Exceso de Peso en Adolescentes
Exceso de Peso en Adolescentes
Exceso de Peso en Adolescentes
net/publication/341775569
CITATIONS READS
0 410
1 author:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Maria Moreno Padilla on 01 June 2020.
TESIS DOCTORAL
Exceso de peso en adolescentes: influencia del
estrés social en el rendimiento neuropsicológico
y efecto de la visualización de imágenes de
alimentos en la activación cerebral y toma de
riesgos.
PRESENTADA POR:
DIRIGIDA POR:
(Marie Curie)
2
3
ÍNDICE
Datos identificativos...................................................................................................................... 6
Autorización .................................................................................................................................. 7
Agradecimientos ........................................................................................................................... 8
Resumen ...................................................................................................................................... 14
I. INTRODUCCIÓN .......................................................................................................... 20
Capítulo 1. Obesidad ............................................................................................................... 22
1. Definición y datos epidemiológicos ............................................................................ 24
2. Factores predisponentes y relevancia clínica del problema ........................................ 25
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad.. 30
1. Rendimiento neuropsicológico .................................................................................... 32
2. Toma de riesgos e impulsividad .................................................................................. 36
Capítulo 3. Estrés social y obesidad ........................................................................................ 42
1. Estrés social en adolescentes con exceso de peso ....................................................... 44
2. Influencia del estrés social sobre la conducta alimentaria........................................... 46
3. Reactividad psicofisiológica al estrés.......................................................................... 49
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad ............................................................................................... 54
1. Singularidades psicobiológicas del neurodesarrollo de la adolescencia ..................... 56
2. Sistemas cerebrales asociados al comportamiento alimenticio ................................... 58
3. La “adicción” a la comida ........................................................................................... 61
4. Estudios de activación cerebral relacionados con la motivación por la comida ......... 65
II. JUSTIFICACIÓN, OBJETIVOS E HIPÓTESIS ............................................................ 68
Capítulo 5. Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis....................................................... 70
1. Justificación y objetivo principal ................................................................................ 72
2. Objetivos específicos................................................................................................... 73
3. Hipótesis ...................................................................................................................... 75
III. MEMORIA DE TRABAJOS .......................................................................................... 78
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess
weight ...................................................................................................................................... 80
1. Introduction ................................................................................................................. 82
2. Methods ....................................................................................................................... 83
3. Results ......................................................................................................................... 88
4. Discussion ................................................................................................................... 93
5. References ................................................................................................................... 97
4
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in adolescents with excess
weight: associations with autonomic responses. ................................................................... 106
1. Introduction ............................................................................................................... 108
2. Method ...................................................................................................................... 110
3. Results ....................................................................................................................... 114
4. Discussion ................................................................................................................. 118
5. References ................................................................................................................. 125
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight
adolescents: relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger ........................... 132
1. Introduction ............................................................................................................... 134
2. Methods ..................................................................................................................... 137
3. Results ....................................................................................................................... 141
4. Discussion ................................................................................................................. 146
5. References ................................................................................................................. 152
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight:
relationship with subjective craving...................................................................................... 160
1. Introduction ............................................................................................................... 162
2. Methods ..................................................................................................................... 165
3. Results ....................................................................................................................... 170
4. Discussion ................................................................................................................. 174
5. References ................................................................................................................. 181
IV. DISCUSIÓN, CONCLUSIONES Y PERSPECTIVAS FUTURAS............................. 190
Capítulo 10. Discusión .......................................................................................................... 192
1. Implicaciones teóricas ............................................................................................... 196
2. Implicaciones clínicas ............................................................................................... 201
3. Fortalezas y limitaciones ........................................................................................... 205
Capítulo 11. Conclusiones..................................................................................................... 206
Capítulo 12. Perspectivas futuras .......................................................................................... 210
V. DOCTORADO INTERNACIONAL ............................................................................ 214
1. Summary ................................................................................................................... 216
2. Conclusions ............................................................................................................... 219
3. Future perspectives.................................................................................................... 220
VI. REFERENCIAS BIBLIOGRÁFICAS .......................................................................... 222
VII. ANEXOS....................................................................................................................... 250
5
Datos identificativos
Licenciada en Psicología
Máster en Intervención Psicológica en ámbitos clínicos y de la salud
Universidad de Jaén
Departamento de Psicología
Área de Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamiento Psicológico
6
Autorización
Garantizan que:
La Tesis Doctoral titulada: “Exceso de peso en adolescentes: influencia del estrés social
María Moreno Padilla, ha sido elaborada bajo nuestra dirección y reúne las condiciones
7
Agradecimientos
Decía Machado que solo se hace camino al andar, y así es. Yo, hace cuatro años, decidí
escoger y caminar este sendero y, mirando atrás, contemplo mis huellas y pienso en lo
mucho que se aprende, al andar, sobre los pasos que volverás y los que no volverás a
atrás en el camino, aprender y seguir adelante. Al final, como decía Machado, son
Y el camino que conlleva la realización de una tesis doctoral no es fácil, pero en este
sendero no estás tú solo, siempre hay personas con las que te cruzas y te alegran el
camino, personas que agradeces todos los días que realicen el camino a tu lado,
personas que están apeadas en la acera y que sabes que cuándo las necesites acudirán a
tu llamada para ofrecerte agua, alimento o cualquier cosa que esté en sus manos para
Este apartado de mi tesis doctoral se trata de eso, de dar las gracias a todas las personas
8
Agradecimientos
conocido nunca a una persona más tenaz, inteligente y dedicado a su trabajo. Gracias
por darme esta oportunidad, por dedicarme parte de tu tiempo y ayudarme siempre que
tema de la neuroimagen. Y, por último, a María José, gracias por todo, por ser un
referente para mí, por pensar en mi futuro cuando esto acabe, por sus palabras de apoyo
y ánimo que siempre me consuelan en los momentos de bajón, por querer siempre lo
mejor para mí y por demostrarme que, aunque el camino pueda llegar a ser muy
complicado en algún momento, hay que seguir caminando. Y a María, por ser su luz en
su propio camino.
los medios suficientes para llevar a cabo el desarrollo de las diferentes investigaciones
del Trinity College de Dublín (Irlanda). Y a Marga, Chris y la pequeña Arianna por
acogerme en su casa durante esos tres meses y hacerme sentir como parte de su familia.
También quiero agradecer a Juan Verdejo, por enseñarme todo lo que sé sobre
neuroimagen y por acogerme siempre con una sonrisa en mis primeros años de tesis
doctoral durante mis viajes a Granada. Las cosas hubieran sido mucho más difíciles sin
ti.
9
Agradecimientos
No puedo olvidarme de mis compañeros de despacho y amigos, esos que en cuatro años
han hecho que los días se pasaran más rápido y que siempre estaban ahí cuando
necesitaba apoyo. Gracias a Pablo, José Andrés, Pedro, Gabi, Rocío Donaire, Sonia,
Loida, Carmen, Teresa y Rocío Linares. En especial quiero agradecer a Pablo por estar
conmigo desde el principio del camino, por ayudarme siempre, por pensar siempre en el
bien del otro y porque tus logros me alegran y sé que los míos también te alegran a ti.
No sé dónde nos llevaran nuestros caminos, solo espero que no se separen. A Rocío
Linares por convertirse en una amiga de esas que llegan tarde en el camino pero que ya
se quedan para siempre. Gracias por ser una amiga en la que apoyarse y confiar, por
poder contante mis éxitos y fracasos, por ser una de las personas más buenas que
conozco, además de trabajadora como nadie, te quiero amiga. A Teresa, por su risa que
ilumina el despacho, por sus preguntas “tecnológicas” que siempre me hacen sentir útil
Por supuesto, agradecer también a todos los participantes de los estudios, sin ellos no
hubiera sido posible. Y a mis alumnos del grado de Psicología, espero que ellos hayan
que es la enseñanza.
También le tengo que agradecer a este trabajo el haberme encontrado con María del
y recuerdos. Gracias por escucharme, entenderme y apoyarme siempre. Este trabajo nos
También quiero agradecer a mis amigas de toda la vida, Isabel, Rocío, os quiero y
aunque este trabajo me ha quitado de muchos momentos con vosotras sabéis que
siempre estaremos juntas. También, dar las gracias a Lucía, porque en la carrera se
10
Agradecimientos
conocen personas maravillosas y algunas tienes la suerte de que se quedan contigo para
Por supuesto quiero agradecer a toda mi familia por acompañarme y entenderme todos
estos años. Pero, en especial, quiero dar unas gracias inmensas, tan grandes que no
tendría suficiente espacio en este apartado para hacerlo, a mis padres y hermana. Ellos
son el motor que me ha dado fuerzas para seguir siempre adelante, cuando he flaqueado
me han infundido ánimos, cuando he tropezado con alguna piedra me han ayudado a
que el miedo al fracaso pulula por cada rincón, es fundamental. Sin ellos, que están
Por último, quiero dar las gracias a mi marido. A Juan Antonio, por apoyarme siempre y
aguantar mis malos ratos, por transmitirme su eterna positividad y hacer que un día
malo se convierta en bueno, por todos estos años de felicidad a tu lado, por ser mi luz en
¡Gracias a todos/as!
11
A mis padres Eduardo y Mercedes
A mi marido Juan Antonio
A mis profesores que me inculcaron su amor por la enseñanza
12
13
Resumen
14
15
Resumen
hambre y saciedad, sin embargo, en las sociedades occidentales actuales qué y cuánto
la obesidad.
comparados con adolescentes con peso saludable. Las funciones ejecutivas permiten
alimenticio.
negativas a nivel de salud (diabetes tipo II, mayor probabilidad de desarrollar obesidad
en la edad adulta y sus perjudiciales consecuencias médicas, etc.) sino que también está
16
Resumen
burlas que reciben por parte de sus iguales referidas a su imagen corporal y que, incluso,
pueden llevar a la marginalización y exclusión social. Por tanto, los adolescentes con
exceso de peso sufren mayor estrés social en su día a día. Numerosos estudios señalan
mecanismos.
segundo lugar para explicar el comportamiento alimenticio, siendo los procesos de toma
demuestran que las personas con exceso de peso tienen un sesgo atencional y mayor
Por otra parte, en los últimos años distintas investigaciones subrayan la superposición
utilizan los mismos mecanismos neurales que modulan la motivación para consumir
alimentos, por lo tanto, existe un paralelismo entre los circuitos cerebrales implicados
17
Resumen
general, los resultados de los estudios de neuroimagen realizados hasta ahora señalan
Tomando en consideración todo lo expuesto, los objetivos de esta tesis doctoral fueron:
Para abordar estos objetivos se llevaron a cabo 4 estudios. Los resultados obtenidos
autonómica ante ese estrés, con respecto a los adolescentes con normopeso (estudio 1 y
2); 2) los adolescentes con exceso de peso toman decisiones más arriesgadas tras la
18
Resumen
los adolescentes con normopeso (estudio 3); y 3) se produce mayor activación de áreas
craving informado por los participantes hacia los alimentos presentados en la tarea
(estudio 4).
Estos resultados podrían resultar de enorme utilidad tanto a nivel teórico, contribuyendo
al avance del conocimiento de los factores que están predisponiendo al aumento de peso
19
I. INTRODUCCIÓN
20
21
Capítulo 1
Obesidad
22
23
Capítulo 1. Obesidad
anormal y excesiva de grasa que puede ser perjudicial para la salud del individuo que lo
utilizar el Índice de Masa Corporal (IMC) como medida de estimación del sobrepeso y
cuadrado de la altura, en metros (kg/m2). Para adultos, la OMS definió los umbrales
indicando que un IMC mayor a 25 kg/m2 está asociado a sobrepeso, y un IMC superior
a 30 kg/m2 a obesidad. En la edad adulta, estos puntos de corte están bien establecidos
ya que, parece ser, que en esta etapa el IMC tiene una alta asociación con la grasa
determinación del IMC es más complicada ya que está asociado de forma más indirecta
la niñez y adolescencia se realiza siguiendo las indicaciones del IOTF (Cole, Bellizzi,
Flegal y Dietz, 2000), las cuáles sugieren utilizar valores de IMC ajustados por edad (2-
a sobrepeso, mientras que se considera obesidad si estos valores superan el percentil 95.
últimas décadas en uno de los principales problemas de salud pública a nivel mundial.
24
Capítulo 1. Obesidad
de 20 años, siendo más frecuente en las mujeres que en los hombres. Según este
sobrepeso, de los cuales, más de 600 millones eran obesos, mientras que 41 millones de
Por su parte, la WOF informa que si se mantiene la tendencia actual se calcula que en
2025 cerca de 2.700 millones de adultos tendrán sobrepeso, más de 1.000 millones
ésta. La última evaluación del Instituto Médico Europeo de la Obesidad en 2014 indicó
que, para esa fecha, el 21,1% de los niños españoles presentaban sobrepeso y el 8,2%
presentaban obesidad, con lo que casi uno de cada tres niños de entre 3 y 12 años tenía
detrás de Gran Bretaña, con mayor porcentaje de niños obesos o con sobrepeso entre los
7 y los 11 años. De este modo, la obesidad se ha convertido en una epidemia que afecta
la severidad de esta, resulta de vital importancia conocer cuáles son los factores
25
Capítulo 1. Obesidad
como el ingreso y gasto calórico. Los factores que pueden desencadenar la obesidad,
demostrado que los genes pueden predisponer al sobrepeso (Locke y cols., 2015), este
solo se produce cuando se combinan con otros factores, como los hábitos alimenticios y
puede explicar el rápido aumento de la obesidad en distintos países del mundo. Por otro
patrones de sueño, así como la total disponibilidad que tenemos de alimentos altamente
2015; Ellulu, Abed, Rahmat, Ranneh y Ali, 2014), son variables a tener en cuenta a la
dedicado a la televisión, etc.), pero también a niños y adolescentes, los cuales pasan
smartphones. Esto provoca que el gasto energético sea bajo y que resulte difícil
deshacerse de las calorías consumidas. Por otro lado, el sueño deficiente es cada vez
más común en los niños y las asociaciones entre la corta duración del sueño en la
(Hasler y cols., 2004; Cappuccio y cols., 2008; Miller, Lumeng & LeBourgeois, 2015).
26
Capítulo 1. Obesidad
estados emocionales negativos (estrés, tristeza, enfado, frustración, soledad, etc.) como
que los drásticos cambios producidos en el entorno y el estilo de vida han modificado la
forma en la que percibimos los alimentos y regulamos su ingesta (Zheng, Lenard, Shin
a otras actividades reforzantes (Zheng y cols., 2009). Además, diversos estudios han
encontrado que los alimentos altamente apetitosos (altos en grasas y/o azúcares) activan
regiones del área de recompensa cerebral, al igual que hacen las drogas de abuso, lo que
conlleva que este tipo de alimentos tengan un valor hedónico y reforzante similar,
Wang, Fawler y Telang, 2008; Volow, Wang, Fowler, Tomasi, Baler, 2011; Volkow,
mayor riesgo cardiovascular en la edad adulta y los costes sociales que no pueden ser
directamente estimados (Lobstein, Baur y Uauy, 2004; Baker, Olsen, Sorensen, 2007;
27
Capítulo 1. Obesidad
En concreto, la diabetes tipo 2, conocida hasta hace poco tiempo como diabetes del
diabetes está estrechamente relacionada con el exceso de peso, por lo tanto, la causa de
este aumento parece ser el incesante crecimiento de la obesidad infantil (DeBoer, 2013;
que cada vez se consumen más alimentos ricos en azúcares y harinas refinadas, la
2.
desarrollar problemas de ajuste social en la adolescencia (Puhl y Heuer, 2009), así como
2009). Por tanto, los adolescentes con exceso de peso sufren de más estrés social que
sus compañeros con normopeso. De este modo, la evaluación de los posibles efectos
decisiones de los adolescentes con exceso de peso son de vital importancia a la hora de
tener un conocimiento más certero acerca de las causas que puedan estar desarrollando
profundamente en el capítulo 3.
Por otro lado, diversos estudios y revisiones recientes confirman que el exceso de peso
28
Capítulo 1. Obesidad
obesidad en la edad adulta (Guo, Wu, Chumlea y Roche, 2002; Singh, Mulder, Twisk,
Van Mechelen y Chinapaw, 2008). Según la OMS, un IMC elevado en la edad adulta es
fueron la principal causa de muerte en 2012), diabetes, trastornos del aparato locomotor
(en especial, osteoartritis) y algunos tipos de cánceres. Los costes médicos asociados a
29
Capítulo 2
Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e
impulsividad en la obesidad
30
31
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
1. Rendimiento neuropsicológico
creación del concepto “adicción a la comida” (Volkow, Wang, Fawler y Telang, 2008;
Volow, Wang, Fowler, Tomasi, Baler, 2011; Volkow, Wang, Tomasi y Baler, 2013). En
“top-down” que normalmente regula las respuestas guiadas por las recompensas está
alterado (Acosta, Manubay y Levin, 2008). Esta interacción anormal entre la regulación
trata de habilidades esenciales para nuestro día a día ya que se ponen en marcha en una
lesiones que afectan a la corteza frontal (Stuss y Levine, 2002), lo que ha llevado a
32
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
razón por la cual en la adolescencia estas funciones están más limitadas que en la edad
recompensa que son inapropiadas para las demandas actuales. Se trata de un constructo
conocimiento de forma espontánea para dar una respuesta adaptada a las exigencias
2003). Por un lado, estudios con resonancia magnética funcional (fMRI) han señalado
que los adolescentes, comparados con los adultos, muestran mayor activación del
consecución (Van Leijenhorst y cols., 2009; Ernst y cols., 2005). Por otro lado, los
33
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
resultados de estudios cognitivos que han empleado fMRI, han mostrado que las
habilidades de control ejecutivo y sus sustratos neurales (p.ej. PFC) están todavía
2007; Waber y cols., 2007; Crone, Bullens, van der Plas, Kijkuit y Zelazo, 2008). Este
desequilibrio hace que la adolescencia sea un período durante el cual la actividad del
daños y el autocontrol (Chambers y cols., 2003). Por lo tanto, los sistemas encargados
comparados con adolescentes con peso saludable. Varios estudios (Kamijo, Khan y
cols., 2012; Kamijo, Pontifex, y cols., 2012) han mostrado una correlación negativa del
señaló que los participantes con bajo control inhibitorio a los 7 años tendían a tener un
IMC mayor a los 15 años. Riggs, Huh, Chou, Spruijt-Metz y Pentz (2012) y Riggs,
Spruijt-Metz, Chou y Petz (2012) señalaron que los niños altamente sedentarios que no
mostraban menor control inhibitorio que los niños activos que consumían frutas y
verduras. Asimismo, distintos estudios han señalado que los adolescentes con obesidad
muestran peor control inhibitorio y, por lo tanto, peor rendimiento en tareas go/no go y
estudiado), Riggs, Huh y cols (2012) y Riggs, Spruijt-Metz y cols (2012) también
34
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
altos en grasas y/o azúcares y el rendimiento en este componente. Por otro lado,
adolescentes con obesidad rendían peor en tareas de memoria de trabajo [Wide Range
Assessment of Learning and Memory (WRAML)] que los adolescentes con peso
que los adolescentes con exceso de peso muestran peor rendimiento en este componente
en una variedad de tareas utilizadas (Trail Making Test, Test de Cartas de Wisconsin,
2010; Lokken, Boeka, Austin, Gunstad y Harmon, 2009; Cserjési, Molnár, Luminet y
2012). Por último, Verdejo-García y cols. (2010) utilizaron la Iowa Gambling Task
(IGT) para evaluar toma de decisiones y observaron que los adolescentes con sobrepeso
normopeso.
déficits cognitivos son anteriores al desarrollo de la obesidad (y, por tanto, pudieran
35
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
tratamiento. En nuestra sociedad actual, llena de alimentos altos en grasas y/o azúcares
tanto, los mecanismos homeostáticos han quedado en segundo lugar para explicar el
adolescencia.
Pérez-García, 2010). El modelo que sigue este cuestionario identifica cuatro vías
a pensar y reflexionar sobre las consecuencias de un acto antes de participar en ese acto
36
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
enfocado en una tarea que puede ser larga, aburrida o difícil. Finalmente, la búsqueda de
sensaciones abarca dos aspectos: (a) la tendencia a disfrutar y llevar a cabo actividades
emocionantes y (b) una apertura a probar nuevas experiencias que pueden o no ser
en los jóvenes, como el consumo de sustancias (p. ej. cigarrillos, alcohol), el juego, la
Brewer y Jones, 2007). Además, la impulsividad hace que sea más difícil resistir la
puede contribuir al exceso de peso. Existe cierta evidencia de que las personas con
obesidad son más propensas a ceder ante las tentaciones y son menos efectivas para
inhibir sus impulsos. Estudios con medidas de autoinforme muestran que las personas
con obesidad son más impulsivas que las personas con normopeso y muestran
cols., 2003). Además, se ha encontrado que los niños obesos son menos capaces de
retrasar la gratificación y, más a menudo, eligen una recompensa inmediata sobre una
recompensa retrasada más grande, siendo este un índice de poco auto-control (Bonato y
Boland, 1983; Best et al., 2012). Investigaciones recientes también han demostrado que
los niños y adolescentes con obesidad son menos efectivos en la inhibición de respuesta
en una tarea “stop-signal”, son más sensibles a la recompensa y toman más riesgos en
37
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
comportamiento más impulsivo (Nederkoorn, Braet, Van Eijs, Tanghe y Jansen, 2006;
Thamotharan, Lange, Zale, Huffhines y Fields, 2013; Davis, Patte, Curtis y Reid, 2010).
2013). El exceso de peso también se asocia con la toma de riesgos ya que los
la tarea IGT (Bechara, 2007), una medida usual de toma de riesgos (Boeka y Lokken,
demostrado que los adolescentes con exceso de peso tienen una respuesta estriatal
hipersensible (Cohen y cols., 2010; Galvan y cols., 2006) y una activación aumentada
Una tarea ampliamente utilizada para evaluar toma de riesgos es la Balloon Analogue
Risk Task (BART) (Lejuez y cols., 2002). En esta tarea, los participantes acumulan
dinero en un banco temporal presionando un botón que infla un globo simulado. Cada
38
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
perder todo el dinero acumulado en ese globo. A diferencia de la tarea IGT, tarea muy
utilizada para la evaluación de la toma de decisiones, en la que cada prueba implica una
elección entre una alternativa de riesgo y otra segura (mediante selección de cartas), la
cada vez que se infla el globo). El riesgo en esta tarea está asociado con la ocurrencia de
(Lejuez, Aklin, Zvolensky y cols., 2003), el tabaquismo (Lejuez, Aklin, Jones y cols.,
Por otra parte, la literatura muestra que distintos contextos o elementos como, por
(Field y Eastwood, 2005; Fox y cols., 2005). Asimismo, varios estudios demuestran que
las personas con exceso de peso tienen un sesgo atencional hacia señales de alimentos
39
Capítulo 2. Rendimiento neuropsicológico, toma de riesgos e impulsividad en la obesidad
altos en grasas y/o azúcares (Hou y cols., 2011; Castellanos y cols., 2009). Por lo que
parece razonable esperar que, del mismo modo que en los individuos adictos a
sustancias, los adolescentes con exceso de peso asuman mayores riesgos al encontrarse
40
41
Capítulo 3
Estrés social y obesidad
42
43
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
que también tiene efectos nocivos en el ámbito social y psicológico. Padecer sobrepeso
(Strauss y Pollack, 2003). Numerosos estudios han encontrado que los adolescentes con
poseen menores niveles de autoestima que sus iguales con peso saludable. En este
(1961) ya en la década de 1960 indicaban que los niños con sobrepeso eran
considerados por los otros niños como los amigos menos deseables.
adolescentes. Dadas las normas estrictas de apariencia entre los adolescentes en cuanto
(2003) se comprobó que los adolescentes con exceso de peso sufrían mayor
Hannan, van den Berg y Eisenberg, 2008). Es más probable que los adolescentes con
44
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
verbal, social y física entre los adolescentes aumenta con el IMC (Janssen y cols.,
2006), siendo los adolescentes con mayor nivel de obesidad especialmente vulnerables a
cuarto de los chicos reportan burlas basadas en el peso por sus compañeros, pero esta
prevalencia aumenta hasta aproximadamente el 60% entre los estudiantes con mayor
Heuer (2011) en el que utilizaron las valoraciones de los estudiantes con peso saludable,
los resultados mostraron que los participantes percibían que el sobrepeso y la obesidad
Las personas con sobrepeso y obesas son altamente estigmatizadas en nuestra sociedad,
Puhl y Heuer, 2009). Para los jóvenes que presentan sobrepeso u obesidad, la
intimidación. Estas experiencias pueden ser explícitas (p. ej. burlas verbales, insultos,
violencia física), o pueden tomar formas más sutiles, como victimización relacional (p.
En resumen, los niños con sobrepeso u obesidad son con frecuencia víctimas de burlas,
45
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
adolescentes con exceso de peso. Las burlas basadas en el peso pueden contribuir a
dramática que ocurre entre los adolescentes que sufren marginalización por su peso es el
Crosby, 2005).
perjudiciales a nivel académico también son frecuentes. La evidencia indica que los
adolescentes que sufren de continuas burlas y acoso sufren mayor absentismo escolar, lo
Muchos factores externos pueden influir en la ingesta de alimentos, entre los que se
comida y que depende de las propiedades organolépticas del alimento como, por
ejemplo, su sabor, olor o apariencia (Pliner y Mann, 2004). Además de esto, es una
creencia comúnmente sostenida que el estrés puede alterar los patrones de alimentación
afectado por el estrés social que sufren las personas con sobrepeso y obesidad. Los
46
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
Cambios biológicos:
- Elevado cortisol
- Grasa visceral
MAYOR
Mayor exposición al
RIESGO DE
estrés psicosocial OBESIDAD
Cambios comportamentales:
- Aumento de la
ingesta calórica
Figura 1. Vías a través de las cuales el estrés puede contribuir al desarrollo de la obesidad.
ingesta de alimentos y pierde peso durante o después del estrés, mientras que la mayoría
de las personas aumentan su ingesta durante el estrés (Stone y Brownell, 1994; Epel y
cols., 2004). Teniendo en cuenta que las personas que viven en países occidentalizados
calóricamente densos, tiene sentido que la mayoría de las personas refieran comer más
durante la situación estresante, en lugar de comer menos. Casi el 50% de una muestra
representativa de EE.UU. afirmó estar preocupada por la cantidad de estrés en sus vidas
poco saludables como fumar y comer como forma de aliviarse de esas situaciones
(Stambor, 2006). El deseo -inducido por el estrés- por alimentos altamente calóricos es
47
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
Así mismo, se ha observado que las personas con exceso de peso presentan
manera impulsiva cuando están bajo un estado emocional negativo (ansiedad, depresión,
estrés, etc.). Como hemos comentado, el estrés agudo puede aumentar la ingesta,
Wardle, 1999; Bjorntorp, 2001). Por ejemplo, un estudio con autoinformes señaló que el
estrés (Oliver y Wardle, 1999). En esta misma línea, un estudio de Jääskeläinen y cols.
(2014) encontró que los adolescentes que se dejaban llevar por el estrés a la hora de
comer tenían una mayor prevalencia de obesidad que los que no lo hacían. Otro estudio
señaló que las mujeres que reportaban mayor estrés crónico también reportaban ser
significativa que sugiere efectos potencialmente perjudiciales del estrés en los patrones
de alimentación (p. ej. omitir las comidas, restringir la ingesta, atracones) y las
Nowson, 2007). Distintas investigaciones muestran que los efectos del estrés pueden ser
diferentes en las personas con peso saludable en comparación con las personas con
obesidad (Block, He, Zaslavsky, Ding y Ayanian, 2009; Lemmens, Rutters, Born y
alimentación debida al estrés se exacerba en las personas con obesidad, mientras que la
Los adolescentes con exceso de peso, los cuales hemos comentado que se encuentran
48
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
grasas y/o azúcares como estrategia de afrontamiento a esa situación. Así, las funciones
cognitivas superiores de control ejecutivo podrían estar alteradas tras estas situaciones
Por otro lado, el modelo “Reward Based Stress Eating” (Adam y Epel, 2007) enfatiza el
sociales (p.ej. después de la Trier Social Stress Task, que se basa en un discurso
público) (Rouach y cols., 2007) al igual que el cortisol u “hormona del estrés” que está
Bjorntorp, 2001).
fisiológica similar a la producida por desafíos de carácter físico. Existen tres sistemas
suprarrenal, conocido como “la hormona del estrés” (Sapolsky, Krey y McEwen, 1986;
49
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
más práctica que la recolección de sangre, ya que este método refleja una tendencia a
que es un método no-invasivo, lo que hace que la recogida de muestras múltiple sea
1994).
