems. We examine three age groups for
mothers: 15–24 years of age (the reference category), 25–34 years of age, and 35 years and
older.
In this analysis, a measure of monthly per
capita household expenditures is used to capture household financial resources. We prefer
expenditures on the grounds that spending levels are likely to more accurately capture levels
of long-term economic resources than income,
which fluctuates seasonally to a greater degree.
The household expenditures variable is logged
to correct for a skewed distribution. The mean
level of monthly expenditures is Rupiah
141,618, which converts to about $50 per
month in 1997.
MOTHERS’ COMMUNITY PARTICIPATION AND CHILD HEALTH
We include measures of maternal kin support to address the issue that women who are
more active in their communities may have better kin ties, and that these kin ties potentially
influence the health of their children. We measure the number of adult siblings women have
living in their villages and create a dichotomous indicator of whether women report having frequent person-to-person contact with
their own mothers in 1997.
This analysis also controls for whether the
child’s mother arrived in the community relatively recently, in which case she may not have
neighborhood ties that provide information or
care-giving that would aid in child rearing. We
measure whether the child’s mother moved in
between waves 1 (1993) and 2 (1997). Nine
percent of women did so.
An important determinant of child health is
the health endowment that he or she is born
with as a function of inherited characteristics
and development in utero. To this end, we include controls for maternal height and for the
child’s birth weight. Maternal height captures
many aspects of the mother’s background that
may be related to her children’s health, including the health behaviors and inputs to which
she was exposed as a child, and, to some extent,
genetic predisposition. Birth weight captures
the health condition of the child during the
pregnancy, which has been shown to have a
strong relationship with children’s physical development in many settings (Conley et al.
2003), including Indonesia. Maternal height is
measured in centimeters; children’s birth
weight is measured in kilograms. For about
one-third of the sample, birth weight data are
not available because the child was not
weighed at birth. In Indonesia, many births occur at home with the assistance of a traditional
midwife, in which case the newborns are typically not weighed (Frankenberg and Thomas
2001). Children without data on birth weight
are assigned the mean birth weight, and an additional variable indicating that the value was
missing is included in the estimations.
Finally, we also consider a more contemporaneous measure of mother’s health (than her
height) given previous research suggesting that
women in poor health are less likely to participate in volunteer activities (Thoits and Hewitt
2001), and, intuitively, the possibility that these
women may be more likely to have children
with poor health. All women in the sample report self-rated health, which predicts chronic
23
disease in many settings, including Indonesia
(Frankenberg and Jones 2004). As such, we also include a control for whether mothers are in
the lower end of the distribution of self-rated
health, which in this sample is reporting average or below average health.
RESULTS
Assessing the Correlates of Community
Participation
Before analyzing the relationship between
community involvement and child health, we
explore the extent to which socioeconomic, demographic, and health-related characteristics
of mothers are associated with the choice to
participate in community activities. Table 3
presents the results for a Tobit regression predicting the number of programs in which
mothers participate while addressing lowerlevel censoring at zero.
In Table 3, maternal education, age, and
height are significantly associated with increases in the number of programs in which a
mother participates. Mothers with six or fewer
years of education participate significantly less
than do mothers with more than six years of
schooling. Both age and height are also positively associated with program participation.
Having more children is negatively associated
with program participation, suggesting that
women with more children may face time constraints.
Living in a household with below median
per capita household expenditures decreases
the expected extent of participation in community programs. Controlling for household resources, community wealth is positively associated with maternal participation. Having
moved between 1993 and 1997 significantly
decreases the expected program participation
in 1997 for Indonesian mothers. Participation,
however, does not appear to differ between urban and rural locations.
Overall, the results displayed in Table 3 suggest a strong positive correlation between socioeconomic status and participation in community activities. Accordingly, it is important
to control for many dimensions of socioeconomic status in the regressions of children’s
health status on mothers’ community participation in order to isolate the relationship between
maternal access to community networks and
child health. We do so in the results that follow.
