Meditation and Cognitive Function Research Study
Meditation and Cognitive Function Research Study
Meditation and Cognitive Function Research Study
OTS Master's Level Projects & Papers STEM Education & Professional Studies
4-2021
Part of the Cognitive Psychology Commons, and the Educational Psychology Commons
MEDITATION AND COGNITIVE FUNCTION RESEARCH STUDY
by
Darrin Landry
MASTERS OF SCIENCE
Approved by:
Darrin Landry
Old Dominion University, 2021
Director: Dr. Michael Kosloski
Although research has shown that long-term guided meditation and mindfulness practices
improve attention, memory, and processing speed, the research on the effects of short-term self-
guided meditation on improving cognitive processing and memory is limited. This study
examined whether a brief self-guided meditation practice improved cognitive processing speed
and memory capacity. The participants engaged in a five-day program which consisted of taking
an online mental speed test and memory recall test, then meditated for 15 minutes a day, for five
days. After completing the five-day meditation program, the participants took the two cognitive
processing tests again. The results on the test scores showed an average increase in three points
in the mental test and two points in the memory test scores. A paired-sample equal variance t-test
determined there was no significant statistical difference made by the five-day meditation
experiment. These findings in this study build on the knowledge and understanding of the impact
of short-term meditation.
practice.
iii
This thesis is dedicated to Harry and Jean Landry. My loving parents who patiently encouraged
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
There are many people who have contributed to the successful completion of this
dissertation. I extend many, many thanks to my committee members for their patience and hours
of guidance on my research and editing of this manuscript. The untiring consistent efforts of my
major advisor Mickey Kosloski, Ph.D. deserve special recognition. Thank you so much for all
your help over the years. All my professors and adjuncts in the Darden Department of Education,
especially Dr. John Baaki, thanks for your professionalism and teachings.
A special mention goes out to my family and friends, especially Jaimee Campbell, who
have supported and encouraged me throughout this journey to getting a Master’s Degree.
vi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
CHAPTER 1 .................................................................................................................................. 1
INTRODUCTION......................................................................................................................... 1
Problem Statement.................................................................................................................................... 3
Research Questions................................................................................................................................... 3
Background and Significance ................................................................................................................... 3
Limitations ................................................................................................................................................ 5
Assumptions ............................................................................................................................................. 5
Definition of Terms .................................................................................................................................. 6
Overview of Chapters ............................................................................................................................... 9
CHAPTER II: REVIEW OF LITERATURE ............................................................................ 9
Benefits of Meditation .............................................................................................................................. 9
Benefits of Meditation on Cognitive Functioning .................................................................................. 11
Benefits of Meditation Measured by Changes in Brain Structure and Activity ..................................... 12
Benefits of Meditation Measured by Cognitive Performance Tests ....................................................... 13
SUMMARY ........................................................................................................................................... 16
CHAPTER III: METHODS AND PROCEDURES ................................................................ 17
Participants ............................................................................................................................................. 17
Instrument Design................................................................................................................................... 18
Statistical Analysis ................................................................................................................................. 19
Summary................................................................................................................................................. 21
CHAPTER IV: FINDINGS........................................................................................................ 21
References .................................................................................................................................... 29
Appendices ................................................................................................................................... 39
vii
List of Tables
Table 1 ................................................................................................................................22
Table 2 ................................................................................................................................23
Table 3 ................................................................................................................................24
Table 4 ................................................................................................................................24
1
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
relaxation or altered state of awareness, has many benefits attributed to its practice (Alexander et
al., 2003; Bertone, 2020; Zeidan et al., 2010). There is a growing body of scientific research
being conducted concerning these benefits in overlapping areas such as psychological health,
well-being, and cognitive function (i.e., the mental processes of attention control, working
memory, processing speed, long-term memory, perception, reading comprehension and problem-
solving.)
anxiety levels, addictive behaviours, depression, stress-cortisol levels, and blood pressure (Baer
et al., 2003; Chung, et al., 2012; Germer et al., 2005; Hayes et al., 2004; Manocha et al., 2011;
Schreiner, & Malcolm, 2008; Singh et al., 2007). Research studies have also demonstrated a
awareness, attention, memory, and mental processing speed (Badart et al., 2018; Chambers et al.,
2008; Ebaid et al., 2017; Heeren et al., 2009; Jha et al., 2007; Mrazek et al., 2013; Rothschild, et
al., 2017). These benefits tend to impact each other; for example, improvements in
psychological health have been shown to improve cognitive function (Bunce et al., 2008;
Llewellyn et al., 2008). Similarly, improvements in one area of cognitive function have an effect
on other areas, such as gray-matter volume, sleep quality, blood flow in the brain, and
psychomotor speed (Cranson et al., 1991; Hölzel et al., 2011; Jedrczak et al., 1986; Pagnoni &
This study focused on the effects of meditation on two areas of cognitive function:
processing speed and working memory. Processing speed is defined as the speed at which one
processes information (Lichtenberger & Kaufman, 2012). Working memory is defined as a small
amount of information that can be held in the mind for cognitive tasks (Nelson, 2014). The term
is often used in the field of psychology to describe and measure intelligence, information
processing, learning, and problem-solving (Jha et al., 2010). It is differentiated from long-term
memory which is the vast amount of information accumulated during one’s life.
The body of research reviewed generally examined the benefits of formal meditation
programs lasting eight weeks or longer, or where the participants had years of meditation
practice prior to the study (Hall, 1999; Helber et al. 2012; Jha et al., 2010; Katherine et al. 2010;
Monk-Turner, 2003). For example, one popular mindfulness based program used by
psychologists and some medical treatment facilities called the Mindfulness-based stress
reduction program (MBSR) lasts eight weeks, followed by a one day six-hour session (Carmody
& Baer 2009). Several studies have been conducted on the effects of a meditation program called
Transcendental Meditation (TM). The participants practice for periods ranging from six months
to 20 months (Alexander et al., 2003). These programs require extensive time and financial
period each day, can improve cognitive function. A short-term self-guided program is available
to a wider population than formal, long-term, supervised meditation program. To some the
former would be more affordable. Demonstrated meditation benefits could extend to job
performance, student test taking, and overall well-being. This research study examined the
3
Problem Statement
Long-term meditation programs lasting eight weeks or longer have established positive
physical and mental benefits of practicing meditation. (Baer et al., 2003; Chung, et al., 2012;
Germer et al., 2005; Hayes et al., 2004; Jha et al., 2010; Manocha et al., 2011; Schreiner, &
Malcolm, 2008; Singh et al., 2007). These are often long-term formal programs guided by a
The purpose of this study was to determine relationship between a self-guided short-term
meditation program and its impact on memory capacity and cognitive processing speed. Does a
Research Questions
RQ1: Does short-term self-guided mediation practice impact cognitive processing speed?
