Medida de Los Procesos de Act
Medida de Los Procesos de Act
Medida de Los Procesos de Act
Summer 2014
Recommended Citation
Gootzeit, Joshua Holubec. "ACT process measures : specificity and incremental value." PhD (Doctor of
Philosophy) thesis, University of Iowa, 2014.
https://doi.org/10.17077/etd.grjd6zw4
by
Joshua Holubec Gootzeit
August 2014
CERTIFICATE OF APPROVAL
_______________________
PH.D. THESIS
_______________
___________________________________
Lilian Dindo
___________________________________
James Marchman
___________________________________
Molly Nikolas
___________________________________
Michael O'Hara
To Beth
ii
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
I would like to thank my advisor, Kristian Markon, for his help and support over
my graduate career. His guidance and his expertise in statistics and psychometrics, and
his patient willingness to sit down and have long discussions about the finer points of
structural equation modeling despite his busy schedule, have made this project possible. I
Nikolas, and Mike O’Hara for their feedback and guidance. I would like to particularly
thank James Marchman, whose invaluable mentorship and clinical training continues to
influence and inspire me, both professionally and personally. I thank my parents for their
unconditional support over the years. Finally, I thank my wife, Beth, for her constant
iii
ABSTRACT
to measure the six processes related to Acceptance and Commitment Therapy’s model of
research settings. However, little research has been done to investigate whether these
from other, seemingly similar constructs such as distress tolerance and coping styles.
functioning, and personality. The structure of these process measures was investigated
across two participant samples. A multi-trait structure of ACT processes was found, with
This structure was found across multiple samples, and measures of these factor
provide guidance for measurement selection and suggest future directions for scale
iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS
v
REFERENCES ..................................................................................................................85
vi
LIST OF TABLES
A14. Table A14. Student T1-T2 Correlations Among Non-ACT Scales ........................151
A15. Parallel Analyses for Scale-Level Factor Analysis of ACT Measures ...................156
A21. Correlations Between Subfactor Loadings for 1st Scale-Level EFA factor ...........169
vii
A25. Item-Level Parallel Analyses for ACT Factor 2 (Awareness) ................................185
A26. Correlations Between Subfactor Loadings for 2nd Scale-Level EFA Factor .........188
A31. Correlations Between Subfactor Loadings for 3rd Scale-Level EFA Factor .........204
A32. Mechanical Turk Subfactors of ACT Factor 3 (Avoidance), Using Promax
Rotation...................................................................................................................205
A35. Correlations Among Subfactor Sum Scores for Mechanical Turk Sample ............214
A36. Correlations Among Subfactor Sum Scores for the Student Sample. ....................215
A37. Factor Analysis of Lower-Order Factor Sum Scores Using Promax Rotation .......217
A38. Mechanical Turk Correlations Between Subfactor Sum Scores and Other
Study Variables .......................................................................................................218
A39. Student Time 1 Correlations Between Subfactor Sum Scores and Other Study
Variables .................................................................................................................220
A40. Student Time 2 Correlations Between Subfactor Sum Scores and Other Study
Variables. ................................................................................................................222
A41. Fit Indices for Hierarchical and Correlated ACT Models ......................................224
A43. Mechanical Turk Partial Correlations Between ACT Subfactors and External
Measures Controlling for Subscales of the ASI, DIS, DTS, and COPE ................226
A44. . Student Time 1 Partial Correlations Between ACT Subfactors and External
Measures Controlling for Subscales of the ASI, DIS, DTS, and COPE. ...............227
A45. Student Time 2 Partial Correlations Between ACT Subfactors and External
Measures Controlling for Subscales of the ASI, DIS, DTS, and COPE ................228
viii
A46. Mechanical Turk R2 Values for Measures of Psychopathology, Personality,
and Functioning ......................................................................................................229
ix
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure
x
1
CHAPTER I.
INTRODUCTION
psychological distress is normative rather than a sign of abnormality, and that efforts to
behavior in order to achieve valued ends, as the primary goal of therapy (Hayes, Strosahl,
The ACT model posits six relevant processes that are targets for change in
therapy. These six processes are all posited to be critical components of psychological
flexibility, and are therefore highly interconnected. This six-part model is known as the
ACT Hexaflex (Hayes et al, 2011; see Figure B1). Many objective personality
questionnaires have been created to measure these proposed ACT processes. Some of
these measures have been extensively used in both clinical and research settings. Indeed,
because the goal of ACT is to effect change in these areas, rather than to reduce
trials due to their hypothesized mediating effect on therapy outcomes (Hayes, Luoma,
Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). However, little research has been done on how these
process measures differ from each other, or from measures of similar psychological
reduction, in that it advocates releasing one’s struggles with unwanted thoughts, feelings,
internal experiences.
Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, and Strosahl (1996) argue that experiential
topographical characteristics of the behavior, and offer limited etiological guidance; even
when treatments have been found to be disorder-specific, the atheoretical nature of the
disorders, Cooper, Russell, Skinner, Frone, and Mudar (1992) investigated a large sample
feelings of distress. They found that the use of avoidant coping strategies predicted
problem drinking behaviors, particularly for men (β weights range from 0.10 to 0.18).
3
Overall, they found that individual-specific characteristics such as the tendency to avoid
such as cutting, which are particularly prevalent in individuals with borderline personality
disorder (Chapman, Gratz, & Brown, 2006). In a study of individuals with a diagnosis of
borderline personality disorder, Chapman, Specht, and Cellucci (2005) found that, in a
study of 117 female inmates, self-harm frequency and borderline symptoms were
in a survey of 75 individuals who engage in deliberate self-harm, 96% report that they do
so, at least in part, for emotional relief (Brown, Comtois, & Linehan, 2002). Similarly,
experiential avoidance may also be associated with full suicide attempts (Baumeister,
1990).
study of panic disorder and emotional avoidance, Tull and Roemer (2007) investigated
individuals with (n = 91) and without (n = 91) a history of uncued panic attacks. They
found that individuals with a history of panic attacks scored statistically significantly
acceptance (η2p = 0.04). Individuals with histories of panic attacks also reported using
functions of worrying. They found that individuals meeting DSM-III-R criteria for GAD
were more likely to report that worry would prevent undesired outcomes, would lead to
better problem solving, and served to distract them from “even more emotional things.”
These researchers suggest that the results indicate an avoidant function of worry for
individuals with GAD because such worry may suppress negative emotions and suppress
Carter (2006) found that a measure of depression was correlated with measures of
nonsocial avoidance (r = 0.37). They also found that a measure of rumination was highly
that depressive rumination, like generalized worry, may serve an avoidant function,
allowing depressed individuals to ruminate rather than to engage behaviors that are
thought suppression indicate that attempts to suppress particular thoughts have several
unintended effects. In their classic study, Wegner, Schneider, Carter, and White (1987)
5
asked participants to either think about white bears (control group) or to suppress
thoughts about white bears for five minutes (thought suppression group); the researchers
found that the thought suppression group had more thoughts about white bears after the
five minutes were up compared with the control group, indicating a rebound effect after
individuals ended their focus on suppression. Additionally, participants can have great
difficulty maintaining suppression when placed under a cognitive load; Wegner and
Erber (1992) found that suppressed thoughts paradoxically become more accessible when
task. Finally, some research suggests attempts to maintain thought suppression increase
the frequency of that thought even in the absence of a cognitive load (e.g., Lavy & Van
example, Wenzlaff and Bates (1998) found that, when placed under a cognitive load,
depressed individuals produce more negative statements and fewer positive statements
compared to non-depressed individuals. In contrast with Lavy and Van den Hout’s (1990)
findings, this difference only occurred when placed under a cognitive load. The
researchers argue that these results suggest a tendency among depressed individuals to
suppression has been found to relate to substance cravings, pain, trauma memories,
Therapy, it is not unique to this model. Other modern behavioral therapies, such as
Therapy (Jacobson & Christensen, 1996) also strongly emphasize the importance of
balancing acceptance with change. Additionally, many humanistic therapists also have
mediate outcomes in the treatment of workplace stress (Bond & Bunce, 2000), diabetes
cessation (Gifford et al., 2004), the reduction of prejudice (Lillis & Hayes, 2007), quality
of life in seizure patients (Lundgren, Dahl, & Hayes, 2008), weight loss (Lillis, Hayes,
al., 2007). These results suggest that changes in acceptance do indeed play a role in
changes in clinical outcome, and that increased acceptance at least partially drives
therapeutic change.
acceptance (the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire) and other measures of clinical
interest. They found that this measure of acceptance is correlated with measures of
depression (r = 0.50), anxiety (r = 0.54), general mental ill-health (r = 0.53), and the
distinct from the more established processes of reappraisal and perceived emotional
control, indicating that the process of accepting is distinct from the process of changing
psychological flexibility, and that many therapy clients become so fixated on past or
future narratives that they pay little attention to present moment processes. Like
acceptance, present moment awareness shares some conceptual similarities with the
concept of mindfulness.
gain purposeful, present moment awareness and to prevent the mind from drifting. While
engaging in mindfulness meditation, both physical and mental experiences are noticed
importance over any other (Kabat-Zinn, 1982). This practice originated in the Mahayana
Buddhist Zen tradition, and was initially used by providers of Western medicine to
reduce stress (Goleman & Schwartz, 1976) and chronic pain (Kabat-Zinn, 1982).
2009); a meta-analysis by Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, and Walach (2004) has been
found that this approach is effective in improving both mental wellbeing (e.g.,
depression, anxiety, and coping styles) and physical wellbeing (e.g., medical symptoms,
physical pain, physical impairment, and quality of life). In all, this meta-analysis found
an overall d value of 0.54 (N = 771) for controlled studies examining mental health
8
variables and a d value of 0.53 (N = 203) for controlled studies examining physical health
variables. When examining pre-post (uncontrolled) studies, they found a d value of 0.50
(N = 894) for mental health variables and a d value of 0.42 (N = 466) for physical health
variables.
Mindfulness practice has also been used to enhance cognitive therapy for
of MBCT argue that mindfulness practice can prevent relapse of depression following
cognitive therapy by giving clients additional tools to notice negative thoughts and to
bring themselves back to the present moment (Teasdale, Segal, & Williams, 1995). In a
meta-analysis of the efficacy of MBCT, Chiesa and Serretti (2011) found that the
rate of relapse in individuals suffering from depression compared with treatment as usual
(OR = 0.36, N= 326). In another meta-analysis, Piet and Houggaard (2011) similarly
found that, in a combined sample size of 593, there was a risk reduction of 43% when
using MBCT for individuals with at least three depressive episodes. This finding,
however, was not obtained for individuals with a history of one or two depressive
episodes, suggesting that MBCT is particularly useful for individuals with a more
outcomes in ACT interventions. Forman, Herbert, Moitra, Yeomans, and Geller (2007)
found that a measure of “acting with awareness” mediated outcomes for an ACT
“describing” mediated outcomes for Cognitive Therapy. These results suggest that
9
mindfulness is important for both ACT and CT, but that different facets of mindfulness
meta-analysis, Giluk (2009) investigated the relation between mindfulness, Big Five
personality traits, and affect. She found that mindfulness was particularly related to
indicate that levels of mindfulness are linked to clinically and functionally relevant
processes.
Defusion
The originators of ACT coined the term “defusion” to refer to the ability to
traditional cognitive behavioral therapy, ACT does not attempt to change or to refute
unwanted thoughts; rather, the goal is to recognize the thought as an example of mental
language that has no intrinsic truth or falsity, and that may be observed without being
taken literally. Thus, an individual becomes “fused” with a thought if he or she believes
that it is a literal representation of the world; in ACT, the therapeutic goal is therefore to
reduce the believability of unhelpful thoughts, rather than to reduce the frequency or to
For example, the believability (but not the frequency) of depressive thoughts was found
to mediate outcome for ACT, but not for CBT, in a study of depressed patients (Zettle &
10
Hayes, 1986; reanalyzed by Hayes et al., 2006). Similarly Varra, Hayes, Roget, and
Fisher (2008) found that an ACT intervention to effect behavior change in substance
al. (2004) found that believability of thoughts mediated an ACT intervention targeting
stigma and burnout in counselors. Finally, Guadiano and Herbert (2006) found that, when
rather than frequency, mediated outcomes. These studies suggest that change in
Little research currently exists investigating the relation between cognitive fusion
found that a measure of cognitive fusion was correlated with the Beck Depression
Inventory (r = 0.69), the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (r = 0.85),
the Symptom Checklist 90 General Severity Index (r = 0.62), and the WHO Brief Quality
of Life Scale (r = -0.45). These results are preliminary, but suggest that cognitive fusion
Self-as-Context
self that goes beyond one’s familiar thoughts, feelings, emotions, and memories.
Becoming aware of the “I” that observes such mental experiences, rather than defining
oneself in terms of them, is an important clinical goal. This self is the self-as-context, or
the observing self. Because this observing self exists independently from mental content,
it is constant and unchanging. Because it is the observer rather than the observed, self-as-
11
because describing it would necessitate adopting a perspective on it that was not one’s
ACT interventions attempt to strengthen this sense of self through the use of
experiential exercises. For example, Harris (2008) uses a series of mindfulness exercises
thinking…[then] be aware that you’re noticing them.”). Metaphors can also be used to
make self-as-context more accessible to the client. For examples, Hayes et al. (2012) uses
and memories are the pieces fighting one another on the chessboard, whereas the noticing
Although few ACT studies examine self-as-context directly, at least one study
suggests that ACT interventions that include self-as-context exercises such as those
described above outperform ACT interventions that do not include such exercises
(Williams, 2006). There are currently no measures explicitly designed to assess patients’
Values
ACT emphasizes helping clients move towards valued areas of life. “Values,” in
ACT terms, refer to verbally constructed, freely chosen life domains, such as family,
friendship, work, leisure, and education that clients find personally meaningful.
with values have been found to have positive clinical effects. For example, ACT values
et al (2008) divided 30 participants into three conditions: the first group received an
emphasized the importance of avoiding pain, and the third received no intervention.
These researchers found that seven of ten participants in the values group could tolerate
maximum pain, whereas only one of ten participants in the avoidance intervention group
and two of ten in the control group could do the same. Similarly, in a study of 171 pain
patients, Vowles and McCracken (2008) investigated the efficacy of values interventions
for chronic pain. They found that changes in values-based action during follow-up is
positive effects. In a study of 243 African American and Caucasian middle school
students, Cohen, Garcia, Apfel, and Master (2006) investigated the use of an exercise to
elicit values from students. They found that minority students who were prompted to
articulate their values had better academic performance compared with minority students
who wrote about and articulated values that they did not rate highly. In a similar study,
physics students. They found a narrowed gender gap for students who received the
intervention, and that female students who received the intervention increased from a C
which they wrote about personally-relevant values unrelated to weight loss. Control
participants wrote about values that they did not rate highly. Individuals who participated
in this exercise lost significantly more weight (d = 0.90) at 2.5 month follow-up
Cresswell et al. (2005) studied the effect of values affirmation exercises on stress
situation by being asked to give a speech. It was found that individuals in the active
condition had significantly lower levels of cortisol (η2 = 0.07) compared with participants
in the control condition. This study suggests that physiological stress markers can be
Finally, Lomore, Spencer, and Holmes (2007) investigated the effects of a values
that, among the participants with low self-esteem, an intervention clarifying shared
values with one’s partner increased feelings of love and regard for one’s partner. Values
of neurotic disorders, and suggests that filling the client’s existential vacuum, helping
14
him or her find meaning, is an important part of treatment (Frankl, 1985). Frankl’s
emphasis on valuing (Sharp, Schulenberg, Wilson, & Murrell, 2004). Likewise, Carl
Rogers suggested that choosing one’s personal values freely, rather than valuing due to
1964). This emphasis on freely chosen values closely mirrors ACT’s conceptualization.