24
JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
TABLE 3. Correlates of Mothers’ Participation in Community Programs: Results from a Tobit
Specification, Indonesian Mothers, 1997 (N = 3,281)
Covariates (1997)
Mother’s education: elementary or less
Mother’s height (cm)
Mother reports having poor or average health
Mother’s number of children
Mother’s number of adult siblings in her village
Mother interacts with her own mother often
Mother’s age: 15–24 years
Number of Community
Programs
–.296
(.087)**
.000
(.007)
–.169
(.098)
.020
(.036)
–.001
(.023)
.093
(.082)
.—
Mother’s age: 25–34 years
Mother’s age: 35 or older
Household below median per capita expenditures
Household moved between 1993–1997
Household in urban area
Mean community per capital expenditures
Constant
Sigma
*p < .05 **; p < .01 (two-tailed tests)
Notes: Controls for province location not shown.
a
Results from a Tobit regression, lower-level censoring at zero. Robust standard errors in brackets.
Source: Indonesian Family Life Survey
Maternal Participation and Children’s
Health
We relate children’s health outcomes to maternal participation in community activities by
estimating fixed-effects regressions of child
height-for-age on a number of individual and
household predictors, including maternal community participation. Table 4 presents the results of four specifications estimated on a sample of children ages 10 and younger in 2000.
All models include community-specific fixedeffects and can thus be interpreted as comparisons among children living within the same
communities, averaged over the 309 communities in our sample. Because many children have
siblings who are also in the sample, the standard errors in these models are calculated using a bootstrap estimator clustered at the level
of the child’s mother, conservatively estimated
with 1,000 repetitions (Efron and Tibshirani
1993).
.397
(.110)**
.525
(.128)**
–.453
(.084)**
–.508
(.137)**
–.089
(.088)
.244
(.079)**
–2.020
(1.171)
1.765
The first specification includes a dichotomous variable measuring whether children’s
mothers participate in any community programs in 1997. The estimated coefficient is
small (.017) and insignificant. The second estimation tests a different parameterization of
participation by asking whether the extent of
mothers’ participation measured by the number
of programs in which she participates is related to her children’s health. The coefficient remains small (.015) and insignificant. Simply
put, these results provide no evidence that, on
average, the health of children benefits from
access to network resources that their mothers
gain by participating in community programs.
However, it may be that participation is more
beneficial for particular subgroups of children.
We now turn to that question.
We introduce interactions between maternal
participation and measures of human and financial capital. We are essentially asking
whether, within communities, the association
MOTHERS’ COMMUNITY PARTICIPATION AND CHILD HEALTH
between maternal participation and child
health varies by indicators of socioeconomic
disadvantage. Model 3 includes an interaction
between mothers’ program participation and
educational attainment, measured as having six
or fewer years of education.4 The interaction
term is positive and statistically significant,
suggesting that the relationship between community participation and children’s health differs within communities by the level of mothers’ education. The first-order term remains insignificant, suggesting that participation is unrelated to children’s height-for-age in families
advantaged with respect to human capital.
Instead, each additional program in which a
child’s mother participates is positively associated with children’s height-for-age only if that
child’s mother is poorly educated. For these
children, each additional program is associated
with having height values that are .052 standard deviations larger, on average (.079 +
–.027 = .052).
It is useful to compare the magnitude of the
coefficient on the participation measure with
the coefficients on well-established predictors
of child height, such as maternal education.
Doing so reveals that the magnitude of the coefficient on participation is sizeable. Although
having a mother who has at most an elementary education is associated with one-fifth of a
standard deviation lower height-for-age values
(compared to having a more educated mother),
each program in which a woman with little education participates reduces the height deficit
associated with low levels of maternal education by about 25 percent. In other words, the
height-for-age difference between children
with less and more educated mothers is .198 if
neither mother participates in community programs. If less-educated mothers participate in
just one community program, the height deficit
of their children is reduced to .146 (.198 – .052
= .146) standard deviations below that of children of well-educated mothers. This result suggests that social capital may mitigate some of
the negative consequences for children’s health
associated with low levels of human capital.