The earliest records of meditation practice (dating back to around 1500 BC) were
found in India within the Vedic schools of Hindu traditions for the spiritual path to
enlightenment (Wynne, 2007). The Chinese Taoists and Indian Buddhist practices began to
develop their own systems in the sixth and fourth centuries BC (Wynne, 2007). Meditation
practices tend to fall into two broad categories; one is an attentional concentrative focus method,
in which one focuses on a thought or activity. The other is a “day-dreaming” type process that
4
involves the practitioner passively observing and allowing any thoughts and feelings to arise
without any judgements or analysis. This usually involves staying in the present moment as a
passive observer. The latter is often referred to as mindfulness. The distinction between the two
is minor because they both involve some degree of attention control. Several formal systems
(Alexander et al., 2003; Bertone, 2020). Mindfulness methods include the Mindfulness-based
stress reduction program, Integrative body–mind training (IBMT), movement meditation, and the
Research on the benefits and effects of meditation are growing. Research on long-term
meditation has shown that it decreases anxiety and depression and increases feelings of well-
being (Baer 2003; Schreiner & Malcolm 2008). When it comes to cognitive function, research
studies have shown improved performance with regards to attention, awareness, memory, and
mental processing (Jha et al., 2007; Jha et al., 2010). Many of these meditation courses involve
programs that last anywhere from eight to 24 weeks. Some meditation programs involve
concentrated retreats that involve full-time commitments lasting two to seven days. For many,
committing to lengthy periods of time, or committing to the financial requirements needed, is not
feasible. This begs the question, is it possible to obtain these benefits with only one week of
meditation practice? If one can improve memory, attention control, mental processing speed, and
increased feelings of well-being, then this would apply to many areas of a person’s life. A
person’s job performance, students’ test taking skills and scores, and general mental and physical
well-being could improve with briefer meditation sessions. There is a business philosophy for
5
workplace practices and personal efficiency (Masaaki, 1986). Small improvements over time
make big improvements overall, but what about small improvements over shorter time periods?
It may mean the difference between achieving a higher grade, time shaved off a time-consuming
work project, or the difference in a competitive promotion. Olympic athletes win events and
races by fractions of an inch or hundredths of a second. Small increments can mean a lot. Small
Limitations
1. This study was limited to a sample of college students currently attending a single
2. This study was limited by the quality and duration of the participants’ meditation sessions,
since it was self-guided and the researcher was not able to monitor their sessions.
4. The online testing score was capped at 100. If a participant scored 100 on the pre-test, there
was no way to accurately measure growth for those that scored 100 on their post-test.
Assumptions
In this study there were several factors and conditions that were assumed to be true and
factual.
2. The participants answered the forms and survey questions honestly and accurately.
4. The participants have responded truthfully and to the best of their ability.
Definition of Terms
Cognitive processing speed, is defined as the speed at which one processes information for
Electroencephalography (EEG) is a test that measures and records the electrical activity of the
brain by placing electrodes on the scalp. These signals are then translated into wave frequencies
on an EEG recording. These waves can then detect brain synchronization or any disorders that
Functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) has been used successfully to monitor
cognitive states and brain activity. Using head-worn electrode sensors, the technique employs
near-infrared light and advanced signal processing to allow real-time, in-task monitoring. The
system not only determines changes in cognitive states by tracking blood hemoglobin levels in
the brain, but also filters non-relevant artifacts, such as the probes' own motion, rendering the
collected data even more accurate. The brains of people can be scanned to see which part of their
brains are being activated, and which portions of the brain receive an increase in blood flow.
d2-concentration and endurance test is a timed test that assesses selective attention. This test
is used in many areas of psychology, which allows for measurements of attention, concentration,
Technology in the 1990’s. The program integrated traditional Chinese medicine practices with
progressive body relaxation, mental imagery, and guided mindfulness training. Tang has used
this form of mindfulness for some of his research studies. (Tang et al., 2007)
7
Meditation is the practice of directing the attention of the mind, in order to relax the body and
calm the mind, which can lead to altered states of consciousness. These mental states positively
improve psychological balance and overall well-being. The practice tends to divide into two
types of methods: a narrow concentrative focus method and a broad, non-focusing, “day
dreaming” type method. The concentrative method can be used on any type of focusing theme
like the breath, exercise, visual images, affirmations, prayers, sounds, etc. The non-focusing
method requires one to simply be passively observant, yet let go of any thought, feeling, analysis
or judgement that comes into consciousness. (Alexander et al., 2003; Bertone, 2020; Zeidan et
al., 2010)
without analysis or judgement. This process can be done in sitting stillness or during low
cognitive load activities like eating, walking, or practicing yoga. (Baer 2003)
practitioners receive training in mindfulness meditation techniques linked with certain physical
stretches and yoga exercises for three hours a day for eight weeks, followed by a one-day retreat.
Progressive Muscle Relaxation (PMR) This program focuses on tensing major muscle groups
for a short period of time, then relaxing them. Over a period of time, the practitioner will become
aware of the body and the condition it is in. Stress induces tension in the body. By practicing
PMR one learns to become aware of unnecessary muscle tension and return to a relaxed state.
