HChui.pdf;jsessionid=35A5F54E223E9732987F051CA44F6550
HChui.pdf;jsessionid=35A5F54E223E9732987F051CA44F6550
HChui.pdf;jsessionid=35A5F54E223E9732987F051CA44F6550
Authors Chui, Helena; Hoppmann, C.A.; Gerstorf, D.; Walker, R.; Luszcz,
M.A.
Citation Chui, H., Hoppmann, C. A., Gerstorf, D., Walker, R. and Luszcz, M.
A., (2014) Social partners and momentary affect in the oldest-old:
The presence of others benefits affect depending on who we are
and who we are with. Developmental Psychology, 50 (3): 728-740.
DOI https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033896
Rights © 2014 APA. “This article may not exactly replicate the final
version published in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record”.
This work is made available online in accordance with publisher policies. Please
refer to the repository record for this item and our Policy Document available from
the repository home page for further information.
To see the final version of this work please visit the publisher’s website. Available
access to the published online version may require a subscription.
Author(s): Chui, H., Hoppmann, C. A., Gerstorf, D., Walker, R. and Luszcz, M. A.
Title: Social partners and momentary affect in the oldest-old: the presence of
others benefits affect depending on who we are and who we are with.
Publication year: 2014
Journal title: Developmental Psychology
Link to original published version: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033896
Citation: Chui, H., Hoppmann, C. A., Gerstorf, D., Walker, R. and Luszcz, M. A.
(2014) Social partners and momentary affect in the oldest-old: The presence of
others benefits affect depending on who we are and who we are with.
Developmental Psychology, 50 (3): 728-740.
Copyright statement: © 2014 APA. “This article may not exactly replicate the
final version published in the APA journal. It is not the copy of record”.
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 1
Social Partners and Momentary Affect in the Oldest-Old: The Presence of Others Benefits Affect
Abstract
There are well-documented links between social relationships and emotional well-being in old
age, but little is known about daily life fluctuations in momentary affective experiences of the
oldest-old while interacting with specific social partners. We examined associations between the
presence of different types of social partners and moment-to-moment fluctuations in affect in the
symptoms, chronic health conditions, and loneliness. Participants (N = 74, M age = 88.7 years,
range = 84 - 102 years, 68% women) provided self-reports concurrently on the presence of social
partners and subjective affective states six times a day for each of seven consecutive days (3,071
occasions, in total). Relative to being with other people, time spent alone was associated with
lower positive affect in the oldest-old. Being with other family members and friends was
associated with more positive affective experiences. Compared to men, women reported more
negative affective experiences when they were with their spouses than when their spouses were
not present. Individuals with more chronic health problems reported more negative affective
experiences when they were with their spouses than not. Participants higher in neuroticism
reported more positive affective experiences when they were with their friends, compared to
times when their friends were not present. Finally, lonelier individuals reported more positive
affective experiences when they were with their spouses than not. These findings suggest that
affective experience is a function of individual differences and the type of social partners oldest-
old adults interact with in everyday life. We discuss how our findings can be generalized to
oldest-olds of different marital statuses taking into account the proportion of widows in our
Social Partners and Momentary Affect in the Oldest-Old: The Presence of Others Benefits
Aspects of social relationships, such as social support and social integration, have major
implications for individuals’ physical and psychological health across the lifespan (Berkman,
Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000). However, little is known about the affective implications of
social interactions in daily life, especially in the oldest-old (Fingerman & Pitzer, 2007). The
purpose of our study was to fill this distinct gap in the literature by examining whether the
absence or the presence of different types of social partners (e.g. spouse and friends), which may
fulfill different relationship functions, are associated with concurrent momentary affective
experiences in the oldest-old. Specifically, we used up to 42 repeated daily life assessments from
74 oldest-old adults to examine concurrent associations between the presence of different types
The link between social relationships and psychological health is well established but the
mechanisms underlying this association are not well understood (Cohen, 2004). Fingerman and
Lang (2004) proposed a cube model to illustrate the interweaving of three dimensions in social
relationships from a lifespan perspective. The structure of social relationships concerns the types
of social partners individuals interact with, e.g. spouse and friends. The processes refer to the
underlying mechanisms of social relationships, such as motivation to engage with others. The
outcomes refer to the benefits and costs of social relationships, including satisfaction and
affective state. Research typically focuses on specific dimensions (e.g. friendships in late life) or
interactions of dimensions (e.g. affective benefits of friendships in late life). The three
dimensions and their associations change and reflect the changing life circumstances of the
individual across the lifespan. For instance, because the loss of a spouse becomes more common
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 4
in late life, the affective benefits of friendships may become more important as individuals age.
The present study focused on the structure-outcome dimensions of the cube model,
specifically, the types of social partners and how they are associated with fluctuations in affect
quality. Findings have shown that the structure of social relationships, e.g. being married, is
associated with physical and mental health outcomes in older adults (Cohen, 2004). To explain
the contribution of social relationships to affective experiences, Larson and colleagues (1986)
pointed to the important distinction between the long-term structure and contributions of social
relationships in a global time frame compared to the immediate sensations and feelings emergent
in social interactions in the present moment. For instance, having more immediate positive
affective experiences with friends was not associated with greater life satisfaction in the global
time frame. To date, a substantial body of research draws from the between-person perspective
(Nesselroade & Molenaar, 2010) and has examined the long-term structure-outcome link, e.g.
marital status and mortality (Cohen, 2004). Few studies have integrated the within-person
perspective and examined momentary affective experiences emergent from ongoing social
interactions (Larson et al., 1986). In order to provide a detailed description of the everyday life
processes and contexts influencing affective experiences in old age (Baltes, Wahl, & Schmid-
Furstoss, 1990; Nesselroade & Molenaar, 2010), the present study adopted a within-person
perspective and examined the momentary affective experiences associated with the presence of
We first review the literature on the link between the long-term structure and outcomes in
social relationships in a global time frame. We then review the literature on the link between the
presence of social partners and affect in the present moment. A substantial body of research has
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 5
examined the long-term structure of social relationships and their associated outcomes (Cohen,
2004). Results from the Australian Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ALSA; Giles, Glonek, Luszcz,
& Andrews, 2005) showed that specific social network types differentially predicted mortality in
older adults. Having more friends was associated with lower mortality hazards over a 10-year
period, whereas no predictive effect was found for networks of children and relatives.