Model 4 tests for an interaction between maternal participation and a measure of whether
the child is from a household in which monthly per capita expenditures are below the median of Indonesian households. The estimates reveal a similar relationship with respect to the
interaction between household resources and
maternal participation. Although children from
25
poor households are at a disadvantage with respect to height, the association between participation and children’s height for age is positive
(.080 – .018 = .062) and significant for individuals from relatively poor households. Each
program in which a mother from a poorer
household participates reduces the initial
height-for-age deficit associated with scarce
resources by nearly 40 percent.
Additional Analyses
Our analysis incorporates a number of
methodological choices designed to reduce the
likelihood that an alternative explanation drives the results presented above. As a further
check that an unmeasured chronic component
of children’s health is not driving mothers’ decision to participate, we restrict our sample to
children born after the 1997 interview and reestimate the models in Table 4 (not shown).
The coefficients on maternal participation
match the direction of those shown for the full
sample and are actually larger in magnitude,
though the considerably smaller sample size
reduces the precision of these estimates.
Nevertheless, the interaction between mother’s
education and community participation is still
significant at the .06 level (p = .057). Our conclusions from these models are substantively
similar: Mothers’ community participation is
positively associated with children’s height only for children whose mothers are less educated and for children who come from poorer
households.
One additional concern that arises when
specifying participation continuously is that
the few women who participate in a large number of programs (e.g., 4 or 5 programs) are actually driving the results. Accordingly, we test
the sensitivity of our results in Table 4 by recoding those women as having participated in
3 programs and re-estimating the results (not
shown). Our findings are nearly identical, further supporting their robustness.
DISCUSSION
We investigate whether social capital produces benefits for those who possess it.
Specifically, we follow recent work that characterizes social capital as an endowment which
exists within communities, but which individuals must access through active social participation. Accordingly, we measure access to social capital as the extent to which a woman participates in local volunteer organizations, and
26
JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
TABLE 4. Height-for-Age as a Function of Maternal Participation, Demographic, and Socioeconomic
Factors, Indonesian Children Age 0 to 10 in 2000 (N = 5,144)
Covariates (1997)
Mother participates in at least one community program
Model 1
.017
(.038)
.—
Number of programs in which mother participates
Model 2
.—
Model 3
.—
Model 4
.—
–.027
(.033)
.079*
(.039)
.—
–.018
(.028)
.—
Number of programs ⫻ Mother’s education elementary or less
.—
.015
(.023)
.—
Number of programs ⫻ Household below median expenditures
.—
.—
Child’s age (years)
Child is male
Child’s birth weight (kg)
Child’s birth weight is missing
Mother’s education (elementary or less)
Mother’s height (cm)
Mother reports poor or average health
Mother’s number of children
Mother’s number of adult siblings living in her village
Mother interacts with her own mother often
Mother’s age: 15–24 years
Mother’s age: 25–34 years
Mother’s age: 35 or older
Household below median per capita expenditures
Household moved between 1993 and 1997
Constant
–.022**
(.006)
–.119**
(.029)
.222**
(.031)
–.095*
(.039)
–.152**
(.041)
.052**
(.003)
–.091
(.063)
–.081**
(.016)
.000
(.010)
–.041
(.037)
.—
–.022**
(.006)
–.119**
(.029)
.222**
(.031)
–.095*
(.039)
–.151**
(.041)
.052**
(.003)
–.090
(.063)
–.081**
(.016)
.000
(.010)
–.042
(.037)
.—
–.022**
(.006)
–.119**
(.029)
.221**
(.031)
–.091*
(.039)
–.198**
(.047)
.052**
(.003)
–.093
(.062)
–.082**
(.016)
.000
(.010)
–.038
(.037)
.—
.080*
(.038)
–.022**
(.006)
–.118**
(.029)
.221**
(.031)
–.092*
(.039)
–.152**
(.041)
.052**
(.003)
–.091
(.062)
–.081**
(.016)
.000
(.010)
–.039
(.037)
.—
.054
(.050)
.208**
(.059)
–.116**
(.039)
.028
(.066)
–9.696**
(.519)
.18
Yes
309
.053
(.050)
.206**
(.059)
–.114**
(.039)
.028
(.066)
–9.700**
(.520)
.18
Yes
309
.056
(.050)
.208**
(.060)
–.115**
(.039)
.027
(.066)
–9.690**
(.519)
.18
Yes
309
.055
(.050)
.207**
(.060)
–.160**
(.046)
.024
(.067)
–9.679**
(.519)
.18
Yes
309
R-squared
Includes community-specific fixed-effect
Number of communities
** p ≤ .01 * p ≤ .05 (two-tailed tests)
Notes: Standard errors (in parentheses) calculated using a bootstrap estimator with 1,000 repetitions.