Sahaja Yoga (SY) meditation is a Hindu-based self-realization technique that attempts to obtain
a state of mental silence and enlightenment through focusing on the present moment. (Dodich et
al. 2019)
Stroop color word task (SCWT) is a neuropsychological test that measures the ability to inhibit
cognitive interference. An example would be the ability to inhibit habits in order to achieve a
goal that has cognitive interference. An example would be the actual Stroop Color Word Task
test. The test goal is to read out loud and name a physical color when it is used to spell the name
of a different color. The test can measure multiple cognitive functions such at attention,
processing speed and cognitive flexibility. (Scarpina, & Tagini, 2017; Stroop, 1935).
minutes a day, twice a day, for one week. Formal programs consist of coaches that guide the
participant through the process efficiently and effectively. (Goodman et al., 2003)
Working memory is defined as a small amount of information that can be held in the mind for
Working Memory Capacity (WMC) is the mental capacity to maintain and manipulate relevant
information towards a goal, without becoming distracted by irrelevant information over short
Yoga-sutras an ancient text that guides the practice of yoga today. The exact date of its creation
is unknown, but has been calculated to have been created between 200 BC and 200 AD by a
person named Patanjali. He traveled throughout India studying and observing the different
systems of yoga that were being practiced then. He put his observations into 200 scientific
principles and philosophies that provided an analysis and structure that is still regarded as one of
Overview of Chapters
The purpose of this study was to determine if a short-term, informal program of self-
literature that will provide a framework of current research studies concerning the benefits of
meditation and in support of the research questions. Chapter III provides a description of the
methods, procedures, and tools used to gather data for this research study. Chapter IV presents
the findings of this study, and Chapter V provides a summary of the study, conclusions, and
future recommendations.
Some cultures have been practicing meditation for centuries, and there have been many
claims and assertions for the benefits of meditation. Both the practice of meditation, and the
research as to its benefits, have been growing. This literature review investigates the benefits of
Benefits of Meditation
A variety of research studies have concluded that meditation is correlated with a wide
scope of benefits including physical, psychological, and cognitive. The benefits of these
meditation interventions are often found together in one study. For example, Sumpter et al.
facility. The initial testing consisted of self-reporting of medical symptoms, emotional control
indicators, and anxiety disorder behaviors. The study found meditation correlated with a
reduction in anxiety disorders like nail-biting and difficulty sleeping, which have effect on
overall mood and well-being. More importantly, the meditation participants also experienced a
Singh et al. (2007) found similar results with three individuals who had a history of
verbal and physical aggressive behaviors. These patients’ behaviors required them to be removed
from society and placed in a hospital. The intervention involved a six-week mindfulness
meditation program that consisted of two sessions per day for six weeks, followed by an audio
self-directed program. This meditation program guided the patients to quickly divert their rising
anger responses, from an upsetting event to a neutral focus point that dissipated their anger.
Following the meditation treatment program, the patients demonstrated the ability to control
themselves and not respond aggressively. They were allowed to integrate back into their
communities and were monitored each month for four years after the intervention. The follow up
four-year assessment found no physically aggressive incidents with any of the three patients.
(There were three incidents of verbal aggression by two out of the three participants; these
incidents were minor and were not serious enough to be readmitted into the hospital.) The
authors of this study recommended that this practice be seriously considered as an integral part
of treatment for psychological issues because of its low cost and ease of implementation.
Research has been conducted in the field of psychology to assess the benefits of
meditation programs that have been integrated into treatment programs. Some studies have
observation practice, have been shown to help patients become aware of their state of mind, and
separate themselves from painful issues they have experienced (Lau & McMain, 2005; Myra et
al., 2005). The integration of a meditation program into psychological treatment has shown
decreased depression, anxiety, and anger in patients (Baer et al., 2003; Germer et al., 2005;
Hayes et al., 2004; Manocha et al., 2011; Schreiner, & Malcolm, 2008; Singh et al., 2007).
11
physical and emotional well-being, self-regulation, and mental health. Meditation interventions
of various lengths and styles have also been shown to have a positive effect on cognitive
functioning (MacLean et al., 2010; Prätzlich et al., 2016; Zeidan et al., 2010)
Cognitive processes are defined as any of the mental activities that are involved in the
acquisition, interpretation, storage, and use of knowledge (Benjafield et al., 2010). This
encompasses a wide scope of mental activities such as attention, perception, learning, problem-
solving, memory, comprehension, and reasoning. Cognitive functions such as working memory,
planning, task switching, inhibitory control, and attention control are often referred to as
executive functions. Meditation research has increased in the field of cognitive neuroscience for
its potential positive effects on these mental functions and processes (Lutz et al., 2009).
Studies that evaluate the improvement of meditation on cognitive function tend to focus on
processing speed, brain wave activity, or task completion. One study by Kozasa et al. (2012)
found that regular meditators have more efficient use of their brains, compared to non-meditators
conducting the same mental task. Participants of the study were broken into two groups: one
group had never meditated before (naïve group); and a group that had an average of eight years
of meditation experience (experienced group). The researchers used two tests to measure
cognitive function: the Stroop color word task (SCWT) and functional near infrared spectroscopy
(fNIRS). The SCWT determines accuracy in completing a timed, mentally challenging task. The
fNIRS monitors electrical activation and blood flow of the brain during activities. The results of
this study showed that the experienced group were more accurate on the SCWT than the naïve
group, and that they used their brains more efficiently. The naïve group required more activation
12
of their brains and more blood flow to certain regions of the brain than the experienced group.
The naïve group did not perform as well as the experienced group on the same tests.
Some studies have shown that long-term meditation leads to structural changes in the
brain (Davidson et al., 2003; Lutz et al., 2004). Davidson et al. (2003) conducted a study of 25
subjects that were tested before, right after, and four months after an eight-week MBSR program.
The researchers used electroencephalography (EEG) to observe electrical brain activity in both
the experimental and control group. They also used the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory test to
measure individual traits and states of anxiety. The meditation group showed greater activation
of the left side of their brains which are associated with adaptive responses to negative or
stressful events. Individuals who show greater activation in these regions of their brains have
been shown to adapt quicker to negative occurrences (Davidson et al., 2000). The meditation
group also showed lowered anxiety levels than the non-meditative group.
Lutz et al. (2004) reported that certain brain-wave patterns, high-amplitude gamma
monitored the brain-wave activities of meditators and non-meditators during states of rest, and
during mental concentrative sessions. Long-time meditators had larger increases in synchronous
brain-wave patterns than the non-meditators during the activities. They also reported that the
meditation group had calmer brain waves (more alpha and theta brain waves) during resting
states than the non-meditators. These results suggest that long-term meditators have
Cotier et al. (2017) found through magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) that elderly people
who meditated for eight weeks showed an increase in brain activity in specific regions of their
13
brains. These regions of their brains were less active before the meditation program began.