Por otro lado, además del cortisol, para evaluar la reactividad ante el estrés en una
SNA. Entre ellas, la frecuencia cardíaca (FC) y la actividad electrodermal (AED) han
del estresor, de los recursos psicosociales que posea el sujeto y de los factores
componentes (Steptoe, 1990; Peters y cols., 1998). La FC se considera una variable muy
50
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
Salvador, 2000; Verdejo-García y cols., 2015), tareas aritméticas (Sloan y cols., 1997),
los cambios cardiovasculares más estables en el tiempo (Swain y Suls, 1996). La AED
ha sido uno de los índices psicofisiológicos más empleado como correlato de procesos
sensibilidad (Wieland y mefferd, 1970), por lo que los cambios en la AED pueden
(Hugdahl, 1995). Los niveles basales de AED pueden variar notablemente entre
analizar un mismo sujeto ante una misma situación, la respuesta disminuye con la
realizados por Lazarus (1966), se encontró un aumento de los niveles tónicos de AED
51
Capítulo 3. Estrés y obesidad
utilizado como indicador de estados de estrés (Clemens y Turpin, 2000) y como índice
(Hugdahl, 1995).
un patrón de activación similar o diferente ante un mismo estímulo, por lo que podrían
52
53
Capítulo 4
Cerebro y obesidad
54
55
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
entre compañeros, participación romántica) (Ernst, Pine y Hardin, 2006). Por lo tanto, el
adolescencia de Ernst y cols. (2006), indica que la adolescencia se caracteriza por una
los problemas de exceso de peso, según este modelo, los adolescentes podrían
56
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
maduración del PFC. La relativa maduración del sistema de recompensa conlleva una
recompensa. Estas actividades a su vez favorecen la maduración del PFC, pero suponen
control de impulsos son necesarias. Debido a esto, los adolescentes podrían ser más
vulnerables a la ingesta de alimentos altos en grasas y/o azúcares debido los déficits
nombrados anteriormente.
Por otro lado, Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover y Casey (2007), además de señalar la
podemos decir que los hábitos alimenticios no saludables podrían ser una conducta de
riesgo hacia la que los adolescentes serían más proclives debido al déficit en el control
toma de riesgos.
Por último, desde el modelo del marcador somático de Damasio (2006), se propone que
en las personas que tienden hacia conductas de riesgo como el consumo de sustancias
futuro” que presentan las personas con antecedentes de abuso de sustancias manifestada
57
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
cuenta lo comentado anteriormente, podría explicar por qué los adolescentes con exceso
de peso no son capaces de tener en cuenta las consecuencias perjudiciales que sus
somáticos), lo que les lleva a seguir teniendo una toma de decisiones alterada y
En resumen, estos modelos tienen varios aspectos en común ya que proponen que los
de la ingesta o el apetito (Kimet y cols., 2006). Sin embargo, a nivel cerebral, no solo el
58
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
por las células receptoras del gusto oral que posteriormente se transmite al núcleo del
tracto solitario (NTS) por fibras sensitivas aferentes. Desde el NTS, la información del
gusto se transmite a múltiples áreas del cerebro posterior (p.ej., el núcleo parabraquial),
estriado, el tálamo y la corteza cerebral) (Kelley, Baldo, Pratt y Will, 2005), que
gustativo secundario) (Rolls, 2005). La respuesta de esta última área a los estímulos del
gusto disminuye a medida que los alimentos son ingeridos, lo que implica la capacidad
Adrews, 2003).
neuronas del VTA. Éstas, a su vez, se proyectan al NAcc, estriado y otras áreas
depende de una red cerebral compuesta por diversas regiones, incluyendo el estriado, el
PFC, el cíngulo anterior, la ínsula o las áreas dopaminérgicas del mesencéfalo (Haber y
59
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
Knutson, 2009). Como hemos comentado, existe cierta especialización dentro de este
sistema, por ejemplo, el estriado ventral está más enfocado a la valoración subjetiva que
2009), mientras que la ínsula anterior parece estar más relacionada con la integración de
grasas y/o azúcares, el individuo tiene que ser capaz de controlar la cantidad de
alimento que ingiere. Así, el comportamiento alimenticio ha dejado de ser una cuestión
y la toma de decisiones juegan un papel muy importante, incluso a veces obviando los
apariencia, sabor, textura, olor, etc. (Zheng y Berthoud, 2007). Por lo tanto, ante la
gran saliencia que tiene el estímulo y conllevar así a una mayor predisposición a la
a la comida y los sistemas encargados del control ejecutivo. Como hemos dicho, las
60
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
en la adolescencia de Ernst y cols., 2006. Además, diferentes estudios han expuesto que
3. La “adicción” a la comida
cerebrales que presentan las personas con trastornos adictivos a sustancias y las
personas que tienen obesidad (Volkow y cols., 2013). Las drogas de abuso utilizan los
mismos mecanismos neurales que modulan la motivación para consumir alimentos, por
lo tanto, existe una superposición entre los circuitos cerebrales implicados en la pérdida
Las neuronas dopaminérgicas residen en los núcleos del cerebro medio (VTA, y
61
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
excitación (tálamo) y control cognitivo (PFC y córtex cingulado) a través de una vasta
de alimentos, y, por otro lado, los péptidos que regulan la ingesta de alimentos también
influyen en los efectos reforzantes de las drogas. Sin embargo, y a diferencia de las
drogas de abuso cuyas acciones están desencadenas por sus efectos farmacológicos
Ambos problemas pueden ser definidos como desordenes en los cuáles la saliencia de
cerebral han empezado a delinear algunos de los circuitos cerebrales superpuestos cuyas
disfunciones pueden ser la base de los déficits observados. Los resultados sugieren que
tanto los individuos obesos como los adictos a sustancias de abuso sufren alteraciones
en las vías dopaminérgicas que regulan los sistemas neuronales asociados no solo con la
Sin embargo, existen corrientes contrarias que han relacionado estos paralelismos entre
62
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
gran cantidad de alimentos que se realiza de forma muy rápida, normalmente estando la
culpa y disgusto. Los atracones pueden desencadenarse por estados de ánimo negativos
que no necesariamente mejoran por el atracón (Stein y cols., 2007). Una advertencia
importante es que, aunque el trastorno por atracón o “binge eating disorder (BED)” se
importancia de evitar el uso simple del IMC como un marcador general para el consumo
el concepto “food addiction” a “eating addiction”, el cual sugiere más bien una adicción
2014). Las personas que comen en exceso generalmente no restringen sus dietas a
alimentos apetecibles (altos en grasas y/o azúcares) parece hacer que los sujetos
alimentaria tiene que actuar de manera responsable, dado que el acceso fácil a alimentos
63
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
predispuestas. Además, los autores que apoyan este cambio de concepto postulan que el
que la comida puede contener sustancias químicas que pueden conducir al desarrollo de
Figura 2. Mecanismos de acción de las drogas de abuso y la comida sobre las vías de
recompensa cerebrales.
En contraste con las drogas de abuso cuyas acciones son desencadenadas por sus directos
efectos farmacológicos en el sistema cerebral de recompensa mediado por la dopamina (área
tegmental ventral, núcleo accumbens y pálido ventral), la regulación del comportamiento
alimenticio y, por tanto, las respuestas a la comida están moduladas por múltiples mecanismos
centrales y periféricos que directamente o indirectamente transmiten a las vías de recompensa
cerebrales, incluidos aquellos envueltos en el placer, aversión, habituación y control cognitivo.
PYY: peptide YY; s. intestines: small intestines; SN: substantia nigra (Adaptado de Volkow y
cols., 2013b).
64
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
utilizando para ello distintos tipos de tareas. Los estudios de actividad cerebral se basan
adquisiciones de fMRI (Ogawa y cols., 1993). De forma resumida, esta técnica se basa
cerebrales. Para ello la persona evaluada simplemente debe realizar una determinada
neuroimagen con fMRI. Estos estudios en participantes sanos han mostrado que la
65
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
En cuanto a los resultados en obesidad, los estudios de fMRI indican que los individuos
con obesidad muestran una mayor activación en la ínsula, el opérculo frontal, el OFC, la
(Stice, Spoor, Bohon, Veldhuizen y Small, 2008). Los datos sugieren que la ínsula y el
de Bruce y cols. (2010) encontró que el grupo de adolescentes con obesidad mostraba
significativamente mayor activación que los adolescentes con un peso saludable ante las
córtex dorsolateral (dlPFC) en adolescentes con obesidad debido al aumento del control
específica go/no go de comida, los resultados mostraron que las adolescentes con
paradigma utilizado.
En otro estudio con fMRI (Stice y cols., 2008), en el que se utilizaba un alimento
apetitoso (batido de chocolate) y una solución insípida, las adolescentes con obesidad
66
Capítulo 4. Cerebro y obesidad
mostraron una mayor activación bilateral que las adolescentes con peso normal en la
consumo real de batido (frente a una solución insípida); estas regiones cerebrales
codifican los aspectos sensoriales y hedónicos de los alimentos. Por otro lado, en un
estudio de Yokum, Ng y Stice (2011) con chicas adolescentes, se observó que el IMC
IMC. Los resultados indicaron que el sobrepeso estaba asociado a un mayor sesgo
atencional a las señales de alimentos y que los adolescentes que mostraban mayor
En general, los resultados de los estudios de neuroimagen realizados hasta ahora señalan
67
II. JUSTIFICACIÓN,
OBJETIVOS E
HIPÓTESIS
68
69
Capítulo 5
Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis
70
71
Capítulo 5. Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis
Los cambios producidos en la sociedad actual en los últimos años han modificado la
nuestro comportamiento alimenticio, sino que otros muchos factores están influyendo
en nuestra toma de decisiones a la hora de comer. Estas variables pueden ser la mayor
adulta (Whitaker, Wright, Pepe, Seidel y Dietz, 1997). Aparte de las perjudiciales
consecuencias del exceso de peso a nivel médico (p.ej., aumento de la diabetes tipo II en
niños y adolescentes), los adolescentes con exceso de peso están expuesto a un mayor
nivel de estrés social (p.ej., burlas, discriminación, bullying, etc.). Dado los efectos que
indagar sobre las consecuencias del estrés social en adolescentes con exceso de peso.
72
Capítulo 5. Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis
con la obesidad podrían estar acentuadas en esta etapa debido a las peculiaridades en el
2. Objetivos específicos
73
Capítulo 5. Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis
normopeso.
Nuestro primer objetivo fue estudiar la influencia del estrés social sobre el rendimiento
anteriormente, el estrés social es mucho más frecuente en esta población y además hay
neuropsicológico en personas con obesidad. Por lo tanto, derivado de los resultados que
exposición al estrés social propiciaba una ejecución más pobre en adolescentes con
exceso de peso en tareas que evaluaban distintas funciones cognitivas. Para ello
objetiva del estrés. Este estudio ha sido publicado en la revista Plos One (Verdejo-
social negativa, ya que los adolescentes con exceso de peso están expuestos con mayor
74
Capítulo 5. Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis
(Moreno-Padilla, M., Fernández-Serrano, M. J., Verdejo-García, A., & Reyes del Paso,
distintos estudios que han mostrado que la visualización de señales relacionadas con el
que, dadas las similitudes encontradas entre los sistemas que regulan la adicción a
comida sobre la toma de decisiones en adolescentes con exceso de peso. Este estudio
esta tesis.
los dos grupos ante una tarea de elección alimenticia y comprobar, si como aparece en
los individuos adictos a sustancias, esta activación está relacionada con el craving por
alimentos con alta saliencia (algos en grasas y/o azúcares). Para abordar este último
objetivo realizamos nuestro cuarto estudio que está bajo revisión en la revista Appetite y
3. Hipótesis
I. Los adolescentes con exceso de peso serán más sensibles a la influencia del
75
Capítulo 5. Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis
exceso de peso.
II. Los adolescentes con exceso de peso serán más sensibles a la influencia del
III. Los adolescentes con exceso de peso mostrarán una mayor toma de decisiones
puntuaciones en impulsividad.
76
Capítulo 5. Justificación, objetivos e hipótesis de la tesis
77
III. MEMORIA DE
TRABAJOS
78
79
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers
attention in adolescents with excess weight
80
81
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
1. Introduction
Adolescents with excess weight suffer substantial social stress including frequent peer
bullying and social marginalization and exclusion [1,2]. Crucially, the degree of
exposure to these social stressors is the most important predictor of poor psychological
adjustment and poor academic achievement in adolescents with obesity [3]. Moreover,
neuroendocrine studies have shown that non-fasting levels of the “hunger hormone”
ghrelin increase in response to social stressors (i.e., the Trier Social Stress Task,
involving a public speak) [4] and that the awakening response of the “stress hormone”
cortisol positively associates with subsequent lipid intake [5]. Therefore, social stress is
a potent determinant of poor cognition and poor food choices in adolescents with excess
weight. This phenomenon could be explained by the harmful impact of social stress on
during adolescence [7,8]. Therefore, examining whether social stress hampers cognition
in adolescents with excess weight is essential for prevention of cognitive decline and
this notion. In this study we examined if a social stressorthe Trier public speaking stress
reflect the function of frontal-limbic systems [9,10] and are longitudinally associate
82
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
with excess weight would show greater cortisol response to the social stressor, and
performance.
2. Methods
Participants
Eighty-four adolescents aged between 12 and 18 years old participated in the study.
They were classified in two groups (Normal weight [n = 42] and Excess weight [n =
42]) based on their age adjusted Body Mass Index (BMI) percentile [13]. Sample size
was estimated through power analysis. The existing evidence about the impact of the
Trier Social Stress Task (TSST) on selected outcome variables was correlational (i.e.,
performance is between 0.3 and 0.4) [14,15]. Therefore, we estimated that in order to
achieve adequate power (80%) to detect a ρH1 = 0.3 association between the
independent variable (stress) and the cognitive outcomes (attention and decision-
making) 84 participants would be required. This sample size was deemed acceptable for
the mixed repeated-measures design. The classification of the two groups was
conducted in alignment with the guidelines of the International Obesity Task Force and
the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Normal weight participants had age
adjusted BMI percentiles in the range between the 5th and the 84th percentile, and
Excess weight participants had age adjusted BMI percentiles 85 (Table 1). Three
participants from the Excess weight group provided invalid cortisol samples, and
therefore the final study sample comprised 42 Normal weight and 39 Excess weight
Participants also completed The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire [16] which was
83
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
external eating, emotional eating and restraint) (Table 1). Participants were recruited
from the paediatrics and endocrinology services of the Hospital “Virgen de las Nieves”
in Granada (Spain), and from schools located in the same geographical area. The
inclusion criteria for participants were defined as follows: (i) age range between 12 and
18 years old; (ii) BMI percentiles falling within the intervals categorized as overweight
assessed by participants and parent’s interviews and the Eating Disorder Inventory [17].
Experimental procedures
Fig 1 displays a schematic representation of the experiment. In order to induce social
stress in the laboratory we utilised a previously validated Virtual Reality version of the
Trier Social Stress Task (TSST) [18]. Participants had to perform a stressing task which
84
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
told that this audience would at- tend the speech and subsequently evaluate its quality.
However, the virtual audience was programmed to look progressively bored and
disappointed with the speech. The speech was followed by a mental calculation test
(serially subtracting 17, starting from 2013). Cortisol levels were measured via saliva
samples collected before onset of the TSST (T1), after completion of the TSST and the
calculation test (10 minutes after TSST onset—T2) and after performance on each of
the attention and decision making cognitive probes (20 and 30 minutes after TSST
onset-T3 and T4- respectively). Cognitive measures were conducted in a fixed order
before TSST onset (pre-TSST, overlapping with T1) and after completion of the TSST
and the calculation test (post-TSST, overlapping with T2). To minimize practice effects,
we utilised parallel versions of all tasks in the post-TSST administration. The original
validation study showed that this virtual reality TSST is able to induce modest but
sizeable increases in cortisol and subjective stress responses [18]. Moreover the virtual
audience tamed the ethical concerns associated with the negative impact of the social
stressor on adolescents’ participants. The Ethics Committee for Human Research of the
Universidad de Granada approved the study. Both participants and parents signed
informed consent.
Cortisol measurement. Participants were told not to smoke, eat or drink coffee for at
least 30 minutes before the experiment. All the experimental sessions were conducted at
85
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
the same time of the day (4–5 pm) based on pilot data obtained in this cohort prior to
study onset indicating that diurnal cortisol levels were stable during these hours. Saliva
cotton), and two plastic tubes that fit one inside the other. Subjects were told to place
cotton salivettes inside their mouth and gently chew and/ or suck on them for 1–3 min
until they became soaked in saliva. The cotton tube was inserted inside the plastic tube,
which was then capped. Saliva samples were stored at -20°C until required for assay.
immunoassay “ECLIA” method. This method is designed for use in Roche Elecsys
Cognitive measures. We utilized three computerized tests: two subtests from the Cam-
(MOT) and Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP), and the Iowa Gambling Task
(IGT) [20]. Al- ternate versions of each test were used in pre-stress and post-stress
administrations.
MOT. The main objective of this test is to provide a baseline measure of the
subjects’ basic motor skills in terms of reaction times and accuracy. After a
demonstration of the correct way to point on the computer screen using the forefinger of
the dominant hand, the subjects must point to a series of stimuli (crosses) popping up in
86
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
component. A white box is displayed in the centre of the computer screen, inside which
digits, from 2 to 9, are displayed in a pseudo-random order, at the rate of 100 digits per
minute. The subject must detect consecutive odd or even sequences of digits (for
example, 2-4-6) and respond by pressing the touch pad. The outcome measures of this
test were response latency and response discriminability (B’) scores, which are sensitive
to attention and impulse control domains respectively. The B’ score is the signal
detection measure of the strength of trace required to elicit a response (range -1.00 to
+1.00). Thus, it is the tendency to respond regardless of whether the target sequence is
present and uses the p(hit) and p(fa) results. A score close to +1.00 indicates that the
making. It involves four decks of cards (A, B, C and D). Each time a participant selects
these rewards, there are probabilistic punishments (monetary losses). Two of the decks
of cards (A and B) produce high immediate gains; however, in the long run, they will
take more money than they give, and are thus considered disadvantageous. The other
two decks (C and D) are considered advantageous, as they result in small, immediate
gains, but will yield more money than they take in the long run. The performance
measure was the net score calculated by subtracting the number of dis- advantageous
choices (decks A and B) from the number of advantageous choices (decks C and D). An
equivalent parallel version of the ABCD task in which decks are labelled K, L, M and N
was utilised in the post-TSST administration. These versions have shown adequate test-
87
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
Visual Analogue Scales (VAS). We used two Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) designed
to rate arousal and stress. For arousal scale the individual must indicate the extent to
which they perceived as active and alert (from nothing active to very active). For stress
scale they must indicate how much stress they feel (from no stress to very much stress).
Statistical analyses
The main hypotheses were examined utilizing mixed repeated measures analyses of
factor, and cortisol levels (as measured in μg/dl) and RVP’s mean response latency and
B’ scores and IGT’s net scores as dependent measures. Cortisol and RVP performance
measures were log-trans- formed (base 10) to meet the normal distribution, but for the
sake of clarity the Figures report non-transformed measures. IGT scores fitted to the
correlation analyses between change scores of cortisol levels (T2—T1) and change
scores of cognitive performance (T2—T1) and between both change scores and
biological and psychological measures. These change measures were non- normally
participants from the Excess weight group (n = 37) and one participant from the Normal
weight group had missing cortisol data at T1 and T2 (n = 41). With regard to cognitive
tests, there was no missing data in the Excess weight group (n = 39), whereas in the
Normal weight group three participants had invalid data for RVP response latency and
IGT (n = 39) at T1 or T2, and three participants had invalid data for RVP B’ (n = 38) at
T1 or T2.
3. Results
88
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
Cortisol response
We found a significant Time x Group interaction on cortisol levels, F (3,74) = 4.36, p =
0.008. Cortisol mildly increased in Excess weight participants after the TSST.
Independent-sample t- tests showed that Excess weight and Normal weight participants
did not significantly differ on cortisol levels before TSST (T1). However, Excess
weight adolescents showed significantly in- creased cortisol levels after TSST (T2), t =
1.94, p = 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.5 (Fig 2). Moreover, cortisol increase between T2 and T1
differences were also statistically significant at T3, t = 2.44, p = 0.02, and T4, t = 2.63, p
= 0.01. However, this effect seems to be driven by decreased cortisol levels in the
Fig 2. Cortisol levels (μg/dl units) in adolescents with excess weight and adolescents with normal weight
before and after exposure to the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST). T1 represents cortisol levels before
TSST; T2 represents cortisol levels immediately after TSST termination; T3 and T4 represents cortisol
levels 10 and 20 minutes after TSST termination.
89
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
Cognitive performance
MOT. Pre-TSST scores showed that both groups had similar baseline response
latencies. Further, both groups showed mild reductions of response latencies between
(1,76) = 6.35, p = 0.01 (Fig 3). Independent-sample t-tests showed that Excess weight
and Normal weight participants did not significantly differ in the pre-TSST measure.
(78) = 1.75, p = 0.08, Cohen’s d = 0.4, with Excess weight participants performing
significantly poorer than Normal weight controls. There was no significant correlation
interaction, F (1,75) = 0.99, p = 0.32. There were no main effects of Time or Group,
although visual inspection shows Excess weight participants performed better than
Normal weight participants in both pre- and post-TSST measures (Fig 3).
F (1,77) = 0.005, p = 0.94. There was a significant main effect of Time, F (1,77) = 6.01,
p = 0.02, indicating that both groups exhibited significantly poorer performance after
the TSST (Fig 3). There was no significant correlation between T2—T1 cortisol levels
90
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
Fig 3. Cognitive performance in adolescents with excess weight and adolescents with normal weight
before and after exposure to the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST). Top panel Y axes represent time in
milliseconds. The Y axis in the bottom-left panel represents signal detection derived Beta scores, ranging
from 0 to 1. The Y axis in the bottom-right panel represents Iowa Gambling Task net scores, ranging
from -60 to +60.
performance in T2—T1. We found a positive correlation between levels of uric acid and
change in RVSP response latency performance between T2 and T1, Spearman’s Rho =
0.46, p = 0.0001, and a negative correlation between thyroxine levels and change in
Iowa Gambling Task performance between T2 and T1, Spearman’s Rho = -0.27, p =
0.03. We also found a negative correlation between scores of external eating and RVSP
response latency performance between T2 and T1, Spearman’s Rho = -0.27, p = 0.02.
Visual Analogue Scales (VAS). We did not find a significant Time x Group
interaction on VAS of arousal or stress but results were in the expected direction, with
both groups showing more subjective arousal and stress after the TSST.
91
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
response. The primary analyses indicated that in the normal weight group cortisol levels
did not change after stress, and therefore there is a concern that cognitive changes were
due to spurious factors. To address this issue, we run additional analyses in the
subsample of participants who showed sizeable increments in cortisol levels after stress,
including 24 participants of the Excess weight group (57% of the original sample) and
20 participants of the Normal weight group (48% of the original sample). The results of
these analyses were coherent with the main findings. We found a significant Time x
Group interaction on RVP’s latency scores, F (1,41) = 6.17, p = 0.02, whereby a drop in
performance was only observed in the Excess weight group. Moreover, there was a
significant correlation between T2—T1 cortisol levels and T2—T1 RVP Response
Fig 4. Correlation between between T2—T1 cortisol levels (X Axis) and T2—T1 RVP Response Latency
(Y Axis) within the subsample of participants showing TSST-induced increases in cortisol levels.
92
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
4. Discussion
We show that social stress specifically increases cortisol levels and hinders attentional
response latency in adolescents with excess weight. Conversely, social stress failed to
after the social stressor. These findings indicate that adolescents who are overweight
and obese have enhanced stress reactivity in response to social stressors, which
selectively impacts on attentional skills. Since adolescents with excess weight are
markedly exposed to social stressors during everyday lives, our findings suggest that
stress immunization strategies should be put in place to prevent the harmful impact of
In agreement with our primary hypothesis, social stress induced greater cortisol
response in overweight and obese adolescents. The effect was mild but the specific
impact on participants with excess weight agrees with the notion that repetitive social
and purportedly of the HPA axis associations with fronto-limbic systems [23–25]. The
discrepancy between our finding of cortisol in- crease and a previous negative finding in
obese adults [26] suggests that adolescence compared to adulthood is a more sensitive
time period for abnormal sensitization of stress systems, likely due to ongoing neural
maturation of these systems [7,27]. Further, both preclinical and clinical evidence
shows that social stressors such as social evaluation and social exclusion are particularly
challenging for adolescents [8,28,29]. The potential mechanisms for the specific impact
of social stress on stress reactivity in adolescents with excess weight include the
93
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
and limbic regions that are essential for stress regulation [32,33]. Our finding is
and obesity-related behaviours [34,35], and of the emerging evidence suggesting that
high levels of stress can longitudinally predict the progression of obesity [36].
adolescents with excess weight. The effect was again mild and pointed to stress-related
hindering of the capacity to get benefit from a repeated administration of the task.
effect size) [37], and this is what we observed in the control group. However, excess
weight adolescents were unable to get benefitted from this repeated administration. The
effect was specific for attention-related latency adjustments, but not for psychomotor-
regulation.6 This notion is consistent with the neural networks interactions between the
HPA axis and medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex regions involved in
attention regulation [38–40]. In support, neuroimaging studies have shown that the
neuroadaptations in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex regions [41]. This
BMI [12], implying that ad- equate control of social stress and/or cognitive boosting of
attentional performance (between T1 and T2) and higher maladaptive eating patterns
94
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
such as external eating, which reflects attentional bias to- wards food related cues.
Further, both social stress and attentional skills are significantly associated with
advantageous social functioning and academic performance [3], and therefore our
finding highlights the potential benefit of controlling social stress to improve social and
adolescents with excess weight and adolescents with normal weight. Since cortisol
performance) it is unlikely that this finding can be attributed to the effects of acute
stress. However, it might be attributed to broader effects of the social stressor, such as
the social evaluation context. The latter notion agrees with previous experimental
evidence showing that adolescents make riskier choices than young adults or adults
when they are under social evaluation [42]. The lack of specificity of our result implies
decision-making in excess weight and normal weight adolescents. In favour of the first
notion, neuroimaging studies have shown that the impact of social evaluation on
regions [43], which are generally sensitized during adolescence. In favour of the second
notion, we have observed that excess weight and normal weight adolescents recruit
different brain circuitries during the pondering of social decisions [44]. Future studies
are warranted to address this question. In any case, our finding might have general
who are overweight or obese have higher exposure to social evaluations [3] and that
95
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
subsequent risky choices are longitudinally associated with weight gain and obesity
[11].
We conclude that social stress response is sensitized in adolescents with excess weight,
hindering their attentional function. The study has important strengths including the
characterization and the group matching of excess weight and normal weight
adolescents, and the objective measurement of stress reactivity with cortisol biomarkers.
particularly important to stress that unlike the original TSST [45], the virtual reality
TSST was not able to induce significant increases of cortisol levels in the control group.
We selected this stressor because it was capable of inducing mild but sizeable stress in
the laboratory at the same time that it reduced the ethical implications of stressing “at
risk” obese adolescents [18]. In agreement with this assumption, our results indicate that
the stress manipulation was actually more effective in obese adolescents (57% of
participants showed increased cortisol levels) than in controls (only 48% of participants
showed increased cortisol levels). There are however several factors that may explain
the variability in stress induction, such as degree of belief in the cover story or degree of
immersion in the virtual reality environment, that were not systematically controlled in
this study. Therefore, further studies are warranted to reassess the validity of this virtual
reality version, and to replicate our findings using TSST versions that are able to
“no-stress” control condition, we cannot ascertain a causal link between stress and
96
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
inhibitory control in adolescents, it is plausible that the mild nature of the stressor
fostered cognitive impulsivity increases rather than (expected) decreases after TSST.