Source: Indonesian Family Life Survey
we ask whether her participation level influences her children’s health. Considering that
possession of social capital may be particularly relevant in settings where women have less
access to other forms of capital, such as formal
education, we use rich data from Indonesia to
answer this question.
The results of this analysis suggest that the
extent of mothers’ participation in volunteer
community programs is significantly and positively associated with children’s health (as indicated by height-for-age), but only for chil-
dren whose mothers have less education, and
for children from relatively poorer households.
The similarities of the findings across these
two measures of disadvantage increase our
confidence in the robustness of the relationship. Moreover, such a relationship is logical
given the mechanisms of advice, information
sharing, health system navigation, and access
to resources that we believe accrue through
participation. These benefits are likely to be of
more value to disadvantaged women.
MOTHERS’ COMMUNITY PARTICIPATION AND CHILD HEALTH
We have taken a number of steps to address
potential methodological pitfalls in relating a
measure of maternal social capital to child
health. Our measure of health is a physical assessment conducted by a trained health worker, rather than a maternal report that could be
subject to systematic error correlated in some
way with women’s choices to participate.
Moreover, we measure the child’s health status
three years after the measure of participation,
so the choice to participate is unlikely to be driven by short-term illness. To help assess
whether these results are driven by variation in
children’s chronic illness that predates and determines the mother’s decision to participate
(and that is not captured by variation in birth
weight), we re-estimate our models for the subset of children born after women’s participation
behavior is measured. We find no attenuation
of the participation coefficients. We have also
included community fixed-effects to control
for all unobservable features of communities
that might simultaneously generate relatively
high levels of maternal participation and better
child health. Finally, we include controls designed to test a number of potentially competing hypotheses by measuring characteristics
that could drive both a woman’s choice to participate in her community and her child’s
health. These include the number of children
she has, the size of her kin network, her own reported health and health background, as well as
several measures of her socioeconomic background.
The remaining stumbling block to concluding that the association we show between social capital and health is indeed a causal one is
that unmeasured features of women may contribute both to the choice to participate and to
children’s health outcomes. For example, some
research finds that people who participate in
voluntary community programs in the United
States are advantaged with respect to psychological well-being measured by happiness,
self-esteem, and depression (Thoits and Hewitt
2001). If these traits also characterize volunteer participants in Indonesia, they may translate into a woman’s general level of motivation
with respect to positively influencing well-being, whether it is that of her children or her
community.
The results of the interaction effects give us
some leverage against this possibility. If our results are a function of the unmeasured motivation of the mother rather than the benefits of
27
information and resources that come from social capital, it must be the case either that (1)
such motivation simultaneously drives mothers’ participation and other behaviors that promote children’s health only for those of low socioeconomic status across multiple dimensions, or (2) motivation drives the decision to
participate for all mothers, but the only children who benefit from their mothers’ motivation are those from disadvantaged households.
While not impossible, these scenarios are more
complicated than our interpretation, which
posits that in households with little educational and economic capital to draw upon, the social capital created through participation
serves as a substitute. Nevertheless, we cannot
conclusively rule out the possibility that some
unmeasured third variable simultaneously affects both participation and children’s health
outcomes, but only for those at low levels of
education and economic resources.
Another key limitation of this study is lack
of specific measures of trust, social support,
and what community participation means to
women in our sample. The Indonesia Family
Life Survey data are rich in many ways, but
they are not geared toward specifically measuring the features of social networks that matter for well-being. Further, our data are limited
with respect to the characteristics of community members with whom individuals interact.