Furthermore, parent-adult children relationships can involve tensions and ambivalence with
consequences for well-being (Fingerman, Pitzer, Lefkowitz, Birditt, & Mroczek, 2008). Among
married older adults, spouses shape each other’s developmental trajectories of depressive
symptoms (Hoppmann, Gerstorf, & Hibbert, 2011) and well-being (Walker, Luszcz, Gerstorf, &
Hoppmann, 2011). In late life, peripheral ties such as healthcare providers, may act as substitutes
for lost spouses and are important for well-being (Fingerman, 2009). However, findings seem to
suggest that older adults tend to have negative interactions with peripheral ties. This may be due
to the tendency of healthcare providers to encourage dependency in older adults which may lead
to resentment, lowered self-esteem, and adverse health outcomes (Smith & Goodnow, 1999). It
is therefore crucial to move beyond an examination of the mere presence or absence of social
relationships in old age and embark on an investigation of how distinct types of social partners
Beyond the global time frame, interactions with different types of social partners are
associated with momentary variations in affect and arousal. Larson and colleagues (1986)
examined the presence of different types of social partners and the associated affect and arousal
types of social partners examined included spouse, children, friends/neighbors, and other. Affect
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 6
and arousal were assessed using bipolar items (e.g. happy-sad, energetic-tired) and z-scores of
affect and arousal were used for analysis. Older adults reported neutral or below average
(relative to individuals’ own means) affect and arousal when with their children or spouses. In
contrast, they reported above average affect and arousal when with friends. Larsen and
colleagues suggested that spouse and family constitute the long-term structural properties of
social relationships, which contribute to individuals’ overall security and satisfaction. In contrast,
friends constitute the short-term process properties of social relationships, which contribute to
individuals’ momentary affective experiences. These findings were consistent with findings on
tension and ambivalence in parent-adult children relationships (Fingerman et al., 2008) and
spousal relationships in old age (Hoppmann et al., 2011; Walker et al., 2011).
To examine the momentary affective experiences associated with social interactions, the
present study extended the original cube model (Fingerman & Lang, 2004) to take into account
within-person variations in the presence of different types of social partners and associated
momentary fluctuations in affect in oldest-old adults’ daily lives. Compared to Larsen and
colleagues’ (1986) study that used a younger and mostly married (66%) sample, our sample
comprised of older (M age = 88.7 years, range = 84 - 102 years) and mostly widowed (68%)
oldest-old adults. Thus, our sample is particularly suited to examine the interactions with
peripheral ties that may substitute for the loss of spouses in the oldest-old. A second point of
difference is that we focused on affect (not arousal) and used unipolar items, instead of bipolar
unidimensional affect structure was timely in the 80’s, however, more recent conceptualizations
of affect assume a multi-dimensional structure and the use of unipolar items (Watson, Clark, &
Tellegen, 1988). Furthermore, we draw from the social convoy model and acknowledge that
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 7
social relationships are in part shaped by individual characteristics (Antonucci, Fiori, Birditt, &
affect in the presence of various types of social partners, which allows us to move towards better
In general, social networks shrink with age (Lang & Carstensen, 1994) and individuals
spend increasing amounts of time alone (Larson, 1990) due to changes in the composition of
social networks (Antonucci et al., 2010) and changes in preference for particular types of social
partners (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999)1. However, being alone does not necessarily
carry negative connotations for at least two reasons. First, there is an important distinction
between solitude and loneliness. Solitude is the objective state of being alone and loneliness is
the subjective feeling of a discrepancy in the quantity or quality of social relationships one has
and those one desires (Jylhä & Saarenheimo, 2010). From adolescence to old age, being alone
becomes more common and less emotionally negative (Larson, 1990). Although loneliness is
often associated with poorer health outcomes (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010), daily experiences of
solitude have been associated with a greater sense of control in older adults (Larson, Zuzanek, &
Mannell, 1985). Second, the affective experiences associated with being alone may depend on
individual characteristics. For instance, being alone may be particularly negative for lonely
individuals because of their unfulfilled social needs (Cacioppo et al., 2000). Because being alone
becomes increasingly common with age, it is important to examine the affective experience of
the oldest-old when they are alone as well as with other people.
important role of between-person differences (Antonucci et al., 2010). Gender, personality, and
health conditions have an influence on who individuals interact with and how they benefit from
their social relationships (Rook, Mavandadi, Sorkin, & Zettel, 2007). Women often have larger
social networks and tend to receive support from multiple sources, while men tend to rely on
their spouses exclusively (Antonucci et al., 2010). Older adults higher in neuroticism report
receiving less social support and are less satisfied with their social interactions (Bolger &
Zuckerman, 1995). Depressive symptoms and chronic health conditions increase with age
(Alexopoulos, 2005; Birditt & Antonucci, 2008) and both are associated with less satisfying
social interactions (Nezlek, Imbrie, & Shean, 1994), including tension and vulnerability in older
couples (Hoppmann et al., 2011). In particular, depressive symptoms are associated with mean
affect and affect variability (Ebner-Priemer & Trull, 2009). Furthermore, lonely individuals may
be particularly distressed when they are alone (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010).
We examined the associations between the presence of different types of social partners
and the momentary affective experiences of oldest-old adults, taking into account individual
and loneliness. Although social interactions with spouses and family may be emotionally
meaningful, there is also evidence suggesting that the presence of spouses and family can be
associated with negative affective experiences in young-old adults (Larson et al., 1986). We
examined the following hypotheses. H1: We expect that the presence of spouses and family will
be associated with lower PA and higher NA in the oldest-old. H2: We expect that the presence of
friends will be associated with higher PA and lower NA in the oldest-old. H3: We expect that
being alone will be associated with lower PA and higher NA for lonely individuals, compared to
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 9
the less lonely. H4: Compared to women, the presence of spouses will be associated with higher
PA and lower NA in men. H5: Compared to individuals with lower neuroticism, higher
neuroticism will be associated with lower PA and higher NA when individuals are with others.