Motivational Interviewing also makes use of valuing by using a client’s individual values
to foster motivation and behavior change (Hettema, Steele, & Miller, 2005). Finally, in
addition to ACT, other modern CBT approaches have emphasized the importance of
values. Some Behavioral Activation (BA) protocols, for instance, share with ACT an
emphasis on values, using values assessment to create and guide activation assignments
0.23) (Wilson, Sandoz, Kitchens, & Roberts, 2010). Similarly, a study of individuals with
chronic pain showed that measures of values discrepancies and values success were
Committed Action
Committed Action refers to the client’s ability to commit to valued change and to take
actions towards it. The other ACT processes, such as acceptance and mindfulness, serve
to help them maintain this movement when mental barriers, such as anxiety, arise.
al. (2013) argue that, although studies have shown that ACT interventions that do not
Twohig, Hayes, & Masuda, 2006; Twohig et al., 2010), such an approach tends to “peel
ACT away from its own model.” Rather, they argue that comparisons between standard
behavioral interventions and full ACT interventions provide a more useful comparison.
and mindfulness techniques rather than in relaxation techniques. For example, in a study
of 60 individuals with panic disorder, Levitt, Brown, Orsillo, and Barlow (2004) divided
participants into three groups; the first group received a brief ACT-based intervention,
the second group received an intervention encouraging them to try to gain control over
uncomfortable feelings by changing their thoughts, and the third group received no
induce panic-like symptoms. Participants who received the brief ACT intervention
reported less subjective distress when controlling for resting levels of distress (ƒ2 = 0.16)
16
and a greater willingness to engage in another, similar task (η2 = 0.12). In a very similar
study, Eifert and Heffner (2003) investigated a sample of 60 participants with high levels
of anxiety sensitivity. One third of the participants were given instruction on mindfulness
and willingness, one third received instruction in relaxation and controlled breathing, and
one third received no clinical intervention. All were then given a series of CO2 breathing
tasks. It was found that the participants who received the ACT-consistent intervention
began each CO2 breathing task more quickly compared with the other groups (η2 = 0.26)
and were more likely to return for additional sessions (η2 = 0.20). These results indicate
constructs have been proposed that relate to how individuals respond or react to their
suffering. Because ACT emphasizes changing one’s response to suffering, rather than the
suffering itself, these constructs may be related to the ACT Hexaflex processes, although
Anxiety Sensitivity
consequences (Reiss, Peterson, Gursky, and McNally, 1986). This “fear of fear” was
disorders. Because this construct focuses on an individual’s beliefs about his or her
experiences. On the other hand, anxiety sensitivity may be a narrower construct than
and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (ρ = 0.54), and is also highly related to other
0.40). This researcher also found that anxiety sensitivity has incremental validity in
predicting most of these disorders (with the exception of OCD) above and beyond the
important risk factor for the development of these disorders. Furthermore, anxiety
sensitivity can be reduced via cognitive behavioral therapy (Smits, Berry, Tart, &
Powers, 2008), suggesting the possibility that the reduction of anxiety sensitivity may
Distress Tolerance
Simons and Gaher (2005) suggest that this construct includes the ability to tolerate
inability to mentally disengage from feelings of distress. This construct appears to have
commonalities with the ACT process of acceptance, as both relate to one’s ability to
may be argued (e.g., Wilson & Dufrene, 2010) that acceptance involves a more open,
distress (Leyro, Zvolensky, & Bernstein, 2010). Distress tolerance has been found to be
an important process in the ability of individuals with substance use disorders to abstain,
Kahler, Strong, and Brown (2005) found a correlation of 0.27 between abstinence
duration and the ability to sit for a lengthy and stressful computer task.
hypothesizes that low distress tolerance is key to the development of this disorder, as it
has been found that individuals with a diagnosis of borderline exhibit lower levels of
with a diagnosis of borderline (n = 17) with normal controls (n = 18), Gratz, Rosenthal,
Tull, Lejuez, and Gunderson (2006) found that, when asked to complete a stressful
likely to terminate the task early (24% versus 0%), and were less likely to report a
willingness to engage in another, similar task (59% versus 89%). They also spent
part of treatment. Additionally, it has been suggested (e.g., Leyro et al., 2010) that many
Discomfort Intolerance
Schmidt et al. (2006) found that individuals with a diagnosis of panic disorder had higher
levels of discomfort intolerance compared with both normal and anxious controls. The
avoidance and to a fear of the physical sensations of fear. These authors also found that
nonclinical individuals reporting higher levels of distress intolerance (n = 44) were more
reactive and reported more symptoms of panic in a high CO2 environment after
= 0.14) (Schmidt, Richey, Cromer, & Buckner, 2007). Discomfort intolerance may also
be related to substance use. In a study of 265 participants, Buckner, Keough, and Schmidt
(2007) found that discomfort intolerance moderates the relationship between depression
20
and substance coping, with depressed individuals with high discomfort intolerance being
Coping Styles
The exact number of coping strategies that are available tend to be measure specific. For
example, some research has divided coping into two broad categories: problem-focused
coping, which focuses on changing the environment, and emotion-focused coping, which
focus on regulating or avoiding stressful emotions (e.g., Folkman & Lazarus, 1980).
Other studies posit a larger variety of more narrowly defined coping styles; for example,
Folkman, Lazarus, Gruen, and DeLongis (1986) measure eight coping styles, consisting
meta-analysis examining the relations between coping styles and the Big Five personality
traits, Connor-Smith and Flachsbart (2007) found that active, engaged coping is weakly
0.13). Conscientiousness was associated with both engaged (r = 0.11) and disengaged (r
= -0.15) coping.
Research shows that maladaptive coping styles are also related to measures of
found that overall psychopathology was associated with rumination (r = 0.49), avoidance
These results were found across a variety of specific psychopathologies. For example,
avoidant coping was correlated with anxiety (r = 0.37), depression (r = 0.48), eating
disorders (r = 0.18), and substance use disorders (r = 0.26), whereas rumination was
correlated with anxiety (r = 0.42), depression (r = 0.55), eating disorders (r = 0.26), and
substance use disorders (r = 0.21). In a follow-up study, these researchers (Aldao &
Overall, these studies demonstrate that coping styles are significantly related to
avoidant coping, Suls and Fletcher (1985) found important differences between short-
term and long-term outcomes. Avoidant coping tended to be more effective for short-
term outcomes, whereas non-avoidant coping tended to be more effective in the long
term. When non-avoidant coping was broken down into sensory monitoring versus
results are consistent with ACT theory, which indicates that avoidance works in the short
term but is ineffective in the long term, and posits a difference between mindfulness
Processes
In general, limited research has been done examining the relations among many
of the aforementioned processes. Although some of the non-ACT related processes (e.g.,
distress tolerance and anxiety sensitivity) have a good deal of research examining
specificity and incremental validity, most measures of ACT processes have almost no
such research.
Gloster, Klotsche, Chaker, Hummel, and Hoyer (2011) tested whether a measure
impairment. Across four samples (total n = 1167), they found that the acceptance did
indeed explain unique variance in impairment and functioning above and beyond the
other measures, with ΔR2 values ranging from 0.024 to 0.108. These results suggest that
acceptance/avoidance might help to explain some kinds of dysfunction above and beyond
participants in order to determine whether these process measures are associated with a
measure of health anxiety. They found that health anxiety is correlated with both
explanatory power after accounting for anxiety sensitivity (ΔR2 = 0.02; P > 0.05).
23
Conversely, anxiety sensitivity had significant explanatory power after accounting for
experiential avoidance (ΔR2 = 0.16). These researchers suggest that these results provide
evidence for the role of maladaptive beliefs, rather than experiential avoidance, in the
Most published ACT measures are at least somewhat related to each other; of
particular note, Gillanders et al. (2013) found that their Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire
(CFQ), a measure of defusion, had a correlation of 0.80 with the Acceptance and Action
that this may be due either to item overlap or to the fact that both questionnaires are
meant to measure different facets of psychological flexibility. These results suggest that
some of the processes under discussion may be difficult to differentiate using self-report
questionnaires.
Patients and research participants may also have a difficult time in practice
healthy research participants and 288 psychiatric outpatients, Gámez, Kotov, and Watson
(2010) found that participants make no distinction between avoidance and distress, even
when being interviewed. Correlations between self-reported distress and avoidance were
0.79 for healthy participants and 0.91 for outpatients, and did not significantly differ from
1.0. They also found that symptoms of avoidance had no incremental value in explaining
psychopathology after accounting for self-reported distress. These results suggest that
measures of avoidance will likely have a great deal of overlap with measures of distress,
investigate the relation between anxiety sensitivity, emotional distress, dysfunction, and
the ACT processes of acceptance, mindfulness, and values. They found that, after
accounting for pain, anxiety sensitivity was a significantly associated with depression
(ΔR2 = 0.33), pain-related anxiety (ΔR2 = 0.26), physical disability (ΔR2 = 0.13),
psychosocial disability (ΔR2 = 0.34), and number of doctor visits (ΔR2 = 0.14). In a
second regression analysis, these researchers first added pain, then measures of ACT-
The ACT-related measures added significant explanatory value over pain intensity
(depression: ΔR2 = 0.51; pain-related anxiety: ΔR2 = 0.45; physical disability: ΔR2 =
0.23; psychosocial disability: ΔR2 = 0.49; doctor visits: ΔR2 = 0.13). After accounting for
the ACT measures, anxiety sensitivity was no longer significantly associated with
physical disability, but had a small incremental value for the other variables (depression:
ΔR2 = 0.05; anxiety: ΔR2 = 0.04; psychosocial disability: ΔR2 = 0.05; doctor visits: ΔR2 =
0.08). These results indicate significant overlap in the explanatory power of these
processes.
acceptance/experiential avoidance has incremental power over the subscales of the brief
and wellbeing. Using a sample of 197 research participants, these researchers conducted
an exploratory factor analysis of the items from the AAQ-II and the COPE. They found
that most of the AAQ-II items loaded with the COPE items measuring maladaptive
coping, indicating that these two processes may not be distinguishable. They found,
25
however, that experiential avoidance did provide some significant incremental value
when explaining measures of perceived stress (ΔR2 = 0.11), physical quality of life (ΔR2
= 0.07), psychological quality of life (ΔR2 = 0.18), social quality of life (ΔR2 = 0.14), and
environmental quality of life (ΔR2 = 0.11), indicating that, while experiential avoidance
Zvolensky, Bernstein, Feldner, and McLeish (2007) found that anxiety sensitivity is
strongly associated with a variety of external variables; some of these relationships were
cognitions: β = -0.61). Significant moderating effects were not found for anhedonic
depression or for body vigilance. These results indicate that low levels of mindfulness
psychopathology. In a follow-up study using the same sample, Kashdan, Zvolensky, and
McLeish (2008) investigated the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and a measure
relationships between anxiety sensitivity and anxious arousal (β = 0.19) and worry (β =
0.13), but not for agoraphobic cognitions. These researchers suggest that mindfulness and
emotional acceptance may have a partial inoculation effect for individuals with high
anxiety sensitivity.
The relationship between discomfort intolerance and anxiety sensitivity has also
been investigated. Schmidt et al. (2007) found that, in a study of 44 research participants,
after taking anxiety sensitivity into account (β = 0.39). Similarly, in a study of 216
intolerance has small but statistically significant incremental value over anxiety
task. However, a follow-up study of the same sample (Kutz, Marshall, Bernstein, &
Zvolensky, 2010) found that neither discomfort intolerance nor distress tolerance had
studies show mixed results in their investigations of the utility of discomfort intolerance
intolerance, and anxiety sensitivity (n = 229). They found a three-factor model emerged,
indicating that these three processes are distinguishable. Anxiety sensitivity and distress
tolerance were found to be related to each other as lower-order factors of a single, higher-
order tolerance/sensitivity factor. They did not find that discomfort intolerance was
highly related to this construct. A measure of negative affectivity was highly related to
this higher-order factor (r = -0.47) as well as to the lower-order distress tolerance factor (r
= -0.43) and anxiety sensitivity factor (r = 0.37). It was not significantly correlated with
the discomfort intolerance factor. Overall, these results suggest that anxiety sensitivity
examined the relationship between anxiety symptoms, anxiety sensitivity, and distress
tolerance in a sample of 418 participants. They found that distress tolerance had small but
27
significant incremental power above and beyond anxiety sensitivity when explaining
symptoms of OCD (β = -0.11), panic (β = -0.11), worry (β = -0.31), and social anxiety (β
= -0.15). In a similar study, Timpano, Buckner, Richey, Murphy, and Schmidt (2009)
examined the relationship between anxiety sensitivity, distress tolerance, and hoarding in
three samples (total n = 745). All were interrelated, but distress tolerance seemed to play
a less important role in hoarding in individuals with low anxiety sensitivity, but increases
0.16), indicating that low distress tolerance is associated with hoarding only when anxiety
important to the development of hoarding behaviors. These results further suggest that
anxiety sensitivity and distress tolerance and distinguishable, despite being highly related.
hampered by the different philosophies of science that are said to underlie ACT and
Stephen Pepper has suggested that science is rooted in one of several world
hypotheses or world views, which guide scientific research and scientific development
(Pepper, 1942; Hayes, Hayes, & Reese, 1988). These world views are implicit in any
Pepper discussed several world hypotheses that commonly guide science; for the
purposes of this discussion, the two most important are mechanism and contextualism.
Each of these can be said that be guided by a “root metaphor” that guides our
relationships between its parts contribute to a unified whole. Although the parts may
interact, these interactions do not change the nature of the parts. Mechanists strive to
create models of a phenomenon of interest, and much of their work involves testing and
refining these models based on how well they correspond with reality. A mechanistic
scientific theory is “true” if this correspondence continues to exist as new facts emerge in
the world. Mechanistic theories are therefore evaluated using an ontological framework.
science underlying their work, much of psychological research is mechanistic. Biglan and
Hayes (1996) discuss a number of areas of psychological research that has an underlying
mechanistic framework. For example, they use Bandura’s (1977) self-efficacy theory as a
in fact been found that responses to self-efficacy measures predict the effects of various
treatments. Biglan and Hayes (1996) argue, however, that the theory is poorly suited to
29
guide the creation of new treatments, and is therefore mechanistic, descriptive, and
ontological.
present contextual factors. Contextualists strive to create models that are functional, and
contextualist theory is “true” if it is functionally useful in this way. For this reason,
gravitation. Newton did not hypothesize that gravity is a hypothetical construct that
mechanistically “exists;” rather, he sought to model how physical bodies influence each
other in order to be able to predict the motion of planets and other heavenly bodies. From
Newton’s theory was true, as it functionally allowed the scientists of the day to predict
the movement of heavenly bodies. Newton’s theory can be contrasted with the
mechanistic theories of Ptolemy and Copernicus, which were purely descriptive, rather
than predictive.
behavior. This theory’s goal is not to model psychological constructs, but to better
30
understand how to predict and influence behavior. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
has grown out of this older behavior analytic tradition (Hayes et al., 2011).
The originators of ACT argue that classic psychometric test theory assumes the
ontological existence of the latent variable being measured. For example, Hayes et al.