Our analysis reveals that women who participate in community programs are more likely to
be advantaged; we can conclude, then, that the
disadvantaged mothers in our sample who participate in community activities have the opportunity to interact with mothers who are better educated and wealthier than themselves.
Nevertheless, we are unable to distinguish between the types of social ties made by the
mothers in our sample. Future research would
benefit from greater attention to the characteristics of women’s networks that matter for children’s health and development.
Our study is also limited in focus to the reduced-form relationship between social participation and children’s health. We do not study
the mechanisms that link these two phenomena, and such research may reveal additional
implications for the household. For example, it
is possible that one of the ways social capital
improves children’s well-being is by increasing
a woman’s knowledge and resources to such a
degree that her relative position in the household also shifts. Examining the gendered na-
28
JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
ture of parenting, economic autonomy, and resource allocation to children is beyond the
scope of this specific study; nevertheless, understanding how social capital influences these
processes warrants future attention.
Despite these limitations, our findings have
several important implications. First, the potential benefits of social capital do not appear
to be limited to the developed world; we
demonstrate that social capital is significantly
associated with health benefits for the disadvantaged in a large, resource-constrained country. Second, the types of participation programs
we examine are not limited to Indonesia.
Similar types of community programs exist in
many other developing contexts (Grootaert and
Bastelaer 2004), but very few studies have examined the health implications for families.
Our findings suggest that these programs may
not only improve village infrastructure (as is
traditionally demonstrated), but may also influence the health of disadvantaged children by
promoting social interaction between village
members.
Third, our findings support Coleman’s
(1988) theory that social capital can differentially influence children’s outcomes by the levels of human capital in a family. In fact, that social capital largely provides benefits only for
children in disadvantaged homes suggests that
it may serve as an important mechanism to reduce socioeconomic-based health disparities
that exist even in poorer contexts such as
Indonesia. As we note, each program in which
mothers participate is associated with a substantial decrease in the socioeconomic-based
deficits in height-for-age.
Importantly, the implications of this interaction for inequality actually extend further.
Because height has been shown to causally influence a number of later-life welfare measures, including earnings (see Strauss and
Thomas 1998), the social capital harnessed
through mothers’ participation may be an important mechanism to reduce the intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic inequality altogether. To our knowledge, very little
work has assessed the role of social capital as
a moderator of intergenerational processes in
health and socioeconomic standing. Our findings point to the value of research in this area.
Certainly as future waves of data from the
Indonesia Family Life Survey become available, it will be worthwhile to assess these children as young adults and examine the role that
social capital plays with respect to inequality in
later-life health and economic welfare.
NOTES
1. For example, the village women’s association Pendidikan Kesejahtraan Keluarga
(PKK) was founded in 1967 by a group of
women, guided by the idea that improving
family welfare by providing village women
with improved basic skills builds the foundation for a better society. As participation
in the PKK grew, the association took on
larger projects and is now found in nearly
every Indonesian village (Prawiro 1998).
2. It is possible that a mother might more actively take advantage of a new community
feature that she learned about through participating in the village improvement program or the women’s association. If this issue is driving our findings, we would expect
to see larger coefficients on indicators of
participation in these specific programs. In
estimations of program-specific models
(with the same interactions and control
measures used in Table 4), we observe that
the coefficient magnitude is relatively similar in size across programs. We find no evidence that our results are being driven by
participation in any single program, including those that could be construed as more
likely to have indirect benefits for children’s
health.
3. For more detail on the LMS method of zscore calculation, see Kuczmarski and colleagues (2002), and, in particular, page 7.
4. We also test interactions between a dichotomous indicator of participation and measures of socioeconomic status. These estimations were worse-fitting according to
BIC goodness-of-fit statistics (Raftery
1995) though the substantive conclusions
from these models are the same.
REFERENCES
Almedom, Astier M. 2005. “Social Capital and
Mental Health: An Interdisciplinary Review.”