H6: Compared to individuals with fewer depressive symptoms and chronic health conditions, the
presence of a spouse will be associated with lower PA and higher NA in individuals with more
Method
Participants
The ALSA Daily-Life Time-Sampling (ADuLTS) Study (Luszcz et al., 2011, November)
is an extension of the Australia Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ALSA; Luszcz et al., 2007). Out
of the 168 active ALSA participants, a subset (N = 95) was invited to take part in ADuLTS based
on three criteria: (a) vision and hearing sufficient to independently complete diaries; (b) Mini-
Mental State Examination ≥ 24 (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975); (c) overall
functioning based on a clinical assessment in the last wave of ALSA. Out of the 95 ALSA
participants, 51 were successfully recruited. Attrition analyses showed that ADuLTS participants
were positively selected, compared to the whole ALSA sample and the eligible participants who
refused to participate (n = 44)2. To increase the sample size, an additional group of community-
dwelling adults (N = 24) was recruited from multiple sources including the school of psychology
older participant pool, retirement villages, and through “snowballing.”3 A total sample of 50
women and 25 men were recruited (M age = 88.65 years, SD = 3.04, range = 83.55 – 102.40).
The participants were mostly community-dwelling (98.7%). Most participants were widowed
(68%), followed by married (25%), de facto (3%), never married (3%), and divorced (1%). Men
(44% married) and women (16% married) differed significantly in marital status, χ2 (1) = 5.51, p
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 10
< .05. Most participants (77%) were born in Australia or the United Kingdom (17%), with the
remainder from other European countries. Participants received an AUD$30 gift card as
compensation. Analyses were based on 74 individuals’ data because one participant did not
Procedure
which they received instructions on completing the within-day paper diaries and on the use of an
electronic beeper that signaled when to complete them. Research assistants met the participants
again at home on Day 2 to make sure they followed the instructions without difficulties.
Participants carried the beeper for one week and completed six self-reports each day. They were
encouraged to contact the research assistant by phone if they had difficulties following the
protocol. Research assistants met the participants at home the day after the final assessment day
to collect diaries and receive feedback from participants and to debrief, thank and remunerate
them.4
On each assessment day, the first self-reports were completed shortly after waking up,
which was self-initiated. The remaining five were completed in response to a beep that occurred
at three-hourly intervals. The beeps were scheduled to minimize conflicts with participants’ daily
routines. Participants were instructed to respond to the beep as soon as possible and within two
hours. Analyses were based on the baseline data and six within-day self-reports for the seven-day
assessment period. The sample provided a total of 3,071 self-reports of the presence of social
partners and affective experiences. Overall compliance was 96%. Participants provided an
average of 40.38 (SD = 4.69) self-reports out of 42 assessment occasions (6 within-day × 7 days).
To ensure compliance with the protocol, i.e. no backfilling, participants used an electronic time
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 11
stamp to record the time that they began and finished each self-report. Participants put each self-
report into an envelope and stamped the time again across the seal.
Measures
Positive and negative affect. In each within-day assessment (not baseline), affect was
measured using nine items: happy, sad, calm, sleepy, anxious, alert, quiet, irritated, and excited
(1 = not at all; 5 = very much). These items were selected based on two criteria: (a) each
displayed considerable within-person variability in past research, and (b) together the items
covered each octant of the affective circumplex (Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, 2006). Drawing from
the Circumplex Model of Emotion (Larsen & Diener, 1992), items were selected to represent
positive (happy, excited, and calm) and negative affect (sad, anxious, and irritated) based on face
validity. Instead of a homogeneous set of items (e.g. high-arousal positive: enthusiastic, excited,
strong), the items were selected to capture the wide range of valence and activation. Similar
items are commonly used in time-sampling affect research (e.g., Hoppmann & Klumb, 2006).
The heterogeneous set of items is expected to have low internal consistency but adequate
longitudinal reliability (Shrout & Lane, 2012). For internal consistency, mean Cronbach’s α
across the 42 within-day assessments was .41 (SD = .10; range = .17-.60) for PA and .67 (SD
= .12; range = .30-.82) for NA. Longitudinal reliability of PA and NA were evaluated using four
indices based on the Generalizability Theory (Cranford et al., 2006; Shrout & Lane, 2011). RKF
= .99 for PA and .99 for NA. R1R = .75 for PA and .78 for NA. RKR = .98 for PA and .98 for NA.
RC = .37 for PA and .36 for NA. The indices show moderate to excellent longitudinal reliability
for PA and NA. The moderate RC suggests that the selected items were sensitive to detecting
moment-to-moment changes in affect. Given the small sample size and limited number of items
used to reduce participant burden, the internal consistency of the 3-item PA and NA scales were
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 12
less than optimal. However, the PA and NA scales show reasonable psychometric properties in
the context of a time-sampling design (Shrout & Lane, 2012). Mean scores of PA and NA for
Types of social interactions in daily life. At each beep, the presence of social partners
was determined by responses to the question “Who are you with?” Participants had multiple
options to check (a) service provider, (b) formal carer, (c) spouse, (d) other family member, (e)
friend, (f) other, and (g) alone. Responses were dummy-coded (0 = not present; 1 = present).
For example, when participants were with their spouse and friend, spouse = 1 and friend = 1. The
other types of social partners were coded 0. Responses to the option “alone” were also dummy-
coded (0 = not alone; 1 = alone). Participants reported being alone 71% of the time (2,171 out
of a total of 3,071 assessments). Participants were most often (17%) with their spouses, followed
by other family members (6%) and friends (3%). The presence of service providers (0.7%),
formal carers (0.03%), and other social partners (1%) were relatively infrequent, so the variable
“peripheral ties” was created by collapsing these responses together. When social partners were
present, only 5% of the time were there multiple types of social partners. Therefore, we did not
examine the interaction effect of multiple social partners, e.g. spouse × friend.
Neuroticism. Neuroticism was assessed during baseline by the 8-item Big Five Inventory
subscale (John & Srivastava, 1999), using a 5-point Likert scale (1 = Disagree Strongly; 5 =
Agree Strongly). Cronbach’s α was .74 (M = 14.51, SD = 4.97). Higher scores indicate more
emotional instability.
item short-version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D;
Andresen, Malmgren, Carter, & Patrick, 1994). Participants responded using a 4-point Likert
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 13
scale (0 = Rarely or none of the time; 4 = Most or all of the time). Cronbach’s α was .62. The
item “My sleep was restless” was dropped because Cronbach’s α was .65 with the remaining
nine items (M = 4.69, SD = 3.46). Higher scores indicate more depressive symptoms. The lower
than expected internal consistency of the CES-D is addressed as a limitation in the discussion
section.