(2012) cite the work of Borsboom, Mellenbergh, and van Heerden (2003), who argue that
the use and measurement of latent variables is difficult to justify without an ontological
philosophical approach. Borsboom et al. (2003) offer three possible ways to interpret a
latent variable. The first is to interpret such a variable as being a “numerical trick,”
essentially a sum score that has no larger reality and that is merely a simple way to
simplify data. The authors reject this interpretation because it requires the assumption
that different item sets cannot measure the same latent variable, rendering psychological
tests ungeneralizable. The authors’ second approach is to treat latent variables as human
constructions; in this view, latent variables are essentially a fiction with no independent
existence other than our perception. They object to this interpretation as well, arguing
that, if there is no independent truth to a theory, any conclusion drawn from a latent
variable must be as true as any other, and that therefore no hypothesis can be falsified.
They therefore argue for a “realist” perspective, which assumes both that the underlying
latent variable is a real construct and that our measure of the latent variable is an
It may be, however, that hypothetical constructs do not necessitate the existence
as a variable hypothesizing the existence of “an entity, process, or event which is not
they serve to summarize.” These definitions would seem to suggest that a test or measure
could be indirectly assessing a process or event rather than an entity, and that this process
variable) if that process encompasses more examples than the test itself.
Hayes et al. (2012) suggest that some psychometric tests can be understood using
consistency suggests that the different behaviors are under the same contextual control.
These authors take as examples two items: “I feel sad” and “I withdraw from people.”
From a contextualist perspective, these two behaviors correlate not because of the
existence of an underlying hypothetical construct (depression) but because they are under
the same contextual control (perhaps a combination of history, genetic vulnerability, and
current life circumstances). If, due to therapy, these contextual controls weaken, the
correlation between these two test items might decrease, causing a drop in coefficient
alpha.
of why these particular processes are contextually related, and why they co-vary across
described above, it seems clear that depression is a hypothetical construct rather than an
intervening variable, as some portion of it exists beyond the processes directly measured
factors, etc.). The contextual factors that cause individual differences in depression are
part of this construct, even if they are not directly measured. A hypothetical construct, in
events and the contextual or etiological factors that link them, and not necessarily a thing
may be used. It should be understood that such approaches and terms are used for the
sake of function, and that the scales and dimensions under discussion should best be
highly related to each other. However, no study has yet tried to create a full structural
these ACT constructs. This allowed the investigation into whether these constructs are
distinguishable from one another, whether the actual factor structure corresponds with the
Hexaflex model described above, and whether a hierarchical structure exists for these
33
constructs. The relationships among these constructs, and the relationships between these
investigated.
all such processes in determining whether these measures have differential specificity or
incremental validity over and above such constructs as anxiety sensitivity, distress
tolerance, discomfort intolerance, or coping styles. The current study investigated the
incremental value of the ACT process measures under investigation. This research will
The results of this research will be valuable in both clinical and research settings
for a number of reasons. First, it will help with scale selection. Clinicians and researchers
often have limited time to administer assessment measures; it is likely, given the state of
that are essentially measuring the same process, while ignoring other measures that may
sample relevant behaviors and processes. The current study’s findings will provide
guidance for instrument selection in such situations. Second, it will provide important
information for future scale development. The development of new instruments in these
domains has been hampered by imprecise knowledge of the exact nature of the processes
and constructs underlying these measures. For this reason, it is probable that new process
measures have been developed that have essentially duplicated previous measures. An
34
instrument or set of instruments that fully samples all ACT-relevant processes may be
developed based on the structure uncovered in this research. Finally, this research may
assist clinicians in selecting ACT-relevant measures that are most related to the processes
under consideration.
Specific Aim 1. I aimed to find the overall structure of the questionnaire measures
that purport to measure the processes and constructs discussed above. I hypothesized that
the scales and subscales measuring ACT processes will be part of a hierarchical structure,
acceptance vs. avoidance, values, committed action, defusion, and present moment
awareness. This hypothesis was consistent with the ACT model proposed by Hayes et al.
(2011). I also hypothesized that values and committed action may not separate into
distinct factors due to the small number of scales available to model these processes.
Specific Aim 2. I also aimed to determine whether the scales under consideration
outcome is related to valued living rather than a reduction of symptoms (Hayes et al.,
2011), I hypothesized that values and committed action will show specificity to
functioning.
35
the included measures. There has been little-to-no research on the short-term stability of
ACT process measures. Because these measures often operationally define these
processes in research settings, it would be fruitful to examine reliability over time. This
allowed me to further measure to what extent these measures are differentiable; if the
test-retest reliabilities are significantly lower than the correlations among the scales, this
would indicate little scale specificity. This also allowed me to examine whether these
determine whether the six-factor ACT Hexaflex model of psychological flexibility can be
recreated structurally, and whether ACT processes are differentiable in this way. Few
questionnaires exist to measure values and committed action, and none exist to
psychological flexibility will emerge, with self-as-context possibly not emerging, given
styles. Little research has been done to test whether ACT processes are distinguishable
from these other psychological processes. I hypothesized that ACT process measures will
be distinguishable from these other, non-ACT processes, and will provide significant
incremental value.
36
CHAPTER II
METHODS
from the University of Iowa (T1 student sample; N = 485) and a community sample
subset of the T1 student sample returned two weeks after completing the initial study to
provide Time 2 data (T2 student sample, N = 342) in order to provide retest information.
participate. Students who signed up for this study were directed to a set of online
questionnaires on the REDCap survey system website hosted by the University of Iowa
(Harris et al., 2009). Students who completed the online survey received one research
credit towards a course requirement. The T1 student sample was 71% female with a mean
and median age of 19. The self-reported racial makeup of this sample was as follows: 2
participants (8.5% of the total) reported a history of mental health or substance abuse
treatment. The breakdown was as follows: 33 treated for depression, 5 for bipolar
disorder, 14 for generalized anxiety disorder, 2 for posttraumatic stress disorder, 4 for
obsessive compulsive disorder, 2 for panic disorder, 1 for social phobia, 7 for other
37
problems with fear or anxiety, 1 for alcohol use problems, 2 for drug use problems, 1 for
borderline personality disorder, and 2 for some other disorder or psychiatric problem.
Two weeks after completing the Time 1 administration of the study, all student
study for a second research credit towards their course requirement. Of these student
participants, about 71% elected to participate in this Time 2 administration. This smaller
subset of the student sample had a mean and median age of 19, and was 74% female. The
identified as Hispanic or Latino of any race. 29 participants (8.5% of the total) reported a
history of mental health or substance abuse treatment. The breakdown was as follows: 24
treated for depression, 2 for bipolar disorder, 11 for generalized anxiety disorder, 1 for
posttraumatic stress disorder, 3 for obsessive compulsive disorder, 4 for other problems
with fear or anxiety, 1 for alcohol use problems, 2 for drug use problems, 1 for borderline
Mechanical Turk System. Mechanical Turk is a service that allows its members to choose
among thousands of paid "microtasks." This service has been found by previous
collected from other community sources (see Paolacci, Chandler, & Ipeirotis 2010;
A total of 345 individuals completed at least part of the survey, and 301
individuals completed the entire survey. The sample was 52% male, and had a mean age
of 33 and a median age of 30. The self-reported racial makeup of the MT sample was as
122 White or Caucasian, 1 Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, 7 multiracial, 3 did not report.
participants (6.4% of the total) reported a history of mental health or substance abuse
treatment. The breakdown was as follows: 17 treated for depression, 1 for bipolar
compulsive disorder, 2 for social phobia, 6 for other problems with fear or anxiety, 4 for
alcohol use problems, 2 for drug use problems, 2 for some other disorder or psychiatric
problem.
Study Measures
measure that was created primarily as a measure of avoidance versus acceptance (Hayes,
psychological flexibility (Hayes et al., 2006). The AAQ has been found to be related to a
measures, including measures of depression, trauma, fear, and generalized anxiety. In all
cases, low levels of acceptance (high levels of avoidance) were related to higher levels of
psychopathology.
39
Bond et al. (2011) developed a revised version of the AAQ. The AAQ-II is a
uses a seven-point Likert response scale. (Example item: “It seems like most people are
handling their lives better than I am.”) These researchers found that the AAQ-II has
improved reliability compared with the original AAQ; across six samples with a total
sample size of 2,816, the average coefficient alpha was found to be 0.84, the average 3-
month and 12-month test-retest reliabilities (investigated in a sample size of 583) were
found to be 0.81 and 0.79, respectively (Bond et al., 2011). The relationship between the
AAQ-II and other, external measures, such as depression and anxiety, remained
essentially unchanged compared with the AAQ, with average correlations of 0.70 with
the Beck Depression Inventory (N = 487), 0.61 with the Beck Anxiety Inventory (N =
206), 0.59 with the White Bear Suppression Inventory (N = 1,661), 0.61 with the DASS
Depression Scale (N = 432), 0.49 with the DASS Anxiety Scale (N=432), 0.57 with the
DASS Stress Scale (N = 432), 0.43 with the General Health Questionnaire-12 (N =
1,661), and 0.70 with the Symptom Checklist-90 Revised (N = 206), indicating good
convergent validity; the measure was also able to predict future absences from work (r =
0.25; N = 583), indicating the measure can predict behaviors longitudinally (Bond et al.,
2011).
suppression that using a five-point Likert response scale (Wegner & Zanakos, 1994).
(Example item: “I wish I could stop thinking of certain things.”) It was designed as a
measure of thought suppression, which the authors hypothesized was related to obsessive
thinking. Across five samples consisting of a total of 2,746 participants, the coefficient
40
alpha ranged from 0.87 to 0.89. In a follow-up study (N = 162), these researchers found
test-retest reliabilities of 0.69, with time intervals ranging from three weeks to three
months. They also found that the WBSI was moderately correlated with the Beck
Depression Inventory (rs range from 0.44 to 0.52), the Maudsley Obsessive Compulsive
Inventory (rs range from 0.38 to 0.40), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (r = 0.53), and
avoidance. The reduction of thought suppression is thought to mediate some of the gains
2007), and is related to other measures of acceptance versus avoidance (Hayes et al.,
2004; Bond et al., 2011; Gámez, Chmielewski, Kotov, Ruggero, & Watson, 2011). Bond
avoidance, and that the WBSI is therefore measuring a narrower construct than the AAQ-
II.
avoidance that uses a six-point Likert response scale (Gámez et al., 2011). (Example
item: “When a negative thought comes up, I immediately try to think of something else.”)
It was designed to be a more comprehensive measure of avoidance than the AAQ or the
with a total sample size of 1,358, the scale-level coefficient alphas averaged 0.83, ranging
The authors found that these six scales are differentially related to various
psychopathology measures, and that, as a whole, the MEAQ had better correlations with
external variables than did the AAQ or the AAQ-II, and the partial correlations between
the MEAQ scales and external variables, when controlling for the AAQ-II, were higher
than the partial correlations between the AAQ-II and external variables when controlling
for the MEAQ. Across two samples with Ns of 314 and 201, the total MEAQ has
correlations of 0.66 and 0.74 with the AAQ-II and 0.54 and 0.56 with the WBSI,
indicating good convergent validity. The authors suggest that, although the MEAQ is a
more comprehensive measure of avoidance compared with the AAQ, the AAQ has come
to be seen as a broader measure of psychological inflexibility in general, and that the two
measures may be tapping into subtly different processes. Because it is a new measure,
little follow-up research has yet been done with the MEAQ.
scale measuring mindfulness, using a six-point Likert response scale (Brown & Ryan,
2003). (Example item: “I find myself doing things without paying attention.”) Across
seven samples with a total sample size of 1,492, the coefficient alphas ranged from 0.80
wellbeing; they found that their measure of mindfulness had moderate negative
correlations with depression (rs range from -0.37 to -0.42), anxiety (rs range from -0.26
42
to -0.42), and neuroticism (r = -0.56), and had moderate positive correlations with self-
esteem (rs range from 0.36 to 0.50), emotional wellbeing (rs range from 0.16 to 0.39),
and physical wellbeing (rs range from 0.25 to 0.51) (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Further
research shows that the MAAS has moderate negative correlations with the AAQ (r = -
0.32) and the WBSI (-0.32) in a sample of 88 individuals, indicating a relationship with
mindfulness which uses a five-point Likert response scale (Cardaciotto, Herbert, Forman,
conscious of them immediately.”) Unlike the MAAS, the PHLMS posits a two-factor
five samples totaling 923 participants, coefficient alphas ranged from 0.75 to 0.86 for the
awareness subscale and from 0.75 to 0.91 for the acceptance subscale.
These researchers found that the PHLMS acceptance scale was significantly
associated with measures of acceptance/avoidance such as the AAQ (rs range from 0.31
to 0.54) and the WBSI (rs range from -0.35 to -0.52), as well as with the Beck Depression
Inventory (rs range from -0.28 to -0.51) and the Beck Anxiety Inventory (rs range from -
0.29 to -0.39). Both the PHLMS awareness scale and the PHLMS acceptance scale
showed an association with the MAAS (acceptance subscale: rs range from 0.17 to 0.32;
mindfulness, using a five-point Likert response scale (Baer et al., 2008). (Example item:
“I watch my feelings without getting lost in them.”) It is the result of previous research
showing that a factor analysis of a large number of self-report items suggests a five-factor
structure of mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006). The FFMQ facets consist of observing,
inner experience. Across four samples that include 1017 participants, Baer et al. (2008)
reported that all subscale coefficient alphas were in the good range (ranging from 0.72 to
0.92), which the exception of the nonreactivity to inner experience scale, whose alphas
The facets have differential relationships with other measures; in another study of
the FFMQ’s psychometric properties, Bohlmeijer, ten Klooster, Fledderus, Veehof, and
Baer (2011) investigated a sample of 376 participants. They found that the different
scales had different correlates. In particular, the nonjudging scale has the strongest
relationship with the AAQ-II (r = -0.54) and the NEO Neuroticism scale (r = -0.46), as
0.25), and positive mental health (r = 0.20). The observe subscale has strong relationships
with the NEO Openness scale (r = 0.44) and positive mental health (r = 0.30). The
describing subscale has significant relationships with the AAQ-II (r = 0.31), openness (r
= 0.30), neuroticism (r = -0.21), and positive mental health (r = 0.37). The act with
awareness subscale has significant correlations with the AAQ (r = 0.30), neuroticism (r =
-0.28), anxiety (r = -0.22), depression (r = -0.20), and positive mental health (r = 0.20).
health (r = 0.23). These results suggest that, while the facet subscales have significant
The Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ) was first developed by Hollon and
Zettle and Hayes (1986), who added a believability scale as a measure of cognitive
fusion, creating the ATQ-B. Each statement in the ATQ-B is rated on a five-point Likert
scale for both frequency and believability. The ATQ-B believability scale has been used
as an outcome measure for ACT clinical trials (e.g., Zettle, Rains, & Hayes, 2011); these
researchers report that, in two samples consisting of 177 mental health patients and 249
non-clinical individuals, the ATQ-B has a coefficient alpha of 0.95 and 0.97,
respectively, and has correlations with the BDI equal to 0.53 and 0.58, respectively. For
the nonclinical sample, the three month test-retest reliability is 0.85. Little additional
fusion, using a seven-point Likert response scale (Gillanders et al., 2013). (Example item:
“I get upset with myself for having certain thoughts.”) In five samples totaling 1,849
individuals, coefficient alphas ranged from 0.88 to 0.93. The authors of the scale have
found that the scale is highly correlated with the AAQ-II (rs range from 0.72 to 0.87),
various measures of mindfulness (rs range from -0.50 to -0.70), and the ATQ-B (r =
0.61). In addition, it is highly correlated with measures of depression (rs range from 0.45
45
to 0.85), and moderately negatively correlated with measures of life satisfaction (rs range
The CFQ is highly correlated with the AAQ-II. However, the authors found that
the CFQ had incremental validity in explaining a measure of distress (ΔR2 = 0.05). The
authors suggest that this high overlap is due to the fact that the AAQ-II is a general
(cognitive fusion). In an item-level factor analysis of the items from the AAQ-II and the
CFQ, the authors found that the items formed two factors in three of their five samples,
ruminating which uses a five-point Likert response scale (Fresco et al., 2007). (Example
item: “I can observe unpleasant feelings without being drawn into them.”). Its authors
define decentering as “the ability to observe one’s thoughts and feelings as temporary,
objective events in the mind, as opposed to reflections of the self that are necessarily
true,” (Fresco et al., 2007), a concept that appears conceptually related to defusion,
decentering scale (11 items) had a coefficient alpha of 0.83, and the rumination scale (9
The authors found that their measure of decentering was correlated the AAQ-II (r
= -0.49) and the Beck Depression Inventory (r = -0.40) (Fresco et al., 2007).