Luders (2014) and Sun et al. (2013) found that meditation reduced the cognitive decline in
elderly individuals and improved several areas of cognitive function. Three different studies
found that a meditation practice showed an increase in gray-matter density and neurological
changes in regions of the brain that control attention, self-regulation, and self-awareness (Dodich
related to increased performance on attention tasks by meditators (Badart et al., 2018; Chambers
et al., 2008; Jha et al., 2007; Kozasa et al. 2012; Moore, & Malinowski, 2009). Long-term
meditation has been shown to benefit attention control, the exclusion of irrelevant information,
working memory capacity, and anxiety control (Badart et al., 2018; Chambers et al., 2008;
Chandla et al., 2013; Hasher et al., 2007; Jha, et al., 2007; Moore, & Malinowski, 2009). Tang
et al. (2007) conducted a study on 40 Chinese undergraduate students who participated in what
the researchers called a five-day IMBT program. This study involved an integration of Chinese
increased attention control, their study also showed a decrease in anxiety, depression, anger,
stress-related cortisol levels, and fatigue. They also reported an overall increase in positive mood
Some studies have shown that meditation improves certain cognitive executive functions.
Moore and Malinowski (2009) conducted a study with 25 experienced meditators from a
Buddhist center and a control group of 25 non-meditators. All the experimental group
participants had gone through a basic Buddhist six-week mindfulness course at a minimum. The
14
control group was 25 non-meditators. By using the d2-Concentration and Endurance Test, and
the SWCT test, their study showed that meditation practices improve cognitive flexibility.
Cognitive flexibility in this case involved the ability to switch between different mental tasks.
The test results indicated that the meditators performed significantly better than the non-
improve cognitive processing speed and satisfaction with Army life. After the participants
they found that information processing speed was increased (though satisfaction with Army-life
was not). Hall (1999) separated an undergraduate psychology class at Hampton University into
two groups: a meditation group and a non-meditation group. Both groups’ overall grade point
average (GPA) did not differ at the beginning of the 16-week semester. Hall found at the end of
her study that students who meditated before and after study sessions had a higher GPA than the
control group who did not meditate. Mrazek et al. (2013) found a reduction in distracting
sustained attention testing, and perceptual sensitivity over time, to assess cognitive functioning.
Their process involved a three-month meditation retreat, where the experienced participants
meditated for five hours a day. They tested both the control group and experimental group pre-
training, mid-training, and post-training. The meditation participants started showing a greater
degree of discriminating the difference between the length of lines than the control group at the
15
ability than the control group, post-training and five months after the experiment. Their study
also added that meditation improves core cognitive processing such as WMC and nonverbal
intelligence.
Jha et al. (2010) also observed that mindfulness meditation training affected WMC. The
study examined how mindfulness meditation training could reduce the stresses and detriments
that U.S. military personnel experience during pre-deployment training. First, the study validated
that the pre-deployment training cycle, and the stresses of upcoming deployments, caused a
stress reduction (MBSR) program, which was created by an officer who had extensive training in
MBSR and other forms of meditation training. Their study found that the mindfulness training
program reduced stress and caused improvements in WMC. They also found that more time
Most of the research studies reviewed here were conducted with meditation programs
lasting eight weeks or more. Research into whether a shorter-term program of mediation would
indicate improvements on cognitive processing is limited. Izzetoglu et al. (2020) used the fNIRS
and the SCWT to assess the improvement of attention, cognitive activity, and behavioral
performance measures in 21 college students who were first-time meditators. Their findings
showed improvement in cognitive processes such as attention control, memory, processing time,
perception, and reasoning, after a single 22-minute meditation session. Tang et al. (2007) found
that an IBMT meditation practice of five days, for 20-minute sessions per day, increased
attention control and decreased stress-related cortisol. The long-term impact of these studies is
unknown.
16
Prätzlich et al. (2016) conducted a study to see if the researchers’ expectations influenced
the outcome of meditation interventions. The study, conducted over a three-day period, had four
groups of participants: two that meditated, and two that did not. Each of the two groups was
separated into two more groups: one received positive comments about the benefits of
meditation; the second group received negative comments about meditating. Interestingly, each
group had performance outcomes that matched the comments and expectations they received.
The positive expectation groups, both meditators and non-meditators, had increased
performance, and the groups who were given negative suggestions had decreased performance.
The study concluded that benefits associated with meditation were correlated more to the content
of the words spoken by the researchers during the sessions than the fact of the meditation itself.
However, the meditators also reported that the mediation sessions produced a heightened state of
awareness, an increase in self-regulation, and a better ability to calm themselves, which the
SUMMARY
Studies of varying lengths, programs, techniques, and tasks have demonstrated the
benefits of meditation on physical, emotional, and cognitive operations. Though researchers have
used different methods of meditation to test its benefits, the meditative systems share a common
element: the practice of controlling the direction and subject of an individual’s thoughts and
attention. Meditation practice leads to more efficient use of the mind, higher attention control,
neurological structural changes within the brain, calmer resting states of the brain, improved
cognitive processing speed, higher grade point averages, and increased working memory
capacity.
17
The purpose of this study was to examine whether a self-guided five-day meditation
program would improve cognitive processing speed and memory capacity as measured by online
tests. The study was formed to assesses if cognitive processing speed and memory capacity test
scores completed before and after five days of mediation showed improvement by statistical
significance.
Chapter III details the population, instrument design, methods of data collection and
statistical analysis.
Participants
The main target participants were adults 18 to 75 years old, regardless of gender. No socio-
economic status requirements were required by the study. Undergraduate and graduate students
were sought through a southeastern university emailing system. Adults from the corporate
workforce were recruited via social/professional networking groups. Adults from the corporate
workforce were required to be 18 years or older. There were no educational requirements, but
Old Dominion University students (ODU subjects) and contacts of the researcher already
identified through social/professional networking groups (non-ODU subjects) were the focus of
efforts to recruit participants. ODU subjects enrolled in science, technology, engineering and
math (STEM) Education and Professional Studies courses at the undergraduate and graduate
level were the primary focus, and were contacted via email solicitation sent through the
respective class professors. Other subjects were solicited through 1) networking site
professional networks).