Chronic health conditions. Chronic health conditions were assessed by responses to the
question “In the past 12 months, has a Medical Doctor ever told you that you suffer from any of
these chronic conditions” on a list of 10 medication conditions, e.g. arthritis and cancer (1 = Yes;
0 = No). The mean number of chronic health conditions was 2.43 (SD = 1.48).
Geriatric Center Morale Scale were administered (PGCMS; Lawton, 1975; Ranzijn & Luszcz,
2000). CFA analyses resulted in a poorly fit model with a negative variance. Cronbach α was .27.
Only one item, “You feel lonelier than you used to feel,” (1 = Agree; 0= Disagree) showed face
validity in the measure of loneliness and was used for further analyses (M = .30, SD = .46). The
lower than expected internal consistency of the “lonely dissatisfaction” subscale is addressed as a
Covariate
Time. Time was used to control for the temporal effect of being in the study on positive
and negative affect. Individuals’ self-report of affect may change as a result of reactance and
habituation during the study (Barta, Tennen, & Litt, 2012). The duration in the study was
measured in the unit of days starting from midnight of Day 1. For example, time = .25 for an
individual who completed the first self-report on Day 1 at 6 am and time = 6.94 when an
To examine the presence of different types of social partners and associated PA and NA,
a typical multilevel modeling analysis would entail a univariate approach. The current study took
the analyses one step further and used a multivariate multilevel modeling approach (Raudenbush
& Bryk, 2002) for its advantages over the univariate approach. First, a multivariate analysis
allows better control of the inflation of Type I error rate. Second, a multivariate approach often
has more statistical power. Third, a multivariate approach allows testing whether PA and NA are
Analyses were performed using SAS PROC MIXED (SAS Institute, 2008). The basic
Level-1 model:
is the affect variable for participant i at time t. and are dummy variables.
= 1. represents the residual components. The level-1 intercept is constrained to 0. Thus, for
each assessment, t, there are two affect variables, i.e. PA and NA, within participants.
Level-2 model:
represents the mean PA across participants across occasions and represents the
mean NA across participants across occasions. and are the level-2 random effects. In
subsequent models, level-1 covariates including time, status of being alone, and the presence of
Gender was coded -.5 = men and .5 = women. Other level-2 covariates were grand-mean
centered. Because the residuals on adjacent assessments are likely to be correlated, we specified
a first-order autoregressive error covariance structure to account for the interdependence of affect
Results
The results section is organized in four parts: (a) descriptive statistics of PA and NA, (b)
the presence of social partners, (c) the status of being alone, individual differences and the
associated PA and NA, and (d) the presence of different types of social partners, individual
Across six (within-day) by seven (days) assessments, mean PA was 3.16 for men (SD
= .55; range = 2.21 – 4.50) and 3.09 for women (SD = .53; range = 2.08 – 4.98). Mean NA was
1.39 for men (SD = .46; range = 1.00 – 2.61) and 1.45 for women (SD = .48; range = 1.00 – 2.70).
Women and men did not significantly differ from each other in mean PA and mean NA.
between- and within-person variability in PA and NA were examined. In Model 1, the random
effects showed that between- and within-person variances were significant for both PA
σ2 = .21, SE = .04; within-person σ2 = .21, SE = .01), all ps < .001. The auto-correlation of affect
scores between adjacent occasions was also significant, AR(1) = .30, SE = .02, p < .001. In
Model 2, the effect of time was added into the model and was significant for PA, γ30 = -.02, p
Participants reported most often being alone (men = 58.8%; women = 75.7%), followed
by being with their spouses (men = 31.7%; women = 8.6%), other family (men = 3.4%; women =
7.0%), friends (men = 3.5%; women = 3.2%), and peripheral ties (men = 2.7%; women = 1.8%).
Compared to men, women reported less often being with their spouses, t(34) = 2.55, p < .05, and
more often being with other family members, t(67) = -2.05, p < .05. Women and men did not
differ in the number of occasions with other types of social partners or being alone.
Table 1 presents results of two nested models. Model 3 tested the effect of being alone
with no level-2 covariates entered. Specifically, being alone was associated with a lower level of
PA, = -.10, t(63) = -2.86, p < .01. The association between being alone and NA was not
significant, = .02, t(63) = .77, p > .05. Results in Model 4 indicate that for both PA and NA,
the effect of being alone has to be interpreted in the context of significant effects of person-
specific characteristics and interactions between being alone and person-specific characteristics.
t(5312) = -2.79, p < .01, and depressive symptoms, = -.04, t(5312) = -2.12, p < .05, reported
lower PA. In addition, individuals higher in loneliness reported higher NA, = .17, t(5312) =
2.28, p < .05. For interaction effects, individuals higher in neuroticism reported higher NA when
they were alone, = .01, t(5312) = 2.06, p < .05. The effects of gender and chronic health
Table 2 presents results of two nested models. Model 5 tested the effects of the presence
of different social partners with no level-2 covariates entered. Level-2 covariates were entered in
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 17
Model 6. Models 5 and 6 are not nested within Models 3 and 4 in the previous section. The
variable “alone” is not entered in Models 5 and 6 because the status of being alone can be
predicted by the presence of social partners. Including “alone” in Models 5 and 6 when variables
indicating the presence of social partners are in the model creates multicollinearity.
In Model 5, higher PA was associated with the presence of other family members,
= .16, t(5428) = 3.96, p < .001, and friends, = .18, t(5428) = 3.52, p < .001. In addition,
lower NA was associated with the presence of friends, = -.10, t(5428) = -1.96, p < .05. The
presence of one’s spouse or peripheral ties was not associated with PA or NA. Results in Model
6 suggest that the association between the presence of social partners and affect has to be
effects between person-specific characteristics and the presence of social partners. Individuals
higher in neuroticism reported lower PA, = -.04, t(5388) = -3.78, p < .001, and higher NA,
= .03, t(5388) = 2.96, p < .01. Individuals higher in depressive symptoms reported lower PA,
= -.05, t(5388) = -2.89, p < .01. In addition, individuals higher in loneliness reported higher
NA, = .20, t(5388) = 2.75, p < .01. For interaction effects, women, = -.32, t(5388) = -
2.42, p < .05, and individuals with more chronic health conditions, = -.11, t(5388) = -2.30, p
< .05, reported lower PA when they were with their spouses. Individuals higher in neuroticism
reported lower NA when they were with friends, = -.03, t(5388) = -2.15, p < .05. In addition,
individuals higher in loneliness reported higher PA when they were with their spouses,
Follow-up analyses were performed to control for possible backfilling based on the time
indicated by the electronic time stamps. Questionnaires that were not sealed with a legible time
stamp on the envelopes were classified as invalid. In addition, the time on the seal had to be
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 18
consistent with the finish time (within five minutes) on the questionnaire. Based on these criteria,
90% of all observations were classified as valid cases. Results of follow-up analyses using only
the valid observations and results based on all observations were substantially identical.