of 150 chronic pain patients, the EQ Decentering scale is correlated with measures of
and psychosocial disability (r = -0.47), suggesting that decentering may play a role in the
The Valued Living Questionnaire (VLQ) is 10-item list of areas of life that might
be valued. Participants rate each area of life in terms both importance and consistency of
behavior using 10-pont Likert response scales (Wilson et al., 2010). (Example domain:
alpha for the importance scale ranged from 0.77 to 0.83, and the coefficient alpha for the
consistency scale ranged from 0.58 to 0.75. In a subset of 57 participants for whom one
to two week test-retest data were collected, the importance scale had a test-retest
reliability of 0.90, and the consistency scale had a test-retest reliability of 0.58. These
results suggest that chosen valued domains are relatively stable, but that efforts to move
towards these domains vary over time. Correlations between the difficulties with valued
living and other measures tend to be relatively low. The VLQ has a correlation of -0.14
with the AAQ, -0.26 with a measure of depression, -0.14 with a measure of anxiety, -0.20
with a measure of hostility, 0.13 with a measure of relationship functioning, and 0.23
with a measure of mental health. These modest correlations suggest that variability in
belief that anxiety has negative consequences rated using a five-point Likert response
scale (Reiss et al., 1986). (Example item: “It scares me when my heart beats rapidly.”) In
an initial validation sample of 127 individuals, it was found that this scale had a test-
retest reliability of 0.75 at two week follow-up. Internal consistency was not reported in
47
this initial study, but other studies have suggested good internal consistency (e.g., α =
The ASI has been found to be a multidimensional scale; Zinbarg et al. (1997)
investigated a sample of 432 participants in order to investigate its structure. They found
the best fit for a hierarchical structure consisting of a higher-order general factor and the
three specific factors of physical concerns, mental incapacitation concerns, and social
concerns. Rodriguez, Bruce, Pagano, Spencer, and Keller (2004) also found support for
this structure in a sample of 206 individuals with anxiety disorder diagnoses. Correlations
among these three lower-order factors ranged from 0.44 to 0.49. They also found
acceptable test-retest correlations (ranging from 0.64 to 0.78 for the subscales and 0.72
for the total score) in a subset of 89 individuals who were re-assessed at a one-year
follow-up.
It has been found that the ASI is associated with the presence of all internalizing
(Naragon-Gainey, 2010). This meta-analysis also showed that the three lower-order
scales have differential specificity. For example agoraphobia is more strongly associated
with the physical (ρ = 0.51) compared with the social (ρ = 0.40) and cognitive (ρ = 0.37)
subfactor. Similarly, depression is more strongly associated with the cognitive subfactor
(ρ = 0.53) compared with the physical (ρ = 0.40) and social (ρ = 0.28) subfactors.
Additionally, social anxiety is more associated with social anxiety sensitivity (ρ = 0.70)
tolerate negative emotional states rated using a five-point Likert scale (Simons & Gaher,
48
2005). (Example item: “I can’t handle being distressed or upset.”) This scale is defined
hierarchically, with a general distress tolerance factor and four lower-order factors,
associated with negative affectivity (Simons & Gaher, 2005). In a validation sample of
823 participants, the coefficient alpha of the overall scale was 0.82, and alphas for the
lower-order factors ranged from 0.70 to 0.82. Six-month test-retest reliability was 0.61,
and men reported higher distress tolerance than women (d = 0.32). Overall distress
tolerance was negatively correlated with negative affectivity (r = -0.57) and substance use
2010), and it has been suggested that more work needs to be done to investigate the
relationship between distress tolerance and other, related constructs (Leyro et al., 2010).
tolerate unwanted physical sensations, rated using a seven-point Likert scale (Schmidt et
al., 2006). (Example item: “I have a high pain threshold.”) These researchers found a
intolerance. In a sample of 1,296 participants, coefficient alpha for the measure was 0.70,
with an alpha of 0.78 for the discomfort intolerance subfactor and an alpha of 0.92 for the
193) samples, correlations between the DIS and the ASI ranged from 0.33 to 0.38,
correlations between the DIS and the Beck Anxiety Inventory ranged from 0.18 to 0.31,
and correlations between the DIS and the Beck Depression Inventory ranged from 0.05 to
0.24.
49
The COPE is a widely used measure of coping (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub,
1989) that broadly measures various coping styles. It is the mostly widely used measure
of coping styles in the literature (Kato, 2013). It is a 53-item scale that is rated using a
four-point Likert scale. It was published with fourteen subscales, consisting of the
seeking social support for instrumental reasons, seeking social support for emotional
participants, coefficient alphas for these scales ranged from 0.45 (mental disengagement)
to 0.92 (turning to religion). Six week test-retest reliabilities, calculated for a subset of
religion). In a meta-analysis, Kato (2013) found that the coefficient alphas of the scales
range from 0.53 (mental disengagement) to 0.91 (turning to religion). This meta-analysis
also examined the external correlates of the COPE scales, and found particularly high
The reliabilities of these fourteen rationally derived scales suggest that they are
not all robust measures, and subsequent factor-analytic studies have suggested that fewer
scales can be used. For example, Lyne and Roger (2000) investigated a sample of 539
individuals to investigate the factor structure of the COPE. They found that the original
scales could not easily be extracted using item-level analyses, and that a simpler three-
scale solution emerged. This new structure consists of rational/active coping (18 items, α
50
(8 items, α = 0.69). These three scales are highly differentiable, with low intercorrelations
(ranging from 0 to 0.21) and differential specificity. In particular, they found that
avoidance coping is the most highly associated with distress (r = 0.35), with emotion
coping having a smaller but statistically significant correlation with distress (r = 0.13) and
The Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10). The K10 is a 10-item measure of
(Kessler et al., 2002). (Example item: “During the last 30 days, about how often did you
In a validation sample of 1,574 participants, the K10 had a coefficient alpha of 0.92. In a
sample of 155 individuals with mental health problems, the K10 had good discrimination
between those with and without a diagnosis based on the Structured Clinical Interview
participants (Donker, van Straten, Marks, & Cuijpers, 2010), the K10 had a correlation of
0.84 with the CES-D. Additionally these researchers found that, in a subset of 157
(CIDI), the K10 and the CES-D were equally effective in predicting the diagnosis of a
depressive disorder.
point Likert response scale (Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning, & Kramer, 2007). A
51
brief form of this measure, consisting of 20 items (example item: “Others have told me
disinhibition (Patrick, Kramer, Krueger, & Markon, 2013). This measure was included to
In a sample of 599 participants, these researchers found that the ESIdis had a coefficient
alpha of 0.94, and, in a sample of 612 individuals, showed correlations with measures of
0.36), alienation (r = 0.60), aggression (r = 0.58), and control (r = -0.59). Overall, the
The Mini-IPIP. The Mini-IPIP is a brief measure of the Big Five personality
Openness (Donnellan, Oswald, Baird, & Lucas, 2006). It consists of 20 items that are
rated using a five-point Likert response scale. This measure was included to investigate
the relationship between personality traits and ACT constructs. In two initial validation
samples with a combined sample size of 2,992, coefficient alphas for the subscales
ranged from 0.65 to 0.82. The authors characterize these measures of internal consistency
convergent correlations between the Mini-IPIP scales and the larger IPIP-FFM scales
were found to be good, ranging from 0.83 to 0.93, indicating that the shorter scales are
correlations of the Mini-IPIP subscales ranged from 0.72 to 0.89 in a sample of 216
individuals. Long-term, six to nine month test-retest correlations ranged from 0.68 to 0.86
with informant reports with correlations ranging from 0.26 to 0.53. Overall, the Mini-
IPIP was found to be an adequate brief measure of the Big Five personality traits.
Schedule (WHODAS-II). Three modules from the 36-item self-report version of the
Organization, 2001). These scales were included to investigate the relationship between
levels of functioning and ACT constructs. These modules measure understanding and
communicating (six items; example item: “In the last 30 days, how much difficulty did
you have in remembering to do important things?”), getting along with other people (five
items; example item: “In the last 30 days, how much difficulty did you have in getting
along with people who are close to you?”), and life activities (eight items; example item:
“In the last 30 days, how much difficulty did you have in getting all the work done that
you need to do?”). All items use a five-point Likert response scale.
of rehabilitation patients with a total sample size of 904, Pösl, Cieza, and Stucki (2007)
found coefficient alphas of 0.83 to 0.87 for understanding and communicating, 0.69 to
0.81 for getting along with others, and 0.94 to 0.97 for life activities. They also found
high correlations with measures of mental symptoms (rs range from -0.40 to -0.68) and
of inconsistent responding, fourteen pairs of items with the highest correlations in the
student Time 1 sample were identified. Pair correlations ranged from 0.54 to 0.64.
(Example item pair: “I think some of my emotions are bad or inappropriate and I
53
shouldn't feel them” and “I tell myself that I shouldn't have certain thoughts.”) Items were
standardized in order to account for different response scales, and the absolute value of
the difference between scores for each pair was calculated. These pair differences were
summed to create a total VRIN score. Individuals who scored highly on this scale can be
than someone who has a low score. Correlations between the VRIN item pairs were
somewhat higher than the pair correlations for the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
Inventory-2, whose VRIN pairs have an average correlation of about 0.40 (Ketterer, Han,
Data Analysis
The data were analyzed using the Mplus and SAS statistical programs. Maximum
likelihood estimators were used for all structural equation modeling. Because most item-
level data used at least a five-point response scale, these data were treated as continuous,
When calculating scale scores, missing item-level data were prorated using other
items in the same scale, such that each missing data point was assumed to be equal to the
When examining the validity of scales and conducting factor analyses, a standard
rule of thumb is to investigate a sample size of at least 300 individuals (Clark & Watson,
(1999) suggest that a sample size as low as 200 is adequate for factor analysis in most
situations, unless both the communalities between variables are low and there are few
variables defining each factor. Due to the high correlations between the constructs under
54
consideration and to the over-determination of the factors investigated in this study, the
sample sizes in this study, which ranged from 342 to 485, were more than adequate for
CHAPTER III
RESULTS
Univariate Statistics
Univariate statistics and group comparisons for all scales are shown in Tables A1
and A2. Coefficient alphas for most scales were in the moderate-to-high range, indicating
good internal consistency. The IPIP scales had slightly lower alphas, ranging from 0.65 to
0.82 across samples. Given the brief nature of these scales, their lower reliabilities were
not surprising. The ASI Social subscale showed notably low internal consistency, with
coefficient alphas ranging from 0.42 to 0.54 across the three samples. These results
indicate that this subscale might not be unidimensional, or it may simply be due to the
scale being relatively short (three items). All ACT-relevant scales had acceptable internal
consistencies.
Group Comparisons
Group comparisons are shown in Table A3. Overall, there were almost no
differences in student responses between Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2); students showed
significantly higher social anxiety sensitivity during Time 1 than they did during Time 2,
but no other significant differences existed. Given the number of comparisons and the
lack of a theoretical rationale for this difference, it is quite possible that this represents a
type I error. Some significant group differences were found between student T1
responses and Mechanical Turk (MT) responses. Overall, the student population reported
significantly lower mental anxiety sensitivity, higher tolerance for physical discomfort,
school. These results indicate a somewhat higher functioning student sample. There was
these results indicate that the Mechanical Turk sample was not significantly more
Correlations
Tables A4, A5, and A6 show the correlations among the ACT-related scales for
the Mechanical Turk sample, the Student Time 1 sample, and the Student Time 2 sample
correlated across the three samples, with correlations ranging from 0.62 to 0.76. Other
correlations were more moderate, even when one would expect a theoretical link. For
example, the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale and the Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale
Awareness scales showed little relationship, ranging from 0.06 to 0.37. The Valued
Living Questionnaire scales showed little relationship with other ACT scales, with no
correlations consistently above 0.25 across the three samples. These results indicate some
Tables A7, A8 and A9 show the correlations among the non-ACT-related scales
across the three samples. Overall, most of the correlations are in the low-to-moderate
range. Notably high correlations include that between the ASI Physical scale and the ASI
Mental scale (rs range from 0.63 to 0.72), those between the DTS scales (rs range from
0.44 to 0.79), that between the K10 depression scale and the Automatic Thoughts
Questionnaire-Frequency (rs range from 0.71 to 0.79), and those between the WHODAS
functioning scales (rs range from 0.49 to 0.78). Overall, these higher correlations are
57
unsurprising given the nature of these scales; indeed most of the high correlations exist
Tables A10, A11, and A12 show correlations between ACT-related scales and
non-ACT-related scales across the three samples. Most correlations fall within the low-
to-moderate range. However, some notably high correlations exist. The Automatic
Thoughts Questionnaire-Frequency has high correlations with the Acceptance and Action
Believability (rs range from 0.63 to 0.89). The high correlations between the Automatic
Believability (rs range from 0.88 to 0.89) are particularly striking, and suggest that,
despite the ostensible difference between the scales, participants have difficulty
possible that experience with an ACT intervention would increase the ability of
individuals to discriminate between these things, but this is speculative. Likewise, the
K10 depression scale has high correlations with the Acceptance and Action
Questionnaire (rs range from 0.60 to 0.74). Again, these results may suggest that
untrained participants might have difficulty distinguishing between suffering on one hand
and the ACT conceptualizations of cognitive fusion and psychological inflexibility on the
other hand, or that the two constructs are simply indistinguishable in a self-report
Tables A13 and A14 show correlations between student T1 scores and student T2
scores for ACT and non-ACT measures, respectively. Test-retest correlations are strong
58
for most scales. As was the case when looking at coefficient alpha, the ASI Social
subscale shows low reliability, with a test-retest correlation of only 0.49, perhaps due in
exploratory factor analysis was conducted. In order to determine the number of factors to
extract, parallel analysis was used using SAS code published by O’Connor (2002). For
each of the three samples, 500 simulations were run using a confidence interval of 95%.
Permutation datasets were generated for each simulation to match the empirical
distribution of the samples, and principal components analysis was used. For each factor,
eigenvalues from the raw data were compared to the average eigenvalues in the simulated
datasets. If the raw eigenvalue was higher than the 95% of the eigenvalues from the
simulated datasets, then the raw eigenvalue was higher than one would expect given
chance. The results of these analyses are shown in Table A15. For the MT and T1
datasets, the first four factors extracted explained significantly more of the variance than
one would expect given chance. For the T2 dataset, the first three factors extracted
explained significantly more of the variance than one would expect given chance.
ACT scales was superior, factor loadings were calculated for each model for each of the
three samples in order the see how well each factor replicated across the three samples.
Table A16 shows the three-factor structure across the three samples. These factors
replicated well, with factor loading correlations ranging from 0.84 to 0.97 (see Table
A17). Table A18 shows the four-factor structure across the three samples. Not every
59
factor in this solution replicated across samples, indicating that this structure is not
reliable (see Table A19). These results indicate that the three-factor solution (shown in
In general, the first factor is most strongly defined by the Acceptance and Action
Fusion Questionnaire, with the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale, the FFMQ Act with
Awareness scale, and the FFMQ Nonjudge scale also loading primarily on this factor.