Future studies are warranted to address these limitations, to expand on the biological,
and to longitudinally assess the relevance of this experimental effect on public health
5. References
treatment in Icelandic sample. J Eval Clin Pract 2012; 18: 465–472. Doi:
ghrelin response to a psy- chological stress challenge does not predict the post-stress
Awakening cortisol re- sponse in lean, obese, and reduced obese individuals: effect of
gender and fat distribution. Obesity 2007; 15: 377–385. PMID: 17299111
97
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes 2014;
8. Tsai SF, Huang TY, Chang CY, Hsu YC, Chen SJ, Yu L, y cols. Social
instability stress differentially af- fects amygdalar neuron adaptations and memory
performance in adolescent and adult rats. Front Behav Neurosci 2014; 8:27. Doi:
16251988
Dissociable aspects of per- formance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following
lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat:
differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity and compulsiv- ity. Behav Brain Res
11. Seeyave DM, Coleman S, Appugliese D, Corwyn RF, Bradley RH, Davidson
NS, y cols. Ability to delay gratification at age 4 years and risk of overweight at age 11
98
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
12. Frazier-Wood AC, Carnell S, Pena O, Hughes SO, O’Connor TM, Asherson P, y
cols. Reaction time but not response inhibition shares genetic influences with later BMI
13. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut-offs for
thinness, overweight and obesity. Pediatr Obes 2012; 7: 284–294. Doi: 10.1111/j.2047-
14. Van den Bos R, Harteveld M, Stoop H. Stress and decision-making in humans:
PMID: 19497677
15. Preston SD, Buchanan TW, Stansfield RB, Bechara A. Effects of anticipatory
stress on decision making in a gambling task. Behav Neurosci 2007; 121: 257. PMID:
17469915
16. Van Strien T., Frijters JER, Bergers GPA, Defares PB. The Dutch Eating
19. Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Lawrence AD, McInnes L, y
cols. A study of perfor- mance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal
99
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
lobe dysfunction in a large sample of nor- mal volunteers: implications for theories of
20. Bechara A. Iowa Gambling Task. Boca Raton, FL, USA: Psychological
and the Iowa gambling task: Ecological validity in individuals with substance
22. Butler TR, Ariwodola OJ, Weiner JL. The impact of social isolation on HPA
axis function, anxiety-like behaviors, and ethanol drinking. Front Integr Neurosci 2014;
23. Jahn AL, Fox AS, Abercrombie HC, Shelton SE, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ, y
24. Liu J, Chaplin TM, Wang F, Sinha R, Mayes LC, Blumberg HP. Stress reactivity
22365466
19225430
100
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
Cortisol response to the Trier Social Stress Test in obese and reduced obese individuals.
20302906
27. Rao U, Hammen C, Ortiz LR, Chen LA, Poland RE. Effects of early and
PMID: 18597740
28. Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Borofsky LA, McNealy K, Pfeifer JH, Dapretto M.
Subgenual anterior cin- gulate responses to peer rejection: a marker of adolescents’ risk
29. Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Pfeifer JH, Dapretto M. Neural responses to
witnessing peer rejection after being socially excluded: fMRI as a window into
30. De Santis AS, Diez Roux AV, Hajat A, Aiello AE, Golden SH, Jenny NS, y
cols. Associations of salivary cortisol levels with inflammatory markers: The Multi-
31. Slavich GM, Way BM, Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE. Neural sensitivity to social
rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci
101
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
32. He Q, Chen C, Dong Q, Xue G, Chen C, Lu ZL, y cols. Gray and white matter
structures in the midcingu- late cortex region contribute to body mass index in Chinese
Garcia A. Brain structural cor- relates of reward sensitive and impulsivity in adolescents
with normal and excess weight. Plos One 2012; 7:e49185. doi:
discounting and percent body fat in Chinese urban young adolescents. Appetite 2014;
36. Kubzansky LD, Bordelois P, Jun HJ, Roberts AL, Cerda M, Bluestone N, y cols.
symptoms and weight status in women. JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71; 44–51. doi:
37. Lowe C., Rabbitt P. (1998). Test\ re-test reliability of the CANTAB and
102
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
39. Kiem SA, Andrade KC, Spoormaker VI, Holsboer F, Czisch M, Sämann PG.
40. Thomason ME, Hamilton JP, Gotlib IH. Stress-induced activation of the HPA
axis predicts connectivity between subgenual cingulate and salience network during rest
in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychi- atry 2011; 52: 1026–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-
41. Hanson JL, Chung MK, Avants BB, Rudolph KD, Shirtcliff EA, Gee JC, y cols.
Structural variations in pre- frontal cortex mediate the relationship between early
childhood stress and spatial working memory. J Neurosci 2012; 32: 7917–7925. doi:
42. Gardner M, Steinberg L. Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky
decision making in ad- olescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev Psychol
risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Dev Sci 2011; 14: F1–
103
Capítulo 6. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight
regions on social decision-making in excess weight adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp (in
press).
45. Kirschbaum C., Pirke K. M., Hellhammer D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress
Test’–a tool for investi- gating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting.
104
105
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive
functions in adolescents with excess weight: associations with
autonomic responses.
Moreno-Padilla, M., Fernández-Serrano, M. J., Verdejo-García, A., & Reyes del Paso,
G. (2018). Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in adolescents with
excess weight: associations with autonomic responses. Annals of Behavioral Medicine.
doi: 10.1093/abm/kay051
106
107
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
1. Introduction
Overweight and obesity in adolescence have sharply increased over recent decades,
reaching epidemic levels (1). The socioeconomic changes that have occurred in recent
decades in Western societies, associated with the unlimited access to food, have
modified the way we perceive food and regulate intake. These processes are
sensory cues (e.g., taste, smell, texture and appearance), availability, motivational and
affective states, pleasure seeking, etc. All of these factors influence what and how much
people eat even when they are not hungry (2). In the last few years, obesity is being
addictions (3), where the motivational value of highly palatable food is significantly
increased, while the top-down or executive control mechanisms that would normally
regulate reward-driven responses are diminished (4, 5). Executive control mechanisms
are relevant to the regulation of eating behavior (6), since they allow for adjustment of
signal processing and executive control functioning has also been related to a tendency
to select immediate and appetizing (high in calories and/or sugar) rewarding choices,
although these have negative consequences in the long term (8, 9). The imbalance
between these two systems can be greater in adolescence, a period characterized by the
addition to the relative maturity of striatal areas responsible for reward processing (10).
Therefore, during adolescence, the activity of rewards system may prevail over that of
108
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
Another factor that can impair top-down control mechanisms is stress. Stress has a
harmful impact on cognitive skills, such as attention, cognitive control and decision-
Furthermore, psychosomatic theories hold that people with obesity tend to eat in
food impulsively) when under negative emotional states (13). Stress can also enhance
the propensity to eat high calorie “palatable” food via its interaction with central reward
pathways (14). For example, ghrelin and cortisol increase in response to social stressors
and influence reward motivation, thus modulating consumption of appetizing food (15,
16)
During adolescence, peer relations are particularly salient and can serve as a robust
source of distress (17). Adolescents with excess weight suffer from social stress, such as
being subjected to frequent teasing about their body (19). Negative stereotypes toward
peers with excess weight begin early in childhood (20) and these social stressors can
negatively affect social adjustment and academic achievement (21). In this context,
Several studies have found deficits in executive functioning in adults and adolescents
with excess weight (22-25). However, to the best of our knowledge, no study has
analyzed the influence of social stress on executive functions in adolescents with excess
weight. Therefore, this study examined the effect of a social stressor on executive
performance in adolescents with excess versus normal weight. For this purpose, the
Trier Public Speaking Stress Social Task (TSST) (26, 27) was used. We analyzed the
109
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
adolescents would show decreased executive performance after exposure to social stress
cognitive inhibition, and shifting (ability to follow different rules in a task and change
were recorded. For this purpose, heart rate (HR) and skin conductance (SC) were
continuously recorded during the TSST. Since adolescents with overweight are more
often exposed to negative peer evaluations than adolescents with normal weight (18,
2. Method
Participants
Sixty adolescents, 25 males and 35 females between 13 and 18 years of age,
participated. They were selected based on their sex and age-adjusted BMI percentile in
accordance with the guidelines of the International Obesity Task Force (IOFT) (28).
Normal weight participants (n=30) had BMIs ranging between the 5th and 84th
percentiles, and excess weight participants (n=30) had BMIs > the 85th percentile. Table
1 displays the socio-demographic, BMI and body fat percentage data. Participants were
recruited from high schools located in Jaén (Spain). They were screened for medical and
were: (i) aged range between 13 and 18 years; (ii) BMI > 5th percentile; and (iii) no
110
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
Executive Measures
Working memory–Letter-Number Sequencing (29): Participants were read a sequence in
which letters and numbers were combined, and were asked to reproduce the sequence,
first putting the numbers in ascending order and then the letters in alphabetical order.
Inhibition and shifting–Five-digit test (FDT (30): The FDT consists of four conditions
within various boxes, which constitutes an interference effect because the boxes contain
groups of digits that do not correspond to their arithmetic value. Finally, in part 4
(shifting), participants have to count or read, depending on whether the outline of the
box is normal (count, 80% of stimuli) or of double thickness (read, 20% of stimuli). The
difference in performance time between part 3 and the mean of parts 1 and 2 (inhibition
score), and the difference in performance time between part 4 and the mean of parts 1
and 2 (shifting score), were considered. Thus, a higher score denotes worse performance
111
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
(i.e., the participant took more time). Errors in parts 3 (inhibition) and 4 (shifting) were
also analyzed.
Social Stress Task (TSST-VR) was used (26). This version of the TSST was previously
used in young people and has been shown to produce a significant increase in subjective
stress and arousal, skin conductance and cortisol levels (27, 31). Participants had to
deliver a speech about their personal characteristics, including both positive and
negative aspects, in front of a simulated audience. The task is divided into two parts
(each 2 min 30 s long). In the first task, the audience was interested and attentive to the
speech, giving nods of understanding to the participant (i.e., positive social evaluation).
In the second part, the audience began to show signs of disagreement with the speech,
talking and murmuring among themselves and criticising the participant´s words (i.e.,
negative social evaluation). The task included four phases: a baseline rest period (3
min), delivery of the task instructions and preparation for the speech by the participant
(3 m), speech during positive social evaluation, and speech during negative social
evaluation. This virtual reality version of the TSST is able to induce modest but
Procedure
After obtaining permission from the high school´s directors, the study was presented to
each class of students and their participation was requested. The students who were
interested in taking part sent us the informed consent form, which was signed by their
parents if they were minors. Then, the participants were assigned to a group and a
specific day on which to complete the experimental session. Six high schools in Jaén
participated in the study. The recruitment rate was approximately 4% of the total
112
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
required to be satiated (having had lunch about 1 hour before) and to not have taken any
caffeine. Weight and height were self-reported by participants for recruitment purposes
and BMI was calculated in the laboratory, using the exact height and weight data
collected on arrival. Body composition measures were also collected using the
Bodystat®1500 monitoring unit. The EDI-2 (32), validated in young people, was
administered to rule out eating disorders (binge eating, anorexia nervosa and bulimia
nervosa). Then, executive functioning measures were conducted before TSST onset
(pre-TSST) and immediately after completion of the TSST (post-TSST). The post-TSST
evaluation was administered immediately after TSST. During the two evaluations,
participants first completed the Letter-Number Sequencing and then the Five Digit Test.
Subjective stress was measured by a visual analogue scale (VAS, ranging from 1 to 10;
no stress to extreme stress) before and after exposure to TSST. The virtual reality TSST
was carried out in a soundproof room, with white walls and without any distracting
stimuli. The equipment consisted of a computer running the program containing the
social scenes, and a projector for their display on the wall. Previous validation studies
indicated increases in skin conductance and salivary cortisol during the task, both when
participants rated task immersion as being higher with the wall-screen presentation
versus the goggles (31). Surround-sound headphones were used to allow perception of
the sound emanating from the room where the audience was situated, and the murmurs
and comments of the listeners. The Ethics Committee of the Universidad de Jaén
approved the study. Both participants and parents signed informed consent forms.
113
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
polygraph (Biopac Systems Inc., USA). HR (beats per minute) was derived from an
attached to the participant’s right mid-clavicle and the lowest left rib (left wrist as the
ground). HR was extracted from ECG recordings using the software AcqKnowledge
3.9.1 (Biopac Systems Inc.) and edited for artifacts (when present) via linear
Ag–AgCl electrodes filled with an inert 0.05 M NaCl electrolyte cream and attached to
the palmar surface of the second and third middle phalanges of the participant’s non-
dominant hand. Two participants (one from each group) had unusable SC recordings.
Statistical analyses
Group comparisons were carried out with Student’s t-test for independent samples.
Responses to the TSST were analyzed by repeated measures ANOVA with Time (pre-
and post-TSST) as the repeated-measures factor and Group (Excess versus Normal
weight) as the between-subject factor. Although the TSST consisted of four phases,
given our specific interest in the effect of social evaluation, HR and SC analyses were
restricted to the difference between the latter two parts of the TSST involving social
scores were computed as the difference between the post- and pre-TSST values.
3. Results
In the whole sample, the change in HR was positively associated with changes in stress
VAS scores (r=0.32, p=0.013), “shifting errors” (r=0.30, p=0.02), “inhibition errors”
114
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
(r=0.38, p=0.003) and the “shifting score” (r=0.26; p=0.046). The change in SC
and positively with the change in stress VAS scores (r=0.26, p=0.047). Finally, the
change in stress VAS scores correlated positively with the change in “inhibition errors”
(r=0.46, p<0.001). BMI was positively associated with post-TSST “inhibition errors”
(r=0.51, p<0.001), “shifting errors” (r=0.32, p=0.001) and stress VAS scores (r=0.31,
p=0.015).
Subjective stress
A Time x Group interaction was found for stress VAS scores (F1,58=9.76, p=0.003,
𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.14). While in adolescents with excess weight stress levels increased from pre- to
post-TSST evaluation (F1,29= 65.89, p<0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.69), the change in adolescents with
normal weight did not reach significance (F1,29=2.66, p=0.115, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.08) (Table 2).
Table 2. Descriptive scores and group comparisons for stress (VAS) and neuropsychological
measures before TSST (PRE-scores) and after TSST (POST-scores)
t p d´
Mean SD Mean SD
115
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
Psychophysiological measures
No group differences were found in HR or SC during the pre-TSST evaluation. A Time
x Group interaction was found for HR (F1,58=8.26, p=0.006, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 = 0.13) (Figure 1). While
HR increased in adolescents with excess weight from the positive to the negative social
evaluation phase of TSST (F1,29=8.45, p=0.007, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.23), no change was observed in
adolescents with normal weight (F1,29=1.16, p=0.29, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.04). A Time x Group
interaction was also observed in SC (F1,56=4.76, p=0.033, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.08) (Figure 2). While
SC decreased in adolescents with normal weight from the positive to the negative social
evaluation phase of the TSST (F1,28=17.15, p<0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.38), no change was observed
116
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
Executive functions
During the pre-TSST evaluation, excess weight participants showed greater scores in
the inhibition condition of FDT (i.e., lower inhibition) than normal-weight participants
(t=2.08, p=0.042, δ=0.54). No other significant differences arose during pre-TSST (see
Table 1). Significant Time x Group interactions were found for Letter-Number
Sequencing (F1,58=16.82, p<0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.23) (Figure 3), “inhibition errors” in FDT
(F1,58=31.34, p<0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.35), “shifting errors” in FDT (F1,58=10.80, p=0.024, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =
0.08) (Figure 4) and “shifting score” in FDT (F1,58=15.47, p=0.039, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.07).
Adolescents with normal weight significantly increased their performance after the
score” (F1,29= 23.02, p<0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.44) and decreased their “inhibition errors” (FDT)
(F1,29=6.59, p=0.01, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.19). By contrast, adolescents with excess weight increased
117
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
their “inhibition errors” (FDT) (F1,29=25.38, p<0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.467) and, marginally, their
4. Discussion
impairments in measures of inhibition and shifting, and higher subjective stress levels,
118
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
negative social evaluation phase (relative to the positive phase) in this group, while no
with normal weight from the positive to the negative social evaluation phase, suggesting
habituation to the situation, but did not change in adolescents with excess weight. Skin
conductance is a variable that usually displays a decrease over the recording period,
denoting habituation to the situation. A flat recording, without any sign of decrease, is
levels. Subjectively, the greater increase in stress levels indicates that adolescents with
excess weight perceive the situation as more stressful than do adolescents with normal
during negative social evaluation, in adolescents with excess weight. The most common
motivation for using a public speaking task is that it elicits a social evaluation-related
threat (34). The inclusion of the two phases of the TSST as a function of feedback from
the audience (positive versus negative) allowed for a more specific analysis of social
evaluation, making our results more innovative. Taken together, these results support
the utility of differentiating between positive versus negative social evaluation during
the TSST for the study of the impact of social stress on autonomic and cognitive
functions.
with excess weight is consistent with a previous study which showed impaired attention
119
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
after TSST in adolescents with excess weight compared to adolescents with normal
weight (27). Negative emotional states in adults are known to impair cognitive capacity;
for example, depressive symptoms in people with obesity impair executive function
(35). Furthermore, emotional eating patterns, which are more prevalent in this
stressful situations evoke negative mood states and impair impulse control. The joint
influence of executive deficits and emotional eating patterns would lead to further
Inhibition, shifting and working memory were negatively affected by social stress in our
excess weight adolescents. This suggests that social stress has a detrimental impact on
executive functioning in these adolescents, and this may influence their eating
second evaluation due to practice effects arising from repeated administration (36, 37).
In fact, in this study working memory improved significantly in the normal weight
group from the pre- to post- TSST evaluation. However, adolescents with excess weight
did not benefit from this learning experience, and in fact their performance decreased. A
previous study (27) using the same experimental protocol also found increases in
evaluation, while excess weight participants were unable to benefit from the practice
effect. These results may be due to the greater levels of stress during the TSST in
adolescents with excess weight. Stress negatively affects abilities that require conscious
(38). Greater cortisol responses to the TSST were found in the previous study (27), and
results of the present study showed higher heart rate and electrodermal reactivity to the
120
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
social stress task in adolescents with excess weight. Furthermore, autonomic reactivity
memory performance in the whole sample. Therefore, the greater autonomic and stress
response in adolescents with excess weight can increase stress interference in this group
and therefore lead to a deficit in learning from the repeated administration of the tasks.
Executive functioning may have multiple direct and indirect influences on obesity in
adolescence. Although available evidence links executive functioning and obesity (24,
39), the specific mechanisms mediating this association are less well-known. Some
behaviours in childhood and adolescence via increasing intake, disinhibiting eating, and
reducing physical activity. The inability to inhibit impulses predicted higher food
intake, a higher body weight and less weight loss after a weight reduction intervention
(40). Deficits in inhibition can impact impulse control and thus the capacity to restrict
intake of appetizing foods (high fat/sugar). Impairments in shifting may influence the
capacity to regulate and modify eating behaviours in order to prevent harmful health
consequences. Furthermore, this deficit may lead to adolescents with excess weight
persisting in their unhealthy eating habits. Impairments in working memory could affect
the ability to maintain cognitive control, making it more difficult to engage in healthy
was associated with reduced orbitofrontal volume and executive dysfunctions, which
were most pronounced in terms of working memory and inhibition (41). Conversely,
executive function skills were positively associated with healthy eating habits, such as
We observed group differences before social stress only in the “inhibition score” (FDT),
121
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
differences were found in shifting or working memory. These results are concordant
obesity versus normal weight adolescents (43). Another study found selective
alterations in inhibition and shifting, but not working memory, in excess weight and
obese adolescents (24). In contrast, others authors found significant differences between
obese and normal adolescents in working memory as well as attention, but not in
As expected, the change in subjective stress was positively associated with the change
in HR, SC and “inhibition errors” (FDT). This suggests that levels of subjective stress
In this way, negative social evaluations may induce a greater increase in stress levels
adolescents relative to those with normal weight. The deleterious influence of negative
social evaluation on executive control in adolescents with excess weight may exacerbate
The change in HR during social evaluation was positively associated with the change in
“shifting errors”, “inhibition errors” and “shifting score” in the whole sample. The
performance (i.e., greater habituation of skin conductance was associated with better
with excess weight. This harmful effect on executive functioning may lead to problems
in real life, such as poor regulation of eating habits. However, studies in adults also
using the TSST did not find differences in HR, blood pressure or cortisol responses
122
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
between obese and normal weight individuals (15). This discrepancy may be due to the
reflect a greater vulnerability to social stress in adolescents than adults. In line with the
greater autonomic response found in our study, a greater cortisol response after the
TSST has been previously found in excess weight than in adolescents with normal
weight (27).
altered balance between the earlier-developing limbic system and the later developing
opinions of peers and general social evaluation become a central aspect for self-image
development (47). Adolescents with excess weight frequently suffer from negative
social evaluations and social stressors during their everyday lives, which may lead to
weight would show a blunted stress response due to habituation to repeated stress
exposure. However, previous studies using this same TSST protocol found greater
increases in salivary cortisol in excess weight than in normal weight adolescents (27).
Therefore, due to all of the factors listed above, adolescents with excess weight are an
important target group for cognitive interventions based on stress regulation strategies,
regard, some evidence already suggests that executive functioning training for obese
123
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
children can improve working memory, inhibition and shifting, being useful in weight-
Regarding its strengths, our study used an innovative strategy to evaluate the impact of
social stress, particularly negative social evaluation, on adolescents with excess weight,
as well as the inclusion of autonomic variables as objective indices of stress. Among the
limitations, we used a virtual reality audience in our TSST instead of the actual public,
which might have decreased the realism of the situation and the stress-elicited
responses. However, this version of the TSST was validated in previous studies and
produced a reliable stress response (26, 27, 31). Furthermore, the inclusion in future
excess weight adolescents. Regarding the study design, the absence of any
conditions might have influenced the results. Therefore, this aspect should be taken into
account in future studies. Additionally, we did not assess factors like emotional eating
executive functioning decrements on the propensity to overeat and become obese in the
In summary, our results showed a harmful impact of social stress, specifically negative
Adolescents with overweight performed worse after TSST in inhibition and shifting
than those with normal weight, and they did not benefit from learning in the domain of
124
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
with excess weight showed greater subjective and autonomic stress responses to
negative social evaluation. Given the relevance of high order executive functions to
self-control of eating behaviour (49, 50), the results presented herein highlight the value
of assessing the social evaluation context, and how it may be associated with
stress-mediated impairment in cognitive functioning could increase the risk for future
5. References
1. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity and trends in
2012;307:483-490.
2. Berthoud HR, Morrison C. The brain, appetite, and obesity. Annu Rev Psychol.
2008;59:55–92.
3. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Baler RD. Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake:
4. Acosta MC, Manubay J, Levin FR. Pediatric obesity: parallels with addiction and
5. Zheng H, Lenard NR, Shin AC, Berthoud HR. Appetite control and energy balance
regulation in the modern world: reward-driven brain overrides repletion signals. Int
J Obes. 2009;33:S8–S13.
125
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
6. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Spitznagel MB, Gordon E. Elevated body
Psicothema. 2010;22:227-235.
Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:48-62.
9. Berthoud HR. Interactions between the “cognitive” and “metabolic” brain in the
10. Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation
2003;160:1041–1052.
11. Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated
12. Davidson RJ, McEwen BS. Social influences on neuroplasticity: stress and
13. Laitinen J, Ek E, Sovio U. Stress-related eating behavior and body mass index and
14. Sominsky L, Spencer SJ. Eating behavior and stress: a pathway to obesity. Front
Psychol. 2014;5:434.
psychological stress challenge does not predict the post-stress urge to eat.
Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32:693-702.
126
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
obese, and reduced obese individuals: effect of gender and fat distribution. Obesity.
2007;15:377-385.
17. Stapinski LA, Araya R, Heron J, Montgomery AA, Stallard P. Peer victimization
18. Puhl RM, King KM. Weight discrimination and bullying. Best Pract Res Clin
20. Cramer P, Steinwert T. Thin is good, fat is bad: how early does it begin? J Appl Dev
Psychol. 1998;19:429-451.
making in obesity: a study using the Gambling Task. Eat Weight Disord.
2006;11:126-132.
2010;18:1572-1578.
127
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
Behav. 2007;10:655-662.
increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess weight. PloS
one. 2015;10:e0123565.
28. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut‐offs for
29. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th edn. Tea Editions:
Madrid, 2005.
García M, Peralta-Ramírez MI. A virtual reality approach to the Trier Social Stress
Madrid, 1998.
33. Roth WT, Ehlers A, Taylor CB, Margraf J, Agras WS. Skin conductance habituation
34. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical
2009;52:535-539.
128
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
36. Collie A, Maruff P, Darby DG, McSTEPHEN MICHAEL. The effects of practice
37. Falleti MG, Maruff P, Collie A, Darby DG. Practice effects associated with the
one week and one month test-retest intervals. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.
2006;28:1095-1112.
38. Luethi M, Meier B, Sandi C. Stress effects on working memory, explicit memory,
and implicit memory for neutral and emotional stimuli in healthy men. Front Behav
Neurosci 2009;2:5.
39. Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of
2014;38:494-506.
restrained eaters only overeat when they are also impulsive. Behav Res Ther.
2009;47:105-110.
42. Riggs NR, Spruijt-Metz D, Chou CP, Pentz MA. Relationships between executive
cognitive function and lifetime substance use and obesity-related behaviors in fourth
129
Capítulo 7. Negative social evaluation impairs executive functions in excess weight adolescents: associations
with autonomic responses
45. Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann New York Acad Sci.
2008;1124:111-126.
46. Casey BJ, Duhoux S, Cohen MM. Adolescence: what do transmission, transition,
47. Nelson EE, Leibenluft E, McClure EB, Pine DS. The social re-orientation of
48. Verbeken S, Braet C, Goossens L, Van der Oord S. Executive function training with
49. Houben K, Jansen A. Training inhibitory control: recipe for resisting sweet
50. Veling H, Aarts H, Papies EK. Using stop signals to inhibit chronic dieters’
130
131
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-
choice task in excess weight adolescents: relationships with
high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Moreno-Padilla, M., Fernández-Serrano, M. J., & Reyes del Paso, G. (2018). Risky
decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
Relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger. Plos One. Under review.
132
133
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
1. Introduction
recent decades [1,2]. Excess weight in adolescents is a strong predictor of adult obesity
[3]. Overweight and obesity, being associated with increased incidence rates of diabetes,
cardiovascular diseases and certain kinds of cancer, are currently the fifth-leading
In the last few decades, drastic changes in the environment and lifestyles have modified
the way we perceive foods and regulate their intake [4]. The availability of a wide range
has led to what, and how much, to eat becoming a decision-making matter. Obesity has
imbalance between motivational and control-inhibition systems [5,6]. From this theory,
appetizing food (high in fat and/or sugar) could cause an imbalance in the interaction of
appetizing food and a weakening of the control-inhibitory system [6]. This deficit in
control and inhibitory influences would lead to impulsive and compulsive intake of
[6].
and impulse control skills strongly modulate goal-directed behaviour [8]. Furthermore,
134
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
control areas (prefrontal areas) are not completely developed and seem to maximize
predisposition to take risks in response to signals for potential reward, which also
confers a probability of unattractive results [9]. In recent years, the concept of risk-
taking has been used to describe impulsive behavior in drug addiction and obesity
[10,11]. Previous studies have repeatedly shown that drug abusers are risk prone, as
taking tasks [13]. Obesity is also associated with greater risk-taking, showing an
association with risky patterns of responses in tasks like the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)
[14].
Adolescents are known to have a tendency to take more and greater risks than
individuals in other age ranges in many life domains, such as unprotected sex, criminal
behavior, dangerous driving, and experimenting with alcohol and other drugs [15].
Furthermore, adolescents who are reward sensitive and have difficulties in controlling
There is increasing evidence that individual differences in the tendency to overeat are
related cues [17]. Neurocognitive studies have shown that obesity and addiction are
response to drug or food cues, respectively [18]. Several studies have analyzed
attentional bias in individuals with obesity, but the results have been inconsistent
[19,20]. When participants were tested in a hungry state, no differences were found, but
135
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
when they were satiated at the time of testing, greater attentional bias was found in
adolescents is scarce. To the best of our knowledge, only one study has found increased
attentional bias and impulsivity to food cues in adolescent girls, as well as reduced
Regarding appetitive motivation, substance use disorders (addiction) and obesity, and
subjective states of craving and hunger, are associated with attentional bias for drug-
that drug-cue reactivity is positively associated with increases in impulsivity and risk-
Decision making in eating behaviour can be studied by food choices tasks. Food
decisions concern what, when, and how much to eat. Food choices can lead to
overconsumption, when there is an increased preference for appetizing food (high in fat
and/or sugar). Therefore, the study of decision making is extremely important in this
population, since decision making based in unhealthy choices can lead to weight more
This study examined the effect of exposure to food pictures, in a food-choice task, on
excess versus normal weight. Risk seeking was assessed through the Balloon Analogue
Risk Task (BART). Riskiness on the BART was related to self-reported engagement in
pictures, performance on the BART and feelings of hunger were evaluated both before
and after the food choice task. We hypothesized that adolescents with excess weight
would show greater risky decision-making after exposure to food pictures, while no
136
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
increases in hunger levels after the food-choice task in excess versus normal weight
2. Methods
Participants
In total, 56 adolescents (24 males and 32 females) aged between 13 and 18 years
participated in the study. They were selected based on their age adjusted body mass
index (BMI) percentile in accordance with the guidelines of the International Obesity
Task Force [27] criteria: normal weight participants (n=29), with age-adjusted BMI
values in the range between the 5th and the 84th percentile, and excess weight
participants (n=27), with age adjusted BMI values above the 85th percentile. However,
the participant of higher weight in the normal weight group has a percentile of 70.