Discussion
Informed by Fingerman and Lang’s (2004) cube model, we examined the link between
the structural aspect and daily life affective experiences of social relationships. Findings showed
that momentary affect is associated with both the type of social partner present and individual
characteristics.
Consistent with previous studies, the oldest-old spent much more of their time alone
(over 70%), compared to adolescents (17%), adults (29%), young-old adults in the US (48%),
and young-old (59%) and old-old adults (62%) in Germany (Baltes et al., 1990; Klumb, 2004;
Larson, 1990). Being alone was associated with lower PA and was a particularly negative
experience for individuals higher in neuroticism. In addition, loneliness may not be eased simply
by being with others. Lonelier individuals reported higher NA in general. Yet higher PA was
observed when the spouse of lonely participants was present. Aside from limitations in statistical
power, this suggests that being with social partners other than one’s spouse may not ease the
unpleasant experience associated with loneliness. Our findings are consistent with the distinction
between solitude and loneliness (Jylhä & Saarenheimo, 2010), that being alone does not
necessarily imply loneliness and individuals may feel lonely in the presence of others (Larson,
1990).
Results showed that being with friends and other family members was associated with
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 19
more positive affective experiences in general. There were several significant interaction effects.
First, when with their spouses, men, but not women, reported higher PA. Second, individuals
with fewer chronic health conditions reported higher PA when with their spouses. Third,
individuals higher in neuroticism reported lower NA when with friends. Finally, individuals
higher in loneliness reported higher PA when with their spouses. We discuss these findings in the
This study has highlighted the importance of examining both person- and situation-
specific characteristics on momentary affect in the oldest-old. Our results suggest that whether
individuals benefit from the presence of others depends on who the individual is (person-specific
characteristics) and who they are with (situation-specific). For person-specific characteristics,
older adults bring to the social relationships a life-time of experiences which have effects on how
social partners interact with them (Fingerman & Pitzer, 2007). For situation-specific
characteristics, not all social ties are beneficial to older adults’ emotional well-being. Different
types of social partners convey distinct emotional meanings and instrumental benefits
(Carstensen et al., 1999; Rook et al., 2007). For instance, older adults tend to spend time with
their spouses and children for routine daily activities (Baltes et al., 1990; Larson et al., 1986). In
contrast, older adults tend to share leisure activities with friends, which lead to substantially
higher positive affect and arousal (Larson et al., 1986). To summarize, our findings indicate that
momentary affective experience in the oldest-old is a function of both situation- and person-
specific characteristics.
Taking into account both situation- and person-specific characteristics, the present study
complements earlier work on social relationships in older adults in several ways. First, relative to
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 20
spouses, family, and friends, we know very little about peripheral ties in old age (Fingerman,
2009). Consistent with the social convoy model (Antonucci et al., 2010), the oldest-old adults did
not often interact with peripheral ties and being with these ties was not significantly associated
with PA or NA. Peripheral ties may be “weak ties” in terms of emotional closeness compared to
“core ties” (Fingerman, 2009). Interactions with peripheral ties are perhaps more driven by
instrumental support in terms of tangible resources, and not emotional support in terms of
companionship and emotional disclosure (Schwarzer & Leppin, 1991). Although the oldest-old
did not gain positive affective experiences from peripheral ties, their presence was not negative,
compared to the presence of one’s spouse for women and individuals with more chronic health
conditions. Findings in young-old adults show that daily interactions with spouses and family
was characterized by negative affective experiences and preoccupied with daily routines (Larson
et al., 1986). However, the present study found that only the presence of one’s spouse for
contrast, the presence of other family members was associated with positive affective
experiences. It is possible that the oldest-old shared different activities with their spouses and
other family members, compared to young-old adults. For example, although the nature of
activities engaged in with one’s spouse was not examined, it may be that they are centered
around caregiving, while interactions with other family members possibly from the younger
generations may involve more fulfilling leisure activities (Baltes et al., 1990). Future
examination of routine vs. leisure activities may explain why the presence of other family
members was associated with negative affective experiences in the young-old but not in the
oldest-old.
Second, the present study highlighted individual differences and the presence of different
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 21
types of social partners and their associated PA and NA. Women reported lower PA than men
when with their spouses. This is consistent with past findings that effects of social relationships
on health outcomes differed between men and women (Shumaker & Hill, 1991). For instance,
the effect of social support on mortality was much stronger in women than in men across age
groups. Because women are more likely than men to be support providers in old age, and
possibly more so in the oldest-old if they are still married, older women may not gain as many
positive emotional experiences when they are the primary caregivers (Shumaker & Hill, 1991).
Third, individuals with more chronic health problems reported more negative affective
experiences when with their spouses. This is consistent with past findings that being the support
recipient because of poor health may create tension with one’s spouse because s/he may perceive
the relationship as not equitable or reciprocal (Rook, 1987). In addition, the experience of
positive affect with a spouse may be undermined because of burdens of chronic illnesses.