Social Science & Medicine 61:943–64.
Barber, Jennifer S., Lisa D. Pearce, Indra Chaudhury, and Susan Gurung. 2002. “Voluntary Associations and Fertility Limitation.” Social Forces
80:1369–1401.
Behrman, Jere R., Hans-Peter Kohler, and Susan
Cotts Watkins. 2002. “Social Networks and
Changes in Contraceptive Use over Time: Evidence from a Longitudinal Study in Rural
Kenya.” Demography 39:713–38.
MOTHERS’ COMMUNITY PARTICIPATION AND CHILD HEALTH
Bolin, Kristian, Björn Lindgren, Martin Lindström,
and Paul Nystedt. 2003. “Investments in Social
Capital—Implications of Social Interactions for
the Production of Health.” Social Science &
Medicine 56:2378–90.
Bowen, John R. 1986. “On the Political Construction of Tradition: Gotong Royong in Indonesia.”
The Journal of Asian Studies 45:545–61.
Carter, Michael R. and John Maluccio. 2003.
“Social Capital and Coping with Economic
Shocks: An Analysis of Stunting in South
African Children.” World Development 31:
1147–63.
Central Bureau of Statistics (Indonesia), State Ministry of Population/National Family Planning
Coordinating Board, Ministry of Health, and
Macro International, Inc. 1998. Indonesia
Demographic and Health Survey 1997. Calverton, Maryland: Central Bureau of Statistics and
Macro International, Inc.
Coleman, James S. 1988. “Social Capital in the
Creation of Human Capital.” American Journal
of Sociology 94:S95–S120.
Conley, Dalton, Kate Strully, and Neil G. Bennett.
2003. The Starting Gate: Birth Weight and Life
Chances. Berkeley, CA: University of California
Press.
Efron, Bradley and Robert J. Tibshirani. 1993. An
Introduction to the Bootstrap. New York, NY:
Chapman and Hall.
Frankenberg, Elizabeth and Nathan R. Jones. 2004.
“Self-Rated Health and Mortality: Does the
Relationship Extend to a Low Income Setting?”
Journal of Health and Social Behavior
45:441–52.
Frankenberg, Elizabeth and Duncan Thomas. 2000.
“The Indonesia Family Life Survey (IFLS):
Study Design and Results from Waves 1 and 2.”
DRU–2238/1–NIA/NICHD.
———. 2001. “Women’s Health and Pregnancy
Outcomes: Do Service Programs Make a Difference?” Demography 38:253–65.
Furstenberg, Frank F. 2005. “Banking on Families:
How Families Generate and Distribute Social
Capital.” Journal of Marriage and Family
67:809–21.
Glewwe, Paul. 1999. “Why Does Mother’s Schooling Raise Child Health in Developing Countries?
Evidence from Morocco.” The Journal of
Human Resources 34:124–59.
Grootaert, Christiaan. 1999. “Local Institutions and
Service Delivery in Indonesia.” Local Level
Working Paper No. 5, Social Development
Department. Washington, DC: World Bank.
Grootaert, Christiaan and Thierry von Bastelaer,
eds. 2004. The Role of Social Capital in Development. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Grossman, Michael. 1972. “On the Concept of
Health Capital and the Demand for Health.” The
Journal of Political Economy 80:223–55.
29
Kana’iaupuni, Shawn Malia, Katharine M. Donato,
Theresa Thompson-Colon, and Melissa Stainback. 2005. “Counting on Kin: Social Networks,
Social Support, and Child Health Status.” Social
Forces 83:1137–64.
Kuczmarski Robert J., Cynthia L. Ogden, Shumei S.
Guo, et al. 2002. “2000 CDC Growth Charts for
the United States: Methods and Development.”
Vital Health Statistics Series 11 Number 246,
National Center for Health Statistics,
Hyattsville, Maryland.
Kunitz, Stephen J. 2004. “Social Capital and
Health.” British Medical Bulletin 69:61–73.