18
Instrument Design
Two online tests were used to assess and score cognitive processing speed and working
memory capacity. For cognitive processing speed, a five-minute timed online Mental Speed Test
1 from Psychology Today was used. The test consisted of word and image pairs and simple
mathematical equations that required a correct or incorrect button response. If the word and
image matched, the participant was to select either correct or incorrect. The same response was
required for simple mathematical equations that were either correct or incorrect. Response time
and accuracy are recorded and assessed by the website. The website summarizes a percentage of
correct responses that were provided by the participants to the researcher for analysis. To assess
memory capacity, the researcher used an online test called the Memory Test Challenge which
consisted of a series of images that appeared at timed intervals. Images that repeated required the
touching of the keyboard space bar (or tapping the screen for tablets and smart-phones).
Response time and accuracy recall were scored and assessed by the Memory Loss website test.
The participants were asked to take the tests once before starting the meditation program and
once again after meditating for 15 minutes a day for five days. After each test their scores were
provided to the researcher by the subjects by filling in their scores on the perspective Google
Form. The scores were assigned a letter-number designator, then organized on an excel
spreadsheet. A paired-sample t-test was performed to understand the difference between the two
scores.
There are a vast number of ways to meditate. For this study, the meditation method used
was a simple breath-focusing technique. The instructions on the study’s website directed the
participant to set a timer for 15 minutes, then focus the mind on this breathing process; inhale for
four seconds, hold the breath for four seconds, exhale for eight seconds, and finally hold the
19
exhale for four seconds. This process is repeated until the timer signals the fifteen minutes are
completed. This is one of the oldest and simplest techniques for meditation which can be traced
back to the yoga-sutras which were organized and recorded by Pantanjali, dated approximately
around 200 BC (White, 2019). That text is still referenced for Yoga and meditation today.
The results of the participants’ initial and post-meditation self-reported test scores were
recorded on two separate Google Forms that were accessed on the study’s web page. Instructions
on how to complete the study and all forms necessary for the study were available on the
research study’s website. Google Forms recorded those entries in a response section. This data
was only accessible to the study’s researcher. Any identifying data of the participant was
throughout the study. The researcher compiled the data from the forms into an Excel
spreadsheet.
Statistical Analysis
RQ1: Does a self-guided short-term mediation practice impact cognitive processing speed?
To find the answer to these two research questions, this experiment involved an online
test to measure cognitive processing speed before and after meditating for five days. The
independent variable for this experiment was the initial test scores taken by the participants
before beginning one week of meditating for 15 minutes each day. The dependent variable was
the test scores taken by the participants after five days of meditation. To address research RQ1,
cognitive processing speed, a five-minute timed online Mental Speed Test 1 from Psychology
20
Today was used (Mental Speed Test - Version 1, n.d.).. The test consisted of word and image
pairs and simple mathematical equations that required a correct or incorrect button response. If
the word and image matched, the participant was to select either correct or incorrect. The same
response was required for simple mathematical equations that were either correct or incorrect.
Response time and accuracy are recorded and assessed by the website.
To address RQ2, memory capacity, the researcher used an online test called the Memory
Test Challenge (Free Short Term Memory Test, 2013) which consisted of a series of images that
appeared at timed intervals. Images that repeated required the touching of the keyboard space bar
(or tapping the screen for tablets and smart-phones). Response time and accuracy recall were
Statistical analysis was performed on the quantitative data collected from the participants
submitted scores. Inferential statistics were performed using Microsoft Excel Data Analysis. The
first analysis was performed to determine whether there was any change in the scores after one
week of meditating. The second analysis was performed to determine whether any such
difference was significant and reliable. A paired-sample equal variance t-test was used to
determine if there was a significant difference between the mean of each of the two groups of
scores (before and after meditation). A third data screening statistics paired-sample equal
variance t-test calculation was done without the scores that had reached the perfect score of 100.
10 total sets of scores had reached 100, four in the mental test and six in the memory test. The
reason for the removal was the total cap on the highest score possible was 100. By eliminating
the scores of 100 in the second data screening calculations, potential false increases were
removed.
21
Summary
Research has shown that formal guided meditation practices consisting of eight weeks or
more impact cognitive processing speed and working memory (Badart et al., 2018; Chambers et
al., 2008; Ebaid et al., 2017; Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., & Philippot, P., 2009; Jha et al., 2007;
This study was conducted to determine if five days of self-guided meditation practice for 15
minutes would impact cognitive processing skills or working memory. Participants took two
online tests, meditated for 15 minutes a day for five days, took the two online tests again, and
then self-reported their scores onto a Google Form provided by the study’s main website. The
data from Google Forms was then used on an Excel spreadsheet to calculate an equal variance t-
The purpose of this study was to determine if a five-day self-guided meditation course
would impact cognitive processing and working memory. 42 participants completed the study,
with an average increase of; three points in the mental test and two points in the memory test
scores. A paired-sample equal variance t-test determined there was no significant statistical
The independent variable in this study was the meditation conducted for 15 minutes a day
for five days. The dependent variable were the changes in cognitive processing speed and
working memory, which were measured by the differences between the participants first pre-
meditation online test scores, and the second set of test scores done after meditating for five
RQ1: Does a self-guided short-term mediation practice impact cognitive processing speed?
To address Research Question 1, the first paired-sample equal variance t-test included all
the scores recorded. Table 1 presents the paired-sample equal variance t-test results that was used
to test the significance of the differences between the pre-and post-meditation scores. The Mental
Test 1 had a mean of 85.44 (M = 85.44, p = 0.13). The results indicated that there was no
significant difference of improvement in scores from the pre-meditation mental test to the post-
Table 1
Mental Test Paired Sample t-test equal variances with scores of 100 included.
To address research question two (RQ2) the first paired-sample equal variance t-test
included all the scores including those participants that reached the top score of 100. Table 2
presents the paired-sample equal variance t-test results that were used to test the significant
differences between the pre-and post-meditation scores that were submitted. The Memory Test 1
had a mean of M = 83.1. The Memory Test 2 had a mean of (M = 86.94, p = 0.09). The results
23
indicated that there was no significant difference of improvement in scores from the pre-
Table 2
Memory Test Paired Sample t-test equal variances with scores of 100 included.