Paradoxically, individuals with poor health are more likely to be socially isolated and not
receiving the support that they need (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010). This has clinical and policy
implications such that services are needed to resolve spousal conflicts arising from care-giving in
Our findings showed that individuals higher in neuroticism reported less negative
affective experiences when they were with friends. Higher neuroticism is associated with
negative social interactions in general (McCrae & Costa, 2003) and interactions with spouse and
family can involve tension and ambiguity (Fingerman et al., 2008; Hoppmann et al., 2011). In
addition, individuals higher in neuroticism may be particularly uncomfortable about seeking out
less familiar people, e.g., counselors (Bolger & Zuckerman, 1995), thus, friends may be the
when they were with their spouses. This is consistent with past research that older adults living
with a spouse reported being less lonely than those living alone or widowed (Savikko, Routasalo,
Tilvis, Strandberg, & Pitkälä, 2005). Our findings provide partial support that individuals may
feel lonely even in the presence of others (De Jong Gierveld, Tilburg, & Dykstra, 2006). Aside
from limitations in statistical power and the relatively infrequent presence of social partners
other than one’s spouse, lonelier individuals did not experience the presence of other types of
social partners differently compared to the less lonely. Our findings suggest that being with one’s
spouse may be particularly beneficial to lonelier individuals’ affective experience. Both societal
arrangements and personal choices determine with whom and how older adults interact
(Fingerman & Lang, 2004), including whether older adults decide to (re)marry or cohabit. Most
older adults without partners live alone and the percentage of older adults living alone is rising
(Wilmoth, 1998). Although the present study did not examine the effect of living arrangement,
our findings have policy implications for the provision of social services to encourage socializing
and possibly partner-seeking in old age, to relieve the negative consequences associated with
Our findings should be considered in light of several limitations. First, our sample is
healthy and mostly community-dwelling. Attrition analyses using ADuLTS participants who
were also in ALSA revealed that ADuLTS participants were positively selected, compared to
those who either did not survive or declined to participate. The positive selection of ADuLTS
effects and making effects harder to detect (Anstey & Luszcz, 2002). Results may not be
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 23
generalizable to oldest-old adults living in residential care where poor health and social isolation
are more common (Suzman, Willis, & Manton, 1992). In addition, we did not have details of
living arrangements, e.g. living alone or with family. Future research is needed to examine
patterns of social interactions and affect among those living in different settings.
Second, brief versions of PA, NA, and depressive symptoms were used to reduce
participant burden. Despite the reasonable psychometric properties of the PA and NA in the
context of a time-sampling design, the limited number of items and the small sample size
psychometric literature (Shrout & Lane, 2011). Specifically, the 3-item PA and NA measures did
not allow the examination of affect vs. arousal, or orthogonal vs. correlated factors in the present
study. Compared to PA, NA showed lower mean and less between- and within-person variance.
The fewer significant effects for NA may in part be due to floor effects5. In addition, the 10-item
CES-D and the 5-item “lonely dissatisfaction” subscale of the PGCMS did not show acceptable
internal consistency. Despite any limitations in the measures, our results were largely consistent
Third, the present study focused on the structure of social relationships and did not
examine other aspects, such as relationship quality (Antonucci et al., 2010). The emotional
benefits from the presence of social partners may be dependent on relationship quality, which is
in part determined by whether individuals are the support provider or recipient in a social
relationship (Ingersoll-Dayton, Morgan, & Antonucci, 1997). Future research should examine
relationship quality and effects of support provider vs. recipient roles on emotional experiences
in the oldest-old. In addition, drawing from Fingerman and Lang’s (2004) cube model of social
relationships, future research may include process variables to further illuminate the underlying
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 24
emotional, motivational, and cognitive processes that link social relationship structures and
psychological outcomes. For instance, coping strategies involved in the daily interactions with
various social partners may explain why lonelier individuals have more positive affective
experiences with their spouses, whereas women and individuals with more chronic health
conditions experience more negative affective experiences with their spouses. It is possible that
lonelier individuals tend to use emotion-focused coping strategies (Lazarus, 1996) and view their
spouses as a source of companionship (Rook, 1990). In contrast, women and individuals with
more chronic health conditions may tend to use problem-focused coping strategies (Lazarus,
1996) in their interactions with their spouses, engaging in instrumental activities centered around
care-giving.
Fourth, given its age range, our sample is comprised mostly of widowed individuals and
the inclusion of widowed, married, and single individuals in our sample allows our findings to be
generalizable to the population of oldest-olds. However, it is possible that among married oldest-
old adults, patterns of social interactions are centered around their spouses’ care-giving needs.
Compared to the widowed, the presence of other family and friends may be particularly
beneficial for the married if this provides relief from care-giving demands. Future research that
oversamples married oldest-old adults is needed to examine whether the pattern of social
The last limitation affects all time-sampling designs, i.e., effects of instrumentation and
procedures which may bias individuals’ responses (Iida, Shrout, Laurenceau, & Bolger, 2012)
and effect sizes (Snijders, 2005). The significant linear effect of time on PA possibly reflected
measurement reactivity (Barta et al., 2012). We adopted several procedures to reduce and control
for measurement reactivity. First, participants were instructed to seal the envelopes to ensure that
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 25
responses of previous self-reports were not accessible to reduce reactivity due to self-monitoring.
Second, to adjust for possible response shift, i.e., participants changing the meaning they assign
to a rating, the linear effect of time on PA and NA was controlled in the models examined. Past
research has used both time-sampling and event-sampling designs to study social partners of
older adults (Larson et al., 1985; Nezlek, Richardson, Green, & Schatten-Jones, 2002). Instead of
responding to prompts at fixed time-intervals as done herein, participants fill out self-reports as
soon as possible after a social interaction in event-sampling studies (Nezlek et al., 2002). Event-
sampling designs generate self-reports of affective reactions and evaluations targeting the social
experiences and social interactions that occurred during the same time interval. Because affective
experiences may or may not be contingent on social interactions that were reported at the same
time, results of our correlational study warrant cautious interpretation. We cannot ascertain
whether the affective experiences reported were reactions to the presence of social partners or
other intervening events. Future time-sampling studies may test lead-lag effects to examine the
causal and cumulative effects of social interactions on affective experiences. In addition, the
present study used fixed intervals and compared to results of a time-sampling study using
random intervals (Klumb, 2004), our sample of oldest-olds reported spending more time alone
(71%) than did the sample of older adults (M age = 80.6 years; 62% of the time alone). Our
participants might have picked beep schedules that sampled times when they were likely to be
alone. Future studies using random time-sampling intervals will be needed to ascertain whether
the age difference in time spent alone is due to differences in the fixed vs. random time-sampling
schedule.
Consistent with similar time-sampling studies on momentary affect in old age (e.g.,
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 26
Charles et al., 2010), effect sizes of our findings are small. Small effect sizes are expected in
manipulated and effects of other variables are controlled. The strength of a time-sampling design
is that the phenomenon of interest is examined as it unfolds in the natural environment, thus
ecological validity is enhanced. Unlike laboratory-based studies, however, our study cannot
control for all other factors that may impact fluctuations in affect. Thus, both time-sampling and
in late life. In addition, intervention studies examining the effect of friendship and caregiver
support programs are needed to explore the practical significance of our findings on affective
experiences.
