Caprara 2005
Caprara 2005
Caprara 2005
191-217
CAPRARA AND
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
STECA ACROSS AGES
PATRIZIA STECA
University of Milan, “Biocca”
The present study tested the posited structural path of influence through which per-
ceived self–efficacy of affect regulation operates in concert with perceived inter-
personal self–efficacy to determine prosocial behavior, which in turn influences
satisfaction with life in four age groups. A strong sense of efficacy in the regulation
of positive and negative affect was associated with a high perceived efficacy in the
management of social relationships and in empathic engagement in others’ emo-
tional experiences. Interpersonal self–efficacy directly affected prosocial behavior
and entirely mediated the influence of affective self–efficacy on it. As predicted,
prosocial behavior directly influenced life satisfaction, showing a higher path of in-
fluence for the oldest group as compared to the other ones. The structural model
provided a better fit for the data than did alternative models.
The literature on well–being and quality of life shows that people gener-
ally feel happy and satisfied with their lives. People judge their lives as
more positive than negative across cultures, regardless of their socioeco-
nomic status, and despite functional disabilities (Chwalisz, Diener, &
This study was partially supported by grants from MURST–Cofin (2000–2002) to Gian
Vittorio Caprara.
The article was prepared while Gian Vittorio Caprara was a Fellow at The Swedish
Collegium for Advanced Study in the Social Sciences, Uppsala, academic year 2001–2002.
We thank Laura Carstensen, Albert Bandura, Daniel Cervone, and Rocío
Fernández–Ballesteros for their comments on the early draft of the present article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gian Vittorio Caprara,
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology 2, University of Rome “La Sapienza,”
via dei Marsi, 78 – 00185 Rome, Italy; E-mail: [email protected].
191
192 CAPRARA AND STECA
Gallagher, 1988; Diener & Diener, 1996; Veenhoven, 1993). The fact that
the documented correlation between objective life conditions and sub-
jective happiness is small has been called the paradox of well–being
(Filipp, 1996; Staudinger, Fleeson, & Baltes, 1999). The paradox seems
more apparent in research on aging and well–being because, despite the
difficulties of aging (e.g., physical deterioration, deaths of old friends
and relatives), older people do not report concomitant decreases in most
aspects of well–being. Numerous studies using cross–sectional data
have found negligible age differences in life satisfaction and other forms
of subjective well–being. (For a complete review, see Diener & Suh,
1998). Some studies have even found an increase in feelings of well–be-
ing among older adults, in that they report less anxiety (Lawton, Kleban,
& Dean, 1993) and have a better balance of positive to negative affect
(Ryff, 1989).
Central questions among life–span researchers concern how people
may maintain a high level of subjective well–being throughout their
lives, and what are the specific personal and social processes that may
contribute to feelings of well–being at different stages of life. In the pres-
ent study, we intend to explore age differences in life satisfaction and to
verify the role played in its promotion by prosocial behavior in concert
with self–efficacy beliefs related to affective and interpersonal domains
of functioning.
Our argument draws upon two primary assumptions, which lead to
two main hypotheses. First, we assume that the capacity to benefit others
contributes to life satisfaction because it is critical to maintaining mutually
rewarding relationships with others. Accordingly, prosocial behavior
should prove particularly beneficial at ages when people’s well–being de-
pends greatly upon support from others. Because being able to recipro-
cate tends to make people less dependent upon the benevolence of others,
the capacity to benefit others is a valuable resource, especially when one
most needs others’ support. Second, we assume that caring about others’
well–being is not just a matter of good character, but rather that it implies
a belief in one’s capacity to deal effectively with oneself and others, both
emotionally and at the level of relationship. Thus, we hypothesize that (1)
prosocial behavior contributes to life satisfaction, especially when aging
increases the importance of support from others; and (2) the more people
believe that they are able to handle their affect and their relationships
effectively, the more they are inclined to behave prosocially.