Data Screening
The second statistics paired-sample equal variance t-test calculation was done without the
scores that had reached the perfect score of 100. 10 total sets of scores had reached 100, four in
the mental test and six in the memory test. The reason for the removal was the highest score
possible was 100, thereby providing a situation whereby measuring any growth indicated by a
score of higher than 100 was not possible. By eliminating the scores of 100 in the second data
To address RQ1 in relation to potential false increases due to the cap of a total score of
100, four test scores were removed from the total pool of 42 submitted scores. Table 3 presents
the paired-sample equal variance t-test results. The Mental Test 1 had a mean of M= 84.32. The
Mental Test 2 had a mean of (M = 87.65, p = 0.15). The results indicated that there was no
significant difference of improvement in scores from the pre-meditation mental test to the post-
Table 3
Mental Test results in which the potential false increases were removed.
To address RQ2 in relation to potential false increases due to the cap of a total score of
100, six test scores were removed from the total pool of 42 submitted scores. Table 4 presents
the paired-sample equal variance t-test results. The Memory Test 1 had a mean of M = 82.77. The
Memory Test 2 had a mean of (M = 86.57, p = 0.10). The results indicated that there was no
significant difference of improvement in scores from the pre-meditation memory test to the post-
Table 4
Memory Test results in which the potential false increases were removed.
CHAPTER V
Summary
The purpose of this study was to determine if a five-day self-guided meditation course
would impact cognitive processing and working memory. All 42 participants completed the
study, with an average increase in three points in the mental test and two points in the memory
test scores. A paired-sample equal variance t-test determined there was no significant statistical
Conclusions
As mentioned above, the literature reviewed generally examined the benefits of formal
guided meditation programs lasting eight weeks or longer, or where the participants had years of
meditation practice prior to the study (Hall, 1999; Helber et al. 2012; Jha et al., 2010; Katherine
et al. 2010; Monk-Turner, 2003). Many of these long-term programs were guided by a coach or
trainer for the duration of the study. Long-term meditation programs lasting eight weeks or
longer have established positive physical and mental benefits of practicing meditation (Baer et
al., 2003; Chung, et al., 2012; Germer et al., 2005; Hayes et al., 2004; Jha et al., 2010; Manocha
This five-day meditation experiment did not show any impact on cognitive processing
speed or memory. Although the participants scores increased by an average of three points on the
mental test and two points on the memory test, these changes were not shown to be statistically
significant. These findings were inconsistent with two other short-term mediation studies done
by Tang et al. (2007) and Izzetoglu et al. (2020). There were several key differences between this
study and the experiment conducted in China by Tang et al. (2007). Their study integrated
26
several additional components. A preparation session was conducted the day before the
experiment began that was guided by a certified meditation coach. The coach mentored
participants in body relaxation techniques, breathing techniques, and mental imagery. The
participants were following a guided meditation CD that included daily instructions and music.
The coach selected the time of day and the place that the group would consistently meditate. The
participants were also monitored by a trainer during each session, and a post-meditation question
and answer discussion was conducted each day. The authors acknowledged the importance of
having a coach monitor the sessions and provide corrective information and guidance. After
each session, the trainers could answer any questions the participants may have had. These
trainers monitored the participants for any body language cues exhibited during the sessions that
would indicate a participant was having trouble meditating. Another note of significance was that
all the participants meditated as a group each day. These authors noted the value of group
dynamics facilitating the outcome of the meditation sessions. They believed this increased the
The use of a guided mediation podcast was one key difference between this study and the
study conducted by Izzetoglu et al. (2020). Their study consisted of the participants sitting in a
chair, taking four tests, meditating following a guided 22-minute podcast, then taking the four
tests again. Their Stroop tests were done immediately after the podcast session. These authors
found an increase in brain hemoglobin oxygenation and a decrease in systolic blood pressure.
They found an increase in scores after the 22-minute meditation session. The technical analysis
techniques of that experiment were not done in this study. The lack of a guided meditation
podcast and immediate testing conducted after the meditation session could be the factors
contributing to the difference in results between their experiment and this one.
27
Several additional factors could have affected the results of this study. The meditation
sessions were not monitored as most of the more formal meditation programs were. That was the
intended goal of this study, to see if a self-guided meditation program only lasting five days
could impact cognitive function and memory. The lack of a coach or mentor during the
meditation sessions may have affected the quality of the meditation, which in turn, could have
affected the impact of meditation on these two mental functions. A guided meditation podcast or
a coach may be a necessary component, especially for first-time meditators, to ensure a higher
quality meditation session. Alcohol consumption, sleep quality, and other potential confounding
stress factors were not monitored throughout the week of the experiment. These unmonitored
factors could have also affected the outcome of the study (Beaunieux, et al., 2014; Vitiello,
1997). Sleep quality and duration were not monitored during the experiment. A lack of sleep or
poor sleep quality could have been a factor in the results of the experiment (Ratcliff, 2009).
Recommendations
Future studies on the short-term effects of meditation on mental functions could benefit
on having the meditation sessions controlled daily by a meditation facilitator. The validity of
whether the participants actually meditated correctly for a whole week could be established with
a facilitator. Alcohol consumption, sleep quality and duration, as well as external and internal
stress factors should be monitored during the length of future experiments. This could reduce
variables that could adversely affect the outcome of the study. A coach or program monitor may
be needed to address meditation difficulties and record the factors listed above that could affect
test results.
Establishing the quality of the meditation session itself through feedback devices that
monitor brain-wave frequency and other electrical feedback devices could establish the level of
28
quality that the participant reaches during the meditation sessions. This could accurately detail
who reached a meditative state and to what level and duration they maintained the session each
day. This would track the authenticity of any impact on test scores that occur. The use of music
while meditating could be a factor in focusing during meditation, especially for first-time
meditators. This could be a variable in which future research might pursue. And lastly, future
participants may need to be recruited from a formal medical treatment program or a formal
guided meditation program for all these recommendations to be fully and realistically applied.
29
References
Allen, M., Dietz, M., Blair, K., van Beek, M., Rees, G., Vestergaard-Poulsen, P., Lutz, A. and
Alexander, C., Walton, K., & Goodman, R., (2003). Walpole study of the transcendental
36(1-4), 97-125.
Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. (2007). Mindfulness-based stress
reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 14, 449–463.
Badart, P., McDowall, J. & Prime, S.L. (2018). Multimodal sustained attention superiority in
Beaunieux, H., Eustache, F., & Pitel, A. L. (2014). The relation of alcohol-induced brain changes
to cognitive function. In Alcohol and the adult brain (pp. 136-155). Psychology Press.