Conclusion
This study shows that the presence of various types of social partners is differentially
relationships play an important role in oldest-old adults’ well-being, it is not the case that the
presence of all social partners is associated with positive affective experiences. Owing to
heterogeneity among individuals, they differ in how much they benefit emotionally in the
presence of various social partners. Our findings attest to the value of including both person- and
well-being in the oldest-old. Future research may substantiate our findings by examining
underlying relationship-specific characteristics, such as daily activities performed alone and with
social partners. In summary, our findings extend the literature by showcasing the dynamic and
References
Alexopoulos, G. S. (2005). Depression in the elderly. The Lancet, 365(9475), 1961-1970. doi:
10.1016/s0140-6736(05)66665-2
Andresen, E. M., Malmgren, J. A., Carter, W. B., & Patrick, D. L. (1994). Screening for
Anstey, K. J., & Luszcz, M. A. (2002). Selective non-response to clinical assessment in the
function and dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 704-709. doi:
10.1002/gps.651
Antonucci, T. C., Fiori, K. L., Birditt, K., & Jackey, L. M. H. (2010). Convoys of social relations:
The Handbook of Life-Span Development (Vol. 2: Social and Emotional Development, pp.
Baltes, M. M., Wahl, H.-W., & Schmid-Furstoss, U. (1990). The daily life of elderly Germans:
Activity patterns, personal control, and functional health. Journal of Gerontology, 45(4),
Barta, W. D., Tennen, H., & Litt, M. D. (2012). Measurement reactivity in diary research. In M.
R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp.
Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000). From social integration to health:
Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science and Medicine, 51, 843-857. doi:
10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00065-4
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 28
Birditt, K., & Antonucci, T. C. (2008). Life sustaining irritations? Relationship quality and
mortality in the context of chronic illness. Social Science and Medicine, 67, 1291-1299.
doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.06.029
Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (1995). A framework for studying personality in the stress process.
Cacioppo, J. T., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, M. H., McClintock, M. K., Malarkey, W. B., Hawkley, L.
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of
066X.54.3.165
Charles, S. T., Luong, G., Almeida, D. M., Ryff, C., Sturm, M., & Love, G. (2010). Fewer ups
and downs: Daily stressors mediate age differences in negative affect. The Journals of
Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65B, 279-286. doi:
10.1093/geronb/gbq002
Cohen, S. (2004). Social relationships and health. American Psychologist, 59, 676-684. doi:
10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676
Cranford, J. A., Shrout, P. E., Iida, M., Rafaeli, E., Yip, T., & Bolger, N. (2006). A procedure for
detect change reliably? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 917-929. doi:
10.1177/0146167206287721
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 29
De Jong Gierveld, J., Tilburg, T. V., & Dykstra, P. A. (2006). Loneliness and social isolation. In
10.1037/a0017075
10.1111/j.1756-2589.2009.00010.x
Fingerman, K. L., & Lang, F. R. (2004). Coming together: A perspective on relationships across
the life span. In F. R. Lang & K. L. Fingerman (Eds.), Growing together: Personal
relationships across the life span (pp. 1-13). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Fingerman, K. L., & Pitzer, L. (2007). Socialization in old age. In P. D. Hastings & J. E. Grusec
Fingerman, K. L., Pitzer, L., Lefkowitz, E. S., Birditt, K. S., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Ambivalent
relationship qualities between adults and their parents: Implications for the well-being of
both parties. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63B,
P362-P371.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini-mental state": A practical
method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric
Giles, L. C., Glonek, G. F. V., Luszcz, M. A., & Andrews, G. R. (2005). Effect of social
networks on 10 year survival in very old Australians: the Australian longitudinal study of
10.1136/jech.2004.025429
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical
doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8
Hoppmann, C. A., Gerstorf, D., & Hibbert, A. (2011). Spousal associations between functional
limitation and depressive symptom trajectories: Longitudinal findings from the Study of
Asset and Health Dynamics Among the Oldest Old (AHEAD). Health Psychology, 30,
Hoppmann, C. A., & Klumb, P. L. (2006). Daily goal pursuits predict cortisol secretion and
mood states in employed parents with preschool children. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(6),
Iida, M., Shrout, P. E., Laurenceau, J.-P., & Bolger, N. (2012). Using diary methods in
Rindskopf & K. J. Sher (Eds.), APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol 1:
Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Morgan, D., & Antonucci, T. (1997). The effects of positive and negative
social exchanges on aging adults. Journal of Gerontology: Social Sciences, 52, S190-
S199.
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 31
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102-138). New York: Guilford Press.
Jylhä, M., & Saarenheimo, M. (2010). Loneliness and ageing: Comparative perspectives. In D.
Dannefer & C. Phillipson (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of social gerontology (pp. 317-
Klumb, P. L. (2004). Benefits from productive and consumptive activities: Results from the
Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (1994). Close emotional relationships in late life: Further
support for proactive aging in the social domain. Psychology and Aging, 9, 315-324.
Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. F. (1992). Promises and problems with the circumplex model of
emotion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion: Review of personality and social psychology (pp.
Larson, R. W. (1990). The solitary side of life: An examination of the time people spend alone
from childhood to old age. Developmental Review, 10, 155-183. doi: 10.1016/0273-
2297(90)90008-r
Larson, R. W., Mannell, R., & Zuzanek, J. (1986). Daily well-being of older adults with friends
Larson, R. W., Zuzanek, J., & Mannell, R. (1985). Being alone versus being with people:
Disengagement in the daily experience of older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 375-
Lawton, M. P. (1975). The Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale: A revision. Journal of
Lazarus, R. S. (1996). The role of coping in the emotions and how coping changes over the life
Luszcz, M. A., Giles, L., Eckermann, S., Edwards, P. A., Browne-Yung, K., & Hayles, C. (2007).
The Australian Longitudinal Study of Ageing: 15 years of ageing in South Australia (pp.
files/Documents/StudyOfAgeing.pdf
Luszcz, M. A., Hoppmann, C., Gerstorf, D., Walker, R., Edwards, P., & Hunter, M. (2011,
November). A week in the lives of oldest-old Australians: Initial findings from ADuLTS
study of development over the life span: Concepts and issues. In W. F. Overton (Ed.),
Handbook of life-Span development: Cognition, biology, and methods (Vol. 1, pp. 30-54).
Nezlek, J. B., Imbrie, M., & Shean, G. D. (1994). Depression and everyday social interaction.