Lin, Nan. 2001. “Building a Network Theory of
Social Capital.” Pp. 3–29 in Social Capital: Theory and Research, edited by N. Lin, K. Cook,
and R.S. Burt. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Lindau, Stacy Tessler, Edward O. Laumann, Wendy
Levinson, and Linda J. Waite. 2003. “Synthesis
of Scientific Disciplines in Pursuit of Health.”
Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 46:
S74–S86.
Macinko, James and Barbara Starfield. 2001. “The
Utility of Social Capital Research on Health
Determinants.” The Milbank Quarterly 79:
387–426.
Miller, Douglas L., Richard Scheffler, Suong Lam,
Rhonda Rosenberg, and Agnes Rupp. 2006.
“Social Capital and Health in Indonesia.” World
Development 34:1084–98.
Morenoff, Jeffrey D. 2003. “Neighborhood Mechanisms and the Spatial Dynamics of Birth
Weight.” American Journal of Sociology 108:
976–1017.
National Center for Health Statistics. 2000. “2000
CDC Growth Charts: United States.” Retrieved
November 20, 2004 (http://www.cdc.gov/
GrowthCharts/).
Portes, Alejandro. 1998. “Social Capital: Its Origins
and Applications in Modern Sociology.” Annual
Review of Sociology 24:1–24.
Prawiro, Radius. 1998. Indonesia’s Struggle for
Economic Development. Kuala Lumpur,
Malaysia: Oxford University Press.
Raftery, Adrian E. 1995. “Bayesian Model Selection
in Social Research.” Sociological Methodology
25: 111–63.
Schady, Norbert. 2001. “Who Participates? The
Supply of Volunteer Labor and the Distribution
of Government Programs in Rural Peru.” Policy
Research Working Paper No. 2671, World Bank,
Washington D.C.
Seeman, Teresa E. 1996. “Social Ties and Health:
The Benefits of Social Integration.” Annals of
Epidemiology 6:442–51.
Shiffman, Jeremy. 2002. “The Construction of
Community Participation: Village Family Planning Groups and the Indonesian State.” Social
Science & Medicine 54:1199–1214.
Strauss, John, Kathleen Beegle, Bondan Sikoki,
Agus Dwiyanto, Yulia Herawati, and Firman
30
JOURNAL OF HEALTH AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Witoelar. 2004. “The Third Wave of the Indonesia Family Life Survey (IFLS): Overview and
Field Report.” WR–144/1–NIA/NICHD.
Strauss, John and Duncan Thomas. 1998. “Health,
Nutrition, and Economic Development.” Journal
of Economic Literature 36:766–817.
Surkan, Pamela J., Louise M. Ryan, Lina M. Carvalho Vieira, Lisa F. Berkman, and Karen E.
Peterson. 2007. “Maternal Social and Psychological Conditions and Physical Growth in LowIncome Children in Piaui, Northeast Brazil”
Social Science & Medicine 64:375–88.
Thoits, Peggy A. and Lyndi N. Hewitt. 2001. “Volunteer Work and Well-Being.” Journal of Health
and Social Behavior 42:115–31.
Wibisana, W., Trihono and S. Nurwati. 1999. “Community Participation in Health Development:
Indonesian Experience.” Jakarta, Indonesia:
Ministry of Health, Republic of Indonesia.
World Bank. 2003. World Development Report
2004: Making Services Work for Poor People.
Washington, DC: World Bank and Oxford University Press.
Yip, Winnie, S.V. Subramanian, Andrew D. Mitchell,
Dominic T.S. Lee, Jian Wang, and Ichiro
Kawachi. 2007. “Does Social Capital Enhance
Health and Well-being? Evidence from Rural
China.” Social Science & Medicine 64:35–49.
Jenna Nobles is a Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Health and Society Scholar at the University of
California, Berkeley and the University of California, San Francisco. Her research interests include the
health and well-being consequences of population change, with a particular focus on migration and family
welfare.
Elizabeth Frankenberg is associate professor of Public Policy and Sociology at the Terry Sanford Institute
of Public Policy at Duke University. Her interests focus on understanding how attributes of families and
communities influence demographic outcomes and other dimensions of well-being in developing countries.