The second factor is defined primarily by the EQ Decentering scale, the FFMQ
Describe scale, the FFMQ Nonreact scale, the FFMQ Observe scale, the MEAQ Distress
Endurance scale, and the PHLMS Awareness scale. Because of the dominance of mindful
The third factor is defined primarily by the MEAQ Behavioral Avoidance scale,
the MEAQ Distraction/Suppression scale, the MEAQ Distress Aversion scale, and the
analyses were performed on the items of scales loading most highly on each of these
three factors. This was operationalized as scales loading >=0.35 on a factor for all three
samples (see Table A16). Using these criteria, the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire,
FFMQ Act with Awareness, and the FFMQ Nonjudge, and the Mindful Attention
60
Awareness Scale were found to be the best markers of Factor 1 (Fusion/Inflexibility). The
EQ Decentering, the FFMQ Observe, the FFMQ Describe, the FFMQ Nonresponse, the
MEAQ Distress Endurance, and the PHLMS Awareness scales were found to be the best
markers of Factor 2 (Awareness). Finally, the MEAQ Behavior Avoidance, the MEAQ
scales were found to be the best markers of Factor 3 (Avoidance). The MEAQ
Importance, and VLQ Consistency scales did not clearly load on one of these factors
Item-level factor analyses were conducted for each of these three clusters of
scales across samples. In order to determine how many lower-order factors to extract for
each factor, correlations between factor loadings were calculated for each solution across
lower-order factors could be extracted that have significantly higher eigenvalues than
expected due to chance (Table A20). However, when correlations between factor
loadings across samples were calculated, only solutions extracting three or four factors
were found to be robust, with high correlations between the loadings across samples
(Table A21). The fourth subfactor extracted was difficult to rationally interpret, and
feeling the way I’m feeling”) and a facet of cognitive fusion (e.g., “I struggle with my
thoughts”). These items were difficult to rationally distinguish from markers of the purer
and the inflexibility/fusion factor (e.g., “I need to control the thoughts that come into my
head” had strong loadings for both factors, despite the oblique rotation used). For the
solution consisted of an Inflexibility subfactor (best defined by AAQ items, CFQ items,
and FFMQ nonjudge items; example item: “I tend to get very entangled in my thoughts”),
worthless”), and a Detachment subfactor (defined by MAAS items and FFMQ Act with
Awareness items; example item: “I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of
For Factor 2 (Awareness), parallel analysis found that up to five or six lower-
order factors could be extracted whose eigenvalues were significantly higher than would
be expected due to chance (Table A25). However, when factor loading correlations
between samples were calculated, it was found that only the first four factors were the
same across samples (Table A26). This four-subfactor solution consisted of a Perspective
Taking subfactor (defined by EQ Decentering items and FFMQ Nonreact items; example
item: “I can observe unpleasant feelings without being drawn into them”), a Committed
Action subfactor (defined by MEAQ Distress Endurance items; example item: “I don’t let
pain and discomfort stop me from getting what I want”), an Expressive Awareness
subfactor (defined by FFMQ Describe items; example item: “It’s hard for me to find the
words to describe what I’m thinking”), and a Physical Awareness subfactor (defined by a
few EQ decentering items, FFMQ Observe items, and PHLMS Awareness items;
62
example item: “I pay attention to sensations, such as the wind in my hair or sun on my
For Factor 3 (Avoidance), parallel analysis found that four lower-order factors
could be extracted that have eigenvalues significantly greater than would be expected due
to chance (Table A30). When factor loading correlations were calculated across samples,
it was found that these four factors were robust and interpretable across all three samples
(Table A31). These four subfactors consisted of Physical Avoidance (defined by MEAQ
Behavioral Avoidance items and some MEAQ Distress Aversion items; example item:
“I’m quick to leave any situation that makes me feel uneasy”), Pain Aversion (defined by
MEAQ Distress Aversion items; example item: “The key to a good life is never feeling
“When upsetting memories come up, I try to focus on other things”), and Mental
Avoidance (defined by PHLMS Acceptance items; example item: “There are aspects of
myself I don’t want to think about”) (Tables A32, A33, and A34).
Sum scores were then calculated for each of the eleven subfactors listed above.
The score for each item was standardized before being added to the sum score in order to
make items using different response formats comparable. For each subfactor, items were
chosen that that had a loading of >=0.35 for all samples, and whose next highest loading
was at least 0.15 lower than the primary loading for all samples (see Tables A22-A24,
Tables A35 and A36 show the correlations among these sum scores for all three
samples, as well as test-retest correlations for the sum scores for the student sample. The
63
test-retest reliabilities for the sum scores tended to be high in the student sample. Overall,
high (rs range from 0.62 to 0.73), with most other correlations among the sum scores
falling in the low-to-moderate range. It is noteworthy that the Mental Avoidance sum
score is highly correlated with Inflexibility (rs range from 0.65 to 0.73), despite being
derived from a different higher-order factor. These results suggest that these sub-factors
An exploratory factor analysis was then performed on these sum scores in order to
recreate the higher-order factor structure (Table A37). In general, the higher-order
surprising, given its initial derivation from the Avoidance higher-order factor. Similarly,
Distraction cross loaded between Avoidance and Awareness, although it tended to load
Correlations between these sum scores and other scales of interest (all scales
whose items were not used for any sum scales, including ACT scales) were calculated
across the three samples (Tables A38-A40). The Inflexibility and Internalizing Belief
sum scores were highly correlated with a wide variety of scales, including measures of
anxiety sensitivity (rs range from 0.48 to 0.63), distress tolerance (rs range from -0.26 to -
0.65), depression (rs range from 0.63 to 0.74), neuroticism (rs range from 0.36 to 0.57),
and functioning (rs range from 0.28 to 0.55). Mental avoidance also correlated highly
with a number of scales, including measures of anxiety sensitivity (rs range from 0.40 to
0.46) and distress tolerance (rs range from -0.33 to -0.59). It was also noteworthy that
64
were not highly correlated with any of the ACT sum scores. The lack of relationship
between measures of values and valuing on one hand and the ACT sum scales on the
For each sample, three item-level CFAs were conducted in order to determine the
best overall structure for the constructs under consideration. The first structure (model 1)
Distraction, and Mental Avoidance, and the 126 items that were used to create the
subfactor sum scales. For model 1, the lower-order factors loaded only on the “parent”
factors from which they were derived (see Figure B2). Given the structure shown in
Table A34, a second hierarchical model (model 2) was tested in which Mental Avoidance
was allowed to load on both Avoidance and Fusion/Inflexibility and Distraction was
allowed to load on both Avoidance and Awareness (see Figure B3 for an illustration of
this structure). The third model tested (model 3) was a correlated factor model, in which
the higher-order factors were not included and the mid-level factors were allowed to
correlate freely (see Figure B4). Because item level data for most scales included at least
a five-point Likert scale, the data were treated as continuous, as suggested by Rhemtulla
et al. (2012). In order to compare the two models, the Akaike's Information Criterion
65
(AIC) and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) were calculated for each model. The
AIC and the BIC are used to compare the goodness of fit for different models. For both
Across all three samples, the correlated model (model 3) showed a better fit
compared with either hierarchical model across all three samples, using both the AIC and
the BIC (see Table A41). These results indicate that these ACT factors are better
structure. Across the three samples, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation
(RMSEA) for this best-fitting model ranged from 0.045 to 0.052, and the Standardized
Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) ranged from 0.067 to 0.078. These values are
within the commonly used acceptable limits set by Hu and Bentler (1999), which
establishes a cutoff of 0.06 for the RMSEA statistic and 0.08 for the SRMR statistic.
The ACT model predicts that Hexaflex processes such as experiential avoidance
correlations between psychopathology measures and ACT sum scores were calculated
across timepoints in the student sample (see Table A42). It was hypothesized that, if ACT
between the Time 1 sum score and the Time 2 psychopathology score would be higher
than the correlation between the Time 1 psychopathology score and the Time 2 sum
score. As can be seen in Table A42, this was not universally the case. Across eleven sum
relationship was found in sixteen cases, and the reverse was found in six cases. However,
the difference tended to be quite small. For the sixteen comparisons that were found to be
in the expected direction, the average difference between the correlations was only 0.04,
with a standard deviation of 0.025. For all 22 comparisons, including the six in which the
relationship was opposite of what was hypothesized, the difference between the
correlations was 0.02 in the hypothesized direction, with a standard deviation of 0.042.
The only relationship in which the difference was more than 0.06 in the expected
direction was that between Expressive Awareness and Depression. For this relationship,
the correlation between Time 1 Expressive Awareness and Time 2 Depression was 0.10
higher than the relationship between Time 2 Expressive Awareness and Time 1
Depression. However, even this difference was not statistically significant (p = 0.14).
Incremental Validity
the ACT sum scores and these external measures were calculated, partialing out variance
styles (see Tables A43-A45). Most of the partial correlations were in the low range, but
some noteworthy partial correlations emerged. The Inflexibility sum score had moderate-
to-high correlations with Depression (rs range from 0.29 to 0.40) and Neuroticism (rs
range from 0.21 to 0.39). Likewise, the Internalizing Belief sum score had notably high
partial correlations with Depression (rs range from 0.38 to 0.51). Additionally, Mental
Avoidance had moderate correlations with Depression (rs range from 0.19 to 0.26) and
67
Neuroticism (rs range from 0.25 to 0.29). Perspective Taking had moderate partial
correlations with Neuroticism (rs range from -0.20 to -0.34). Expressive Awareness had
moderate partial correlations with Openness/Imagination (rs range from 0.15 to 0.29).
Finally, Physical Avoidance had moderate partial correlations with Extraversion (rs range
from -0.22 to -0.27). These results indicate some incremental value among the ACT Sum
Scores.
For all criterion variables, squared multiple correlations first calculated for measures of
anxiety sensitivity, distress tolerance, discomfort intolerance, and coping, then for the
eleven ACT sum scores, and then for all of the explanatory variables combined. It was
found that the ACT sum scores were nearly universally better than the combined ASI,
DTS, DIS, and COPE scores in explaining these criterion variables. On average, the ACT
sum scores explained ΔR2 = 0.056 more of the variance than the non-ACT measures for
the Mechanical Turk sample (SD = 0.070), ΔR2 = 0.059 for the Student Time 1 sample
(SD = 0.039), and ΔR2 = 0.042 for the Student Time 2 sample (SD = 0.058).
ACT sum scores added incremental power above and beyond measures of anxiety
incremental value for the ACT sum scores was ΔR2 = 0.114 for the Mechanical Turk
sample (SD = 0.064), ΔR2 = 0.106 for the Student Time 1 sample (SD = 0.024), and ΔR2
= 0.102 for the Student Time 2 sample (SD = 0.034). These results indicate that the ACT
68
functioning.
69
CHAPTER IV
DISCUSSION
The aim of this study was to investigate the interrelationships among ACT
process measures in order to determine whether the ACT Hexaflex model emerges when
processes and other, similar constructs such as distress tolerance, anxiety sensitivity, and
coping styles, and to investigate whether ACT processes can be differentiated in self-
report format. Additionally, the psychometric properties of the measures, including test-
committed action, defusion, and present moment awareness. It was found that most of
these Hexaflex processes were distinguishable using factor analysis; however it was also
found that a simple correlated model fit the data better than a hierarchical model, and that
a questionnaire measure of values was only minimally related to measures of other ACT
processes. It was also hypothesized that the ACT process measures would have
was highly associated with internalizing psychopathology and neuroticism, but only
ACT process measures would have be distinguishable from and have incremental
discomfort intolerance, and coping styles. It was found that ACT process measures do
indeed have incremental validity over and above these measures when explaining
Scale Relationships
these results can be used to begin to determine the overall structure and interrelationships
inflexibility), the CFQ (a measure of cognitive fusion), and the ATQ-B (another measure
of cognitive fusion and the believability of depressive thoughts). All three measures were
separate psychological inflexibility from cognitive fusion. Gillanders et al. (2013) suggest
that this is due to the structure of the Hexaflex model, as cognitive fusion is a facet of
Hexaflex model, such as measures of mindfulness and avoidance, have much more
modest associations with the AAQ. These results also suggest that participants have great
difficulty separating these processes from the frequency and intensity of suffering, or that
71
current measures lack the specificity to make such a distinction. These results are
consistent with the findings of Gámez et al. (2010), who found that participants do not
report measures.
The strong associations between these processes may indicate that these scales are
measuring the same hypothetical constructs. This would indicate that ACT measures may
not “exist” separately from psychopathology, but rather are simply facets of or alternative
perspective, it is possible that these processes are distinct, but correlate highly because
they are under the control of the same contextual factors, or because one causes the other
(Hayes et al., 2012). For instance, an individual may report high cognitive fusion and
high depression because both are caused by the same genetic and social vulnerabilities, or
because the fusion has caused the depression. Because an explicit goal of ACT is to
weaken this contextual control, these relationships might weaken in individuals who have
Additionally, it is noteworthy that the associations between the AAQ and the
MEAQ subscales are moderate rather than strong. In the literature, the AAQ is sometimes
inflexibility. The associations between the AAQ and the MEAQ are consistent with those
found by Gámez et al. (2011). These results suggest that the AAQ measure a broader
Another striking finding when looking at simple scale correlations is that the
Valued Living Questionnaire scales have low correlations with process measures that are
psychometric weaknesses in the scale itself; the VLQ is often used as a clinical
instrument to help the ACT therapist and the client collaboratively explore the client’s
values. The VLQ may not be a good measure of client values without this collaboration.
Alternatively, the low associations may suggest a looser relationship between values and
other Hexaflex processes, such as acceptance and defusion. A third possibility is that the
relationship between chosen values and other processes of interest might be expected to
Another aim of the study was to investigate the structure of the ACT processes
under investigation in order to determine whether the Hexaflex model could be derived
structurally, and to investigate whether these processes were distinguishable. To this end,
exploratory factor analyses of ACT process measures were conducted at the scale level.
Items from scales defining each higher-order factor were further factor analyzed in order
A stable, three-factor model found at the scale level across three samples suggests
that many of these processes are indeed distinguishable. This is a particularly important
finding because the three factors appear to correspond to Hexaflex processes; cognitive
fusion, present awareness, and avoidance were found to be distinguishable at the scale
experiential avoidance, loaded on the fusion factor rather than the avoidance factor,
73
inflexibility, and that this process cannot be distinguished from cognitive fusion when
using questionnaire measures. Overall, the Hexaflex model holds up quite well in this
initial factor analysis; three of the six ACT processes (fusion, awareness, and avoidance)
are represented. The VLQ scales, representing values, were not strongly associated with
other scales, and were therefore not well modeled by any factor. Self-as-context and
committed action likely did not emerge because they were underrepresented at the scale
level.