Nezlek, J. B., Richardson, D. S., Green, L. R., & Schatten-Jones, E. C. (2002). Psychological
well-being and day-to-day social interaction among older adults. Personal Relationships,
Ranzijn, R., & Luszcz, M. (2000). Measurement of subjective quality of life of elders.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data
Rook, K. S. (1987). Reciprocity of social exchange and social satisfaction among older women.
Rook, K. S., Mavandadi, S., Sorkin, D. H., & Zettel, L. A. (2007). Optimizing social
Park & A. Spiro (Eds.), Psychology of health psychology and aging (pp. 267-285). New
York: Guilford.
Savikko, N., Routasalo, P., Tilvis, R. S., Strandberg, T. E., & Pitkälä, K. H. (2005). Predictors
Schwarzer, R., & Leppin, A. (1991). Social support and health: A theoretical and empirical
10.1177/0265407591081005
Shrout, P. E., & Lane, S. P. (2011). Psychometrics. In M. R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.),
Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 302-320). New York: Guilford.
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 34
Shumaker, S. A., & Hill, D. R. (1991). Gender differences in social support and physical health.
Smith, J., & Goodnow, J. J. (1999). Unasked-for support and unsolicited advice: age and the
Snijders, T. A. B. (2005). Power and sample size in multilevel linear models. In B. S. Everitt &
Suzman, R. M., Willis, D. P., & Manton, K. G. (Eds.). (1992). The oldest old. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Tsai, J. L., Knutson, B., & Fung, H. H. (2006). Cultural variation in affect valuation. Journal of
Walker, R., Luszcz, M., Gerstorf, D., & Hoppmann, C. (2011). Subjective well-being dynamics
in couples from the Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging. Gerontology, 57, 153-160.
doi: 10.1159/000318633
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures
of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social
Wilmoth, J. M. (1998). Living arrangement transitions among America's older adults. The
Wolff, J. L., & Kasper, J. D. (2006). Caregivers of frail elders: Updating a national profile. The
Footnotes
1
We recognize that the Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (SST) is valuable in the
interpretation and prediction of affective experiences in relation to social partners from the
lifespan perspective. However, we decided not to rely on SST because testing propositions from
the SST requires making use of future time perspective, which was not available in our study.
2
Attrition analyses showed that participants who took part in ADuLTS were positively
selected, among all ALSA participants. Using data from ALSA (waves 1, 3, 6, 9, and 11),
ADuLTS participants were younger, better educated, having better cognitive functioning, self-
rated health, and functional health, and lower depressive symptoms, compared to those who
either did not survive or refused to take part in ADuLTS. Effects of sample selectivity was
primarily due to mortality rather than drop-out for other reasons such as refusal to participate
(Luszcz et al., 2011, November). Using data from ALSA Wave 11 (2010), among the 95 ALSA
participants eligible to take part in ADuLTS, those who declined to participate (n = 44) were
significantly different from those who participated (n = 51) in age and free recall. ADuLTS
participants were significantly younger, t(92) = -2.12, p < .05, (M = 88.55 years, SD = 2.57 vs.
M = 89.81 years, SD = 3.19) and scored higher in free recall, t(93) = 2.55, p < .01, (M = 7.18, SD
= 1.51, vs. M = 6.27, SD = 1.81) than those who refused to participate. They did not differ from
each other in scores of attention and calculation, recall, cued recall, and Mini Mental State
Examination.
3
The ALSA participants and the additional group from the community did not differ
significantly from each other in the variables of interest, including age (ALSA group: M = 89.04,
SD = 2.50, community group: M = 88.02, SD = 3.83, t(32.70) = 1.19, p > .05), gender (ALSA
group: 64% women; community group: 71% women, χ2 (1) = .10, p > .05), neuroticism (ALSA
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 37
group: M = 14.60, SD = 4.79, community group: M = 14.33, SD = 5.43, t(40.74) = .21, p > .05),
12.60, t(43.86) = 1.06, p > .05), chronic health conditions (ALSA group: M = 2.22, SD = 2.13,
community group: M = 2.75, SD = 2.02, t(46.60) = -1.49, p > .05), and loneliness (ALSA group:
27% agree, community group: 38% agree, χ2 (1) = .37, p > .05).
4
Participants provided feedback by answering four questions (1 = not at all; 5 = very
much): (a) “Do you think that the alarm was loud enough?”, M = 3.86, SD = 1.34; (b) “Did the
people around you react negatively when you completed the questionnaires?”, M = 1.27, SD
= .75; (c) “To what extent did the questionnaire completion interfere with your daily routines?”,
M = 2.49, SD = 1.22; and (d) “Did your response to the questions result in you changing your
high face validity) were substantively identical to those reported in the main text.
6
Additional analyses were performed to further examine results of Model 6, using a
subsample of participants who were either married or in a de facto relationship (n = 21, 28% of
the whole sample; total number of observations = 808). Results related to the presence of spouse
were substantially identical with those reported herein, except that the interaction effect of
Spouse × Lonely on PA, = .11, t(1523) = 1.35, p > .05, became non-significant, and the
interaction effect of Spouse × Neuroticism on PA, = .03, t(1523) = 2.52, p < .05, became
significant. These effects are still in the same direction as in the original analyses. This
convergent evidence indicates that the effects related to the presence of spouse are relatively
solid although this is not true for the loneliness findings. However, we caution against over-
interpreting results of the additional analyses due to the very small sample size.
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 38
Table 1
Multilevel Model Results of Situation-Specific (Being Alone vs. Not) and Person-Specific
Model 3 Model 4
95% CI 95% CI
Goodness-of-fit
SE = standard error; Neuro = Neuroticism; Chronic = Chronic health conditions; AR(1) = First-
order auto-regression; AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion.
Gender was coded -.5 = men and .5 = women. CES-D, neuroticism, chronic health conditions,
Results of random effects were not shown for simplicity. Results of random effects can be
Table 2
Multilevel Model Results of Situation-Specific (Being with Different Social Partners) and
Model 5 Model 6
95% CI 95% CI
Goodness-of-fit
Social Partners and Affect in the Oldest-Old 43
SE = standard error; Neuro = Neuroticism; Chronic = Chronic health conditions; AR(1) = First-
order auto-regression; AIC = Akaike information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion.
Gender was coded -.5 = men and .5 = women. CES-D, neuroticism, chronic health conditions,
Results of random effects were not shown for simplicity. Results of random effects can be