Socio-demographic, BMI, waist-hip ratio and fat percentage data are displayed in Table
1. Participants were recruited from high schools located in Jaén (Spain). The inclusion
criteria were: (i) aged between 13 and 18 years; and (ii) no history of neurological or
137
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Instruments
Self-reported measures
- Spanish version of the short UPPS-P impulsive behavior scale [28]: the UPPS-P is a
sensation seeking, lack of perseverance, lack of premeditation and urgency (positive and
negative). Each item on the UPPS-P is rated on a four-point scale ranging from 1
act rashly to obtain reinforcement when experiencing positive emotions, while Negative
negative affect. Sensation seeking describes individuals' tendency to seek out novel,
complex, and intense sensations and experiences, and a predisposition to take risks to
realise these experiences. Lack of Premeditation refers to the tendency to think and
reflect on the consequences of an act before engaging in that act or taking a decision.
- A visual analogue scale (VAS) designed to rate hunger levels. Participants had to
indicate how hungry they were feeling on a scale ranging from 1 to 10 (not hungry to
very hungry).
Risk-taking task
The BART [29] is a 20-trial computerized task that models real-world risk behavior
according to the concept of balancing the potential for reward and harm [29]. The
the screen. With each pump, the participant obtains 25 cents and the balloon increases
slightly in size. However, each balloon also has a concealed probability of exploding
after an unspecified number of pumps. Participants were told that at some point each
138
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
balloon would burst. Before the balloon explodes, the participant can press “Collect
money,” which saves his or her earnings to a permanent bank. If the balloon explodes
before the participant collects the money, all earnings for that balloon are lost, and the
next balloon is presented. Each successful click increases the participants’ temporary
payoff but increases the risk of the balloon exploding. Thus, each pump confers not
In this version of the task, the maximum number of pumps possible for a given balloon
was 128, thus the probability of the balloon exploding on Pump 1 was 1/128. If there
was no explosion after this first pump, the probability of explosion on Pump 2 was
1/127, and so on up until the 128th pump. Accordingly, the average break point or
Dependent variables are the average number of pumps of unexploded balloons and the
Food-choice task
A food preference decision-making task was used in this study. Two types of food
pictures were utilized: appetizing (high levels of fats and/or sugars) and healthy.
Appetizing cues included, for instance, sausages and chocolate and healthy cues
included, for instance, fruits and salads. In each trial, pairs of pictures of these different
types of foods were presented in three conditions (appetizing vs. healthy, appetizing vs.
appetizing and healthy vs. healthy). Participants had to choose between the two options
by pressing a computer keyboard. Each trial begins with a fixation cross which lasts
from 3 to 6 seconds, varying between trials. Then, the images of the two options appear
for 5 seconds (one on the left side of the screen and the other one on the right side of
screen, with the positions of the appetizing and healthy foods varying among trials). The
order of presentation of the images was counterbalanced across the participants. Then,
139
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
the fixation cross was represented. There were a total of 30 choice trials, with 10
choices for each decision category, preceded by four practice trials. The outcome
Procedure
Height, weight and body composition measures (Bodystat®1500 monitoring unit) were
performed the food-choice task, and immediately after, the BART was administrated
again (post-task). Subjective hunger evaluation (VAS) was carried out before (and after
UPPS-P) (pre-task) and after completion of the food-choice task (post-task). The Ethics
Committee for Human Research of the Universidad de Jaén approved the study. Both
Statistical analyses
Group comparisons were carried out with Student t-test for independent samples. BART
and hunger measures were analyzed by repeated measures ANOVA with Time (pre- and
post-task) as the repeated-measures factor and Group (Excess vs. Normal weight) as the
the covariables of Sensation Seeking and Positive Urgency in order to assess the
influence of these variables on the pre- to post- change. Associations between variables
with the PROCESS macro for SPSS. To assess the significance of partial mediation
effects, confidence intervals from the bootstrapping estimation techniques were used.
For a significant meditational effect, the limits of confidence interval should not include
the 0 value [30, 31]. In order to simplify these analyses, change scores were calculated
140
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
3. Results
Self-reported measures
The groups differed in two dimensions of the impulsivity questionnaire (UPPS-P),
sensation seeking (t=2.17, p=0.034, δ=0.58) and positive urgency (t=2.14, p= 0.037,
δ=0.56), with greater scores in the excess versus normal weight adolescents (Table 2).
A Time x Group interaction was found for hunger VAS scores (F1,54 = 8.56, p =0.005,
𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.14). Although both groups showed significant increased hunger levels, the
increase from the pre- to post-task evaluation was greater in excess weight (F1,26= 33.72,
p<0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.57) versus normal weight adolescents (F1,28=17.37, p< 0.001, η2p =0.38)
(Table 2).
effect on the change in hunger levels in the whole sample (F1,53= 4.13, p=0.047,
𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.072). Furthermore, a Sensation Seeking x group x time (F1,53= 8.40, p=0.001,
𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.241) interaction was found. In order to analyze this interaction we explored the
effect of the covariable in each group separately. Sensation Seeking influenced the
change in hunger levels in the excess weight group (F1,25 = 7.97, p=0.009, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.242),
but not in the normal weight group (F1,27= 0.03, p=0.869, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.001). Regarding food
choices, excess weight adolescents chose significantly more appetizing foods than
141
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Table 2. Means and standard deviations (SD) of impulsivity (UPPS-P), hunger (VAS)
measures, appetizing and healthy choices. Results of the group comparisons (t and p)
are also displayed.
unexploded balloons (F1,54= 5.68, p=0.021, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.10) (Fig 1) and exploded balloons
(F1,54= 7.38, p=0.009, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.12) (Fig 2). The average number of pumps of unexploded
balloons (F1,26= 14.57, p=0.001, 𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.36) and exploded balloons (F1,26= 6.33, p=0.018,
𝜂𝜂𝑝𝑝2 =0.20) increased in excess weight adolescents after the food-choice task, while no
group differences in the BART were observed in the pre-task evaluation (exploded
above-described differential responses to the food choice task led to increased risk-
taking (exploded balloons) in the excess versus normal weight adolescents during the
142
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Fig 1. Average Number of Pumps on Unexploded Balloons in the Pre-Task and Post-
function of group.
with the change in the number of exploded balloons, the number of exploded balloons
after the food-choice task and the average number of pumps on unexploded balloons
143
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
before and after the food-choice task. Sensation Seeking was positively associated with
the change in hunger levels, the number of appetizing choices made in the food choice
task, the number of exploded balloons and the average number of pumps on unexploded
balloons after the food-choice task. Positive Urgency, Lack of Perseverance and UPPS-
P total scores were positively correlated with the number of appetizing choices. Finally,
BMI was positively associated with the change in the number of exploded balloons, the
number of exploded balloons after the food-choice task, Positive Urgency, Sensation
In the excess weight group (Table 4), the change in hunger levels was positively
associated with the change in the number of exploded balloons, the number of exploded
balloons after the food-choice task, the average number of pumps on unexploded
balloons before and after the food-choice task and sensation seeking scores. Sensation
Seeking was positively associated with the number of exploded balloons after the food
choice task. UPPS-P total scores were correlated with the number of appetizing choices.
Finally, Lack of Perseverance was positively associated with BMI and the number of
exploded balloons before the food choice task. In the normal weight group (Table 5),
Negative Urgency, Lack of Perseverance and UPPS-P Total scores correlated positively
with the number of appetizing choices. Finally, BMI was positively associated with
Lack of Premeditation.
Results of mediation analysis showed that the change in hunger levels mediated the
difference in the number of exploded balloons between the pre- and post-
and not in normal weight participants (Bootstrapping Lower Limit Confidence Interval
144
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Table 3. Pearson correlations between variables in the whole sample are displayed.
Table 4. Pearson correlations between variables in excess weight group are displayed.
n=29 Hunger_ UPPSP_ UPPSP_ UPPSP_ UPPSP_ UPPSP_ UPPSP_ BMI
Normal weight Change T U+ U- SS LPrem LPers
145
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Table 5. Pearson correlations between variables in normal weight group are displayed.
4. Discussion
Results showed that after the food-choice task adolescents with excess weight displayed
increased values in the two risk-taking measures of the BART than adolescents with
normal weight. Adolescents with excess weight also showed a greater increase in
hunger levels (VAS scores) after exposure to the food-choice task. Furthermore, excess
weight adolescents showed greater scores in Positive Urgency and Sensation Seeking
found between the change in hunger feelings, risk-taking and impulsivity measures.
food cues, since the food-choice task led to an increase in risk-taking in these
individuals. Yeomans and Brace (2015) [32] showed similar results in a study
to their scores in on the disinhibition and restraint scales of the Three Factor Eating
146
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Questionnaire (TFEQ) [33]. They found that exposure to food cues led to a greater risk
comparison with restrained women. However, they found group differences in BART
measures both before and after food cue-exposure, while we only observed differences
after the food-choice task. The pre-task discrepancy may be due to differences in the
studies samples, as they selected their sample based on uncontrolled eating (TFEQ),
To our knowledge, this is the first study to analyse the influence of food cues
visualization on risk-taking in adolescents with excess weight. The fact that excess
weight adolescents increase risky decision-making after food exposure may be relevant
to our understanding of the role of food cues in the development of unhealthy eating
behaviour to maintain a positive mood. For example, undergraduate college students are
more likely to drink on days of celebration than during the week [35], and individuals
may also engage in risky drinking to enhance a pre-existing positive mood [36]. This
hypothesis is in line with our results of greater positive urgency (reward seeking under a
positive mood) in excess weight adolescents. In line with this result, Fernández-Serrano
y cols. (2011) [37], using the Iowa gambling task (IGT) in polysubstance users, found
negative images while performing the IGT) to a level similar to that observed in
controls, while they increased their risky decisions on the IGT during a positive
147
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Food cues could be associated with greater reward value in adolescents with excess
weight than in normal weight adolescents. Evidence points to greater neural reactivity in
the reward system in obese versus normal weight individuals during high-calorie food
visualization [38]. Therefore, appetizing food cues may evoke an approach response to
behaviours [39]. In fact, in our study, a greater number of appetizing choices in the
food-choice task were found in excess versus normal weight adolescents. Therefore, this
purported greater underlying reactivity to appetizing food cues in obese individuals may
exploded balloons and average number of pumps on unexploded balloons) and the
change in hunger levels in the whole sample and in the excess weight group
particularly, but not in the normal weight group. Besides, the change in hunger levels
was also positively associated with the average number of pumps of unexploded
balloons after the food-choice task in adolescents with excess weight. Therefore, food
excess weight adolescents, what may cause alterations in impulses control and hinder
believe that his/her body is in a state of homeostatic imbalance that must be restored
through the intake of food. This may increase the predisposition to overeat in current
society, given the ubiquitousness of full of fatty/sweet food cues. Therefore, these
results suggest that greater hunger feelings may predispose to enhanced risk-taking in
148
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
excess weight adolescents, which can lead to greater seeking of the reward
Impulsivity measures and its associations with hunger levels and appetizing choices
Excess weight adolescents showed greater scores in Positive Urgency and Sensation
traits in obese adolescents is scarce. In the two studies available on adolescents with
excess weight, no groups differences in impulsivity were found [7, 40]. As a possible
explanation for these differences versus the current study, the mean age of the excess
weight participants in these previous studies was 14.19±1.38 and 14.22±1.4 years, lower
than that in our study (15.28±1.82 years). The mean age of our study is characterized by
greater freedom and less control by parents, so it is more likely that adolescents around
this age develop behaviours such as searching for new experiences or immediate
adolescent alcohol use, cigarette smoking and marijuana use have been reported, with
older adolescents being more likely to engage in these types of risky behaviours [41].
Conversely, Nazarboland and Fath (2015) [42] found greater Sensation Seeking in
Sensation Seeking was positively associated with the change in hunger levels in excess
weight participants. Furthermore, this variable mediated the difference in hunger levels
in this group between the pre- and post- food-choice task evaluations. This suggests that
the impulsivity trait may not only be associated with eating preferences, but also with
changes in subjective feelings of hunger, which could stimulate overeating and the
149
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
Regarding to this, Sensation Seeking has long been associated with elevated drug intake
hunger feelings in excess weight adolescents may support the hypothesis of a greater
reactivity to food signals (i.e., increased seeking for rewards and positive reinforcement)
in these participants.
A preference for appetizing food in the food-choice task was associated with
impulsivity measures in our whole sample. Specifically, Sensation Seeking and Positive
Urgency (both related to greater reward sensitivity), which may indicate a mediational
evidence. Davis y cols. (2007) [44] found in women ranging from normal weight to
obese that reward sensitivity was positively linked to overeating and high sugar-fat food
preferences. Nederkoorn y cols. (2010) [45] found that participants with greater
impulsivity gained more weight during a 1-year period. It has been proposed that
[46]. All of this evidence suggests that exposure to appetitive food cues, via interaction
with impulsivity traits, may play an important role in the development of unhealthy
eating behaviours. In modern societies, given the high availability of, and frequent
exposure to, high calorie foods, individuals with high reward sensitivity are predisposed
to consumption beyond their caloric needs. The enhanced preference for fat-sweet foods
weight [47].
As the incentive salience of appetizing food cues increases, seeking out and consuming
this type of food becomes an important goal, exceeding feeding homeostatic regulation
[48]. This represents a risky behaviour, since consuming foods high in fat and/or sugar
is associated with weight gain in children and adolescents and, therefore, increased risk
150
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
of obesity [49]. Macchi, MacKew and Davis (2017) [50] assessed eating habits and risk-
taking (BART) in adolescents and found that choices on the BART were riskier in
adolescents who made unhealthier food choices. These findings are congruent with
studies observing that adolescents with higher risk-taking on the BART consistently
Limitations
Regarding limitations of the study, there are a number of issues that need to be
addressed in future studies, like differentiating among obese, overweight and normal
weight participants, the inclusion of objective eating behaviour measures (in the home
and/or the lab), and the fulfilment of a more exhaustive decision-making evaluation.
weight gain. Finally, the lack of a control group not exposed to the food-choice task
manipulation makes it difficult to discern whether the changes in hunger feelings and
decision-making are due to the visualization of food, the mere passage of time or the
repeated administration of the test. Future studies will be necessary to address this
Conclusions
In summary, the results showed that excess weight adolescents increased their risky
decision-making after food-choice task exposure, where this was associated with an
greater scores in impulsivity measures, which were positively associated with the
risk factor for the development of future health problems and obesity. In current western
societies, given the high availability of, and exposure to, high-calorie foods, decision-
151
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
making has become a crucial factor in maintaining healthy eating habits. Since risk-
make healthy decisions to prevent future obesity. Impulse control and decision-making
5. References
1. Lee H, Lee D, Guo G, Harris KM. Trends in body mass index in adolescence and
601–608.
2. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity and trends in
2012;12: 483–490.
3. Singh AS, Mulder C, Twisk JWR, van Mechelen W, Chinapaw MJM. Tracking
4. Zheng H, Lenard NR, Shin AC, Berthoud HR. Appetite control and energy
5. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Tomasi D, Baler RD. Obesity and addiction:
6. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Tomasi D, Baler R. Food and drug reward:
152
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
9. MacPherson L, Magidson JF, Reynolds EK, Kahler CW, Lejuez CW. Changes in
10. Koritzky G, Yechiam E, Bukay I, Milman U. Obesity and risk taking. A male
11. Lane SD, Cherek DR. Analysis of risk taking in adults with a history of high risk
12. Ball SA, Carroll KM, Rounsaville BJ. Sensation seeking, substance abuse, and
13. Yechiam E, Busemeyer JR, Stout JC, Bechara A. Using cognitive models to map
16. Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and
153
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
18. Volkow ND, Baler RD. NOW vs LATER brain circuits: implications for obesity
20. Doolan KJ, Breslin G, Hanna D, Gallagher AM. Attentional bias to food-related
visual cues: Is there a role in obesity? Proc Nutr Soc. 2014;74: 37–45.
21. Nijs IMT, Muris P, Euser AS, Franken IHA. Differences in attention to food and
22. Batterink L, Yokum S, Stice E. Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory
2010;52: 1696–1703.
patients, restrained eaters and healthy samples. Proc Nutr Soc. 2015;74: 99–114.
24. Field M, Cox WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its
25. Jones JD, Vadhan NP, Luba RR, Comer SD. The effects of heroin administration
154
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
26. Lejuez CW, Aklin WM, Zvolensky MJ, Pedulla CM. Evaluation of the Balloon
27. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut‐offs for
short Spanish version of the UPPS-P impulsive behaviour scale. Trastor Adict.
2012;14: 73–78.
29. Lejuez CW, Read JP, Kahler CW, Richards JB, Ramsey SE, Stuart GL, y cols.
30. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects
in simple mediation models. Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput. 2004;36: 717-
731.
31. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and
2008;40: 879-891.
32. Yeomans MR, Brace A. Cued to act on impulse: more impulsive choice and risky
33. Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary
34. Yuen KSL, Lee TMC. Could mood state affect risk-taking decisions? J Affect
155
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
35. Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, Goldman MS. Up close and personal:
36. Cooper ML, Agocha VB, Sheldon MS. A motivational perspective on risky
2000;68: 1059–1088.
2011;217: 331–339.
38. Stoeckel LE, Weller RE, Cook EW, Twieg DB, Knowlton RC, Cox JE.
39. Gray JA. The psychology of fear and stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press; 1987.
adolescents with normal and excess weight. PloS one. 2012;7: e49185.
156
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
43. Zuckerman M. Sensation seeking and the endogenous deficit theory of drug
just eat what you like? Weight gain over a year is predicted by an interactive
effect of response inhibition and implicit preference for snack foods. Health
47. Epstein LH, Leddy JJ. Food reinforcement. Appetite. 2006;46: 22–25.
48. Berridge KC. ‘Liking’and ‘wanting’food rewards: brain substrates and roles in
Relationship between raised BMI and sugar sweetened beverage and high fat
51. Lejuez CW, Aklin WM, Bornovalova MA, Moolchan ET. Differences in risk-
157
Capítulo 8. Risky decision-making after exposure to a food-choice task in excess weight adolescents:
relationships with high-calorie food preferences and hunger
53. Hanson KL, Thayer RE, Tapert SF. Adolescent marijuana users have elevated
1087.
158
159
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in
adolescents with excess weight: relationship with subjective
craving
Moreno-Padilla, M., Verdejo-Román, J., Fernández-Serrano, M. J., & Reyes del Paso,
G., Verdejo-García, G. (2018). Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in
adolescents with excess weight: relationship with subjective craving. Appetite. Under
review.
160
161
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
1. Introduction
The prevalence of adolescent overweight and obesity has steeply increased over the last
two decades, reaching epidemic levels (1). In most cases, overweight and obesity are the
result of food choices characterized by high calorie intake. When these choices are
consolidated at an early age, obesity is more severe and associated with worse long-term
consequences (2).
dietary choices are complex and poorly understood. Food intake is influenced by a
variety of factors besides homeostatic regulation, like sensory cues (e.g., taste, smell,
texture and appearance), availability, motivational and affective states, pleasure seeking,
etc. All of these aspects influence what, and how much, humans eat even when they are
satiated. These factors are associated with specific patterns of regional cerebral blood
Previous studies have demonstrated that brain activation in response to food pictures is
a useful measure to examine both sensitivity to food cues and vulnerability to the
brain regions associated with reward value processing (e.g. dorsal/ventral striatum,
orbitofrontal cortex [OFC]), as well as with the representation of internal body states
differential responses to food cues associated with weight status (6, 8, 9). Obese
participants in multiple brain regions, including reward system-related ones, like the
162
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
prefrontal cortex (PFC), OFC, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), insula, amygdala, and
striatum during hunger states (10); the PFC, caudate, hippocampus, and temporal lobe
immediately after eating (8); and the striatum, insula, hippocampus, and parietal lobe
during neutral appetitive states (neither hungry or satiated) (6). In addition, differential
activation to food types (high- and low-calorie) has also been examined in relation to
Another way to study brain activation during eating behaviour is by using food choice
paradigms. Food decisions concern what, when, and how much to eat. Food choices can
lead to overconsumption, when more energy is consumed than expended. The brain
Several neural processes are involved in feeding behaviour. Firstly, the visual system is
very important to guide food selection. Inputs from the visual system elicit a specific
pattern of brain activation related to preparation for food ingestion, which evokes the
desire to eat, as well as cognitive processes such as memory retrieval and hedonic
evaluation of the specific food (11, 12). Secondly, visual food cues activate the reward
neural circuitry (e.g., PFC, OFC, amygdala, dorsal and ventral striatum, hypothalamus,
and insula) (10). Moreover, high-calorie food cues specifically elicit a greater response
Recent theoretical models highlight that decision-making skills are a key factor in
controlling caloric intake in modern environments, since these are characterized by open
access to food and strong media-driven appeals to eat high-calorie food (15). Decision-
making skills are particularly pertinent in the case of adolescents, in whom transitions in
163
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
risk (16). Neuroimaging studies have confirmed that adolescents have hypersensitive
striatal responses to reward prediction (17, 18) and high activation of brain areas
However, the neural correlates of food choices in adolescents have been less well-
studied. This matter is particularly important, as the probability that an obese adolescent
develops into an obese adult is much higher than that of a normal-weight adolescent
(20). Moreover, once people have become overweight or obese, it is quite difficult for
them to regress to a stable healthy weight. The important increase in the prevalence of
obesity in children and adolescents, the complications of overweight / obesity for health
and the greater tendency to continue being overweight or obese in adulthood make
prevention of obesity the alternative of choice and the optimal strategy to stop the
In this study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to assess brain
regions associated with food choices between appetizing (i.e., high sugar, high fat) and
plain food in adolescents with excess weight (i.e., overweight and obese) versus normal
weight participants, in comparison with the normal weight ones, would show greater
neural responsiveness in the corticolimbic reward system during food choices between
appetizing and plain foods (i.e., OFC, ACC, insula, ventral striatum and amygdala). We
also hypothesized that this choice-evoked activation in these areas would correlate with
164
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
2. Methods
Participants
Seventy-three adolescents (age range: 14-19 years) participated in the study. They were
classified into two groups, excess weight [n=38] (27 adolescents with obesity and 11
adolescents with overweight) or normal weight [n=39], according to their age- and sex-
adjusted body mass index (BMI) percentile, following the International Obesity Task
Force (IOFT) criteria (21). There were no significant differences in age or sex between
The recruitment of participants was carried out throughout the province of Granada
(Andalusia, Spain) in hospitals and high schools, as well as via press and radio
advertisements. The inclusion criteria were defined as follows: (i) aged between 14 and
19 years; (ii) BMI percentiles falling within the intervals categorized as overweight or
obesity (≥85: Excess weight group), or normal weight (5 to 84: Normal weight group);
assessed via interviews with participants and their parents; (iv) absence of any history or
current evidence of eating disorders (e.g., binge eating, bulimia nervosa, anorexia
nervosa) assessed with the Eating Disorders Inventory-2 (EDI-2) and (v) absence of any
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee for Human Research of the
Universidad de Granada. Both the participants and their parents signed an informed
consent form.
165
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
Table 1
tc/chi p-value
Excess weight (n=38) Normal weight (n=39) squared
Mean (SDb) Mean (SD)
Age 16.47 (1.66) 16.58 (1.36) -0.30c 0.768
Sex (%men/women 47.37/52.63 48.72/51.28 0.00d 0.991
a
BMI 29.89 (3.72) 21.36 (2.07) 12.58c < 0.001
Fat (%) 29.03 (10.28) 15.61 (7.39) 6.56c < 0.001
a:
Body Mass Index; : Standard Deviation; : value of Student’s t; : value of Chi-square χ2
b c d
Procedure
This study consisted of two sessions. In the first session, all participants were pre-
acquisition. The participants tasted all foods included later in the fMRI task in order to
become familiar with the specific foods (and their corresponding flavours, textures and
sizes) that were going to be used in the fMRI tasks. The purpose of the tasting was to
establish a context closer to real life in the food choice task. Then, they had to rate the
different foods on a 1- to 10-point self-report scale indexing how much they liked each
meal.
In the second session, we conducted the fMRI task. When participants arrived at the lab,
we used an automated scale (Tanita BC-420 GP Supplies Ltd., London) to measure their
weight and body fat percentage. Body fat percentage was estimated via Bioelectrical
Impedance Analysis. All of the sessions were carried out at the same time of day (4
p.m.), and always fMRI tasks were carried out between one and three hours after lunch.
The teenagers finished their lessons in the high school about 2.30 p.m., after that they
took lunch and at 4 p.m. they started the session. Given the study had a larger protocol
that included more measures assessed before fMRI, participants usually started the
fMRI tasks at about 5 p.m. Just before the fMRI session, before beginning the task
166
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
inside the scanner, and after finishing the fMRI session, participants rated their hunger
from 0 to 100 points on a visual analogue scale in response to the question of “how
fMRI task
A food preference decision-making task was used. The food pictures used in the task
were taken earlier in the tasting session. All pictures were shot ad hoc for the study
using standardized presentation and lighting conditions. Therefore, all images were
matched for visual properties and serving size (about a portion to all food images).
Three types of food were utilized: appetizing (food with high levels of fats and sugars),
plain (defined as natural food or low in fats and/or sugars) and functional (foods that are
prepared not only for their nutritional characteristics but also to fulfil a specific
function, such as improving health and reducing the risk of disease). Appetizing cues
included, for instance, sausages, chocolate, cake, cheese and chips and plain cues
included, for instance, fruits, yoghurt, cereals and salads. In each trial, pairs of these
different types of food were presented to participants (appetizing vs. plain, appetizing
vs. functional and plain vs. functional). Participants were instructed to choose between
these two options taking into account their own preference for one or the other meal.
The question presented was: “If you had to eat one of these foods, which would you
choose?” Each trial began with a fixation cross, which appeared for 4 seconds. Then,
images of the two options appeared for 5 seconds (one on the left side of the screen and
the other one on the right side). The order of presentation of the images was
counterbalanced among the participants. Then, the fixation cross was presented again
(Fig 1). There were a total of 36 choice trials with 12 choices for each decision type.
Participants were instructed to press a button in order to choose the food that they
167
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
display goggles (Resonance Technology Inc., Northridge, CA, USA), and responses
were recorded with the Evoke Response Pad System (Resonance Technology Inc.).
Participants were instructed to press the button with their thumb if they preferred the
food on the left side, or the button with their forefinger for food on the right side.
According to the objectives of this study, we focused only on the choice between
appetizing food and plain food. The primary behavioral measure was the number of
After the fMRI session, participants assessed their "craving" for each food presented
earlier in the scanner on a 9-points scale (1, they did not desire the meal; 9, desired the
meal excessively). Valence and arousal for each meal were also assessed via Self-
Assessment Manikin (SAM) (22). The stimuli were presented using a computer task
programmed using e-Prime software, in which each stimulus was presented on the
screen for 5 seconds. The difference of score between subjective ratings of craving in
response to appetizing versus plain food (referred to as “Appetising vs. Plain Craving”)
was calculated.
Figure 1
Schematic representation of the fMRI task through depiction of the sequence of one
experimental trial
168
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
The Netherlands), equipped with an eight-channel phased-array head coil, was used to
repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 35 ms, field of view (FOV) = 230 ×
230 mm, 96 × 96 matrix, flip angle = 90°, 21 4 mm axial slices, 1 mm gap, 162 scans. A
slices, TR = 8.3 ms, TE = 3.8 ms, flip angle = 8°, FOV = 240 × 240, 1 mm3 voxels) was
abnormalities.
Square, London, UK), running on MATLAB R2009 (MathWorks, Natick, MA). Prior to
preprocessing, all images were visually inspected for artifacts. Preprocessing included
reslicing to the first image of the time series, slice timing correction, normalization
Gaussian kernel (full width at half maximum = 8 mm). No participant was excluded due
either direction.