When the items in the scales representing each of these three factors are
themselves subjected to factor analysis, eleven stable lower-order factors emerge that can
be observed across the three study samples. The Inflexibility/Fusion factor can be broken
into an Inflexibility subfactor (defined by items from the AAQ, CFQ, and FFMQ
Nonjudge), an Internalizing Belief subfactor (defined by items from the ATQ-B), and a
detachment subfactor (defined mostly by items from the MAAS). These results further
suggest that the AAQ and the CFQ are either measuring the same process or measuring
two processes that are under such similar contextual control as to be indistinguishable
when assessed using questionnaire measures. Overall, these subfactors all seem
The Awareness factor can be broken into four lower-order factors at the item
Decentering scale), Expressive Awareness (defined by items from the FFMQ Describe
scale), Committed Action (defined by items from the MEAQ Distress Endurance scale),
and Physical Awareness (defined by items from the FFMQ Observe scale). These
74
subfactors are important, as they may represent additional Hexaflex processes. The
Perspective Taking subfactor (defined by items such as “I can separate myself from my
thoughts and feelings” and “I can observe unpleasant feelings without being drawn into
them”) may be conceptually related to ACT’s self-as-context, or the self that exists
beyond ever-changing mental content. This facet is self is thought to be related to the
Additionally, the Committed Action subfactor, defined by some of the items from the
important, I won’t quit even if things get difficult”) appears to be measuring the ACT
both appear to be facets of present moment awareness. Thus, these subfactors appear to
represent the Hexaflex processes of present moment awareness, self as context, and
committed action. It is unclear why committed action is most closely associated with
awareness and self-as-context, but it may be that the ability to be mindful and to
dissociate one’s sense of self from one’s unpleasant mental content is an important
prerequisite for engaging in committed action while feeling distress. If this is the case, it
would make conceptual sense for these three points of the Hexaflex to be strongly
related.
Mental Avoidance. Physical Avoidance, defined by some items from the MEAQ
Behavioral Avoidance scale, and Pain Aversion, defined by some items from the MEAQ
Distress Aversion scale, and Mental Avoidance, defined by items from the PHLMS
75
Distraction/Suppression scale (sample item: “When upsetting memories come up, I try to
focus on other things”) also appears conceptually related to the Hexaflex process of
awareness.
Of the six points of the ACT Hexaflex, as many as five emerge in these item-level
context may be related to the perspective taking factor that is defined by some items from
the EQ. Values did not emerge in the initial factor analysis due to the VLQ’s inability to
strongly correlate with other measures. It is possible that this is due to the process of
valuing being under the control of fundamentally different contextual factors compared
with the other ACT Hexaflex processes. Alternatively, it may be that the VLQ has low
Another aim of the study was to investigate the nature of these ACT processes. To
this end, sum scores of the eleven lower-order scales were calculated, and their
determine if a three-factor structure emerges, the scores were re-factor analyzed. Despite
emerging from the Avoidance higher-order factor, the Mental Avoidance sum score cross
Distraction appeared to cross-load with both the Awareness and the Avoidance higher-
order factors. Item-level confirmatory factor analysis suggests that a simple correlated
factor model better accounts for the data compared with a hierarchical model, even when
taking these cross loadings into account. These results suggest that these processes are all
contextual factors controlling different groups of processes, these results suggest that the
contextual factors underlying these processes have significant overlap across the different
processes.
are most strongly related to psychopathology and poor functioning. Again, it is likely that
common contextual factors account for these findings. Given that the study participants
have not undergone ACT interventions, the strong relationship between fusion-related
processes and measures of suffering and functioning makes conceptual sense in light of
the ACT model. The very nature of cognitive fusion is that individuals have difficulty
relationships would be weaker among individuals who have experience with ACT
associations were in the expected direction (greater awareness and committed action were
associated with less psychopathology and greater functioning), but the relationships were
not particularly strong compared with the Cognitive Fusion and Mental Avoidance’s
77
relationships with these variables. Physical Awareness and Distraction fared even more
results may indicate that Acceptance and Defusion are particularly important Hexaflex
processes for increasing levels of functioning. It also suggests that mindfulness exercises
may be particularly useful when they help clients to increase their ability to verbally
describe their ongoing experiences, as this facet of present awareness appears to be the
longitudinal analyses were conducted in order to determine whether the ACT processes at
observed, the results were neither statistically nor clinically significant. These results may
be because the two week test-retest period is too short to detect cause and effect
relationships. Alternatively, it is possible that the ACT processes do not play the
hypothesized causal role, but rather vary along with psychopathology. To truly shed light
would be necessary.
Despite the test-retest interval likely being too short to examine cause and effect
relationships, the simple longitudinal course of the ACT processes under consideration is
informative. The ACT process sum scores were quite reliable over time compared with
many of the study scales. These results offer preliminary evidence that these processes
are “traitlike” in the sense of being relatively stable over time. The reliabilities compare
favorably to that of the Mini IPIP scales; however, it is possible that longer and more
78
robust measures of the Big Five personality traits would show higher reliability
(Donnellan et al., 2006). The stability of these processes over time suggests that they
This study also aimed to determine whether ACT process measures have
intolerance, anxiety sensitivity, and coping styles. It was found that ACT process
Mental Avoidance). These results suggest that cognitive fusion may be particularly
differentiable from non-ACT processes. Overall, the eleven ACT sum scores had
significant incremental value when added to the combined ASI, DTS, DIS, and COPE (a
total of twelve scales) in explaining most of the external study variables, including
functioning. These results indicate that these process measures are not merely slight
variations of these other constructs, but rather add unique information about individual
differences.
General Discussion
This study set out to determine the validity and utility of ACT process measures,
to determine whether these processes can be distinguished from one another when self-
report measures are used, and to investigate whether there is evidence for the ACT
Hexaflex model in the structure of these self-report measures. Strengths of this study
79
include the use of two large samples, collection of two week longitudinal data, and the
together. Because of these strengths, this study was very well suited to answer questions
Overall, the results were positive for these process measures. Measures of ACT
processes have incremental value above and beyond measures of seemingly similar
intolerance, and coping styles. Further, it was found that the ACT measures were
operationalized by the VLQ, were not strongly related to any other study measure,
including measures of ACT processes. The other ACT processes were related to some
degree. Many of these Hexaflex processes, such as avoidance, awareness, and fusion,
could be broken into several interrelated processes. Committed action and self-as-context
could not. This may be due to there being fewer items that were explicitly measuring
At the measure level, there was some conceptual overlap. Most notably, the AAQ
and the CFQ could not be distinguished from one another, either at the scale level or at
the item level. These two scales were therefore either measuring the same process or
were measuring two different processes (psychological inflexibility and cognitive fusion)
center of the Hexaflex, the AAQ is often used as a standalone measure of ACT-related
outcomes research for ACT interventions, this study’s structural analyses suggest that it
does not fully measure a number of Hexaflex processes, including present moment
avoidance; however, the results of this study suggest that it is much more closely related
to the ACT process of cognitive fusion, as it is nearly indistinguishable from the CFQ and
highly related to the ATQ-B. Additionally, these measures are strongly related to a
measure of depression. These results suggest that naïve test takers may not be able to
Overall, in order to fully assess and track these Hexaflex processes, several
measures must be used. Some measures can be used to assess multiple Hexaflex
processes. The FFMQ can be used to measure both mindful awareness and
committed action; the PHLMS can be used to measure both mindful awareness and
constructs. Such a comprehensive ACT measure could be developed relatively easily, and
the current study suggests that the ACT Hexaflex processes can be readily differentiated
at the scale level. The current study offers clear guidance to distinguish four Hexaflex
Committed Action subfactor), and Present Moment Awareness (related to the Expressive
Awareness subfactor) are clearly modeled in this study. The current study may also offer
guidance for measuring Perspective Taking using a self-report measure; however, more
research is needed to confirm the hypothesis that such a measure is associated with the
whether Values can be fully modeled using self-report measures, and, if so, how this can
best be done.
The results of this study are relevant to future research. From a purely
distinguished at the scale level, and the most ACT outcome studies that only use the
AAQ to measure changes in participants are not assessing all relevant processes. It is
recommended that future outcome studies begin to take these findings into account by
of Gámez et al. (2010) that naïve participants have difficulty distinguishing between ACT
processes and symptoms of psychopathology. Hayes et al. (2012) suggest that, since an
explicit goal of ACT is to loosen the associations between these processes, these relations
would change due to ACT interventions. Future research should test this hypothesis by
including a sample that has undergone ACT interventions. ACT interventions may also
change the relationships found in this study in other ways, such as by strengthening the
associations between self-reported values and other Hexaflex processes. A study in which
82
the structure of these process measures was examined before and after an ACT
This study also included a two week longitudinal component in order to test
relationships over time. Although this demonstrated that the test-retest correlations for
ACT processes were robust, suggesting that they are more trait-like than state-like, causal
relationships between the ACT processes and measures of psychopathology could not be
established. This may be due to the short timeframe between the test and the retest.
Future research should examine whether ACT processes affect future psychopathology
This study also did not examine behavioral measures of the Hexaflex processes.
could be examined, full construct validity for these process measures has not been
established. Although it has been established that these processes are distinguishable and
that self-report measures of these processes have incremental validity, in future research
it would be fruitful to examine differential correlates between ACT process measures and
behavioral measures. Some such research has already been done. For example, Gratz et
al. (2006) found that a behavioral measure of willingness to experience distress correlated
r = -0.76 with the AAQ. Testing whether this relationship is stronger than that between
measures have also been tested; Frewen, Evans, Maraj, Dozois, and Partridge (2008)
was periodically queried during a mindful meditation exercise. This behavioral measure
had only a modest (r = 0.34) association with the MAAS. Overall, there has been limited
research to compare whether such measures are more valid or reliable than self-report
measures.
Conclusion
The current study had multiple strengths, including two large independent
samples, a longitudinal component, and a large number of scales to fully investigate the
structure, incremental validity, stability over time, and clinical utility of ACT process
measures. It was found that the ACT Hexaflex model could largely be recreated
operationalized by the VLQ, were not related to the other processes. These processes
psychopathology, personality, and functioning. It was also found that these processes
were stable and had high reliabilities over time. It was also found that measures of
psychological inflexibility and cognitive fusion had very strong relationships with a
distinguish between this ACT process and psychological distress in a self-report format.
control on symptoms of depression. Additionally, it was found that most ACT outcome
studies only poorly measure these ACT processes; most such studies use the AAQ-II,
which, of the ACT Hexaflex processes, is most closely related to cognitive fusion. Other
84
measures may be used to more completely track the changes caused by these
interventions. Future research should also investigate how well these process measures
REFERENCES
Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). When are adaptive strategies most predictive
of psychopathology? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(1), 276-281.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-
report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27-
45.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., et al. (2008).
Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and
nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15(3), 329-342.
Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Review, 97(1), 90-
113.
Bernstein, A., Zvolensky, M. J., Vujanovic, A. A., & Moos, R. (2009). Integrating
anxiety sensitivity, distress tolerance, and discomfort intolerance: A hierarchical
model of affect sensitivity and tolerance. Behavior Therapy, 40(3), 291-301.
Biglan, A., & Hayes, S. C. (1997). Should the behavioral sciences become more
pragmatic? The case for functional contextualism in research on human behavior.
Applied and Preventive Psychology, 5(1), 47-57.
Bohlmeijer, E., ten Klooster, P. M., Fledderus, M., Veehof, M., & Baer, R. (2011).
Psychometric properties of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire in depressed
adults and development of a short form. Assessment, 18(3), 308-320.
Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. C., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., et
al. (2011). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action
Questionnaire–II: A revised measure of psychological flexibility and acceptance.
Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 1-38.
Bond, F. W., & Bunce, D. (2000). Mediators of change in emotion-focused and problem-
focused worksite stress management interventions. Journal of Occupational Health
Psychology, 5(1), 156-163.
86
Borkovec, T., & Roemer, L. (1995). Perceived functions of worry among generalized
anxiety disorder subjects: Distraction from more emotionally distressing topics?
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26(1), 25-30.
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G. J., & Van Heerden, J. (2003). The theoretical status of
latent variables. Psychological Review, 110(2), 203-219.
Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Dillworth, T. M., & Marlatt, G. A. (2007). The role of
thought suppression in the relationship between mindfulness meditation and alcohol
use. Addictive Behaviors, 32(10), 2324-2328.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its
role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
84(4), 822-848.
Brown, M. Z., Comtois, K. A., & Linehan, M. M. (2002). Reasons for suicide attempts
and nonsuicidal self-injury in women with borderline personality disorder. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 198-202.
Buckner, J. D., Keough, M. E., & Schmidt, N. B. (2007). Problematic alcohol and
cannabis use among young adults: The roles of depression and discomfort and
distress tolerance. Addictive Behaviors, 32(9), 1957-1963.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical Turk A new
source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 6(1), 3-5.
Cardaciotto, L. A., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E., & Farrow, V. (2008). The
assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance. Assessment, 15(2), 204-
223.
Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: Consider
the brief cope. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4(1), 92-100.
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A
theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2),
267-283.
Chapman, A. L., Gratz, K. L., & Brown, M. Z. (2006). Solving the puzzle of deliberate
self-harm: The experiential avoidance model. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
44(3), 371-394.
87
Chapman, A. L., Specht, M. W., & Cellucci, T. (2005). Borderline personality disorder
and deliberate Self‐Harm: Does experiential avoidance play a role? Suicide and Life-
Threatening Behavior, 35(4), 388-399.
Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2011). Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy for psychiatric
disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 187(3), 441-
453.
Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale
development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309-319.
Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. (2006). Reducing the racial
achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science, 313(5791), 1307-
1310.
Connor-Smith, J. K., & Flachsbart, C. (2007). Relations between personality and coping:
A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 1080-1107.
Cooper, M. L., Russell, M., Skinner, J. B., Frone, M. R., & Mudar, P. (1992). Stress and
alcohol use: Moderating effects of gender, coping, and alcohol expectancies. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 101(1), 139-152.
Creswell, J. D., Welch, W. T., Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Gruenewald, T. L., &
Mann, T. (2005). Affirmation of personal values buffers neuroendocrine and
psychological stress responses. Psychological Science,16(11), 846-851.
Cribb, G., Moulds, M. L., & Carter, S. (2006). Rumination and experiential avoidance in
depression. Behaviour Change, 23(3), 165-176.
Daughters, S. B., Lejuez, C., Kahler, C. W., Strong, D. R., & Brown, R. A. (2005).
Psychological distress tolerance and duration of most recent abstinence attempt
among residential treatment-seeking substance abusers. Psychology of Addictive
Behaviors, 19(2), 208-211.
Donker, T., van Straten, A., Marks, I., & Cuijpers, P. (2010). Brief self-rated screening
for depression on the internet. Journal of Affective Disorders, 122(3), 253-259.
Donnellan, M. B., Oswald, F. L., Baird, B. M., & Lucas, R. E. (2006). The mini-IPIP
scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big Five factors of personality.
Psychological Assessment, 18(2), 192-203.
Eifert, G. H., & Heffner, M. (2003). The effects of acceptance versus control contexts on
avoidance of panic-related symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 34(3), 293-312.
88
Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Gruen, R. J., & DeLongis, A. (1986). Appraisal, coping,
health status, and psychological symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 50(3), 571-579.
Foody, M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2012). The role of self in
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy. In L. McHugh, & I. Stewart (Eds.), The self
and perspective taking (pp. 125-142). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications
Inc.
Forman, E. M., Herbert, J. D., Moitra, E., Yeomans, P. D., & Geller, P. A. (2007). A
randomized controlled effectiveness trial of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy
and cognitive therapy for anxiety and depression. Behavior Modification, 31(6), 772-
799.
Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., Van Dulmen, M. H. M., Segal, Z. V., Ma, S. H., Teasdale,
J. D., et al. (2007). Initial psychometric properties of the Experiences Questionnaire:
Validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior Therapy, 38(3), 234-
246.
Frewen, P. A., Evans, E. M., Maraj, N., Dozois, D. J., & Partridge, K. (2008). Letting go:
Mindfulness and negative automatic thinking. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
32(6), 758-774.
Gámez, W., Chmielewski, M., Kotov, R., Ruggero, C., & Watson, D. (2011).