Benjafield, J. G., Smilek, D., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Cognition (4th ed.). Oxford University
Press.
Benedict, R. H., DeLuca, J., Phillips, G., LaRocca, N., Hudson, L. D., Rudick, R., & Multiple
Bertone, H., 2020. Which Type of Meditation Is Right for You? Health line.
https://www.healthline.com/health/mental-health/types-of-meditation
30
Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007).
Bunce, D., Tzur, M., Ramchurn, A., Gain, F., & Bond, F. W. (2008). Mental health and cognitive
Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2009). How long does a mindfulness-based stress reduction program
need to be? A review of class contact hours and effect sizes for psychological distress.
Chandla, S. S., Sood, S., Dogra, R., Das, S., Shukla, S. K., & Gupta, S. (2013). Effect of short-
and heart rate variability. Journal of the Indian Medical Association, 111, 662–665.
Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training
on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32,
303–322.
Chung, S. C., Brooks, M. M., Rai, M., Balk, J. L., & Rai, S. (2012). Effect of Sahaja yoga
meditation on quality of life, anxiety, and blood pressure control. Journal of Alternative
Cotier, F.A., Zhang, R. & Lee, T. (2017). A longitudinal study of the effect of short-term
Cranson, R. W., Orme-Johnson, D. W., Gackenbach, J., Dillbeck, M. C., Jones, C. H., &
Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Kalin, N. H. (2000). Emotion, plasticity, context, and
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller, D., Santorelli, S. F.,
Urbanowski, F., Harrington, A., Bonus, K., & Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain
564–570.
Dodich, A., Zollo, M., Crespi, C., Cappa, S., Laureiro M., Daniella, F., (2019). Short-term Sahaja
Yoga meditation training modulates brain structure and spontaneous activity in the
of the mind, context of meditation practice and modes of existential awareness. Frontiers
in Psychology, 7, 1788.
Ebaid, D., Crewther, S. G., MacCalman, K., Brown, A., & Crewther, D. P. (2017). Cognitive
processing speed across the lifespan: beyond the influence of motor speed. Frontiers in
MemoryHealthCheck. https://www.memorylosstest.com/free-short-term-memory-tests-
online/
32
Gard, T., Holzel, B. K. & Lazar, S. W. (2014). The potential effects of meditation on age related
cognitive decline: a systematic review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
1307, 89–103.
Germer, C. K., Siegel, R. D., & Fulton, P. R. (Eds.). (2005). Mindfulness and psychotherapy.
Guilford Press.
Goodman, R. S., Walton, K. G., Orme-Johnson, D. W., & Boyer, R. (2014). The transcendental
Hall, P. D. (1999). The effect of meditation on the academic performance of African American
Hasher, L., Lustig, C., & Zacks, R. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In
Hayes, S. C., Follette, V. M., & Linehan, M. M. (2004). Mindfulness and acceptance:
Helber, C., Zook, N. A., & Immergut, M. (2012). Meditation in higher education: Does it
Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., & Philippot, P. (2009). The effects of mindfulness on executive
47, 403–409.
33
Hölzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T., & Lazar, S.
Izzetoglu, M., Shewokis, P., Tsai, K., Dantoin, P., Sparango, K., & Min, K. (2020). Short-term
608.
Jedrczak, A., Toomey, M., & Clements, G. (1986). The TM‐Sidhi programme, age, and brief tests
Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness meditation modifies subsystems
Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the
Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., ... &
Lau, M. A., & McMain, S. F. (2005). Integrating mindfulness meditation with cognitive and
Lichtenberger, E. O., & Kaufman, A. S. (2012). Essentials of WAIS-IV assessment (Vol. 96). John
Llewellyn, D., Lang, I., Langa, K., & Huppert, F. (2008). Cognitive function and psychological
well-being: Findings from a population-based cohort. Age and Ageing, 37(6), 685-689.
Luders, E., Toga, A. W., Lepore, N., & Gaser, C. (2009). The underlying anatomical correlates of
Luders, E., Clark, K., Narr, K. L., & Toga, A. W. (2011). Enhanced brain connectivity in long-
Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., & Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term
Lutz, A., Slageter, H. A., Rawlings, N. B., Francis, A. D., Greischer, L. L., & Davidson, R. J.
(2009). Mental training enhances attentional stability: Neural and behavioral evidence.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients
MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Zanesco, A. P., Jacobs, T. L., ... &
Kozasa, E. H., Sato, J. R., Lacerda, S. S., Barreiros, M. A., Radvany, J., Russell, T. A., Sanches,
L. G., Mello, L. E., & Amaro, E., Jr. (2012). Meditation training increases brain
Kozasa, E. H., Lacerda, S. S., Menezes, C., Wallace, B. A., Radvany, J., Mello, L. E., & Sato, J.
MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Zanesco, A. P., Jacobs, T. L., King,
B. G., Rosenberg, E. L., Sahdra, B. K., Shaver, P. R., Wallace, B. A., Mangun, G. R., &
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610371339
Mallya, S., & Fiocco, A. J. (2016). Effects of mindfulness training on cognition and well-being
Manocha, R., Black, D., Sarris, J., & Stough, C. (2011). A randomized, controlled trial of
meditation for work stress, anxiety and depressed mood in full-time workers. Evidence-
https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/tests/iq/mental-speed-test-version-1
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in
Monk-Turner, E., (2003). The benefits of meditation: Experimental findings. The Social Science
Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness
training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind
Myra Weiss, J., Siegel, E. (2005). Mindfulness-based stress reduction as an adjunct to outpatient
Nelson Cowan. (2014). Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and
Pagnoni, G., & Cekic, M. (2007). Age effects on gray matter volume and attentional performance
Ratcliff, R., & Van Dongen, H. (2009). Sleep deprivation affects multiple distinct cognitive
Rothschild, S., Kaplan, G., Golan, T., & Barak, Y. (2017). Mindfulness meditation in the Israel
Defense Forces: Effect on cognition and satisfaction with life-A randomized controlled
Sahdra, B. K., MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Shaver, P. R., Rosenberg, E. L., Jacobs, T. L.,
Zanesco, A. P., King, B. G., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Mangun, G. R., Lavy, S.,
Wallace, B. A., & Saron, C. D. (2011). Enhanced response inhibition during intensive
Scarpina, F., & Tagini, S. (2017). The stroop color and word test. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 557.