Data analysis
Group comparisons of sociodemographic, task, and self-reported variables were
For the neuroimaging analysis, the conditions of interest were modelled from the time at
which the food choice was presented to the time at which participants responded.
169
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
Baseline was modelled as the time that the fixation cross was on the screen. Task
regressors were convolved with the SPM8 canonical hemodynamic response function.
The key contrast of interest was “appetizing vs. plain > baseline”, defined in first-level
One-sample t-tests were conducted to assess intra-group activations (healthy weight and
using two-sample t-tests. The statistical threshold used for all fMRI analyses, (i.e: intra-
and between-group analyses) was p< 0.05 false discovery rate (FDR) whole-brain
Finally, in order to examine the association between choice-evoked brain activation and
subjective food craving, the peak beta eigenvalues from each cluster of significant brain
differences between groups were extracted for each participant and correlated with the
3. Results
170
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
Table 2
Means (±SD) of food choices and self-reported measures (tasting, valence, arousal, craving
and hunger).
Brain activation during appetizing vs. plain food choices compared to baseline (fixation
cross)
Both groups show extensive activation in brain regions in response to the appetizing
versus plain food choices, including areas of the frontal cortex (dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex [vlPFC]), occipital cortex (visual cortex), and subcortical regions (thalamus,
171
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
participants
The excess weight group, compared to the normal weight group, displayed increased
cortex, dorsal ACC, insula and dorsal caudate during food choice processing (see Table
3). The normal-weight group had no additional activation versus the excess weight
Table 3
Brain regions that show greater activation in “appetizing versus plain choices > baseline”
in excess weight group than in the normal weight group.
a
Region Side MNI coordinates Ke t-value
X Y Z
b
dlPFC Right 32 34 36 133 4.12
Left -34 44 10 199 5.26
f c
STCx Right 60 -20 2 364 4.58
Left -52 -30 6 131 4.19
d
Hippocampus Right 42 -16 -18 945 4.46
g c
MTC Right 46 -36 2 364 4.37
d
Putamen Right 34 -16 0 945 4.34
h
SFCx Right 10 36 48 241 4.32
d
Thalamus Right 10 -8 2 945 4.3
Left -6 -20 0 152 3.82
GlobusPallidus Left -12 -2 -4 51 4.22
i
ITC Left -60 -4 -18 93 4.15
j e
OFC Right 38 48 -6 182 4.13
k e
vlPFC Right 30 30 -20 182 3.96
l
Rostral ACC Right 6 48 4 91 3.88
ACC Left -10 36 16 96 3.82
Insula Right 40 6 -4 48 3.7
DorsalCaudate Right 12 10 8 23 3.74
172
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
Figure 2
1: dlPFC: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; 2: Putamen; 3: Insula; 4: vlPFC: ventrolateral prefrontal cortex;
5: OFC: orbitofrontal cortex; 6: SFC: superior frontal cortex; 7: Thalamus; 8: Dorsal Caudate; 9: Rostral
ACC: rostral anterior cingulate cortex; 10: ACC: anterior cingulate cortex; 11: Thalamus; 12: Globus
Pallidus; 13: STC: superior temporal cortex; 14 ITC: inferior temporal cortex.
In the excess-weight group “Appetising vs. Plain Craving” correlated with appetizing
caudate, putamen and thalamus while in the normal-weight group only insula was
correlated with “Appetising vs. Plain Craving” (see Table 4). We performed an FDR
adjustment for multiple comparisons and the regions that remained significant were the
right dlPFC, dorsal caudate and superior temporal cortex only in the excess weight
group.
173
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
Table 4
Correlations between craving scores (Appetizing vs. Plain Craving) and “appetizing
versus plain choices > baseline” brain activation as a function of group. Only areas with
significant correlations are displayed.
Excess weight Normal weight Fisher
x y Z
b
dlPFC Left -34 44 10 0.04 0.35 0.43 -0.136 0.017
a
Right 32 34 36 0.001 0.53 0.819 -0.04 0.004
c a
TSC Right 60 -20 2 0.003 0.49 0.84 0.035 0.017
d
TMC Right 46 -36 2 0.04 0.34 0.221 0.209 0.274
Putamen Right 34 -16 0 0.01 0.43 0.385 0.149 0.097
e
TIC Left -60 -4 -18 0.03 0.38 0.628 0.083 0.092
f
vlPFC Right 30 30 -20 0.03 0.38 0.388 0.148 0.145
Thalamus Left -6 -20 0 0.02 0.4 0.892 -0.023 0.03
g
ACC Left -10 36 16 0.009 0.44 0.381 0.15 0.089
Insula Right 40 6 -4 0.03 0.38 0.049 0.33 0.405
a
DorsalCaudate Right 12 10 8 0.004 0.48 0.738 0.058 0.025
a
: These results survived FDR correction for multiple comparison;b: Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex; c: Temporal
Superior Cortex; d: Temporal Medial Cortex; e: Temporal Inferior Cortex; f: Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex; g:
Anterior Cingulate Cortex
4. Discussion
We found that adolescents with excess weight, compared to those with normal weight,
have higher brain activation in frontal, striatal, insular and mid-temporal regions during
choices between appetizing and plain food cues and, furthermore, this activation
correlated with subjective measures of craving. Neural responses in these regions during
food cue exposure in the obese participants were consistent with previous studies (6, 10,
23-25), but there is a lack of studies focused on food choices in adolescents with excess
the step prior to the development of obesity in adulthood. Furthermore, the context in
which food choices are made is a novel field of research within this population.
Decision making with respect to food choices may be considered the basis of healthy
eating habits.
174
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
During brain activation in our food choice task, we observed significantly increased
neural responses in the excess-weight group in the dlPFC and vlPFC, which are
implicated in cognitive control (26, 27). Research in the decision-making and self-
control literatures points to the particular importance of these regions (28-30). In fact, it
is possible that the greater activation in these areas relates to inhibitory processes in
response to the general belief that high-calorie foods are unhealthy and should be
avoided. This increased activation in the inhibitory system may be a reaction to greater
reward processing, as evoked by the high calorie stimulus (10). This fact may indicate
appropriate behavioural control (31). In fact, our findings are consistent with Davids y
cols (2010) (25) who also found increased activation of dlPFC to food cues in obese
children and also explained this fact as a greater inhibitory control effort. Therefore, the
greater inhibitory control may be the cause of the lack of group differences in
behavioural measures. Furthermore, adolescents with excess weight may present a bias
regarding their eating choices, since adolescents are more susceptible to social
desirability because of the importance given to their peer’s opinions. Social desirability
has been described as a tendency to overestimate desirable traits and behaviors and
development (33). In this case, adolescent with excess weight may think that to choose
appetizing food cues is frowned upon due to his body image and, therefore, they end up
choosing the healthy option. Klesges y cols, (2004) (34) reported that participants
175
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
Another part of the PFC showing increased activation in our excess weight group was
the OFC. This area plays an important role in food-related neural circuitry and responds
preferentially to high-calorie food cues (6, 10). The OFC integrates multiple sources of
Grabenhorst and Rolls (2011) (35), the reward value is ‘passed’ onward from the OFC
to higher cognitive regions such as vlPFC, dlPFC and ACC. Regarding our results,
increased activation in the OFC in excess weight adolescents may indicate greater
processing of high-calorie food cues in terms of reward. The ACC, which was also
more activated by the appetizing versus healthy food choice in our study, is thought to
integrate this reward value with information about planned or anticipated actions and
On the other hand, the excess weight group showed increased activation in the insula.
The insula and OFC are interconnected as the primary and secondary gustatory cortex.
A large number of studies found that insula is involved in craving (36), with specific
reactivity to visual food cues, as well as anticipation and consumption of foods in obese
individuals (37), and environmental drug cues in drug abusers (38). These results are in
accordance with previous studies (13, 14, 39) showing the involvement of temporo-
and Stice (2011) (40) found that youth who showed elevated reward circuitry
Finally, we found greater activation in the dorsal striatum (caudate and putamen) in the
appetizing versus plain food choice. This area is a part of the habit learning system,
which has previously been linked to lowered dopamine D2 receptor availability in obese
176
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
The reward deficiency theory has been advanced to clarify how a baseline hypo
(47). This model has also been proposed for individuals with excess weight, similarly
involved in encoding the pleasure associated with food intake. Highly appetizing foods
and drugs of abuse directly affect the mesolimbic system, with consumption of each
model of obesity posits that repeated pairings of reward from food intake and cues that
circuitry to food cues, contributing to craving and overeating (52). Therefore, in our
study, food choice-induced anticipation of palatable food can elevate activity in the
In general, our findings showed greater brain reactivity to food cues in adolescents with
excess weight. Neural food cue reactivity as indexed by fMRI has been shown to be
prospectively associated with food choice (54, 55), snack consumption (5), future
weight gain in adolescent girls (40), and women (56), weight status in women (57) and
preliminary results that show that regional brain activity elicited by food cues of high
receive real-time feedback of this activity through changes in the cue’s visible size, also
As a second result, we found a positive relation between the variable “Appetizing vs.
Plain Craving” and brain activation in the food choice task only in the excess weight
177
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
group. This result might imply that the desire and subjective value that excess weight
adolescents give to food cues is important, and related to the cognitive processing that
they engage in when choosing between appetizing and plain food cues. This outcome is
consistent with the results of Jastreboff y cols. (2013) (60), who found that in obese but
not lean individuals, food craving correlated positively with neural activity in
it is not only people with obesity show greater food reactivity and subsequent craving,
but also overweight (without reaching obesity levels). On the other hand, in a food
choice processing, normal weight adolescents seem to give less importance to the
subjective value of food cues given their lack of associations between craving and brain
This fact may be due to the social desirability as we have commented previously. In a
recent systematic review, Boswell and Kober, (2016) (61) demonstrated a robust
Overall, their results suggested that food cue reactivity and craving explained a
food and food cues in the modern ‘toxic food environment’ may function as conditioned
stimuli that serve as triggers for increased food consumption and can lead to weight
All of our results support the “food addiction model”. Brain areas showing food-
craving studies. The relationship between the dorsal striatal network and food craving
was significant in the overweight group in this study, in accordance with the notion of
178
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
dlPFC activation and craving, which may suggest that participants who require more
inhibitory control during the appetizing-plain food choice show more craving for this
high-calorie food (and hence there is a need for greater control). In summary, a bunch of
However, there are several limits to these similarities between drug and food addiction
The main conclusion of this study is that adolescents with excess weight have increased
activation in several regions involved in reward and emotional salience when they are
faced to choose between appetizing versus plain food. The results also suggest that
adolescents with excess weight ascribe greater importance to the incentive value of
appetizing food, since subjective craving correlated with the brain regions activated
during food choices, although no differences in behavioral craving were found between
groups. The difference between groups in relation to craving was only observed at brain
level.
The findings of the present study have treatment implications. Interventions for obese
adolescents should not solely focus on prominent or physical symptoms, but should also
target basic cognitive control functions and processes related to the emotion-processing
in patients with excess weight. Our results also show the importance of interventions
improve the outcomes of obesity treatments. The ability to resist temptations and
eating behaviors. Cue reactivity and craving to food cues may be directly relate to real-
life behavior, given that their effect on subsequent food-related outcomes is comparable
179
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
with real food exposure. Therefore, to promote a reduction in exposure to food cues in
the environment of adolescents with excess weight could be an important target for
Furthermore, more research into the neural correlates of food choice may provide better
insight into the effects of age, sex, and weight on food-related decision-making
processes, and provide targets for healthy eating interventions. Since an overweight
This study had some limitations, such as the type of contrast that we used (appetizing
and plain food cues vs. baseline), since some of the activations are not specifically
related to decision making. However, this limitation was partially overcome due to the
fact that significant correlations were found with the craving measure. On the other
hand, although we observed clear and extensive differences between our obese and
possible to determine whether the observed effects represent the causes or consequences
compare the results obtained between participants in fasted and satiated states. Finally,
the effects of the menstrual cycle should be taken into account in future studies, since a
number of studies have reported that brain activation and decision making processes are
modulated by the hormonal stage of the menstrual cycle during exposure to food cues
(65, 66).
180
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
Conclusion
In summary, this neuroimaging study strongly supports the hypothesis of behavioural
stimuli associated with high-calorie foods. It might be expected, for example, that an
would predict weight gain, especially in subjects at risk for obesity. Conversely, the
successful anti-obesity therapy, and whether such neuroadaptation would lead or follow
Author contributions: MMP and JVR carried out the experiments and analyzed data.
MMP wrote the paper with the contributions and approval of all authors.
Funding: This work was supported by projects grants from the Andalusian Council of
5. References
1. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of obesity and trends in
2012;307:483-490.
181
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
2. Reilly JJ, Kelly J. Long-term impact of overweight and obesity in childhood and
3. Korner J, Leibel RL. To eat or not to eat—how the gut talks to the brain. N Engl
J Med 2003;349:926-928.
4. Castellanos EH, Charboneau E, Dietrich MS, y cols. Obese adults have visual
attention bias for food cue images: evidence for altered reward system
5. Lawrence NS, Hinton EC, Parkinson JA, Lawrence AD. Nucleus accumbens
2012;63:415-422.
7. Stoeckel LE, Kim J, Weller RE, Cox JE, Cook EW, Horwitz B. Effective
395.
8. Bruce AS, Holsen LM, Chambers RJ, y cols. Obese children show
182
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
10. Stoeckel LE, Weller RE, Cook EW, Twieg DB, Knowlton RC, Cox JE.
11. Van der Laan LN, De Ridder DTD, Viergever MA, Smeets PA. The first taste is
12. Smeets PA, Charbonnier L, van Meer F, van der Laan LN, Spetter MS. Food
induced brain responses and eating behaviour. Proc Nutr Soc 2012;7:511–520.
13. Killgore WD, Young AD, Femia LA, Bogorodzki P, Rogowska J, Yurgelun-
Todd DA. Cortical and limbic activation during viewing of high-versus low-
14. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Thanos PK, Fowler JS. Similarity between obesity and
15. Morrison CD, Berthoud HR. Neurobiology of nutrition and obesity. Nutr Rev
2007;65:517-534.
17. Cohen JR, Asarnow RF, Sabb FW, y cols. A unique adolescent response to
18. Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, y cols. Earlier development of the accumbens
183
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
19. Van Leijenhorst L, Moor BG, de Macks ZAO, Rombouts SA, Westenberg PM,
20. Guo SS, Wu W, Chumlea WC, Roche AF. Predicting overweight and obesity in
21. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut‐offs
22. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the
2012;518:106-110.
24. Martin LE, Holsen LM, Chambers RJ, y cols. Neural mechanisms associated
with food motivation in obese and healthy weight adults. Obesity 2010;18:254–
260.
Obes 2010;34:94–104.
26. Levy BJ, Wagner AD. Cognitive control and right ventrolateral prefrontal
27. Miller EK. The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Rev
Neuroscience 2000;1:59.
184
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
28. Kable JW, Glimcher PW. The neural correlates of subjective value during
in DLPFC and VMPFC adjusts to the available decision time. Eur J Neurosci
2012;35:1065-1074.
31. Hutcherson CA, Plassmann H, Gross JJ, Rangel A. Cognitive regulation during
32. Dadds MR, Perrin S, Yule W. Social desirability and self-reported anxiety in
1998;26:311-317.
33. Brener ND, Billy JO, Grady WR. Assessment of factors affecting the validity of
34. Klesges LM, Baranowski T, Beech B, y cols. Social desirability bias in self-
reported dietary, physical activity and weight concerns measures in 8-to 10-year-
old African-American girls: results from the Girls Health Enrichment Multisite
35. Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET. Value, pleasure and choice in the ventral prefrontal
36. Garavan H. Insula and drug cravings. Brain Struct Func 2010;214:593-601.
185
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
2000;157:1789-1798.
1280.
elevated weight and future weight gain: an fMRI study. Obesity 2011;19:1775-
1783.
41. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Logan J, y cols. Brain dopamine and obesity. The
Lancet 2001;357:354-357.
42. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, y cols. Decreases in dopamine receptors but
1594–1598.
43. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, y cols. Decreased striatal dopaminergic
833.
44. Volkow ND, Wise RA. How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat
Neurosci 2005;8:555–560
receptors in the ventral striatum and central processing of alcohol cues and
46. Martinez D, Broft A, Foltin RW, y cols. Cocaine dependence and D2 receptor
186
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
47. Blum K, Braverman ER, Holder JM, y cols. Reward deficiency syndrome: A
biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and
49. Hernandez L, Hoebel BG. Food reward and cocaine increase extracellular
1988;42:1705-1712.
dorsal striatum correlates with meal pleasant- ness ratings in healthy human
51. Rada P, Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly releases
52. Berridge KC. ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ food rewards: brain substrates and roles in
53. Pelchat ML, Johnson A, Chan R, Valdez J, Ragland JD. Images of desire: Food-
54. Mehta S, Melhorn SJ, Smeraglio A, y cols. Regional brain response to visual
food cues is a marker of satiety that predicts food choice. Am J Clin Nutr
2012;96:989-999.
55. Van der Laan LN, De Ridder DT, Viergever MA, Smeets PA. Appearance
matters: neural correlates of food choice and packaging aesthetics. PloS one
2012;7:e41738.
187
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
56. Demos KE, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM. Individual differences in nucleus
accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual
high-calorie food images predicts weight status among women. Int J Obes
2013;37:1435.
58. Murdaugh DL, Cox JE, Cook III EW, Weller RE. fMRI reactivity to high-
59. Ihssen N, Sokunbi MO, Lawrence AD, Lawrence NS, Linden DE.
60. Jastreboff AM, Sinha R, Lacadie C, Small DM, Sherwin RS, Potenza MN.
61. Boswell RG, Kober H. Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and
62. Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, y cols. The global obesity pandemic: shaped
63. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Baler RD. Reward, dopamine and the control of food
64. Polivy J, Herman CP. 2014. Eating in response to external cues. Managing and
2014;12:181-192.
188
Capítulo 9. Increased food choice-evoked brain activation in adolescents with excess weight: relationships with
subjective craving
65. Barr SI, Janelle KC, Prior JC. Energy intakes are higher during the luteal phase
66. Frank TC, Kim GL, Krzemien A,Van Vugt DA. Effect of menstrual cycle phase
92.
189
IV. DISCUSIÓN,
CONCLUSIONES Y
PERSPECTIVAS
FUTURAS
190
191
Capítulo 10
Discusión
192
193
Capítulo 10. Discusión
Uno de los principales retos del siglo XXI a nivel mundial es el abordaje del problema
prevalencia de este problema es, sin duda, uno de los principales desafíos científicos
Observamos, sin embargo, que existía un escaso número de estudios sobre aquellos
adolescencia. Partiendo de esta premisa, los objetivos de esta tesis pueden resumirse
principalmente en tres. El primero estaba dirigido a analizar el efecto del estrés social,
decisiones de riesgo en adolescentes con exceso de peso, así como analizar la asociación
activación cerebral ante una tarea de elección alimenticia en adolescentes con exceso de
peso, así como a estudiar la asociación de esta activación cerebral con el craving
194
Capítulo 10. Discusión
diferencias entre los adolescentes con exceso de peso y los adolescentes con normopeso,
En relación con nuestro primer objetivo, este se dividió en dos objetivos específicos que
se corresponden con los dos primeros estudios que conforma esta tesis. El primer
estaba asociado a un mayor aumento del cortisol en este grupo. En cuanto al segundo
estudio, los resultados también revelaron un efecto perjudicial del estrés social en el
déficit estuvo asociado con una mayor reactividad autonómica en el grupo de exceso de
peso.
En relación con nuestro segundo objetivo, los resultados de nuestro tercer estudio
mostraron que los adolescentes con exceso de peso presentaban mayor cantidad de
elecciones de alimentos apetitosos (altos en grasas y/o azúcares) así como mayor
de peso, así como una asociación positiva entre la toma de riesgos y la cantidad de
195
Capítulo 10. Discusión
Por último, en relación a nuestro tercer objetivo, los resultados de nuestro cuarto estudio
mostraron que los adolescentes con exceso de peso tenían mayor activación cerebral en
áreas relacionadas con la recompensa al visualizar una tarea de elección alimenticia. Así
con el craving que mostraban los participantes por los alimentos presentados en la tarea.
En conjunto, estos resultados tienen una serie de implicaciones tanto teóricas como
1. Implicaciones teóricas
En relación a las implicaciones del primer estudio, los resultados mostraron que los
selectivamente en sus capacidades atencionales. Por otro lado, nuestros resultados son
reactividad del cortisol y los comportamientos relacionados con la ganancia de peso (Lu
y cols., 2014, van Strien, Roelofs & de Weerth, 2013), y la emergente evidencia que
obesidad (Kubzansky y cols., 2014). Así mismo, el efecto perjudicial del estrés social en
menor mejoría del rendimiento atencional tras el estresor social en el grupo de exceso
externa, que refleja un sesgo atencional hacia las señales relacionadas con los alimentos.
196
Capítulo 10. Discusión
Además, tanto el estrés social como el déficit en las habilidades atencionales están
igual que en el primer estudio, que los adolescentes con exceso de peso mostraban
mayor reactividad ante el estrés social, y en concreto ante las evaluaciones negativas,
rendimiento ejecutivo del grupo con exceso de peso, el cual estaba asociado con la
implicaciones son similares a las del estudio anterior, pero hemos de añadir que las
comportamiento, por tanto, creemos que estos resultados son cruciales para entender por
qué el impacto del estrés social puede derivar en un peor control de impulsos y, por
tanto, en una posible sobreingesta. De hecho, los déficits ejecutivos han sido asociados
alimentación desinhibida, y reducción del ejercicio físico). Por tanto, estos déficits
pueden conllevar que los adolescentes con exceso de peso persistan en sus hábitos
En cuanto a las implicaciones teóricas del tercer estudio, los resultados sugieren que los
adolescentes con exceso de peso tienen mayor reactividad a las señales alimenticias, ya
como en los niveles de hambre en este grupo. Además, el grupo con exceso de peso
197
Capítulo 10. Discusión
encontraron en toda la muestra asociaciones positivas entre los cambios en los niveles
de hambre y la toma de riesgos, así como asociaciones con las variables de impulsividad
y el IMC. Hasta nuestro conocimiento, este es el primer estudio que analiza la influencia
mostraron mayor nivel de urgencia positiva, lo que indica una mayor tendencia a la
búsqueda de refuerzos bajo estados de ánimo positivos. Estos resultados están en línea
con los hallazgos de Fernández-Serrano y cols. (2011), los cuales mostraban que los
una mayor preferencia por los alimentos apetitosos en la tarea de elección alimenticia
de alimentos altos en grasas y/o azúcares y por tanto a la ganancia de peso (Davis y
cols., 2007; Nederkoorn, Houben, Hofmann, Roefs & Jansen, 2010). Toda esta
alta disponibilidad y la frecuente exposición a alimentos con alto contenido calórico, las
ignorando sus necesidades calóricas. La mayor preferencia por los alimentos grasos y/o
dulces se explica por su mayor valor reforzante, especialmente en personas con exceso
198
Capítulo 10. Discusión
tanto, con un mayor riesgo de obesidad (Millar y cols., 2014). Por último, el mayor
incremento del hambre en el grupo con exceso de peso y la asociación positiva entre la
toma de riesgos y los niveles de hambre sugiere que el nivel subjetivo de hambre puede
En cuanto a las implicaciones teóricas del cuarto estudio, los hallazgos aportados por
los patrones de activación durante la neuroimagen indican que los adolescentes con
exceso de peso presentan una mayor activación de áreas relacionadas con la recompensa
entre imágenes alimenticias, lo que podría indicar una mayor sensibilidad ante las
señales de comida. La mayoría de los estudios que han encontrado mayor activación
han realizado en participantes con obesidad (Scharmüller, Übel, Ebner & Schienle,
2012; Connolly y cols., 2013; Rothemund y cols., 2007; Stoeckel y cols., 2008). Por
incrementado en personas con IMCs asociados a sobrepeso, sin tener que llegar a
centran en adultos, por lo que hemos abordado de manera más profunda los factores
Los adolescentes con exceso de peso también mostraron mayor activación en el dlPFC.
Nuestra explicación de este hecho está basada en otros estudios que sugieren que existe
un mayor esfuerzo para redirigir su preferencia alimenticia por lo que utilizan un mayor
control inhibitorio, así como por el concepto de deseabilidad social (Davids y cols.,
199
Capítulo 10. Discusión
2010; Klesges y cols., 2004). Esto explicaría que no haya diferencias conductuales en
las elecciones alimenticias entre los dos grupos. Por otro lado, la mayor activación en el
OFC que muestran los adolescentes con exceso de peso está asociado, según los
(Yokum, Ng & Stice, 2011). En cuanto a la mayor a activación del estriado dorsal, este
syndrome RDS” que proponen que existe un nivel basal más bajo de DA en el sistema
sustancias, y que resulta en una falta de placer o refuerzo proveniente de actividades que
normalmente si lo proporcionan (Volkow y cols, 1996; 1997; Volkow & Wise, 2005;
Heinz y cols., 2004; Martínez y cols., 2004). Siguiendo este modelo, los alimentos
así un aumento del placer (Small, Jones-Gotman & Dagher, 2003; Rada, Avena &
Hoebel, 2005). Por lo tanto, en nuestro estudio, la anticipación inducida por la elección
reforzante. Así mismo, la mayor reactividad cerebral ante señales alimenticias ha sido
(Lawrence, Hinton, Parkinson & Lawrence, 2012; Yokum y cols., 2011; Demos,
Heatherton & Kelley, 2012; Murdaugh, Cox, Cook & Weller, 2012). Por tanto, nuestros
alimenticio se refiere.
200
Capítulo 10. Discusión
informado por los participantes con exceso de peso. Este resultado podría implicar que
el deseo y el valor subjetivo que los adolescentes con exceso de peso otorgan a las
señales alimenticias son muy importantes, ya que están estrechamente asociados con el
procesamiento cerebral que realizan ante una tarea de elección alimenticia. Además, la
evidencia sugiere que la reactividad ante las señales de alimentos junto con el craving
explica una cantidad importante de la varianza en los resultados relacionados con las
intervenciones en exceso de peso (Boswell & Kober, 2016), mostrándose como fuertes
predictores en este sentido. Como conclusión, todos estos resultados apoyan el modelo
2. Implicaciones clínicas
intervención.
podemos concluir que la presencia y alta frecuencia del estrés social en adolescentes
201
Capítulo 10. Discusión
manejo del estrés, así como herramientas alternativas para afrontar las situaciones de
consecuencias negativas que el estrés social crónico pueda estar causando en el origen o
rendimiento académico que como hemos visto anteriormente también se puede ver
afectado negativamente. En segundo lugar, derivado del tercer estudio podemos afirmar
que en los adolescentes con exceso de peso se produce un aumento en sus niveles de
razonamiento más profundo sobre las ventajas e inconvenientes tanto a corto como a
largo plazo de cada elección. De esta manera, los adolescentes podrían llegar a adquirir
las habilidades necesarias para cambiar sus hábitos alimenticios basados en alimentos
que ofrecen una recompensa inmediata por aquellas opciones saludables que ofrecen
una recompensa beneficiosa para su salud más a largo plazo. En general, son necesarios
esto impactará en sus comportamientos alimenticios. Por último lugar, en relación a los
202
Capítulo 10. Discusión
que en los adolescentes con exceso de peso presentan mayor activación de áreas
implica que los adolescentes con exceso de peso intentan inhibir la conducta no
dlPFC como estrategia para reducir el craving por la comida y la consecuente ingesta
(Uher y cols., 2005; Barth y cols., 2011; Forcano, Mata, de la Torre & Verdejo-García,
2018). Como conclusión, los resultados de esta tesis evidencian el papel crítico de la
reactividad que presentan los adolescentes con exceso de peso a este tipo de señales y su
asociación con variables como el hambre, toma de riesgos o craving. Por tanto,
203
Capítulo 10. Discusión
comparables con la exposición a comida real. Por tanto, consideramos que promover
con exceso de peso debe ser un objetivo cardinal en las políticas de prevención de la
cerebral provocada por señales de alimentos de alto valor hedónico se reduce durante el
alimenticias, mostrando una reducción significativa del hambre después de una auto-
Linden, 2017). Por tanto, nuestros resultados indican que sería adecuado implementar
individuo con el fin de poder adquirir estrategias que permitan resistir a la tentación tras
altos en grasas y/o azúcares, así como las señales alimenticias derivadas de esta
Yokum y Stice, 2010; Nederkoorn y cols, 2006, 2010; Lowe y Fisher, 1983) que
apuntan que la obesidad podría estar relacionada con una mayor reactividad emocional
perjudicial del estrés social, la mayor toma de riesgos tras la exposición a señales de
204
Capítulo 10. Discusión
ambiente.