Development of a measure of experiential avoidance: The Multidimensional
Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 692-713.
Gámez, W., Kotov, R., & Watson, D. (2010). The validity of self-report assessment of
avoidance and distress. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 23(1), 87-99.
Gaudiano, B. A., & Herbert, J. D. (2006). Acute treatment of inpatients with psychotic
symptoms using acceptance and commitment therapy: Pilot results. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 44(3), 415-437.
Gifford, E. V., Kohlenberg, B. S., Hayes, S. C., Antonuccio, D. O., Piasecki, M. M.,
Rasmussen-Hall, M. L., et al. (2004). Acceptance-based treatment for smoking
cessation. Behavior Therapy, 35(4), 689-705.
Gillanders, D. T., Bolderston, H., Bond, F. W., Dempster, M., Flaxman, P. E., Campbell,
L., et al. (2013). The development and initial validation of the Cognitive Fusion
Questionnaire. Behavior Therapy, 45(1), 83-101.
89
Gloster, A. T., Klotsche, J., Chaker, S., Hummel, K. V., & Hoyer, J. (2011). Assessing
psychological flexibility: What does it add above and beyond existing constructs?
Psychological Assessment, 23(4), 970-982.
Gratz, K. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., Tull, M. T., Lejuez, C., & Gunderson, J. G. (2006). An
experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 850-855.
Gregg, J. A., Callaghan, G. M., Hayes, S. C., & Glenn-Lawson, J. L. (2007). Improving
diabetes self-management through acceptance, mindfulness, and values: A
randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(2),
336-343.
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-Based Stress
Reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
57(1), 35-43.
Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J. G. (2009).
Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and
workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. Journal
of Biomedical Informatics, 42(2), 377-381.
Harris, R. (2008). The happiness trap: How to stop struggling and start living. Boston,
Massachusetts: Trumpeter Books.
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 44(1), 1-25.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2011). Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy: The process and practice of mindful change The Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Bissett, R., Roget, N., Padilla, M., Kohlenberg, B. S., Fisher, G., et al.
(2004). The impact of acceptance and commitment training and multicultural
training on the stigmatizing attitudes and professional burnout of substance abuse
counselors. Behavior Therapy, 35(4), 821-835.
Hayes, S. C., Hayes, L. J., & Reese, H. W. (1988). Finding the philosophical core: A
review of Stephen C. Pepper's world hypotheses: A study in evidence1. Journal of
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50(1), 97-111.
Hayes, S. C., Levin, M. E., Plumb-Vilardaga, J., Villatte, J. L., & Pistorello, J. (2013).
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy and contextual behavioral science:
Examining the progress of a distinctive model of behavioral and cognitive therapy.
Behavior Therapy, 44(2), 180-198.
Hayes, S. C., Nelson, R. O., & Jarrett, R. B. (1987). The treatment utility of assessment:
A functional approach to evaluating assessment quality. American Psychologist,
42(11), 963-974.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., Wilson, K. G., Bissett, R. T., Pistorello, J., Toarmino, D., et al.
(2004). Measuring experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model.
The Psychological Record, 54(4), 553-578.
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996).
Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach
to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6),
1152-1168.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1-55.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2009). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind
to face stress, pain, and illness Delta.
Kanter, J. W., Manos, R. C., Bowe, W. M., Baruch, D. E., Busch, A. M., & Rusch, L. C.
(2010). What is behavioral activation?: A review of the empirical literature. Clinical
Psychology Review, 30(6), 608-620.
Karekla, M., & Panayiotou, G. (2011). Coping and experiential avoidance: Unique or
overlapping constructs? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
42(2), 163-170.
Kashdan, T., Zvolensky, M., & McLeish, A. (2008). Anxiety sensitivity and affect
regulatory strategies: Individual and interactive risk factors for anxiety-related
symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 429-440.
Kato, T. (2013). Frequently used coping scales: A Meta‐Analysis. Stress and Health
Keough, M. E., Riccardi, C. J., Timpano, K. R., Mitchell, M. A., & Schmidt, N. B.
(2010). Anxiety symptomatology: The association with distress tolerance and
anxiety sensitivity. Behavior Therapy, 41(4), 567-574.
Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., Mroczek, D. K., Normand, S. L. T.,
et al. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in
non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32(6), 959-976.
Ketterer, H. L., Han, K., Hur, J., & Moon, K. (2010). Development and validation of
culture-specific Variable Response Inconsistency and True Response Inconsistency
Scales for use with the Korean MMPI-2. Psychological Assessment, 22(3), 504-519.
Kollman, D. M., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2009). The construct validity of
acceptance: A multitrait-multimethod investigation. Behavior Therapy, 40(3), 205-
218.
Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., & Kramer, M. D. (2007).
Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: An integrative
quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum.Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 116(4), 645-666.
Kutz, A., Marshall, E., Bernstein, A., & Zvolensky, M. J. (2010). Evaluating emotional
sensitivity and tolerance factors in the prediction of panic-relevant responding to a
biological challenge. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(1), 16-22.
Lappalainen, R., Lehtonen, T., Skarp, E., Taubert, E., Ojanen, M., & Hayes, S. C. (2007).
The impact of CBT and ACT models using psychology trainee therapists A
preliminary controlled effectiveness trial. Behavior Modification, 31(4), 488-511.
92
Lavy, E. H., & Van den Hout, Marcel A. (1990). Thought suppression induces intrusions.
Behavioural Psychotherapy, 18(4), 251-258.
Levitt, J. T., Brown, T. A., Orsillo, S. M., & Barlow, D. H. (2004). The effects of
acceptance versus suppression of emotion on subjective and psychophysiological
response to carbon dioxide challenge in patients with panic disorder. Behavior
Therapy, 35(4), 747-766.
Leyro, T. M., Zvolensky, M. J., & Bernstein, A. (2010). Distress tolerance and
psychopathological symptoms and disorders: A review of the empirical literature
among adults. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 576-600.
Lillis, J., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Applying acceptance, mindfulness, and values to the
reduction of prejudice: A pilot study. Behavior Modification, 31(4), 389-411.
Lillis, J., Hayes, S. C., Bunting, K., & Masuda, A. (2009). Teaching acceptance and
mindfulness to improve the lives of the obese: A preliminary test of a theoretical
model. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37(1), 58-69.
Logel, C., & Cohen, G. L. (2012). The role of the self in physical health testing the effect
of a values-affirmation intervention on weight loss. Psychological Science, 23(1),
53-55.
Lomore, C. D., Spencer, S. J., & Holmes, J. G. (2007). The role of shared-values
affirmation in enhancing the feelings of low self-esteem women about their
relationships. Self and Identity, 6(4), 340-360.
Lundgren, T., Dahl, J., & Hayes, S. C. (2008). Evaluation of mediators of change in the
treatment of epilepsy with acceptance and commitment therapy. Journal of
Behavioral Medicine, 31(3), 225-235.
Lyne, K., & Roger, D. (2000). A psychometric re-assessment of the COPE questionnaire.
Personality and Individual Differences, 29(2), 321-335.
MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., & Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor
analysis. Psychological Methods, 4(1), 84-99.
Mason, W., & Suri, S. (2011). Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical
Turk. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 1-23.
93
McCracken, L. M., & Yang, S. (2006). The role of values in a contextual cognitive-
behavioral approach to chronic pain. Pain, 123(1), 137-145.
Miyake, A., Kost-Smith, L. E., Finkelstein, N. D., Pollock, S. J., Cohen, G. L., & Ito, T.
A. (2010). Reducing the gender achievement gap in college science: A classroom
study of values affirmation. Science, 330(6008), 1234-1237.
O'Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of
components using parallel analysis and Velicer's MAP test. Behavior Research
Methods, Instrumentation, and Computers, 32, 396-402.
Paez-Blarrina, M., Luciano, C., Gutiérrez-Martínez, O., Valdivia, S., Ortega, J., &
Rodríguez-Valverde, M. (2008). The role of values with personal examples in
altering the functions of pain: Comparison between acceptance-based and cognitive-
control-based protocols. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(1), 84-97.
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running experiments on Amazon
Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(5), 411-419.
Patrick, C. J., Kramer, M. D., Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2013). Optimizing
efficiency of psychopathology assessment through quantitative modeling:
Development of a brief form of the externalizing spectrum inventory. Psychological
Assessment, 25(4), 1332-1348.
Piet, J., & Hougaard, E. (2011). The effect of Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for
prevention of relapse in recurrent major depressive disorder: A systematic review
and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 1032-1040.
Pösl, M., Cieza, A., & Stucki, G. (2007). Psychometric properties of the WHODAS-II in
rehabilitation patients. Quality of Life Research, 16(9), 1521-1531.
94
Reiss, S., Peterson, R. A., Gursky, D. M., & McNally, R. J. (1986). Anxiety sensitivity,
anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 24(1), 1-8.
Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical
variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and
categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological
Methods, 17(3), 354-373.
Rodriguez, B. F., Bruce, S. E., Pagano, M. E., Spencer, M. A., & Keller, M. B. (2004).
Factor structure and stability of the anxiety sensitivity index in a longitudinal study
of anxiety disorder patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(1), 79-91.
Rogers, C. R. (1964). Toward a modern approach to values: The valuing process in the
mature person. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68(2), 160-167.
Sharp, W., Schulenberg, S. E., Wilson, K. G., & Murrell, A. R. (2004). Logotherapy and
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT): An initial comparison of values-
centered approaches. International Forum for Logotherapy, 27(2), 98-105.
Simons, J. S., & Gaher, R. M. (2005). The Distress Tolerance scale: Development and
validation of a self-report measure. Motivation and Emotion, 29(2), 83-102.
Smits, J. A., Berry, A. C., Tart, C. D., & Powers, M. B. (2008). The efficacy of cognitive-
behavioral interventions for reducing anxiety sensitivity: A meta-analytic review.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(9), 1047-1054.
Stewart, I., Villatte, J., & McHugh, L. (2012). Approaches to the self. In L. McHugh, & I.
Stewart (Eds.), The self and perspective taking (pp. 3-35). Oakland, CA: New
Harbinger Publications Inc.
Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1985). The relative efficacy of avoidant and nonavoidant coping
strategies: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 4(3), 249-288.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does Cognitive Therapy
prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training
help? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 25-39.
95
Timpano, K. R., Buckner, J. D., Richey, J. A., Murphy, D. L., & Schmidt, N. B. (2009).
Exploration of anxiety sensitivity and distress tolerance as vulnerability factors for
hoarding behaviors. Depression and Anxiety, 26(4), 343-353.
Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2007). Emotion regulation difficulties associated with the
experience of uncued panic attacks: Evidence of experiential avoidance, emotional
nonacceptance, and decreased emotional clarity. Behavior Therapy, 38(4), 378-391.
Twohig, M. P., Hayes, S. C., & Masuda, A. (2006). Increasing willingness to experience
obsessions: Acceptance and commitment therapy as a treatment for obsessive-
compulsive disorder. Behavior Therapy, 37(1), 3-13.
Twohig, M. P., Hayes, S. C., Plumb, J. C., Pruitt, L. D., Collins, A. B., Hazlett-Stevens,
H., et al. (2010). A randomized clinical trial of Acceptance and Commitment
Therapy versus progressive relaxation training for obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78(5), 705-716.
Varra, A. A., Hayes, S. C., Roget, N., & Fisher, G. (2008). A randomized control trial
examining the effect of acceptance and commitment training on clinician willingness
to use evidence-based pharmacotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 76(3), 449-458.
Vujanovic, A. A., Zvolensky, M. J., Bernstein, A., Feldner, M. T., & McLeish, A. C.
(2007). A test of the interactive effects of anxiety sensitivity and mindfulness in the
prediction of anxious arousal, agoraphobic cognitions, and body vigilance.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(6), 1393-1400.
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects
of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5-13.
Wilson, K. G., Sandoz, E. K., Kitchens, J., & Roberts, M. (2011). The Valued Living
Questionnaire: Defining and measuring valued action within a behavioral
framework. The Psychological Record, 60(2), 249-272.
Wilson, K. G., & Dufrene, T. (2010). Things might go terribly, horribly wrong: A guide
to life liberated from anxiety. Oakland, California: New Harbinger Publications, Inc.
Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1986). Dysfunctional control by client verbal behavior: The
context of reason-giving. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 4, 30-38.
Zettle, R. D., Rains, J. C., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). Processes of change in Acceptance and
Commitment Therapy and Cognitive Therapy for depression: A mediation reanalysis
of Zettle and Rains. Behavior Modification,35(3), 265-283.
Zinbarg, R. E., Barlow, D. H., & Brown, T. A. (1997). Hierarchical structure and general
factor saturation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index: Evidence and implications.
Psychological Assessment, 9(3), 277-284.
Zvolensky, M. J., Vujanovic, A. A., Bernstein, A., & Leyro, T. (2010). Distress
tolerance: Theory, measurement, and relations to psychopathology. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 19(6), 406-410.