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR
Prosocial behavior refers to voluntary actions undertaken to benefit oth-
ers. It includes a variety of behaviors, such as sharing, donating, caring,
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR ACROSS AGES 193
FIGURE 1. Conceptual model of the path of influence exerted by efficacy beliefs across do-
mains of functioning.
to build enduring personal resources, not only at the physical and intel-
lectual levels, but also at the social and psychological levels. Recent lon-
gitudinal studies suggest that optimism and the experience and expres-
sion of positive emotions are associated with positive long–term
outcomes and a longer life (Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001; Harker &
Keltner, 2001; Maruta, Colligan, Malinchoc, & Offord, 2000; Peterson,
Seligman, Yurko, Martin, & Friedman, 1998).
The expression of positive emotions constitutes a signal of friendliness
and sociability that favors mutuality and helping behavior (Frijda &
Mesquita, 1994; Isen, 1987; Keltner & Kring, 1998). The experience and
expression of positive emotions also promotes engagement with others,
cooperation among individuals and groups, and positive intimate rela-
tionships (Berry & Willigham, 1997; Carstensen, Gottman, & Levenson,
1995; Cunningham, 1988; Isen, 1987; Watson, Clark, McIntyre, &
Hamaker, 1992). Over time the repeated experience and expression of
positive affect accompanied by proper strategies to manage negative af-
fect contributes to satisfying and lasting interpersonal relations that pro-
mote individual well–being by direct and indirect pathways. In addi-
tion, a vast literature attests to the importance of interpersonal relations
in the formation of personal identity and in the promotion of health and
well–being over the entire life span (Caprara & Cervone, 2000).
Whatever the various emphases placed by different theoretical ap-
proaches on the influence of interpersonal relationships in the develop-
ment and functioning of individuals, there is no doubt that they play a
critical role in mediating feelings and cognitions, and in setting the con-
ditions for actions. In short, they make life worth living (Bateson, 1972;
Hinde, 1997; Sullivan, 1953).
Contemporary developments that extend the early conceptualiza-
tions of Vygotsky (1934/1962), Ainsworth (1967), and Bowlby (1969,
1973, 1980) further emphasize the importance of early interpersonal re-
lationships in the development of cognition (Rogoff, 1998; Van Geert,
1998), affect (Sroufe, 1996), and relational bonds (Bartholomew, 1993;
Hazan & Shaver, 1994). Most research on affect has focused on the fre-
quency of different emotional experiences, and most research on inter-
personal relations has centered on the quality of relational experiences
and their outcomes. To our knowledge, the systematic study of mastery
in emotional and interpersonal experiences and the study of the influ-
ence that the belief in self–efficacy exerts on the management of affect
and interpersonal emotions are mostly due to social cognitive theory. A
number of findings have demonstrated the impact of self–efficacy be-
liefs on the successful handling of family, school, work, and social rela-
tionships (Bandura, 1997). New studies on affective self–regulatory be-
lief (Bandura et al., 2003; Caprara et al., 1999, 2003) have broadened
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR ACROSS AGES 199
METHOD
PARTICIPANTS
The participants in this study were 259 men and 253 women, ranging in
age from 20 to 87. They were recruited by an Italian national survey and
were divided into four age groups: young adults (n = 135, mean age =
27.30, SD = 5.51 ), adults (n = 120, mean age = 44.21, SD = 4.02), mid-
dle–aged adults (n = 130, mean age = 56.70, SD = 4.53) and elderly adults
(n = 127, mean age = 73.13, SD = 5.13).
The participants varied widely in demographic and socioeconomic
background. Twenty-three percent were unmarried, 63% were married,
3% were divorced, and 11% were widows or widowers. Education
ranged from 5 to 18 years; generally, elderly adults had a lower level of
education than the other age groups; the level was higher for men in the
older age groups and higher for women in the younger ones. Seventeen
percent were in the professional or managerial ranks, 23% were mer-
chants or employees in various types of businesses, 5% were workers,
11% were students, 17% were housewives, 25% were retired, and 2%
were unemployed.
MEASURES
Participants completed a set of scales measuring the variables of theoret-
ical interest.