3. Fortalezas y limitaciones
depresión, y trastornos alimenticios como bulimia o trastorno por atracón) fueron causa
Estos resultados podrían estar limitados por algunos condicionantes. Por un lado, la
utilización de varias muestras por lo que hay que tener precaución a la hora de trasladar
205
Capítulo 11
Conclusiones
206
207
Capítulo 11. Conclusiones
A partir de los resultados obtenidos en los distintos estudios de esta tesis, se derivan las
siguientes conclusiones:
atencional en los adolescentes con exceso de peso. Este deterioro está asociado a
con exceso de peso. Este deterioro está asociado una mayor reactividad
electrodermal.
3. Los adolescentes con exceso de peso presentan mayor reactividad a las señales
cuales están asociados con el aumento del hambre y la toma de riesgos en este
grupo.
4. Los adolescentes con exceso de peso presentan una mayor activación en áreas
una tarea alimenticia. Esta activación, en este grupo, se asocia positivamente con
algunos de los factores que pueden estar influyendo en el exceso de peso, además de
208
Capítulo 11. Conclusiones
además de las singularidades psicobiológicas propias de esta etapa. Todos estos factores
controlar la tentación derivada de la continua exposición a éstos. Además, dado que los
el pronóstico.
209
Capítulo 12
Perspectivas futuras
210
211
Capítulo 12. Perspectivas futuras
Las conclusiones derivadas de esta tesis nos permiten generar nuevas preguntas de
investigación que creemos sería interesante explorar en estudios futuros. Entre ellas
podríamos destacar:
ambos.
uno con normopeso. Esta metodología permitiría que los resultados obtenidos
visualización de éstos.
212
Capítulo 12. Perspectivas futuras
cognición.
con exceso de peso. Diferentes estudios han mostrado que padecer obesidad y
rendimiento ejecutivo. Sin embargo, los estudios son escasos, con muestra
213
V. DOCTORADO
INTERNACIONAL
214
215
Doctorado internacional
1. Summary
The increase in the obesity prevalence has become, in recent decades, according to
the World Health Organization, one of the main public health problems worldwide.
people regulated their intake according to their metabolic states of hunger and
satiety, however, in today's western societies what and how much to eat has become
a decision-making matter. Recent studies suggest that the change in current lifestyle,
based on sedentary lifestyle and unhealthy eating habits, is responsible for the
Excess weight and obesity have also increased exponentially in childhood and
peculiarities. In this stage, behaviours aimed at the reward search and propensity to
risk are frequent, as well as a decrease in executive control and the ability to
regulate behavior effectively. In this regard, various studies confirm the existence of
adolescents with healthy weight. The executive functions allow a better regulation
adulthood and its harmful medical consequences, etc.) but it is also associated with a
social stress increase due mainly to the frequent teasing referred to their body image
that they receive from their peers and that can even lead to marginalization and
social exclusion. Therefore, adolescents with excess weight suffer greater social
216
Doctorado internacional
stress in their day to day. Numerous studies point to the detrimental effect of stress
on cognitive performance. Also, stress may also alter eating patterns through various
mechanisms.
second place to explain the eating behavior, being the decision-making processes
important role in obesity during childhood and adolescence. Different studies have
shown that the cues visualization related to drug use produces an increase in
impulsivity levels and induces a greater risk taking, increasing as a consequence the
show that people with excess weight have an attentional bias and greater reactivity
creation of the concept "food addiction". The drugs of abuse use the same neural
parallelism between the brain circuits involved in the loss of control and excessive
food intake that characterizes obesity and compulsive drug use. The alteration of the
dopamine brain circuits is central in these two pathologies. Specifically, the brain
obesity. During exposure to highly appetizing foods, reward circuit areas may
promote greater dopamine release due to the great salience of the stimulus and thus
217
Doctorado internacional
results of fMRI studies carried out so far point to an increased response in areas of
the reward circuit, both in adults and in overweight adolescents, when processing
food images, especially those with a high fat content and sugars.
Taking into consideration all previous, the objectives of this doctoral thesis were: 1)
adolescents with excess weight and adolescents with normal weight, 2) analyse the
adolescents with normal weight, y 3) analyse brain processing during food choices
and its relation to subjective craving, in excess weight adolescents and normal
weight adolescents.
To address these objectives, 4 studies were carried out. The results obtained
reactivity to this stress compared to adolescents with normal weight (study 1 and 2); studies
2) adolescents with excess weight make more risky decisions after exposure to food
cues and have higher levels of impulsivity than adolescents with normal weight
(study 3); and 3) there is greater activation of brain areas related to the reward
circuit during the exposure to food-choice task in the group of excess weight
adolescents and an association between activation in these areas and the craving
reported by the participants towards the foods presented in the task was found (study
4).
218
Doctorado internacional
adolescence, as well as to clinical level, promoting new treatments that take into
2. Conclusions
From the results obtained in the different studies of this thesis, the following
1. Social stress produces deterioration in tasks that measure the attention capacity
3. Adolescents with excess weight have a greater reactivity to food cues since after
their visualization they increase their hunger levels and they present a greater
risk taking. In addition, adolescents with excess weight present higher scores in
which are associated with increased hunger and risk taking in this group.
4. Adolescents with excess weight have greater activation in brain areas related to
the reward circuit when they are exposed to a food task. This activation, in this
group, is positively associated with craving for the foods shown in the task.
In general, our results are innovative since they help to point out some of the factors that
219
Doctorado internacional
greater social stress, risk taking and impulsivity, in addition to the psychobiological
peculiarities of this stage. All these factors make adolescence a critical period when it
particular, with overeating. These results also reveal the importance of taking into
account the highly caloric foods cues (images, advertisements, shop windows, etc.) as a
promote strategies for control the temptation derived from the continuous exposure to
them. In addition, since children and adolescents with excess weight have a high
unhealthy foods and the formation of healthy eating habits in these stages is crucial to
3. Future perspectives
The conclusions derived from this thesis allow us to generate new research questions
that we believe would be interesting to explore in future studies. Among them we could
highlight:
1. Deepen the knowledge of the effects of social stress on the eating behavior of
foods high in fats and / or sugars leads to a greater intake of these foods, and
3. Study more deeply through fMRI the neural correlates of elections with real
foods. Analyse the brain activity during the whole process, that is before making
the choice and when the food is consumed and observe the differences between
220
Doctorado internacional
a group of adolescents with excess weight and one with normal weight. This
real life.
weight towards high caloric foods in order to control the temptation, and see if
the change in the hedonic value that is given to food has consequences in brain
5. Conduct studies using the tDCS technique to try to reduce craving through the
the individual; and study the feasibility of including this technique in obesity
treatment programs.
related to "emotional eating". Thus, reducing the social stress impact would also
influence the reduction of the detrimental effects that chronic stress produces on
cognition.
excess weight. Different studies have shown that obesity and having the allele
However, studies are scarce, with insufficient sample and are not focused on
221
VI. REFERENCIAS
BIBLIOGRÁFICAS
222
223
Referencias bibliográficas
Acosta, M.C., Manubay, J., & Levin, F.R. (2008). Pediatric obesity: parallels with
80-96.
Adam, T.C., & Epel, E.S. (2007). Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiology &
Al'Absi, M., & Arnett, D.K. (2000). Adrenocortical responses to psychological stress
Anzman, S.L., & Birch, L.L. (2009). Low inhibitory control and restrictive feeding
Baker, J.L., Olsen, L.W., & Sorensen, T.I. (2007). Childhood body-mass index and the
357, 2329-2337.
Barth, K., Rydin-Gray, S., Kose, S., Borckardt, J., O'Neil, P., Shaw, D., ... & George,
M. (2011). Food cravings and the effects of left prefrontal repetitive transcranial
2, 9.
Batterink, L., Yokum, S., & Stice, E. (2010). Body mass correlates inversely with
Berthoud, H.R. (2007). Interactions between the “cognitive” and “metabolic” brain in
224
Referencias bibliográficas
Best, J.R., Theim, K.R., Gredysa, D.M., Stein, R.I., Welch, R.R., Saelens, B.E., ... &
Block, J.P., He, Y., Zaslavsky, A.M., Ding, L., & Ayanian, J.Z. (2009). Psychosocial
Boeka, A.G., & Lokken, K.L. (2006). The Iowa gambling task as a measure of decision
Bonato, DP., & Boland, F.J. (1983). Delay of gratification in obese children. Addictive
Behaviors, 8, 71-74.
Boswell, R.G., & Kober, H. (2016). Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and
Brogan, A.M.Y., Hevey, D., & Pignatti, R. (2010). Anorexia, bulimia, and obesity:
shared decision making deficits on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). Journal of
Bruce, A.S., Holsen, L.M., Chambers, R.J., Martin, L.E., Brooks, W.M., Zarcone, J.R.,
... & Savage, C.R. (2010). Obese children show hyperactivation to food pictures
225
Referencias bibliográficas
Bruce, A.S., Martin, L.E., & Savage, C.R. (2011). Neural correlates of pediatric obesity.
Bunge, S.A., & Wright, S.B. (2007). Neurodevelopmental changes in working memory
Cappuccio, F.P., Taggart, F.M., Kandala, N.B., Currie, A., Peile, E., Stranges, S., &
Carrillo, E., Moya-Albiol, L., González-Bono, E., Salvador, A., & Gómez-Amor, J.
Castellanos, E.H., Charboneau, E., Dietrich, M.S., Park, S., Bradley, B.P., Mogg, K., &
Cowan, R.L. (2009). Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue
Chaiton, M., Sabiston, C., O'loughlin, J., McGrath, J.J., Maximova, K., & Lambert, M.
Chambers, R.A., Taylor, J.R., & Potenza, M.N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry
226
Referencias bibliográficas
Clements, K., & Turpin, G. (2000). Life event exposure, physiological reactivity, and
Cohen, J.R., Asarnow, R.F., Sabb, F.W., Bilder, R.M., Bookheimer, S.Y., Knowlton,
Cole, T.J., Bellizzi, M.C., Flegal, K.M., & Dietz, W.H. (2000). Establishing a standard
Connolly, L., Coveleskie, K., Kilpatrick, L.A., Labus, J.S., Ebrat, B., Stains, J., ... &
Mayer, E.A. (2013). Differences in brain responses between lean and obese
Craig, A.D., & Craig, A.D. (2009). How do you feel--now? The anterior insula and
Cramer, P., & Steinwert, T. (1998). Thin is good, fat is bad: How early does it begin?
Crone, E.A., Bullens, L., Van der Plas, E.A.A., Kijkuit, E J., & Zelazo, P.D. (2008).
Cserjési, R., Molnár, D., Luminet, O., & Lénárd, L. (2007). Is there any relationship
227
Referencias bibliográficas
Dallman, M.F., Pecoraro, N., Akana, S.F., La Fleur, S.E., Gomez, F., Houshyar, H., ...
& Manalo, S. (2003). Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of “comfort food”.
Dallman, M.F., Pecoraro, N.C., & la Fleur, S.E. (2005). Chronic stress and comfort
19, 275-280.
Crítica.
Davids, S., Lauffer, H., Thoms, K., Jagdhuhn, M., Hirschfeld, H., Domin, M., ... &
94.
Davis, C., Patte, K., Curtis, C., & Reid, C. (2010). Immediate pleasures and future
54, 208-213.
Davis, C., Patte, K., Levitan, R., Reid, C., Tweed, S., & Curtis, C. (2007). From
DeBoer, M.D. (2013). Obesity, systemic inflammation, and increased risk for
228
Referencias bibliográficas
Delgado‐Rico, E., Río‐Valle, J.S., González‐Jiménez, E., Campoy, C., & Verdejo‐
DelParigi, A., Pannacciulli, N., Le, D.N., & Tataranni, P.A. (2005). In pursuit of neural
risk factors for weight gain in humans. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, 50-55.
Demos, K.E., Heatherton, T.F., & Kelley, W.M. (2012). Individual differences in
nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and
Eaton, D.K., Lowry, R., Brener, N.D., Galuska, D.A., & Crosby, A.E. (2005).
Associations of body mass index and perceived weight with suicide ideation and
Ellulu, M., Abed, Y., Rahmat, A., Ranneh, Y., & Ali, F. (2014). Epidemiology of
Obesity, 2, 2.
Epel, E., Jimenez, S., Brownell, K., Stroud, L., Stoney, C., & Niaura, R.A.Y. (2004).
Are stress eaters at risk for the metabolic syndrome?. Annals of the New York
229
Referencias bibliográficas
Ernst, M., Nelson, E.E., Jazbec, S., McClure, E.B., Monk, C.S., Leibenluft, E., ... &
Pine, D.S. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and
Ernst, M., Pine, D.S., & Hardin, M. (2006). Triadic model of the neurobiology of
Faith, M. S., Allison, D. B., & Geliebter, A. (1997). Emotional eating and obesity:
Farooqi, I.S., Bullmore, E., Keogh, J., Gillard, J., O'rahilly, S., & Fletcher, P.C. (2007).
Leptin regulates striatal regions and human eating behavior. Science, 317, 1355-
1355.
Flegal, K.M., & Ogden, C.L. (2011). Childhood obesity: are we all speaking the same
166S.
Forcano, L., Mata, F., de la Torre, R., & Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2018). Cognitive and
Reviews.
230
Referencias bibliográficas
Fox, H.C., Talih, M., Malison, R., Anderson, G.M., Kreek, M.J., & Sinha, R. (2005).
Frequency of recent cocaine and alcohol use affects drug craving and associated
891.
Frazier‐Wood, A.C., Carnell, S., Pena, O., Hughes, S.O., O'Connor, T.M., Asherson, P.,
Galvan, A., Hare, T., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B.J. (2007). Risk‐taking and the
Galvan, A., Hare, T.A., Parra, C.E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., & Casey, B. J.
6885-6892.
Gilbert, S.J., & Burgess, P.W. (2008). Executive function. Current Biology, 18, R110-
R114.
Goldberg, A.D., Becker, L.C., Bonsall, R., Cohen, J.D., Ketterer, M.W., Kaufman, P.G.,
... & Pepine, C.J. (1996). Ischemic, hemodynamic, and neurohormonal responses
231
Referencias bibliográficas
2409.
Goran, M.I., Ball, G.D., & Cruz, M.L. (2003). Obesity and risk of type 2 diabetes and
Gottfried, J.A., O'doherty, J., & Dolan, R.J. (2003). Encoding predictive reward value in
Griffiths, L.J., Wolke, D., Page, A.S., & Horwood, J.P. (2006). Obesity and bullying:
different effects for boys and girls. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 91, 121-
125.
Guo, S.S., Wu, W., Chumlea, W.C., & Roche, A.F. (2002). Predicting overweight and
obesity in adulthood from body mass index values in childhood and adolescence.
Haber, S.N., & Knutson, B. (2010). The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and
Haines, J., Neumark‐Sztainer, D., Hannan, P. J., Berg, P., & Eisenberg, M.E. (2008).
Obesity, 16.
Hasler, G., Buysse, D.J., Klaghofer, R., Gamma, A., Ajdacic, V., Eich, D., ... & Angst,
J. (2004). The association between short sleep duration and obesity in young
232
Referencias bibliográficas
Hayden‐Wade, H.A., Stein, R.I., Ghaderi, A., Saelens, B.E., Zabinski, M.F., & Wilfley,
Hebebrand, J., Albayrak, Ö., Adan, R., Antel, J., Dieguez, C., de Jong, J., ... & van der
47, 295-306.
Heinz, A., Siessmeier, T., Wrase, J., Hermann, D., Klein, S., Grüsser-Sinopoli, S.M., ...
the ventral striatum and central processing of alcohol cues and craving.
Hopko, D.R., Lejuez, C.W., Daughters, S.B., Aklin, W.M., Osborne, A., Simmons,
B.L., & Strong, D.R. (2006). Construct validity of the balloon analogue risk task
Hou, R., Mogg, K., Bradley, B.P., Moss-Morris, R., Peveler, R., & Roefs, A. (2011).
External eating, impulsivity and attentional bias to food cues. Appetite, 56, 424-
427.
University Press.
Ihssen, N., Sokunbi, M.O., Lawrence, A.D., Lawrence, N.S., & Linden, D.E. (2017).
233
Referencias bibliográficas
Jääskeläinen, A., Nevanperä, N., Remes, J., Rahkonen, F., Järvelin, M.R., & Laitinen, J.
14, 321.
Janssen, I., Craig, W.M., Boyce, W.F., & Pickett, W. (2004). Associations between
Jastreboff, A.M., Potenza, M.N., Lacadie, C., Hong, K.A., Sherwin, R.S., & Sinha, R.
(2011). Body mass index, metabolic factors, and striatal activation during
Kamijo, K., Khan, N.A., Pontifex, M.B., Scudder, M.R., Drollette, E.S., Raine, L.B., ...
& Hillman, C.H. (2012). The relation of adiposity to cognitive control and
Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M.B., Khan, N.A., Raine, L.B., Scudder, M.R., Drollette, E.S., ...
Kelley, A.E., & Berridge, K.C. (2002). The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance
Kelley, A.E., Baldo, B.A., Pratt, W.E., & Will, M.J. (2005). Corticostriatal-
234
Referencias bibliográficas
Kim, M.S., Pak, Y.K., Jang, P.G., Namkoong, C., Choi, Y S., Won, J.C., ... & Kim, Y
Kirschbaum, C., Prussner, J.C., Stone, A.A., Federenko, I., Gaab, J., Lintz, D., ... &
Klesges, L.M., Baranowski, T., Beech, B., Cullen, K., Murray, D.M., Rochon, J., &
results from the Girls Health Enrichment Multisite Studies (GEMS). Preventive
Kohlisch, O., & Schaefer, F. (1996). Physiological changes during computer tasks:
Kringelbach, M.L., O’Doherty, J., Rolls, E.T., & Andrews, C. (2003). Activation of the
235
Referencias bibliográficas
Kubzansky, L.D., Bordelois, P., Jun, H.J., Roberts, A.L., Cerda, M., Bluestone, N., &
Kuhmann, W., Boucsein, W., Schaefer, F., & Alexander, J. (1987). Experimental
943.
Laitinen, J., Ek, E., & Sovio, U. (2002). Stress-related eating and drinking behavior and
body mass index and predictors of this behavior. Preventive Medicine, 34, 29-
39.
Lawrence, N.S., Hinton, E.C., Parkinson, J.A., & Lawrence, A.D. (2012). Nucleus
women and increased body mass index in those with reduced self-control.
Lawyer, S.R. (2013). Risk taking for sexual versus monetary outcomes using the
Lejuez, C.W., Aklin, W.M., Jones, H.A., Richards, J.B., Strong, D.R., Kahler, C.W., &
Read, J.P. (2003). The balloon analogue risk task (BART) differentiates smokers
236
Referencias bibliográficas
Lejuez, C.W., Aklin, W.M., Zvolensky, M.J., & Pedulla, C.M. (2003). Evaluation of the
Lejuez, C.W., Bornovalova, M.A., Daughters, S.B., & Curtin, J.J. (2005). Differences in
impulsivity and sexual risk behavior among inner-city crack/cocaine users and
Lejuez, C.W., Read, J.P., Kahler, C.W., Richards, J.B., Ramsey, S.E., Stuart, G.L., ... &
Applied, 8, 75.
Lemmens, S.G., Rutters, F., Born, J.M., & Westerterp-Plantenga, M.S. (2011). Stress
Lobstein, T., Baur, L., & Uauy, R. (2004). Obesity in children and young people: a
Locke, A.E., Kahali, B., Berndt, S.I., Justice, A.E., Pers, T.H., Day, F.R., ... & Croteau-
Chonka, D.C. (2015). Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for
Lokken, K.L., Boeka, A.G., Austin, H.M., Gunstad, J., & Harmon, C.M. (2009).
Lowe, M.R., & Fisher, E.B. (1983). Emotional reactivity, emotional eating, and obesity:
237
Referencias bibliográficas
Lu, Q., Tao, F., Hou, F., Zhang, Z., Sun, Y., Xu, Y., ... & Zhao, Y. (2014). Cortisol
reactivity, delay discounting and percent body fat in Chinese urban young
Maayan, L., Hoogendoorn, C., Sweat, V., & Convit, A. (2011). Disinhibited eating in
Marrero, A.F., al'Absi, M., Pincomb, G.A., & Lovallo, W.R. (1997). Men at risk for
hypertension show elevated vascular resistance at rest and during mental stress.
Martinez, D., Broft, A., Foltin, R.W., Slifstein, M., Hwang, D.R., Huang, Y., ... &
Martinez-Gomez, D., Moreno, L.A., Romeo, J., Rey-López, J.P., Castillo, R., Cabero,
M.J., ... & AVENA Study Group. (2011). Combined influence of lifestyle risk
105-111.
Millar, L., Rowland, B., Nichols, M., Swinburn, B., Bennett, C., Skouteris, H., &
beverage and high fat food consumption among children. Obesity, 22.
Miller, A.L., Lumeng, J.C., & LeBourgeois, M.K. (2015). Sleep patterns and obesity in
238
Referencias bibliográficas
Miller, J.W., Naimi, T.S., Brewer, R.D., & Jones, S.E. (2007). Binge drinking and
associated health risk behaviors among high school students. Pediatrics, 119,
76-85.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N.P., Emerson, M.J., Witzki, A.H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T.D.
(2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to
41, 49-100.
Montagu, J.D. (1963). Habituation of the psycho-galvanic reflex during serial tests.
Moya, S., & Salvador, A. (2001). Respuesta cardíaca y electrodérmica ante estresores
Murdaugh, D.L., Cox, J.E., Cook III, E.W., & Weller, R.E. (2012). fMRI reactivity to
Nederkoorn, C., Braet, C., Van Eijs, Y., Tanghe, A., & Jansen, A. (2006). Why obese
children cannot resist food: the role of impulsivity. Eating Behaviors, 7, 315-
322.
Nederkoorn, C., Houben, K., Hofmann, W., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2010). Control
yourself or just eat what you like? Weight gain over a year is predicted by an
interactive effect of response inhibition and implicit preference for snack foods.
239
Referencias bibliográficas
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Falkner, N., Story, M., Perry, C., Hannan, P.J., & Mulert, S.
O'Doherty, J.P., Deichmann, R., Critchley, H.D., & Dolan, R.J. (2002). Neural
Ogawa, S., Menon, R.S., Tank, D.W., Kim, S.G., Merkle, H., Ellermann, J.M., &
Oliver, G., & Wardle, J. (1999). Perceived effects of stress on food choice. Physiology
Passamonti, L., Rowe, J.B., Schwarzbauer, C., Ewbank, M.P., Von Dem Hagen, E., &
appetizing foods: the neural basis of a risk factor for overeating. Journal of
Pauli-Pott, U., Albayrak, Ö., Hebebrand, J., & Pott, W. (2010). Association between
inhibitory control capacity and body weight in overweight and obese children
Pearce, M.J., Boergers, J., & Prinstein, M.J. (2002). Adolescent obesity, overt and
240
Referencias bibliográficas
Pérez-Rodrigo, Bartrina, A., Majem, S., Moreno, & Rubio, D. (2006). Epidemiology of
Peters, M.L., Godaert, G.L., Ballieux, R.E., van Vliet, M., Willemsen, J.J., Sweep, F.C.,
23, 1-17.
Pliner, P., & Mann, N. (2004). Influence of social norms and palatability on amount
food choice, physical activity and energy balance. Physiology & Behavior, 86,
603-613.
Puhl, R., & Brownell, K.D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity. Obesity, 9, 788-
805.
Puhl, R.M., & Heuer, C.A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity,
17, 941-964.
the school setting: Emotional reactions and coping behaviors. Journal of Youth
Puhl, R.M., Luedicke, J., & Heuer, C. (2011). Weight‐based victimization toward
241
Referencias bibliográficas
Rada, P., Avena, N.M., & Hoebel, B.G. (2005). Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly
Rich, S.S., Essery, E.V., Sanborn, C.F., DiMarco, N.M., Morales, L.K., & LeClere,
Richardson, S.A., Goodman, N., Hastorf, A.H., & Dornbusch, S.M. (1961). Cultural
241-247.
Riggs, N.R., Huh, J., Chou, C.P., Spruijt-Metz, D., & Pentz, M.A. (2012). Executive
Riggs, N.R., Spruijt-Metz, D., Chou, C.P., & Pentz, M.A. (2012). Relationships
between executive cognitive function and lifetime substance use and obesity-
Rolls, E. (2008). Functions of the orbitofrontal and pregenual cingulate cortex in taste,
Rolls, E. T. (2005). Taste, olfactory, and food texture processing in the brain, and the
242
Referencias bibliográficas
Rothemund, Y., Preuschhof, C., Bohner, G., Bauknecht, H.C., Klingebiel, R., Flor, H.,
& Klapp, B.F. (2007). Differential activation of the dorsal striatum by high-
Rouach, V., Bloch, M., Rosenberg, N., Gilad, S., Limor, R., Stern, N., & Greenman, Y.
(2007). The acute ghrelin response to a psychological stress challenge does not
Rydén, A., Sullivan, M., Torgerson, J.S., Karlsson, J., Lindroos, A.K., & Taft, C.
Sahoo, K., Sahoo, B., Choudhury, A.K., Sofi, N.Y., Kumar, R., & Bhadoria, A.S.
Sapolsky, R.M., Krey, L.C., & McEwen, B.S. (2002). The neuroendocrinology of stress
and aging: the glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Science's SAGE KE, 2002, 21.
Scharmüller, W., Übel, S., Ebner, F., & Schienle, A. (2012). Appetite regulation during
Sidle, D.A., Lipp, O.V., & Dall, P.J. (1996). The effects of task type and task
Singh, A.S., Mulder, C., Twisk, J.W., Van Mechelen, W., & Chinapaw, M.J. (2008).
243
Referencias bibliográficas
Sloan, R.P., DeMeersman, R.E., Shapiro, P.A., Bagiella, E., Kuhl, J.P., Zion, A.S., ... &
Small, D.M., Veldhuizen, M.G., Felsted, J., Mak, Y.E., & McGlone, F. (2008).
Spear, L.P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations.
Stein, R.I., Kenardy, J., Wiseman, C.V., Dounchis, J.Z., Arnow, B.A., & Wilfley, D.E.
Steinberg, L. (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: what changes, and why?. Annals of the
(Eds.), Stress and Medical Procedures (pp. 1-24). Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
244
Referencias bibliográficas
Steptoe, A., Cropley, M., & Joekes, K. (1999). Job strain, blood pressure and response
Stice, E., Figlewicz, D.P., Gosnell, B.A., Levine, A.S., & Pratt, W.E. (2013). The
Stice, E., Spoor, S., Bohon, C., Veldhuizen, M.G., & Small, D.M. (2008). Relation of
reward from food intake and anticipated food intake to obesity: a functional
Stoeckel, L.E., Weller, R.E., Cook III, E.W., Twieg, D.B., Knowlton, R. C., & Cox, J.E.
Stone, A.A., & Brownell, K.D. (1994). The stress-eating paradox: multiple daily
Striegel‐Moore, R.H., Cachelin, F.M., Dohm, F.A., Pike, K.M., Wilfley, D.E., &
Fairburn, C.G. (2001). Comparison of binge eating disorder and bulimia nervosa
Sturm, R. (2002). The effects of obesity, smoking, and drinking on medical problems
Stuss, D.T., & Levine, B. (2002). Adult clinical neuropsychology: lessons from studies
Swain, A., & Suls, J. (1996). Reproducibility of blood pressure and heart rate reactivity:
245
Referencias bibliográficas
Thamotharan, S., Lange, K., Zale, E.L., Huffhines, L., & Fields, S. (2013). The role of
Therrien, F., Drapeau, V., Lalonde, J., Lupien, S.J., Beaulieu, S., Tremblay, A., &
obese individuals: effect of gender and fat distribution. Obesity, 15, 377-385.
Tomiyama, A.J., Dallman, M.F., & Epel, E.S. (2011). Comfort food is comforting to
those most stressed: evidence of the chronic stress response network in high
Torres, S.J., & Nowson, C.A. (2007). Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and
Uher, R., Yoganathan, D., Mogg, A., Eranti, S.V., Treasure, J., Campbell, I.C., ... &
Van den Berg, L., Pieterse, K., Malik, J.A., Luman, M., Van Dijk, K.W., Oosterlaan, J.,
Van Leijenhorst, L., Zanolie, K., Van Meel, C.S., Westenberg, P.M., Rombouts, S.A.,
& Crone, E.A. (2009). What motivates the adolescent? Brain regions mediating
Van Strien, T., Roelofs, K., & de Weerth, C. (2013). Cortisol reactivity and distress-
246
Referencias bibliográficas
Psicothema.