37
Schreiner, I., & Malcolm, J. (2008). The benefits of mindfulness meditation: Changes in
emotional states of depression, anxiety, and stress. Behaviour Change, 25(3), 156-168.
Singh, N., Lancioni, G., Winton, A., Adkins, A., Wahler, R., Sabaawi, M., & Singh, J. (2007).
Individuals with mental illness can control their aggressive behavior through mindfulness
Sumter, M., Monk-Turner, E., & Turner, C. (2009). The benefits of meditation practice in the
Sun, J., Kang, J., Wang, P., & Zeng, H. (2013). Self‐relaxation training can improve sleep quality
and cognitive functions in the older: A one‐year randomized controlled trial. Journal of
Tang, Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S. Lu, Q., Fuk, W.T., Poontang, Q., Sui, D., Rothbart,
M., Fan, M., Posner, M. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and
Teper, R., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: The importance
Vitiello, M. (1997). Sleep, alcohol and alcohol abuse. Addiction Biology, 2(2), 151-158.White,
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S., Diamond, B., David, Z. and Goolkasian, P., 2010. Mindfulness
Appendices
Welcome to our research study. Thank you for participating. This study is being
conducted to find out if a brief meditation session consisting of five, 15-minute sessions for five
days, will improve cognitive processing. You will take a few tests that take about 90 seconds
each, meditate for 15 minutes for five days, fill out a quick survey after each session, and on the
final day, take those tests again. This should only take about 20 minutes out of each day.
Research has shown that long-term meditation and mindfulness practices improve focus,
attention and memory, the research on the effects of short-term meditation on improving
cognitive processing is limited. This study will help provide information and knowledge for the
regulatory practice that induces a state of relaxation and may produce altered states of awareness.
Meditation practices tend to fall into two broad categories; an attentional concentrative focus
method or an awareness-based process that involves the practitioner observing and allowing any
There are many techniques of mediation. It is often beneficial to experiment and find a
method or technique that works best for you. For this study to be consistent, we would like you
3. Meditate every day for 15 minutes. The process to follow can be found here.
4. On the fifth day after meditating, take the Post-training tests here.
How to meditate
1. Pick a consistent time. Many chose the morning after a good night’s rest, before
the day gets going. Evening times may not be ideal, due to a stressful, long day
or fatigue from work.
2. Find a comfortable position. Sitting is often the best to prevent yourself from
falling asleep. A common occurrence with first-time meditators. Keep your back
upright and straight. Feet are flat on the floor, hands resting comfortably on your
thighs. When you are ready, close your eyes to reduce visual distractions.
3. Set your countdown timer to 15 minutes, with a pleasant alarm to sound at the
end of your session.
Start your session by focusing on the relaxing breath exercise.
4. The relaxing Breath or the 4-4-8-4 (forty-four eighty-four) Breath:
4.1 Inhale for 4 seconds.
4.2 Hold your breath for 4 seconds.
4.3) Exhale for 8 seconds.
4.4) Hold your breath for 4 seconds.
Repeat
You count the seconds in your head by saying “one-thousand one, one-thousand
two, etc.”
5. The wandering mind.
In the beginning, you may be able to focus well on your breath because the
technique is new. As soon as your mind becomes use to it, it may start to wander.
It is very common for the mind to wander to different things, other than focusing
on your breath. The key is to not get frustrated, or turn it into a forceful process.
This wandering mind is not a personal defect or disability, it is a survival
mechanism.
42
Relax, acknowledge what thought comes to mind, and return to focusing on your
breathing. You will get better at focusing, the more you practice it. Some find it
helpful to have a notepad and pen within arm’s reach, to write down what comes
into your mind, if it is important to note or remember. This way, if it is important,
and you have it recorded for future reference, it will allow the mind to stop
reminding you about it.
Any external distracting sounds can be used as a reminder for you to focus on
the breath. Your mind may start day-dreaming, and an external sound may bring
you out of that day-dream and back to the present moment.
6. Enjoy the process, feel good about it, no matter how “well” you performed it.
43
I am writing to seek participants for a short research study into whether 15 minutes of
meditation per day for five days can improve memory and cognitive processing. I would be
grateful if you would please forward the below to your students, and any other party you think
may be interested in participating. Thank you in advance.
Dear Students:
Please see below information about how you can participate in a voluntary research study that
will test whether 15 minutes of meditation per day for five days can improve memory and
cognitive processing. All participants will be entered to win a $25 Amazon Gift Card. Your
participation in this study is voluntary and anonymous. You must be 18 years or older to
participate.
What will the study entail? The study participants will take two brief three-minute online
tests, then meditate for 15 minutes per day for five days and repeat the tests, taking note of all
test results and recording them in an online form.
How much time will it take? A few minutes to read the brief instructions, take the online
tests, and record the results; and 15 minutes per day for five days for the meditation.
Where can I find out more details about the study, and how to participate? Detailed
information about the study, how to participate, and where to write for further information or
questions, is found here: https://sites.google.com/odu.edu/diao/home
Warmest regards,
Darrin Landry
45
Subj: Meditation Research Study Seeks Participants (You Could Win a $25 Amazon Gift Card)
Dear Colleagues,
I am writing to seek participants for a short research study into whether 15 minutes of meditation per
day for five days can improve memory and cognitive processing. I would be grateful if you would
please forward the below text to those you think might be interested in participating, on a voluntary
basis. Thank you in advance for circulating this email amongst your staff and colleagues.
Dear [staff]:
Please see below information about how you can participate in a research study that will test whether
15 minutes of meditation per day for five days can improve memory and cognitive processing. Your
participation in this study is voluntary and anonymous.
All participants will be entered to win a $25 Amazon Gift Card. You must be 18 years or older to
participate.
What will the study entail? The study participants will take two brief three-minute online tests, then
meditate for 15 minutes per day for five days, then repeat the tests, taking note of all test results and
recording them in an online form.
How much time will it take? A few minutes to read the brief instructions, take the online tests (each
takes approximately three minutes), and record the results; and 15 minutes per day for five days for
the meditation.
Where can I find out more details about the study, and how to participate? Detailed information
about the study, how to participate, and where to write for further information or questions, is found
here: https://sites.google.com/odu.edu/diao/home
Warmest regards,
Darrin Landry