Affective Self–Regulatory Efficacy. Affective self–regulatory efficacy
was measured by 17 items concerning the perceived capability to man-
age one’s emotional life (Caprara & Gerbino, 2001). Perceived self–effi-
cacy to manage positive affect was measured by 8 items in terms of the per-
ceived ability to express liking and affection toward others, to express
enthusiasm and enjoyment, and to feel satisfaction at personal accom-
plishments. The sample item, “I can feel gratified over achieving what I
set out to do” assessed perceived efficacy to express satisfaction. Per-
ceived self–efficacy to regulate negative affect was assessed by 9 items in
terms of the perceived capability to manage negative affect in the face of
anxiety–arousing threats, anger provocation, rejection, and disrespect,
and to control worry when things go wrong. “I can get over irritation
quickly for wrongs I have experienced” was a sample item.
Interpersonal Self–Regulatory Efficacy. Interpersonal self–regulatory ef-
ficacy was measured by 24 items concerning the perceived ability to man-
age one’s interpersonal relationships (Caprara, Gerbino, & Delle Fratte,
2001). Perceived social self–efficacy was measured by 14 items in terms of the
perceived capability to express personal opinions in groups, to share per-
sonal experiences with others, to invite people to go out together, to know
202 CAPRARA AND STECA
people in a new situation, and to help others to integrate into one’s circle of
friends. “I can share an interesting good experience I had with other peo-
ple,” was a sample item. Perceived empathic self–efficacy was measured by
10 items in terms of the perceived ability to sense another person’s feelings
and need for emotional support, to experience emotions from another per-
son’s perspective, to respond empathetically to others’ distress and misfor-
tune, and to be sensitive to how one’s actions affect the feelings of others.
The sample item, “I can experience how a person in trouble feels,” assessed
perceived empathic capability for distress.
Participants rated the strength of their self–efficacy beliefs on 5–point
scales ranging from 1 = perceived incapability to 5 = complete self–assurance.
A principal components factor analysis with Oblimin rotation revealed
a four–factor structure, corresponding to the posited four domains of
self–efficacy functioning, each representing a single factor. The alpha
reliability coefficients for the self–efficacy factor scales were uniformly
high for each age group. The coefficients were .82, .86, .90, and .80 for
managing positive affect; .85, .85, .87, and .81 for regulating negative af-
fect; .89, .91, .93, and .87 for social efficacy; and .82, .89, .89, and .87 for
empathic efficacy.
Prosocial Behavior. Participants rated their prosocialness on a 16–item
scale (with 5–point response) that assessed their degree of helpfulness,
sharing, consoling, supportiveness, and cooperativeness (Caprara et al.,
in press; Caprara, Steca, Zellli, & Capanna, in press). “I try to help oth-
ers” and “I try to console people who are sad” are sample items. Factor
analysis revealed a single factor structure; the alpha reliability coeffi-
cients for the four age groups were .90, .93, .90, and .91.
Satisfaction with Life . Participants rated their life satisfaction on the
five–item Satisfaction With Life scale (Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Grif-
fin, 1985). Participants rated on a 7–point scale the extent to which they
feel generally satisfied with life. “In most ways my life is close to my
ideal” and “So far I have gotten the important things I want in my life”
are sample items. The alpha reliability coefficients for the four age
groups were .86, .91, .88, and .82.
Participants also completed a brief Physical Health scale composed of
six items that measure the frequency of some health-related behaviors.
The response format varies from 1 = never in my life to 5 = more than fif-
teen times during the past year. “I has been sick for more than two days”
and “I had a general checkup” are sample items.