Verdejo-García, A., Lozano, Ó., Moya, M., Alcázar, M.Á., & Pérez-García, M. (2010).
scale: reliability, validity and association with trait and cognitive impulsivity.
Social stress increases cortisol and hampers attention in adolescents with excess
M.J., Cruz, F., Pérez‐García, M., ... & Campoy, C. (2010). Selective alterations
within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity, 18, 1572-
1578.
Volkow, N., Wang, G.J., Fowler, J.S., Tomasi, D., & Baler, R. (2011). Food and drug
Volkow, N.D., & Wise, R.A. (2005). How can drug addiction help us understand
247
Referencias bibliográficas
Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.J., Fowler, J.S., & Telang, F. (2008). Overlapping neuronal
Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.J., Fowler, J.S., Logan, J., Gatley, S.J., Hitzemann, R., ... &
Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.J., Fowler, J.S., Logan, J., Hitzemann, R., Ding, Y.S., ... &
1594-1598.
Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.J., Tomasi, D., & Baler, R.D. (2013). Obesity and addiction:
Volkow, N.D., Wang, G.J., Tomasi, D., & Baler, R.D. (2013). The addictive
Waber, D.P., De Moor, C., Forbes, P.W., Almli, C.R., Botteron, K.N., Leonard, G., ... &
Brain Development Cooperative Group. (2007). The NIH MRI study of normal
Wardle, J., & Gibson, E.L. (2002). Impact of stress on diet: processes and implications.
BMJ Books.
Clinical Management.
248
Referencias bibliográficas
Whitaker, R.C., Wright, J.A., Pepe, M.S., Seidel, K.D., & Dietz, W.H. (1997).
Predicting obesity in young adulthood from childhood and parental obesity. New
Wieland, B.A., & Mefferd, R.B. (1970). Systematic changes in levels of physiological
<http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs311/en/index.html>.
do/policy-prevention/>.
Yokum, S., Ng, J., & Stice, E. (2011). Attentional bias to food images associated with
elevated weight and future weight gain: an fMRI study. Obesity, 19, 1775-1783.
Zheng, H., & Berthoud, H.R. (2007). Eating for pleasure or calories. Current Opinion in
Pharmacology, 7, 607-612.
Zheng, H., Lenard, N.R., Shin, A.C., & Berthoud, H.R. (2009). Appetite control and
Ziauddeen, H., & Fletcher, P.C. (2013). Is food addiction a valid and useful concept?.
249
VII. ANEXOS
ARTÍCULOS PUBLICADOS
250
251
RESEARCH ARTICLE
a11111
Abstract
Objective
To experimentally examine if adolescents with excess weight are more sensitive to social
stress and hence more sensitive to harmful effects of stress in cognition.
OPEN ACCESS
Introduction
Adolescents with excess weight suffer substantial social stress including frequent peer bullying
and social marginalization and exclusion [1,2]. Crucially, the degree of exposure to these social
stressors is the most important predictor of poor psychological adjustment and poor academic
achievement in adolescents with obesity [3]. Moreover, neuroendocrine studies have shown
that non-fasting levels of the “hunger hormone” ghrelin increase in response to social stressors
(i.e., the Trier Social Stress Task, involving a public speak) [4] and that the awakening response
of the “stress hormone” cortisol positively associates with subsequent lipid intake [5]. There-
fore, social stress is a potent determinant of poor cognition and poor food choices in adoles-
cents with excess weight. This phenomenon could be explained by the harmful impact of social
stress on cognitive skills such as attention, cognitive control and decision-making, which con-
tribute to obesity-related behaviours in adolescents [6]. The harmful impact of persistent social
stressors on cognition in adolescents with obesity is likely to be enduring as stress induces neu-
roadaptations in prefrontal and limbic regions particularly during adolescence [7,8]. Therefore,
examining whether social stress hampers cognition in adolescents with excess weight is essen-
tial for prevention of cognitive decline and hence progression of obesity. However, to date no
studies have experimentally assessed this notion. In this study we examined if a social stressor-
the Trier public speaking stress task- specifically increases cortisol levels and hampers cognitive
performance in adolescents with excess weight compared to adolescents with normal weight.
We specifically assessed the impact of social stress on outcome measures of attention, cognitive
inhibition and decision-making. We selected these outcomes because they reflect the function
of frontal-limbic systems [9,10] and are longitudinally associated with weight gain in pediatric
populations [11,12]. We hypothesized that adolescents with excess weight would show greater
cortisol response to the social stressor, and greater detrimental impact of social stress on atten-
tion and decision-making performance.
Methods
Participants
Eighty-four adolescents aged between 12 and 18 years old participated in the study. They were
classified in two groups (Normal weight [n = 42] and Excess weight [n = 42]) based on their
age adjusted Body Mass Index (BMI) percentile [13]. Sample size was estimated through power
analysis. The existing evidence about the impact of the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST) on se-
lected outcome variables was correlational (i.e., the association between TSST-induced cortisol
changes and decision-making performance is between 0.3 and 0.4) [14,15]. Therefore, we esti-
mated that in order to achieve adequate power (80%) to detect a ρH1 = 0.3 association between
the independent variable (stress) and the cognitive outcomes (attention and decision-making)
84 participants would be required (S1 Fig). This sample size was deemed acceptable for the
mixed repeated-measures design. The classification of the two groups was conducted in align-
ment with the guidelines of the International Obesity Task Force and the Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention: Normal weight participants had age adjusted BMI percentiles in the
range between the 5th and the 84th percentile, and Excess weight participants had age adjusted
BMI percentiles 85 (Table 1). Three participants from the Excess weight group provided in-
valid cortisol samples, and therefore the final study sample comprised 42 Normal weight and
39 Excess weight participants. Participants’ socio-demographic characteristics, BMIs, percent-
age fat and blood count obtained biochemical parameters are as well displayed in Table 1. Par-
ticipants also completed The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire [16] which was used to
Table 1. Descriptive scores for the demographic, biometric and blood count characteristics of adolescents with excess and normal weight.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0123565.t001
characterise psychological traits relevant to maladaptive eating behaviours (i.e., external eating,
emotional eating and restraint) (Table 1).
Participants were recruited from the paediatrics and endocrinology services of the Hospital
“Virgen de las Nieves” in Granada (Spain), and from schools located in the same geographical
area. The inclusion criteria for participants were defined as follows: (i) age range between 12
and 18 years old; (ii) BMI percentiles falling within the intervals categorized as overweight or
obesity (85—Excess weight group), or normal weight (5–85—Normal weight group); and
(iii) absence of history or current evidence of neurological or psychiatric disorders, assessed by
participants and parents interviews and the Eating Disorder Inventory [17]. All participants
had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.
Experimental procedures
Fig 1 displays a schematic representation of the experiment. In order to induce social stress in
the laboratory we utilised a previously validated Virtual Reality version of the Trier Social
Stress Task (TSST) [18]. Participants had to perform a stressing task which consisted of deliv-
ering a speech about personal characteristics including both positive and negative aspects of
themselves in front of a simulated audience. Participants were told that this audience would at-
tend the speech and subsequently evaluate its quality. However, the virtual audience was pro-
grammed to look progressively bored and disappointed with the speech. The speech was
followed by a mental calculation test (serially subtracting 17, starting from 2013). Cortisol lev-
els were measured via saliva samples collected before onset of the TSST (T1), after completion
of the TSST and the calculation test (10 minutes after TSST onset—T2) and after performance
on each of the attention and decision making cognitive probes (20 and 30 minutes after TSST
onset-T3 and T4- respectively). Cognitive measures were conducted in a fixed order before
TSST onset (pre-TSST, overlapping with T1) and after completion of the TSST and the
calculation test (post-TSST, overlapping with T2). To minimize practice effects, we utilised
parallel versions of all tasks in the post-TSST administration. The original validation study
showed that this virtual reality TSST is able to induce modest but sizeable increases in cortisol
and subjective stress responses [18]. Moreover the virtual audience tamed the ethical concerns
associated with the negative impact of the social stressor on adolescents’ participants. The Eth-
ics Committee for Human Research of the Universidad de Granada approved the study. Both
participants and parents signed informed consent.
Cortisol measurement. Participants were told not to smoke, eat or drink coffee for at least
30 minutes before the experiment. All the experimental sessions were conducted at the same
time of the day (4–5 pm) based on pilot data obtained in this cohort prior to study onset indi-
cating that diurnal cortisol levels were stable during these hours. Saliva was collected via a com-
mercially available device: Salivette Cortisol (Sarstedt, Numbrecht, Germany). This device is
composed of a cotton tube (similar to dental cotton), and two plastic tubes that fit one inside
the other. Subjects were told to place cotton salivettes inside their mouth and gently chew and/
or suck on them for 1–3 min until they became soaked in saliva. The cotton tube was inserted
inside the plastic tube, which was then capped. Saliva samples were stored at -20°C until re-
quired for assay. Samples were analyzed at the University Hospital, using the electrochemilu-
minescence immunoassay “ECLIA” method. This method is designed for use in Roche
Elecsys 1010/2010 automated analyzers and in the Elecsys MODULAR NALYTICS E170 mod-
ule. We computed two different metrics from each cortisol sample (microgram/deciliter and
nanomol/liter). The correlation between both metrics at the different time points ranged from
0.8 and 0.9.
Cognitive measures. We utilized three computerized tests: two subtests from the Cam-
bridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) [19], Motor Screening (MOT)
and Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP), and the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) [20]. Al-
ternate versions of each test were used in pre-stress and post-stress administrations.
MOT. The main objective of this test is to provide a baseline measure of the subjects’ basic
motor skills in terms of reaction times and accuracy. After a demonstration of the correct way
to point on the computer screen using the forefinger of the dominant hand, the subjects must
point to a series of stimuli (crosses) popping up in turn. The outcome measure of this test was
response latency.
RVP. This is a test of visual sustained attention with an impulse control component. A
white box is displayed in the centre of the computer screen, inside which digits, from 2 to 9, are
displayed in a pseudo-random order, at the rate of 100 digits per minute. The subject must de-
tect consecutive odd or even sequences of digits (for example, 2-4-6) and respond by pressing
the touch pad. The outcome measures of this test were response latency and response
discriminability (B’) scores, which are sensitive to attention and impulse control domains re-
spectively. The B’ score is the signal detection measure of the strength of trace required to elicit
a response (range -1.00 to +1.00). Thus, it is the tendency to respond regardless of whether the
target sequence is present and uses the p(hit) and p(fa) results. A score close to +1.00 indicates
that the subject gave few false alarms.
IGT. This is a computer task measuring reward/punishment based decision-making. It in-
volves four decks of cards (A, B, C and D). Each time a participant selects a card, a specified
amount of play money is awarded. However, interspersed among these rewards, there are prob-
abilistic punishments (monetary losses). Two of the decks of cards (A and B) produce high im-
mediate gains; however, in the long run, they will take more money than they give, and are
thus considered disadvantageous. The other two decks (C and D) are considered advantageous,
as they result in small, immediate gains, but will yield more money than they take in the long
run. The performance measure was the net score calculated by subtracting the number of dis-
advantageous choices (decks A and B) from the number of advantageous choices (decks C and
D). An equivalent parallel version of the ABCD task in which decks are labelled K, L, M and N
was utilised in the post-TSST administration. These versions have shown adequate test-retest
reliability and ecological validity in relation to decision-making [21].
Visual Analogue Scales (VAS). We used two Visual Analogue Scales (VAS) designed to
rate arousal and stress. For arousal scale the individual must indicate the extent to which they
perceived as active and alert (from nothing active to very active). For stress scale they must in-
dicate how much stress they feel (from no stress to very much stress). We used the mean scores
of each dimension.
Statistical analyses
The main hypotheses were examined utilizing mixed repeated measures analyses of variance
including Time as the repeated-measures factor, Group as the between-groups factor, and cor-
tisol levels (as measured in μg/dl) and RVP’s mean response latency and B’ scores and IGT’s
net scores as dependent measures. Cortisol and RVP performance measures were log-trans-
formed (base 10) to meet the normal distribution, but for the sake of clarity the Figures report
non-transformed measures. IGT scores fitted to the normal distribution as assessed by Kolgo-
morov-Smirnov tests. We also performed correlation analyses between change scores of corti-
sol levels (T2—T1) and change scores of cognitive performance (T2—T1) and between both
change scores and biological and psychological measures. These change measures were non-
normally distributed and therefore we applied Spearman’s rank correlation analyses. Two par-
ticipants from the Excess weight group (n = 37) and one participant from the Normal weight
group had missing cortisol data at T1 and T2 (n = 41). With regard to cognitive tests, there was
no missing data in the Excess weight group (n = 39), whereas in the Normal weight group
three participants had invalid data for RVP response latency and IGT (n = 39) at T1 or T2, and
three participants had invalid data for RVP B’ (n = 38) at T1 or T2.
Results
Cortisol response
We found a significant Time x Group interaction on cortisol levels, F (3,74) = 4.36, p = 0.008.
Cortisol mildly increased in Excess weight participants after the TSST. Independent-sample t-
tests showed that Excess weight and Normal weight participants did not significantly differ on
cortisol levels before TSST (T1). However, Excess weight adolescents showed significantly in-
creased cortisol levels after TSST (T2), t = 1.94, p = 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.5 (Fig 2). Moreover,
cortisol increase between T2 and T1 correlated with amount of fat, Spearman’s Rho = 0.30,
Fig 2. Cortisol levels (μg/dl units) in adolescents with excess weight and adolescents with normal
weight before and after exposure to the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST). T1 represents cortisol levels
before TSST; T2 represents cortisol levels immediately after TSST termination; T3 and T4 represents cortisol
levels 10 and 20 minutes after TSST termination.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0123565.g002
p = 0.01. Between-group differences were also statistically significant at T3, t = 2.44, p = 0.02,
and T4, t = 2.63, p = 0.01. However, this effect seems to be driven by decreased cortisol levels in
the Normal weight group (Fig 2).
Cognitive performance
MOT. Pre-TSST scores showed that both groups had similar baseline response latencies.
Further, both groups showed mild reductions of response latencies between the pre-TSST mea-
sure and the post-TSST measure (Fig 3).
RVP—Response latency. We found a significant Time x Group interaction, F (1,76) =
6.35, p = 0.01 (Fig 3). Independent-sample t-tests showed that Excess weight and Normal
weight participants did not significantly differ in the pre-TSST measure. However, they showed
marginally significant differences in the post-TSST measure, t (78) = 1.75, p = 0.08, Cohen’s
d = 0.4, with Excess weight participants performing significantly poorer than Normal weight
controls. There was no significant correlation between T2—T1 cortisol levels and T2—T1 RVP
Response Latency.
RVP—Response discriminability. We did not find a significant Time x Group interac-
tion, F (1,75) = 0.99, p = 0.32. There were no main effects of Time or Group, although visual in-
spection shows Excess weight participants performed better than Normal weight participants
in both pre- and post-TSST measures (Fig 3).
Decision-making—IGT. We did not find a significant Time x Group interaction, F (1,77)
= 0.005, p = 0.94. There was a significant main effect of Time, F (1,77) = 6.01, p = 0.02, indicat-
ing that both groups exhibited significantly poorer performance after the TSST (Fig 3). There
was no significant correlation between T2—T1 cortisol levels and T2—T1 IGT performance.
Correlations between biological and psychological measures and cognitive performance
in T2—T1. We found a positive correlation between levels of uric acid and change in RVSP
response latency performance between T2 and T1, Spearman’s Rho = 0.46, p = 0.0001, and a
negative correlation between thyroxine levels and change in Iowa Gambling Task performance
between T2 and T1, Spearman’s Rho = -0.27, p = 0.03. We also found a negative correlation
Fig 3. Cognitive performance in adolescents with excess weight and adolescents with normal weight
before and after exposure to the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST). Top panel Y axes represent time in
milliseconds. The Y axis in the bottom-left panel represents signal detection derived Beta scores, ranging
from 0 to 1. The Y axis in the bottom-right panel represents Iowa Gambling Task net scores, ranging from -60
to +60.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0123565.g003
between scores of external eating and RVSP response latency performance between T2 and T1,
Spearman’s Rho = -0.27, p = 0.02.
Visual Analogue Scales (VAS). We did not find a significant Time x Group interactions
on VAS of arousal or stress but results were in the expected direction, with both groups show-
ing more subjective arousal and stress after the TSST (S2 Fig).
Post-hoc analyses in the subsample of participants showing enhanced cortisol re-
sponse. The primary analyses indicated that in the normal weight group cortisol levels did
not change after stress, and therefore there is a concern that cognitive changes were due to spu-
rious factors. To address this issue, we run additional analyses in the subsample of participants
who showed sizeable increments in cortisol levels after stress, including 24 participants of the
Excess weight group (57% of the original sample) and 20 participants of the Normal weight
group (48% of the original sample). The results of these analyses were coherent with the main
findings. We found a significant Time x Group interaction on RVP’s latency scores, F (1,41) =
6.17, p = 0.02, whereby a drop in performance was only observed in the Excess weight group
(See S2 Fig). Moreover, there was a significant correlation between T2—T1 cortisol levels and
T2—T1 RVP Response Latency (Spearman’s Rho = 0.25, punilateral = 0.05) (Fig 4).
Discussion
We show that social stress specifically increases cortisol levels and hinders attentional response
latency in adolescents with excess weight. Conversely, social stress failed to show significant ef-
fects on attention response discriminability. Moreover, both excess weight and normal weight
adolescents displayed poorer decision-making performance after the social stressor. These
Fig 4. Correlation between between T2—T1 cortisol levels (X Axis) and T2—T1 RVP Response Latency (Y Axis) within the subsample of
participants showing TSST-induced increases in cortisol levels.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0123565.g004
findings indicate that adolescents who are overweight and obese have enhanced stress reactivity
in response to social stressors, which selectively impacts on attentional skills. Since adolescents
with excess weight are markedly exposed to social stressors during everyday lives, our findings
suggest that stress immunization strategies should be put in place to prevent the harmful im-
pact of social stress on cognition and therefore on progression of obesity.
In agreement with our primary hypothesis, social stress induced greater cortisol response in
overweight and obese adolescents. The effect was mild but the specific impact on participants
with excess weight agrees with the notion that repetitive social stress may induce sensitization
of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis [22] and purportedly of the HPA axis associ-
ations with fronto-limbic systems [23–25]. The discrepancy between our finding of cortisol in-
crease and a previous negative finding in obese adults [26] suggests that adolescence compared
to adulthood is a more sensitive time period for abnormal sensitization of stress systems, likely
due to ongoing neural maturation of these systems [7,27]. Further, both preclinical and clinical
evidence shows that social stressors such as social evaluation and social exclusion are particu-
larly challenging for adolescents [8,28,29]. The potential mechanisms for the specific impact of
social stress on stress reactivity in adolescents with excess weight include the additive or syner-
gistic interactions between social stress and inflammation [30,31] and/or between social stress
and obesity-related neuroadaptations in anterior cingulate and limbic regions that are essential
for stress regulation [32,33]. Our finding is particularly relevant in view of the significant asso-
ciation between cortisol reactivity and obesity-related behaviours [34,35], and of the emerging
evidence suggesting that high levels of stress can longitudinally predict the progression of obe-
sity [36].
We also showed a significant impact of social stress on attentional performance in adoles-
cents with excess weight. The effect was again mild and pointed to stress-related hindering of
the capacity to get benefit from a repeated administration of the task. Previous findings indicate
that repeated administration of CANTAB attentional tests is associated with significant im-
provements in performance (of at least 0.3 in Cohen’s d effect size) [37], and this is what we ob-
served in the control group. However, excess weight adolescents were unable to get benefitted
from this repeated administration. The effect was specific for attention-related latency adjust-
ments, but not for psychomotor-related reaction times. Therefore, it suggests a detrimental im-
pact of stress on attention regulation.6 This notion is consistent with the neural networks
interactions between the HPA axis and medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex
regions involved in attention regulation [38–40]. In support, neuroimaging studies have shown
that the impact of stress on executive attention is mediated by structural (gray matter) neuroa-
daptations in prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex regions [41]. This stress-related
attentional hurdle has a high translational value, as individual differences in response latencies
to attentional probes are longitudinally associated with increases in BMI [12], implying that ad-
equate control of social stress and/or cognitive boosting of attentional resources may contrib-
ute to prevent chronic obesity. This notion is consistent with our finding of significant
correlations between less improvement of attentional performance (between T1 and T2) and
higher maladaptive eating patterns such as external eating, which reflects attentional bias to-
wards food related cues. Further, both social stress and attentional skills are significantly associ-
ated with advantageous social functioning and academic performance [3], and therefore our
finding highlights the potential benefit of controlling social stress to improve social and career
outcomes in the long-term.
Furthermore, we found poorer decision-making after the social stressor in both adolescents
with excess weight and adolescents with normal weight. Since cortisol levels dropped between
T3 and T4 (the time window of decision-making task performance) it is unlikely that this find-
ing can be attributed to the effects of acute stress. However, it might be attributed to broader ef-
fects of the social stressor, such as the social evaluation context. The latter notion agrees with
previous experimental evidence showing that adolescents make riskier choices than young
adults or adults when they are under social evaluation [42]. The lack of specificity of our result
implies that the impact of social evaluation on decision-making is mediated by neural mecha-
nisms that are similarly sensitized in adolescents regardless of BMI/weight status, or that differ-
ent neural mechanisms mediate a similar impact of social evaluation on decision-making in
excess weight and normal weight adolescents. In favor of the first notion, neuroimaging studies
have shown that the impact of social evaluation on decision-making is mediated by increased
activation of ventral striatal and orbitofrontal regions [43], which are generally sensitized dur-
ing adolescence. In favor of the second notion, we have observed that excess weight and normal
weight adolescents recruit different brain circuitries during the pondering of social decisions
[44]. Future studies are warranted to address this question. In any case, our finding might have
general implications for prevention of obesity during adolescence as we know that adolescents
who are overweight or obese have higher exposure to social evaluations [3] and that subsequent
risky choices are longitudinally associated with weight gain and obesity [11].
We conclude that social stress response is sensitized in adolescents with excess weight, hin-
dering their attentional function. The study has important strengths including the experimen-
tal design, the power-informed sample size, the detailed phenotypic characterization and the
group matching of excess weight and normal weight adolescents, and the objective measure-
ment of stress reactivity with cortisol biomarkers. However, the results should be as well ap-
praised in light of relevant limitations. It is particularly important to stress that unlike the
original TSST [45], the virtual reality TSST was not able to induce significant increases of corti-
sol levels in the control group. We selected this stressor because it was capable of inducing mild
but sizeable stress in the laboratory at the same time that it reduced the ethical implications of
stressing “at risk” obese adolescents [18]. In agreement with this assumption, our results indi-
cate that the stress manipulation was actually more effective in obese adolescents (57% of par-
ticipants showed increased cortisol levels) than in controls (only 48% of participants showed
increased cortisol levels). There are however several factors that may explain the variability in
stress induction, such as degree of belief in the cover story or degree of immersion in the virtual
reality environment, that were not systematically controlled in this study. Therefore, further
studies are warranted to reassess the validity of this virtual reality version, and to replicate our
findings using TSST versions that are able to unequivocally reproduce the original TSST stress
induction. Moreover, in absence of a “no-stress” control condition, we cannot ascertain a caus-
al link between stress and cognitive performance. However, we base our interpretation on pre-
vious evidence showing that improvement (rather than stability or decrease) in performance is
typically expected in “no-stress” repeated administration designs [37]. A related limitation is
the negative finding in relation to cognitive impulsivity. Since mild arousal improves inhibitory
control in adolescents, it is plausible that the mild nature of the stressor fostered cognitive im-
pulsivity increases rather than (expected) decreases after TSST. Future studies are warranted to
address these limitations, to expand on the biological, psychological and socio-economic medi-
ators of the impact of social stress on cognition, and to longitudinally assess the relevance of
this experimental effect on public health indicators of the progression of obesity.
Supporting Information
S1 Fig. Power analysis calculations.
(TIF)
S2 Fig. Visual Analogue Scales for arousal—left panel- and stress—right panel- in excess
and normal weight adolescents before and after the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST).
(TIF)
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: AVG. Performed the experiments: MMP EDR JSRV.
Analyzed the data: AVG. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MGR. Wrote the
paper: AVG MFS FLT.
References
1. Ludwig DS. Childhood obesity—the shape of things to come. N Engl J Med 2007; 357(23): 2325–2327.
PMID: 18057334
2. Strauss RS, Pollack HA. Social marginalization of overweight children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
2003; 157: 746–752. PMID: 12912779
3. Gunnarsdottir T, Njardvik U, Olafsdottir AS, Craighead L, Bjarnason R. Chilhood obesity and co-morbid
problems: effects of Epstein’s family-based behavioral treatment in Icelandic sample. J Eval Clin Pract
2012; 18(2): 465–472. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2010.01603.x PMID: 21210895
4. Rouach V, Bloch M, Rosenberg N, Gilad S, Limor R, Stern N, et al. The acute ghrelin response to a psy-
chological stress challenge does not predict the post-stress urge to eat. Psychoneuroendocrinology
2007; 32(6): 693–702. PMID: 17560728
5. Therrien F, Drapeau V, Lalonde J, Lupien SJ, Beaulieu S, Tremblay A, et al. Awakening cortisol re-
sponse in lean, obese, and reduced obese individuals: effect of gender and fat distribution. Obesity
2007; 15(2): 377–385. PMID: 17299111
6. Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related
behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes 2014; 38(4):494–506. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.142
PMID: 23913029
7. Davidson RJ, McEwen BS. Social influences on neuroplasticity: stress and interventions to promote
well-being. Nat neurosci 2012, 15(5): 689–695. doi: 10.1038/nn.3093 PMID: 22534579
8. Tsai SF, Huang TY, Chang CY, Hsu YC, Chen SJ, Yu L, et al. Social instability stress differentially af-
fects amygdalar neuron adaptations and memory performance in adolescent and adult rats. Front
Behav Neurosci 2014; 8:27. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00027 PMID: 24550802
9. Bechara A. Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive per-
spective. Nat neurosci 2005; 8(11): 1458–1463. PMID: 16251988
10. Chudasama Y, Passetti F, Rhodes SEV, Lopian D, Desai A, Robbins TW. Dissociable aspects of per-
formance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate,
infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity and compulsiv-
ity. Behav Brain Res 2003; 146(1): 105–119.
11. Seeyave DM, Coleman S, Appugliese D, Corwyn RF, Bradley RH, Davidson NS, et al. Ability to delay
gratification at age 4 years and risk of overweight at age 11 years. Arch Pediatr Adoles Med 2009;
163(4): 303–308. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.12 PMID: 19349558
12. Frazier-Wood AC, Carnell S, Pena O, Hughes SO, O’Connor TM, Asherson P, et al. Reaction time but
not response inhibition shares genetic influences with later BMI in 7–10 years-old. Obesity (in press).
13. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut-offs for thinness, overweight
and obesity. Pediatr Obes 2012; 7(4): 284–294. doi: 10.1111/j.2047-6310.2012.00064.x PMID:
22715120
14. Van den Bos R, Harteveld M, Stoop H. Stress and decision-making in humans: performance is related
to cortisol reactivity, albeit differently in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34(10):
1449–1458. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.016 PMID: 19497677
15. Preston SD, Buchanan TW, Stansfield RB, Bechara A. Effects of anticipatory stress on decision making
in a gambling task. Behav Neurosci 2007; 121(2): 257. PMID: 17469915
16. Van Strien T., Frijters JER, Bergers GPA, Defares PB. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire
(DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. International Journal of
Eating Disorders 1986; 5(2): 295–315.
17. Garner DM. EDI 2: inventario de trastornos de la conducta alimentaria: manual. Tea ediciones: Ma-
drid, 1994.
18. Kelly O, Matheson K, Martinez A, Merali Z, Anisman H. Psychosocial stress evoked by a virtual audi-
ence: relation to neuroendocrine activity. CyberPsychol Behav 2007; 10(5): 655–662. PMID:
17927533
19. Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Lawrence AD, McInnes L, et al. A study of perfor-
mance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of nor-
mal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Cambridge
Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998; 4(5): 474–90. PMID:
9745237
20. Bechara A. Iowa Gambling Task. Boca Raton, FL, USA: Psychological Assessment Resources: 2007.
21. Verdejo-Garcia A, Bechara A, Recknor EC, Perez-Garcia M. Decision-making and the Iowa gambling
task: Ecological validity in individuals with substance dependence. Psychol Belg 2006; 46(1/2): 55–78.