RESULTS
Before conducting the analyses, we examined the data for univariate
and multivariate variables’ distributions, using the procedure devised
TABLE 1. Means and Standard Deviations of the Different Variables for Men and Women in the Four Age Groups
203
Social self–efficacy 3.65 (.49) 3.43 (.57) 3.24 (.58) 3.22 (.59) 3.27 (.62) 3.20 (.73) 3.15 (.50) 3.16 (.63)
Empathic self–efficacy 3.70 (.44) 3.67 (.40) 3.57 (.54) 3.76 (.45) 3.59 (.44) 3.65 (.57) 3.38 (.55) 3.57 (.51)
Prosocial behavior 3.53 (.57) 3.57 (.48) 3.42 (.57) 3.67 (.61) 3.62 (.47) 3.68 (.57) 3.30 (.67) 3.46 (.62)
Satisfaction with life 4.78 (1.16) 4.51 (1.40) 4.63 (1.40) 4.72 (1.48) 5.05 (1.27) 5.04 (1.26) 4.91 (1.43) 4.66 (1.15)
204 CAPRARA AND STECA
PATTERN OF INFLUENCES
We tested the posited structural model on the covariance matrix with the
EQS program (Bentler, 2001). To test age differences in the posited paths,
we analyzed the structural model by using the multiple groups model
approach, which estimated simultaneously the same pattern of relation-
ships among variables in the four age groups. In this approach, equiva-
lence among different samples is evaluated by constraints that impose
identical estimates for the model’s parameters (Byrne, 1994;
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR ACROSS AGES 205
TABLE 2. Correlation Matrix of the different variables, for the total sample
1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Self–efficacy to manage
positive affect —
2. Self–efficacy to regulate
negative affect .20*** —
3. Social self–efficacy .60*** .29*** —
4. Empathic self–efficacy .55*** .30*** .45*** —
5. Prosocial behavior .43*** .20*** .40*** .60*** —
6. Satisfaction with life .24*** .27*** .20*** .23*** .25*** —
ALTERNATIVE MODELS
Although the refined model provided an excellent fit for the empirical
data, plausible alternative models were also tested. One structural
model gave causal primacy to prosocial behavior, affecting the various
forms of self–efficacy beliefs, which in turn affect life satisfaction. This
model provided a poorer fit to the data. It yielded a significant chi
square, χ2 (248, 512) = 474.789, p < .001, and fared less well on the other
indices of goodness of fit, with an NNFI = .91, a CFI = .92, and an RMSEA
= .042.
A second plausible model reversed the direction of causation, with life
satisfaction influencing prosocial behavior, which in turn influences the
different forms of efficacy beliefs. Even this model provided a poorer fit
for the data. It yielded a significant chi square, χ2 (249, 512) = 489.254, p <
.001, and fared less well on the other indices of goodness of fit, with an
NNFI = .91, a CFI = .92, and an RMSEA = .044.
into account, thus adding new elements that may help to promote suc-
cessful aging. Nor do our findings contradict other findings concerning
the importance of interpersonal relations for successful aging.
It may seem surprising that older adults score lower in empathic
self–efficacy compared to other groups. Although one might expect
more charitable wisdom from the elderly, awareness of one’s own vul-
nerability may recommend prudence in identifying with the misery of
others so that succor does not turn into contagion. As proposed by the
“model of selective optimization with compensation” (Baltes & Baltes,
1990) and by socioemotional selectivity theory (Carstensen, 1991, 1992,
1995), the capacity to select activities and relations to maximize social
and emotional gains and minimize social and emotional risks is particu-
larly decisive in preserving healthy functioning in old age. In particular,
socioemotional selectivity theory predicts that as age increases and time
is perceived as more limited, there is a change in emotional and interper-
sonal experiences connected to a new balance of interpersonal goals.
Those related to the acquisition of knowledge assume less relevance,
and emotionally meaningful goals are increasingly pursued. The selec-
tion of social partners becomes characterized by a greater engagement in
smaller, but more emotionally centered, social networks; this allows an
efficacious way to avoid negative emotional states and to optimize posi-
tive ones (Carstensen, 1991, 1992, 1995; Carstensen, Isaacowitz, &
Charles, 1999). In agreement with these approaches, our findings point
to the great contribution of prosocial behavior to life satisfaction.
As time goes on and as social choices are made with increasing selec-
tivity, with less emphasis on the novelty of activities and situations, and
with more concern for the quality of relationships, the capacity to have
restricted and selected groups of significant others becomes increas-
ingly important. However, it is unlikely that people are able to have re-
warding relationships, even if few in number, unless they are able to cul-
tivate these relationships with mutual trust, confidence, and support. It
is likely that the more restricted the number of relationships, the more
important the investment one puts into them; and the more satisfaction
one derives from them, the more important becomes the capacity to
nourish them. This probably explains the considerable amount of vari-
ance in life satisfaction accounted for by caring, sharing, and helping in
old age, in comparison with other ages. Nevertheless, we have the im-
pression that this has not been fully appreciated in the literature on
successful aging.
Obviously, some people are more inclined than others to behave
prosocially, and some situations foster prosocial behavior more than
others, because the value assigned to prosocial behavior in its various
forms may significantly change across cultures. Our findings show that
212 CAPRARA AND STECA
REFERENCES
(1999). Age differences in personality across the adult life span: Parallels in five cul-
tures. Developmental Psychology, 35, 466–477.
Midlarsky, E. (1991). Helping as coping. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Prosocial behavior (pp.
238–264). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Midlarsky, E., & Hannah, M. E. (1989). The generous elderly: Naturalistic studies of dona-
tions across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 346–351.
Midlarsky, E., & Kahana, E. (1994). Altruism in later life. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Mroczek, D., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A de-
velopmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
75, 1333–1349.
Mussen, P., & Eisenberg, N. (2001). Prosocial development in context. In A. C. Bohart & D.
J. Stipek (Eds.), Constructive and destructive behavior (pp. 103–126). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Pasupathi, M., Henry, R. M., & Carstensen, L. L. (in press). Age and ethnicity differences in
storytelling to young children: Emotionality, relationality, and socialization. Psy-
chology and Aging.
Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E. P., Yurko, K. H., Martin, L. R., & Friedman, H. S. (1998).
Catastrophizing and untimely death. Psychological Science, 9, 127–130.
Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & D. Kuhn
& R. S. Siegker (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2: Cognition, perception,
and language (5th ed., pp. 679–744). New York: Wiley.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Beyond Ponce de Leon and life satisfaction: New directions in quest of
successful aging. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 35–55.
Salovey, P. (1992). Mood–indiced self–focused attention. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 62, 699–707.
Schroeder, D. A., Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., & Piliavin, J. A. (1995). The psychology of help-
ing and altruism: Problems and puzzles. New York: McGraw Hill.
Scott–Lennox, J. A., & Scott–Lennox, R. D. (1995). Sex–race differences in social support
and depression in older low–income adults. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation
modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 199–216). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sills, D. L. (1968). International encyclopedia of social sciences. New York: The Macmillan
Company & The Free Press.
Smith, T. W. (1992). Hostility and health: Current status of a psychosomatic hypothesis.
Health Psychology, 11, 139–150.
Sroufe, A. (1996). Emotional development. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Staudinger, U. M., Fleeson, W., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). Predictors of subjective physical
health and global well–being: Similarities and differences between the United
States and Germany. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 305–319.
Stephan, W. G., & Finlay, K. (1999). The role of empathy in improving intergroup relations.
Journal of Social Issues, 55, 729–743.
Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychology. New York: Norton.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1989). Using multivariate statistics. New York: Harper &
Row.
Taylor, S. E., Peplau, L. A., & Sears, D. O. (2000). Social psychology. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46,
35–57.
Van Geert, P. (1998). A dynamic systems model of basic development mechanisms: Piaget,
Vygotsky, and beyond. Psychological Review, 105, 634–677.
Veenhoven, E. (1993). Happiness in nations. Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Risbo.
PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR ACROSS AGES 217
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language (Trans.) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original
work published 1934)
Watson, D. (1988). Intraindividual and interindividual analyses of positive and negative
affect: Their relation to health complaints, perceived stress, and daily activities.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1020–1030.
Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford.
Watson, D., Clark, L., McIntyre, C., & Hamaker, S. (1992). Affect, personality, and social ac-
tivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 1011–1025.
Wentowski, G. (1981). Reciprocity and the coping strategies of older people. Gerontologist,
21, 600–609.
Williams, S. L. (1995). Self–efficacy, anxiety, and phobic disorders. In J. E. Maddux (Ed.),
Self–efficacy, adaptation, and adjustment: Theory, research and application (pp. 69–107).
New York: Plenum.
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology, the new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Wilson, E. O. (1978). On human nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wylie, C. M. (1984). Contrasts in the health of elderly men and women: An analysis of re-
cent data for whites in the United States. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 32,
670–675.