22. Butler TR, Ariwodola OJ, Weiner JL. The impact of social isolation on HPA axis function, anxiety-like
behaviors, and ethanol drinking. Front Integr Neurosci 2014; 7:102. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00102
PMID: 24427122
23. Jahn AL, Fox AS, Abercrombie HC, Shelton SE, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ, et al. Subgenual prefrontal
cortex activity predicts individual differences in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity across different
contexts. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67(2): 175–181. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.039 PMID: 19846063
24. Liu J, Chaplin TM, Wang F, Sinha R, Mayes LC, Blumberg HP. Stress reactivity and corticolimbic re-
sponse to emotional faces in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012; 51(3): 304–312.
doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.12.014 PMID: 22365466
25. Root JC, Tuescher O, Cunningham-Bussel A, Pan H, Epstein J, Altemus M, et al. Frontolimbic function
and cortisol reactivity in response to emotional stimuli. Neuroreport 2009; 20(4): 429–434. doi: 10.
1097/WNR.0b013e328326a031 PMID: 19225430
26. Therrien F, Drapeau V, Lalonde J, Lupien SJ, Beaulieu S, Doré J, et al. Cortisol response to the Trier
Social Stress Test in obese and reduced obese individuals. Biol Psychol 2010; 84(2): 325–329. doi:
10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.013 PMID: 20302906
27. Rao U, Hammen C, Ortiz LR, Chen LA, Poland RE. Effects of early and recent adverse experiences on
adrenal response to psychosocial stress in depressed adolescents. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64(6):
521–526. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.012 PMID: 18597740
28. Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Borofsky LA, McNealy K, Pfeifer JH, Dapretto M. Subgenual anterior cin-
gulate responses to peer rejection: a marker of adolescents’ risk for depression. Dev Psychopathol
2011; 23(1): 283–292. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000799 PMID: 21262054
29. Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Pfeifer JH, Dapretto M. Neural responses to witnessing peer rejection after
being socially excluded: fMRI as a window into adolescents' emotional processing. Dev Sci 2013;
16(5): 743–759 doi: 10.1111/desc.12056 PMID: 24033579
30. De Santis AS, Diez Roux AV, Hajat A, Aiello AE, Golden SH, Jenny NS, et al. Associations of salivary
cortisol levels with inflammatory markers: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Psychoneuroen-
docrinology 2012; 37(7): 1009–1018. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.11.009 PMID: 22178583
31. Slavich GM, Way BM, Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE. Neural sensitivity to social rejection is associated
with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci usa 2010; 107(33): 14817–14822.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009164107 PMID: 20679216
32. He Q, Chen C, Dong Q, Xue G, Chen C, Lu ZL, et al. Gray and white matter structures in the midcingu-
late cortex region contribute to body mass index in Chinese young adults. Brain Struct Funct 2013; Oct
22. [Epub ahead of print]
33. Moreno-Lopez L, Soriano-Mas C, Delgado-Rico E, Rio-Valle JS, Verdejo-Garcia A. Brain structural cor-
relates of reward sensitive and impulsivity in adolescents with normal and excess weight. Plos One
2012; 7(11):e49185. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049185 PMID: 23185306
34. Lu Q, Tao F, Hou F, Zhang Z, Sun Y, Xu Y, et al. Cortisol reactivity, delay discounting and percent body
fat in Chinese urban young adolescents. Appetite 2014; 72: 13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.09.019
PMID: 24080189
35. Van Strien T, Roelofs K, de Weerth C. Cortisol reactivity and distress-induced emotional eating. Psy-
choneuroendocrinology 2013; 38(5): 677–684. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.08.008 PMID: 22999262
36. Kubzansky LD, Bordelois P, Jun HJ, Roberts AL, Cerda M, Bluestone N, et al. The weight of traumatic
stress: A prospective study of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and weight status in women.
JAMA Psychiatry 2014; 71(1); 44–51. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2798 PMID: 24258147
37. Lowe C., Rabbitt P. (1998). Test\ re-test reliability of the CANTAB and ISPOCD neuropsychological
batteries: theoretical and practical issues.Neuropsychologia, 36(9), 915–923. PMID: 9740364
38. Baeken C, Vanderhasselt MA, De Raedt R. Baseline ‘state anxiety’influences HPA-axis sensitivity to
one sham-controlled HF-rTMS session applied to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Psychoneur-
oendocrinology 2011; 36(1): 60–67. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.06.006 PMID: 20599325
39. Kiem SA, Andrade KC, Spoormaker VI, Holsboer F, Czisch M, Sämann PG. Resting state functional
MRI connectivity predicts hypothalamus-pituitary-axis status in healthy males. Psychoneuroendocrinol-
ogy 2013; 38(8): 1338–1348. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.11.021 PMID: 23279846
40. Thomason ME, Hamilton JP, Gotlib IH. Stress-induced activation of the HPA axis predicts connectivity
between subgenual cingulate and salience network during rest in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychi-
atry 2011; 52(10): 1026–1034. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02422.x PMID: 21644985
41. Hanson JL, Chung MK, Avants BB, Rudolph KD, Shirtcliff EA, Gee JC, et al. Structural variations in pre-
frontal cortex mediate the relationship between early childhood stress and spatial working memory. J
Neurosci 2012; 32(23): 7917–7925. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0307-12.2012 PMID: 22674267
42. Gardner M, Steinberg L. Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in ad-
olescence and adulthood: an experimental study. Dev Psychol 2005; 41(4): 625. PMID: 16060809
43. Chein J, Albert D, O’Brien L, Uckert K, Steinberg L. Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing
activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Dev Sci 2011; 14(2): F1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.
01035.x PMID: 21499511
44. Verdejo-Garcia A, Verdejo-Roman J, Lagos FM, Lacomba JA, Schmidt-Rio-Valle J, Soriano-Mas C.
Dysfunctional involvement of emotion and reward brain regions on social decision-making in excess
weight adolescents. Hum Brain Mapp (in press).
45. Kirschbaum C., Pirke K. M., Hellhammer D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investi-
gating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–2),
76–81. PMID: 8272203
Abstract
Background Adolescents with excess weight suffer social deterioration of executive functioning in adolescents
stress more frequently than their peers with normal with excess weight. Evoked increases in subjective stress
weight. and autonomic responses predicted decreased executive
Purpose To examine the impact of social stress, specifi- function. Deficits in executive skills could reduce cogni-
cally negative social evaluation, on executive functions in tive control abilities and lead to overeating in adolescents
adolescents with excess weight. We also examined asso- with excess weight. Strategies to cope with social stress
ciations between subjective stress, autonomic reactivity, to prevent executive deficits could be useful to prevent
and executive functioning. future obesity in this population.
Methods Sixty adolescents (aged 13–18 years) classified
into excess weight or normal weight groups participated. Keywords Obesity • Adolescence • Social stress
We assessed executive functioning (working memory, • Executive functions • Autonomic reactivity
inhibition, and shifting) and subjective stress levels
before and after the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST). The
TSST was divided into two phases according to the feed- Introduction
back of the audience: positive and negative social evalu-
ation. Heart rate and skin conductance were recorded. Overweight and obesity in adolescence have sharply
Results Adolescents with excess weight showed poorer increased over recent decades, reaching epidemic levels [1].
executive functioning after exposure to TSST compared The socioeconomic changes that have occurred in recent
with adolescents with normal weight. Subjective stress decades in Western societies, associated with the unlim-
and autonomic reactivity were also greater in adoles- ited access to food, have modified the way we perceive
cents with excess weight than adolescents with normal food and regulate intake. These processes are increasingly
weight. Negative social evaluation was associated with influenced by a variety of factors besides homeostatic
worse executive functioning and increased autonomic regulation, like sensory cues (e.g., taste, smell, texture
reactivity in adolescents with excess weight. and appearance), availability, motivational and affect-
ive states, pleasure seeking, and so on. All of these fac-
Conclusions The findings suggest that adolescents with
tors influence what and how much people eat even when
excess weight are more sensitive to social stress trig-
they are not hungry [2]. In the last few years, obesity is
gered by negative evaluations. Social stress elicited
being increasingly considered as a brain-related dysfunc-
tion similar to that occurring in addictions [3], where the
María J. Fernández-Serrano
motivational value of highly palatable food is signifi-
[email protected] cantly increased, while the top-down or executive control
mechanisms that would normally regulate reward-driven
1
Department of Psychology, Universidad de Jaén, 23071 Jaén, responses are diminished [4, 5]. Executive control mech-
Spain anisms are relevant to the regulation of eating behavior
2
School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of
[6], as they allow for adjustment of behavior in a flex-
Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences, Monash University, ible way in situations that require a change in a strong
Melbourne, Australia habitual response or resistance to temptation [7]. The
abnormal interaction between reward signal processing executive performance after exposure to social stress
and executive control functioning has also been related relative to normal weight adolescents. Outcome meas-
to a tendency to select immediate and appetizing (high in ures were working memory, cognitive inhibition, and
calories and/or sugar) rewarding choices, although these shifting (ability to follow different rules in a task and
have negative consequences in the long term [8, 9]. The change between them). In addition, subjective and phys-
imbalance between these two systems can be greater in iological (autonomic) indexes of stress were recorded.
adolescence, a period characterized by the relative imma- For this purpose, heart rate (HR) and skin conduct-
turity of the prefrontal cortex, responsible for executive ance (SC) were continuously recorded during the TSST.
control, in addition to the relative maturity of striatal As overweight adolescents are more often exposed to
areas responsible for reward processing [10]. Therefore, negative peer evaluations than adolescents with nor-
during adolescence, the activity of rewards system may mal weight [18, 19], we expected greater increases in
prevail over that of executive control mechanisms [11]. perceived stress, HR, and SC in excess versus normal
Another factor that can impair top-down control weight participants during the TSST. Furthermore, neg-
mechanisms is stress. Stress has a harmful impact on ative associations between stress-induced subjective and
cognitive skills, such as attention, cognitive control, physiological responses and post-TSST executive per-
and decision making, which may contribute to obesi- formance were hypothesized.
ty-related behaviors in adolescents [12]. Furthermore,
psychosomatic theories hold that people with obesity Method
tend to eat in response to emotional distress, showing
an “emotional eating pattern” (i.e., consuming food Participants
impulsively) when under negative emotional states [13].
Stress can also enhance the propensity to eat high calorie Sixty adolescents, 25 males and 35 females between 13
“palatable” food via its interaction with central reward and 18 years of age, participated. They were selected
pathways [14]. For example, ghrelin and cortisol increase based on their sex and age-adjusted body mass index
in response to social stressors and influence reward moti- (BMI) percentile in accordance with the guidelines of the
vation, thus modulating consumption of appetizing food International Obesity Task Force (IOFT) [28]. Normal
[15, 16]. weight participants (n = 30) had BMIs ranging between
During adolescence, peer relations are particularly the 5th and 84th percentiles, and excess weight partici-
salient and can serve as a robust source of distress [17]. pants (n = 30) had BMIs greater than the 85th percentile.
Adolescents with excess weight suffer from social stress, Table 1 displays the sociodemographic, BMI, and body
such as bullying or social marginalization-exclusion, fat percentage data. Participants were recruited from
more frequently than their peers [18], being subjected high schools located in Jaén (Spain). They were screened
to frequent teasing about their body [19]. Negative ste- for medical and developmental conditions, medication
reotypes toward peers with excess weight begin early in use, and learning disabilities. Inclusion criteria were (i)
childhood [20], and these social stressors can negatively age range between 13 and 18 years, (ii) BMI >5th per-
affect social adjustment and academic achievement [21]. centile, and (iii) no history of neurological, psychiatric,
In this context, study of the detrimental influence of or eating disorders (measured using the Eating Disorder
social stress on executive functions may be of crucial Inventory [EDI-2]). All participants had normal or cor-
importance to understand deficient diet-related decision rected-to-normal vision.
making and poor emotional-regulation-related overeat-
ing in adolescents. Executive Measures
Several studies have found deficits in executive func-
tioning in adults and adolescents with excess weight [22– Working memory—Letter-Number Sequencing [29]
25]. However, to the best of our knowledge, no study
has analyzed the influence of social stress on executive Participants were read a sequence in which letters and
functions in adolescents with excess weight. Therefore, numbers were combined, and were asked to reproduce
this study examined the effect of a social stressor on the sequence, first putting the numbers in ascending
executive performance in adolescents with excess ver- order and then the letters in alphabetical order. The sum
sus normal weight. For this purpose, the Trier Public of the correct answers was considered.
Speaking Stress Social Task (TSST) [26, 27] was used.
We analyzed the specific influence of negative social Inhibition and shifting—Five-Digit Test (FDT) [30]
evaluation on executive functioning and autonomic The FDT consists of four conditions of increasing com-
responses in overweight adolescents. We hypothesized plexity. Conditions 1 and 2 evaluate processing and
that excess weight adolescents would show decreased response speed. In Condition 3 (inhibition), participants
the goggles [31]. Surround-sound headphones were used four phases, given our specific interest in the effect of
to allow perception of the sound emanating from the social evaluation, HR and SC analyses were restricted
room where the audience was situated, and the murmurs to the difference between the latter two parts of the
and comments of the listeners. The Ethics Committee of TSST involving social evaluation (positive vs. negative
the Universidad de Jaén approved the study. Both partic- social evaluation). Associations between variables were
ipants and parents signed informed consent forms. analyzed by Pearson’s correlations. To simplify the cor-
relation analysis, change scores were computed as the
Psychophysiological Data Acquisition and Processing difference between the post- and pre-TSST values.
Table 2 Descriptive scores and group comparisons for stress (VAS) and neuropsychological measures before TSST (pre-scores) and after
TSST (post-scores)
Mean SD Mean SD t p d´
VAS visual analogue scale; TSST Trier Social Stress Task; FDT Five-Digit Test.
Psychophysiological Measures
Fig. 1. Mean heart rate (beats per minute [BPM]) during the Fig. 3. Working memory scores (Letter-Number Sequencing)
positive and negative social evaluation phases of the Trier Social before and after the Trier Social Stress Task. Bars indicate stand-
Stress Task. Bars indicate standard error of the mean. ard error of the mean.
harmful health consequences. Furthermore, this deficit and normal weight individuals [15]. This discrepancy
may lead to adolescents with excess weight persisting in may be due to the noninclusion of specific positive–neg-
their unhealthy eating habits. Impairments in working ative evaluation phases in their TSST, or may reflect a
memory could affect the ability to maintain cognitive greater vulnerability to social stress in adolescents than
control, making it more difficult to engage in healthy adults. In line with the greater autonomic response found
activities and intervention programs. Finally, disinhibited in our study, a greater cortisol response after the TSST
eating in obese adolescents was associated with reduced has been previously found in excess weight than in ado-
orbitofrontal volume and executive dysfunctions, which lescents with normal weight [27].
were most pronounced in terms of working memory and Executive functioning is still developing during ado-
inhibition [41]. Conversely, executive function skills were lescence, as prefrontal areas reach full development at
positively associated with healthy eating habits, such as maturity [45]. A growing body of literature suggests an
fruit and vegetable intake, and physical activity [42, 43]. altered balance between the earlier-developing limbic
We observed group differences before social stress only system and the later-developing frontal/executive system
in the “inhibition score” (FDT), with lower performance [46] during adolescence. Furthermore, in this period,
in excess weight adolescents. However, no differences the opinions of peers and general social evaluation
were found in shifting or working memory. These results become a central aspect for self-image development [47].
are concordant with a previous study reporting selective Adolescents with excess weight frequently suffer from
alterations in inhibition in adolescents with obesity ver- negative social evaluations and social stressors during
sus normal weight adolescents [43]. Another study found their everyday lives, which may lead to greater vulnera-
selective alterations in inhibition and shifting, but not bility to social stress, especially if a negative social evalu-
working memory, in excess weight and obese adolescents ation component is included. It would be reasonable to
[24]. In contrast, other authors found significant differ- assume that adolescents with excess weight would show
ences between obese and normal adolescents in working a blunted stress response due to habituation to repeated
memory as well as attention, but not in intelligence or stress exposure. However, previous studies using this
verbal fluency [44]. Discrepancies between studies may same TSST protocol found greater increases in salivary
be due to differences in testing methods, samples, and cortisol in excess weight than in normal weight adoles-
levels of BMI. cents [27]. These results suggest the development of a
As expected, the change in subjective stress was posi- sensitization process to social stress in adolescents with
tively associated with the change in HR, SC, and “inhibi- excess weight.
tion errors” (FDT). This suggests that levels of subjective Therefore, due to all of the factors listed above, ado-
stress may modulate both psychophysiological responses lescents with excess weight are an important target group
and executive-inhibition functions. In this way, negative for cognitive interventions based on stress regulation
social evaluations may induce a greater increase in stress strategies, executive function improvement and preven-
levels and autonomic responsiveness, and a reduction tion of harmful eating behaviors. In this regard, some
of inhibition capacity, in excess weight adolescents rela- evidence already suggests that executive functioning
tive to those with normal weight. The deleterious influ- training for obese children can improve working mem-
ence of negative social evaluation on executive control ory, inhibition, and shifting, being useful in weight-loss
in adolescents with excess weight may exacerbate diffi- maintenance [48].
culties in eating behavior control, eventually triggering Regarding its strengths, our study used an innovative
overconsumption. strategy to evaluate the impact of social stress, particu-
The change in HR during social evaluation was pos- larly negative social evaluation, on adolescents with excess
itively associated with the change in “shifting errors,” weight, as well as the inclusion of autonomic variables
“inhibition errors,” and “shifting score” in the whole as objective indices of stress. Among the limitations, we
sample. The change in SC was negatively associated with used a virtual reality audience in our TSST instead of the
the change in Letter-Number Sequencing performance actual public, which might have decreased the realism of
(i.e., greater habituation of SC was associated with bet- the situation and the stress-elicited responses. However,
ter working memory). These results suggest that modula- this version of the TSST was validated in previous stud-
tion of autonomic activity by social stress may index, or ies and produced a reliable stress response [26, 27, 31].
additionally influence, executive functioning in adoles- Furthermore, the inclusion in future studies of a non-
cents with excess weight. This harmful effect on execu- stress control condition (also with two cognitive evalua-
tive functioning may lead to problems in real life, such tions) is recommended to rule out more possible general
as poor regulation of eating habits. However, studies in disruption of cognitive processes in excess weight ado-
adults also using the TSST did not find differences in lescents. Regarding the study design, the absence of
HR, blood pressure, or cortisol responses between obese any counterbalancing of the order of presentation of
positive versus negative feedback conditions might have 2. Berthoud HR, Morrison C. The brain, appetite, and obesity.
influenced the results. Therefore, this aspect should be Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:55–92.
3. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Baler RD. Reward, dopamine and the
taken into account in future studies. In addition, we control of food intake: Implications for obesity. Trends Cogn
did not assess factors such as emotional eating or loss Sci. 2011;15:37–46.
of control in eating behavior, which may be relevant to 4. Acosta MC, Manubay J, Levin FR. Pediatric obesity:
overeating. Finally, longitudinal studies are necessary Parallels with addiction and treatment recommendations.
to better analyze the influence of stress-induced execu- Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2008;16:80–96.
5. Zheng H, Lenard NR, Shin AC, Berthoud HR. Appetite
tive functioning decrements on the propensity to over- control and energy balance regulation in the modern world:
eat and become obese in the future in adolescents with Reward-driven brain overrides repletion signals. Int J Obes
excess weight. (Lond). 2009;33(suppl 2):S8–S13.
In summary, our results showed a harmful impact of 6. Gunstad J, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Tate DF, Spitznagel MB,
social stress, specifically negative social evaluation, on Gordon E. Elevated body mass index is associated with
executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr
executive functioning of adolescents with excess weight. Psychiatry. 2007;48:57–61.
Overweight adolescents performed worse after TSST in 7. Verdejo-García A, Bechara A. [Neuropsychology of execu-
inhibition and shifting than those with normal weight, tive functions]. Psicothema. 2010;22:227–235.
and they did not benefit from learning in the domain of 8. Verdejo-García A, Bechara A. A somatic marker theory of
working memory, in contrast to adolescents with normal addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56(suppl 1):48–62.
9. Berthoud HR. Interactions between the “cognitive” and
weight. Furthermore, in association with the observed “metabolic” brain in the control of food intake. Physiol
decreases in neuropsychological performance, adoles- Behav. 2007;91:486–498.
cents with excess weight showed greater subjective and 10. Chambers RA, Taylor JR, Potenza MN. Developmental
autonomic stress responses to negative social evaluation. neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A crit-
Given the relevance of high order executive functions to ical period of addiction vulnerability. Am J Psychiatry.
2003;160:1041–1052.
self-control of eating behavior [49, 50], the results pre- 11. Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic model of the neuro-
sented herein highlight the value of assessing the social biology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychol Med.
evaluation context, and how it may be associated with dif- 2006;36:299–312.
ferential changes in cognitive functioning in overweight 12. Davidson RJ, McEwen BS. Social influences on neuroplas-
adolescents. This stress-mediated impairment in cogni- ticity: Stress and interventions to promote well-being. Nat
Neurosci. 2012;15:689–695.
tive functioning could increase the risk for future obesity. 13. Laitinen J, Ek E, Sovio U. Stress-related eating and drinking
Interventions aimed at social stress coping strategies and behavior and body mass index and predictors of this behav-
improvement of executive functions could be useful in the ior. Prev Med. 2002;34:29–39.
prevention of obesity in adulthood. 14. Sominsky L, Spencer SJ. Eating behavior and stress: A path-
way to obesity. Front Psychol. 2014;5:434.
Acknowledgements This work has not received any funding. 15. Rouach V, Bloch M, Rosenberg N, et al. The acute ghrelin
response to a psychological stress challenge does not pre-
dict the post-stress urge to eat. Psychoneuroendocrinology.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
2007;32:693–702.
16. Therrien F, Drapeau V, Lalonde J, et al. Awakening corti-
Authors’ Contributions M. M. Padilla carried out the experi- sol response in lean, obese, and reduced obese individuals:
ments, and all of the authors analysed the data and wrote the Effect of gender and fat distribution. Obesity (Silver Spring).
article. 2007;15:377–385.
17. Stapinski LA, Araya R, Heron J, Montgomery AA, Stallard
Ethical Approval All procedures performed in this study involv- P. Peer victimization during adolescence: Concurrent and
ing human participants were in accordance with the ethical stand- prospective impact on symptoms of depression and anxiety.
ards of the Ethics Committee of the Universidad de Jaén and with Anxiety Stress Coping. 2015;28:105–120.
the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or compar- 18. Puhl RM, King KM. Weight discrimination and bullying.
able ethical standards. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2013;27:117–127.
19. Puhl RM, Luedicke J, Heuer C. Weight-based victimization
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained froma all par- toward overweight adolescents: Observations and reactions
ticipants included in the study. of peers. J Sch Health. 2011;81:696–703.
20. Cramer P, Steinwert T. Thin is good, fat is bad: How early
Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical does it begin? J Appl Dev Psychol. 1998;19:429–451.
Standards The authors declare no conflicts of interest. 21. Gunnarsdottir T, Njardvik U, Olafsdottir AS, Craighead L,
Bjarnason R. Childhood obesity and co-morbid problems:
Effects of Epstein’s family-based behavioural treatment in an
Icelandic sample. J Eval Clin Pract. 2012;18:465–472.
22. Boeka AG, Lokken KL. Neuropsychological performance
References of a clinical sample of extremely obese individuals. Arch Clin
Neuropsychol. 2008;23:467–474.
1. Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of 23. Pignatti R, Bertella L, Albani G, Mauro A, Molinari E,
obesity and trends in body mass index among US children Semenza C. Decision-making in obesity: A study using the
and adolescents, 1999-2010. JAMA. 2012;307:483–490. gambling task. Eat Weight Disord. 2006;11:126–132.
24. Verdejo‐García A, Pérez‐Expósito M, Schmidt‐Río‐Valle J, one month test-retest intervals. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.
et al. Selective alterations within executive functions in ado- 2006;28:1095–1112.
lescents with excess weight. Obesity. 2010;18:1572–1578. 38. Luethi M, Meier B, Sandi C. Stress effects on working mem-
25. Wu M, Brockmeyer T, Hartmann M, Skunde M, Herzog W, ory, explicit memory, and implicit memory for neutral and
Friederich HC. Reward-related decision making in eating emotional stimuli in healthy men. Front Behav Neurosci.
and weight disorders: A systematic review and meta-analy- 2008;2:5.
sis of the evidence from neuropsychological studies. Neurosci 39. Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN.
Biobehav Rev. 2016;61:177–196. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related
26. Kelly O, Matheson K, Martinez A, Merali Z, Anisman behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond).
H. Psychosocial stress evoked by a virtual audience: 2014;38:494–506.
Relation to neuroendocrine activity. Cyberpsychol Behav. 40. Jansen A, Nederkoorn C, van Baak L, Keirse C, Guerrieri R,
2007;10:655–662. Havermans R. High-restrained eaters only overeat when they
27. Verdejo-Garcia A, Moreno-Padilla M, Garcia-Rios MC, are also impulsive. Behav Res Ther. 2009;47:105–110.
et al. Social stress increases cortisol and hampers atten- 41. Maayan L, Hoogendoorn C, Sweat V, Convit A. Disinhibited
tion in adolescents with excess weight. Plos One. 2015;10: eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal
e0123565. volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity (Silver
28. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body Spring). 2011;19:1382–1387.
mass index cut-offs for thinness, overweight and obesity. 42. Riggs NR, Spruijt-Metz D, Chou CP, Pentz MA.
Pediatr Obes. 2012;7:284–294. Relationships between executive cognitive function and life-
29. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4th ed. time substance use and obesity-related behaviors in fourth
Madrid, Spain: Tea Editions; 2005. grade youth. Child Neuropsychol. 2012;18:1–11.
30. Sedó M. Five Digits Test. Madrid, Spain: Tea Editions; 2008. 43. Kittel R, Schmidt R, Hilbert A. Executive functions in ado-
31. Montero-López E, Santos-Ruiz A, García-Ríos MC, lescents with binge-eating disorder and obesity. Int J Eat
Rodríguez-Blázquez R, Pérez-García M, Peralta-Ramírez Disord. 2017;50:933–941.
MI. A virtual reality approach to the trier social stress test: 44. Cserjési R, Molnár D, Luminet O, Lénárd L. Is there any
Contrasting two distinct protocols. Behav Res Methods. relationship between obesity and mental flexibility in chil-
2016;48:223–232. dren? Appetite. 2007;49:675–678.
32. Garner DM. EDI-2: Inventario de Trastornos de la Conducta 45. Casey BJ, Jones RM, Hare TA. The adolescent brain. Ann N
Alimentaria. Madrid, Spain: TEA; 1998. Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:111–126.
33. Roth WT, Ehlers A, Taylor CB, Margraf J, Agras WS. Skin 46. Casey BJ, Duhoux S, Malter Cohen M. Adolescence: What
conductance habituation in panic disorder patients. Biol do transmission, transition, and translation have to do with
Psychiatry. 1990;27:1231–1243. it? Neuron. 2010;67:749–760.
34. Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol 47. Nelson EE, Leibenluft E, McClure EB, Pine DS. The social
responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of labora- re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on
tory research. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:355–391. the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychol Med.
35. Cserjési R, Luminet O, Poncelet AS, Lénárd L. Altered execu- 2005;35:163–174.
tive function in obesity. Exploration of the role of affective 48. Verbeken S, Braet C, Goossens L, van der Oord S. Executive
states on cognitive abilities. Appetite. 2009;52:535–539. function training with game elements for obese children:
36. Collie A, Maruff P, Darby DG, McStephen M. The effects of A novel treatment to enhance self-regulatory abilities for
practice on the cognitive test performance of neurologically weight-control. Behav Res Ther. 2013;51:290–299.
normal individuals assessed at brief test-retest intervals. J Int 49. Houben K, Jansen A. Training inhibitory control. A recipe
Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9:419–428. for resisting sweet temptations. Appetite. 2011;56:345–349.
37. Falleti MG, Maruff P, Collie A, Darby DG. Practice effects 50. Veling H, Aarts H, Papies EK. Using stop signals to inhibit
associated with the repeated assessment of cognitive func- chronic dieters’ responses toward palatable foods. Behav Res
tion using the CogState battery at 10-minute, one week and Ther. 2011;49:771–780.