97
APPENDIX A
TABLES
98
100
Externalizing 482 35.23 7.23 0.81 339 34.78 7.67 0.85
Table A2. Continued
101
VLQ Importance 482 79.92 12.57 0.83 340 78.55 13.97 0.87
Table A2. Continued
102
103
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. AAQ --
2. ATQ Believability 0.73 --
3. CFQ 0.73 0.68 --
4. EQ decentering -0.29 -0.24 -0.44 --
5. FFMQ Observe 0.05 0.12 0.10 0.38 --
6. FFMQ Describe -0.35 -0.25 -0.27 0.36 0.30 --
7. FFMQ Act with Awareness -0.61 -0.56 -0.61 0.27 -0.02 0.37 --
8. FFMQ Nonjudge -0.52 -0.53 -0.55 0.08 -0.35 0.18 0.52 --
9. FFMQ Nonreact -0.11 -0.02 -0.28 0.61 0.32 0.23 -0.01 -0.08
10. MEAQ Behavior Avoidance 0.32 0.24 0.35 -0.03 0.16 -0.12 -0.25 -0.43
11. MEAQ Distress Aversion 0.26 0.23 0.32 -0.01 0.22 0.03 -0.16 -0.44
12. MEAQ Procrastination 0.48 0.39 0.48 -0.25 -0.04 -0.36 -0.53 -0.40
13. MEAQ Distraction/Suppression 0.00 0.00 -0.02 0.31 0.29 0.06 0.04 -0.29
14. MEAQ Repression/Denial 0.46 0.44 0.39 -0.06 -0.07 -0.44 -0.52 -0.45
15. MEAQ Distress Endurance -0.26 -0.21 -0.30 0.52 0.33 0.31 0.20 0.02
16. MAAS -0.49 -0.44 -0.52 0.47 0.27 0.40 0.61 0.25
17. PHLMS Awareness -0.04 0.01 -0.01 0.44 0.68 0.40 0.14 -0.20
18. PHLMS Acceptance 0.38 0.34 0.36 0.07 0.27 -0.13 -0.33 -0.60
19. WBSI 0.55 0.46 0.59 -0.20 0.18 -0.25 -0.46 -0.57
20. VLQ Importance -0.21 -0.16 -0.17 0.31 0.21 0.16 0.20 -0.08
21. VLQ Consistence -0.14 -0.15 -0.21 0.33 0.15 0.22 0.21 0.03
105
Table A4. Continued
9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
9. FFMQ Nonreact --
10. MEAQ Behavior Avoidance 0.06 --
11. MEAQ Distress Aversion 0.02 0.70 --
12. MEAQ Procrastination -0.12 0.50 0.34 --
13. MEAQ Distraction/Suppression 0.30 0.56 0.58 0.18 --
14. MEAQ Repression/Denial 0.10 0.37 0.27 0.52 0.14 --
15. MEAQ Distress Endurance 0.53 0.03 0.09 -0.29 0.44 -0.11 --
16. MAAS 0.23 -0.07 0.04 -0.37 0.19 -0.44 0.35 --
17. PHLMS Awareness 0.37 0.22 0.25 -0.10 0.40 -0.20 0.50 0.37
18. PHLMS Acceptance 0.13 0.55 0.56 0.39 0.63 0.36 0.15 -0.09
19. WBSI -0.05 0.54 0.54 0.48 0.44 0.42 0.02 -0.30
20. VLQ Importance 0.13 0.06 0.19 -0.06 0.32 0.03 0.18 0.23
21. VLQ Consistence 0.15 0.08 0.11 -0.09 0.17 0.04 0.13 0.19
106
Table A4. Continued
17 18 19 20 21
17. PHLMS Awareness --
18. PHLMS Acceptance 0.38 --
19. WBSI 0.20 0.72 --
20. VLQ Importance 0.23 0.13 0.01 --
21. VLQ Consistence 0.17 0.00 -0.06 0.63 --
Note. Ns range from 302 to 333. AAQ = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; ATQ =
Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire; CFQ = Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire; FFMQ =
Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire; MAAS = Mindful Attention Awareness Scale;
MEAQ = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire; PHLMS =
Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale; VLQ = Valued Living Questionnaire; WBSI = White
Bear Suppression Inventory
107
Table A5. Correlations among ACT scales--Student Time 1
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. AAQ --
2. ATQ Believability 0.70 --
3. CFQ 0.72 0.62 --
4. EQ decentering -0.41 -0.31 -0.50 --
5. FFMQ Observe 0.14 0.18 0.14 0.16 --
6. FFMQ Describe -0.30 -0.22 -0.29 0.36 0.19 --
7. FFMQ Act with Awareness -0.44 -0.40 -0.42 0.20 -0.24 0.24 --
8. FFMQ Nonjudge -0.58 -0.53 -0.64 0.32 -0.30 0.24 0.46 --
9. FFMQ Nonreact -0.10 -0.07 -0.27 0.46 0.33 0.26 -0.06 0.06
10. MEAQ Behavior Avoidance 0.32 0.25 0.32 -0.16 0.05 -0.21 -0.33 -0.33
11. MEAQ Distress Aversion 0.43 0.34 0.44 -0.19 0.13 -0.18 -0.33 -0.44
12. MEAQ Procrastination 0.36 0.29 0.26 -0.22 0.08 -0.26 -0.52 -0.27
13. MEAQ Distraction/Suppression 0.22 0.11 0.22 -0.05 0.09 -0.09 -0.26 -0.34
14. MEAQ Repression/Denial 0.37 0.32 0.33 -0.16 -0.05 -0.55 -0.44 -0.40
15. MEAQ Distress Endurance -0.14 -0.13 -0.20 0.36 0.29 0.29 0.14 0.06
16. MAAS -0.39 -0.36 -0.44 0.28 -0.11 0.29 0.67 0.40
17. PHLMS Awareness 0.04 0.06 0.05 0.24 0.59 0.29 -0.11 -0.12
18. PHLMS Acceptance 0.51 0.45 0.60 -0.25 0.22 -0.22 -0.45 -0.65
19. WBSI 0.54 0.45 0.60 -0.34 0.24 -0.22 -0.49 -0.59
20. VLQ Importance -0.12 -0.13 -0.07 0.20 0.07 0.16 0.06 0.09
21. VLQ Consistence -0.23 -0.20 -0.21 0.34 0.04 0.21 0.16 0.14
108
Table A5. Continued
9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
9. FFMQ Nonreact --
10. MEAQ Behavior Avoidance -0.10 --
11. MEAQ Distress Aversion -0.13 0.57 --
12. MEAQ Procrastination -0.04 0.51 0.34 --
13. MEAQ Distraction/Suppression -0.02 0.46 0.56 0.20 --
14. MEAQ Repression/Denial -0.09 0.41 0.40 0.41 0.27 --
15. MEAQ Distress Endurance 0.33 -0.23 -0.09 -0.25 0.15 -0.29 --
16. MAAS -0.01 -0.20 -0.24 -0.39 -0.19 -0.40 0.14 --
17. PHLMS Awareness 0.24 0.05 0.08 0.03 0.09 -0.21 0.30 0.06
18. PHLMS Acceptance -0.13 0.37 0.54 0.31 0.58 0.40 -0.01 -0.40
19. WBSI -0.11 0.28 0.41 0.33 0.41 0.29 0.03 -0.53
20. VLQ Importance -0.03 -0.07 0.10 -0.16 0.17 -0.19 0.26 0.16
21. VLQ Consistence 0.12 -0.05 -0.05 -0.21 0.06 -0.13 0.19 0.25
109
Table A5. Continued
17 18 19 20 21
17. PHLMS Awareness --
18. PHLMS Acceptance 0.28 --
19. WBSI 0.21 0.71 --
20. VLQ Importance 0.13 0.02 0.00 --
21. VLQ Consistence 0.09 -0.10 -0.18 0.47 --
Note. Ns range from 478 to 481. AAQ = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; ATQ =
Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire; CFQ = Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire; FFMQ = Five
Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire; MAAS = Mindful Attention Awareness Scale; MEAQ =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire; PHLMS = Philadelphia Mindfulness
Scale; VLQ = Valued Living Questionnaire; WBSI = White Bear Suppression Inventory
110
Table A6. Correlations among ACT scales--Student Time 2
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. AAQ --
2. ATQ Believability 0.68 --
3. CFQ 0.76 0.67 --
4. EQ decentering -0.44 -0.38 -0.49 --
5. FFMQ Observe 0.12 0.18 0.12 0.33 --
6. FFMQ Describe -0.36 -0.32 -0.37 0.45 0.32 --
7. FFMQ Act with Awareness -0.45 -0.38 -0.50 0.21 -0.26 0.26 --
8. FFMQ Nonjudge -0.57 -0.46 -0.69 0.26 -0.25 0.25 0.49 --
9. FFMQ Nonreact -0.15 -0.14 -0.33 0.56 0.42 0.32 -0.06 0.02
10. MEAQ Behavior Avoidance 0.45 0.29 0.42 -0.21 0.11 -0.28 -0.37 -0.40
11. MEAQ Distress Aversion 0.47 0.30 0.42 -0.23 0.09 -0.20 -0.32 -0.43
12. MEAQ Procrastination 0.49 0.40 0.44 -0.25 0.12 -0.34 -0.56 -0.37
13. MEAQ Distraction/Suppression 0.36 0.17 0.36 -0.12 0.15 -0.09 -0.27 -0.42
14. MEAQ Repression/Denial 0.46 0.43 0.44 -0.30 -0.10 -0.56 -0.38 -0.39
15. MEAQ Distress Endurance -0.18 -0.22 -0.25 0.39 0.26 0.42 0.16 0.13
16. MAAS -0.46 -0.39 -0.49 0.32 -0.05 0.37 0.67 0.39
17. PHLMS Awareness -0.01 0.00 -0.03 0.39 0.69 0.41 -0.09 -0.11
18. PHLMS Acceptance 0.56 0.40 0.60 -0.23 0.23 -0.21 -0.45 -0.68
19. WBSI 0.57 0.41 0.62 -0.27 0.16 -0.22 -0.47 -0.60
20. VLQ Importance -0.18 -0.29 -0.16 0.20 0.01 0.19 0.12 0.09
21. VLQ Consistence -0.27 -0.27 -0.22 0.18 -0.02 0.25 0.23 0.15
111
Table A6. Continued
9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
9. FFMQ Nonreact --
10. MEAQ Behavior Avoidance -0.08 --
11. MEAQ Distress Aversion -0.13 0.66 --
12. MEAQ Procrastination -0.05 0.57 0.46 --
13. MEAQ Distraction/Suppression 0.01 0.57 0.65 0.41 --
14. MEAQ Repression/Denial -0.09 0.47 0.44 0.53 0.27 --
15. MEAQ Distress Endurance 0.35 -0.14 -0.08 -0.16 0.23 -0.32 --
16. MAAS 0.08 -0.27 -0.23 -0.44 -0.18 -0.45 0.24 --
17. PHLMS Awareness 0.37 0.03 0.06 -0.02 0.15 -0.27 0.40 0.16
18. PHLMS Acceptance -0.06 0.48 0.54 0.45 0.62 0.39 -0.02 -0.35
19. WBSI -0.10 0.45 0.47 0.46 0.57 0.37 0.07 -0.45
20. VLQ Importance -0.01 0.01 0.11 -0.10 0.15 -0.15 0.15 0.28
21. VLQ Consistence 0.00 -0.05 0.01 -0.20 0.06 -0.16 0.12 0.33
112
Table A6. Continued
17 18 19 20 21
17. PHLMS Awareness --
18. PHLMS Acceptance 0.29 --
19. WBSI 0.13 0.73 --
20. VLQ Importance 0.17 -0.01 -0.06 --
21. VLQ Consistence 0.12 -0.13 -0.16 0.61 --
Note. Ns range from 337 to 340. AAQ = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; ATQ =
Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire; CFQ = Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire; FFMQ = Five
Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire; MAAS = Mindful Attention Awareness Scale; MEAQ =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Questionnaire; PHLMS = Philadelphia Mindfulness
Scale; VLQ = Valued Living Questionnaire; WBSI = White Bear Suppression Inventory
113
114
1 2 3 4 5
1. ASI Physical --
2. ASI Mental 0.72 --
3. ASI Social 0.48 0.43 --
4. ATQ Frequency 0.50 0.56 0.24 --
5. COPE Active Coping -0.08 -0.04 0.09 -0.21 --
6. COPE Emotion-Focused Coping 0.28 0.34 0.17 0.25 0.32
7. COPE Avoidant Coping 0.43 0.53 0.22 0.50 0.08
8. DIS Tolerance -0.04 0.01 0.02 0.03 0.20
9. DIS Avoidance 0.24 0.29 0.16 0.03 0.13
10. DTS Tolerance -0.23 -0.25 -0.19 -0.18 0.21
11. DTS Appraisal -0.40 -0.41 -0.22 -0.44 0.30
12. DTS Absorbtion -0.32 -0.33 -0.19 -0.31 0.25
13. DTS Regulation -0.19 -0.22 -0.30 -0.06 0.01
14. Externalizing 0.42 0.46 0.21 0.55 -0.15
15. K10 0.49 0.55 0.23 0.79 -0.23
16. IPIP Extraversion -0.07 -0.01 -0.03 -0.22 0.24
17. IPIP Agreeableness -0.05 -0.13 0.04 -0.15 0.25
18. IPIP Conscientiousness -0.30 -0.34 -0.08 -0.42 0.24
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.33 0.37 0.12 0.52 -0.39
20. IPIP Openness -0.24 -0.28 -0.05 -0.24 0.18
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.42 0.57 0.22 0.56 -0.13
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.38 0.46 0.20 0.51 -0.16
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.31 0.41 0.13 0.46 -0.15
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.30 0.45 0.16 0.46 -0.12
25. VRIN 0.18 0.22 0.25 0.19 -0.15
Table A7. Continued
6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
6. COPE Emotion-Focused Coping --
7. COPE Avoidant Coping 0.49 --
8. DIS Tolerance 0.00 0.03 --
9. DIS Avoidance 0.09 0.12 -0.08 --
10. DTS Tolerance -0.12 -0.15 0.24 -0.03 --
11. DTS Appraisal -0.17 -0.36 0.16 0.01 0.64 --
12. DTS Absorbtion -0.19 -0.30 0.18 -0.05 0.70 0.78 --
13. DTS Regulation -0.10 -0.06 0.11 -0.10 0.44 0.44 0.48 --
14. Externalizing 0.20 0.47 0.02 0.01 -0.12 -0.35 -0.27 -0.05
15. K10 0.31 0.57 0.06 0.01 -0.24 -0.51 -0.43 -0.09
16. IPIP Extraversion 0.17 -0.06 0.07 0.07 0.11 0.17 0.20 0.00
17. IPIP Agreeableness 0.24 -0.13 0.08 0.09 0.15 0.14 0.13 -0.01
18. IPIP Conscientiousness -0.11 -0.40 0.04 0.05 0.22 0.29 0.30 0.04
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.16 0.28 -0.06 -0.04 -0.38 -0.52 -0.48 -0.20
20. IPIP Openness -0.12 -0.26 0.11 0.00 0.13 0.19 0.14 0.01
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.33 0.62 0.03 0.05 -0.17 -0.39 -0.25 -0.10
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.22 0.55 0.05 0.03 -0.16 -0.37 -0.29 -0.11
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.25 0.46 0.00 0.05 -0.17 -0.30 -0.25 -0.10
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.24 0.52 0.01 0.07 -0.15 -0.33 -0.24 -0.08
25. VRIN 0.08 0.31 -0.06 -0.02 -0.13 -0.24 -0.22 -0.18
115
Table A7. Continued
14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21
14. Externalizing --
15. K10 0.60 --
16. IPIP Extraversion -0.01 -0.18 --
17. IPIP Agreeableness -0.31 -0.17 0.27 --
18. IPIP Conscientiousness -0.47 -0.45 0.13 0.28 --
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.27 0.62 -0.32 -0.14 -0.34 --
20. IPIP Openness -0.39 -0.29 0.08 0.36 0.27 -0.19 --
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.56 0.63 0.08 -0.21 -0.43 0.34 -0.36 --
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.49 0.59 -0.05 -0.26 -0.39 0.35 -0.32 0.78
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.45 0.51 0.08 -0.22 -0.48 0.30 -0.34 0.76
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.45 0.52 0.04 -0.15 -0.41 0.28 -0.31 0.78
25. VRIN 0.33 0.26 0.03 -0.18 -0.19 0.15 -0.22 0.31
116
Table A7. Continued
22 23 24 25
22. WHODAS Getting Along --
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.72 --
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.67 0.73 --
25. VRIN 0.28 0.24 0.27 --
Note. Ns range from 272 to 342. ASI = Anxiety Sensitivity Index; ATQ = Automatic
Thoughts Questionnaire; DIS = Discomfort Intolerance Scale; DTS = Distress
Tolerance Scale; IPIP = International Personality Item Pool; K10 = Kessler
Psychological Distress Scale; WHODAS = World Health Organization Disability
Assessment; VRIN = Variable Response Inventory
117
118
1 2 3 4 5
1. ASI Physical --
2. ASI Mental 0.63 --
3. ASI Social 0.41 0.38 --
4. ATQ Frequency 0.42 0.62 0.27 --
5. COPE Active Coping 0.01 -0.05 0.08 -0.17 --
6. COPE Emotion-Focused Coping 0.19 0.11 0.03 0.05 0.36
7. COPE Avoidant Coping 0.35 0.50 0.16 0.50 0.07
8. DIS Tolerance -0.07 0.02 0.12 0.03 0.16
9. DIS Avoidance 0.27 0.22 0.15 0.14 0.09
10. DTS Tolerance -0.32 -0.36 -0.19 -0.37 0.15
11. DTS Appraisal -0.41 -0.53 -0.27 -0.50 0.16
12. DTS Absorbtion -0.37 -0.43 -0.20 -0.48 0.13
13. DTS Regulation -0.32 -0.30 -0.25 -0.28 -0.02
14. Externalizing 0.22 0.32 0.20 0.47 -0.11
15. K10 0.39 0.53 0.28 0.71 -0.17
16. IPIP Extraversion -0.10 -0.08 -0.03 -0.15 0.15
17. IPIP Agreeableness 0.00 -0.17 0.04 -0.13 0.24
18. IPIP Conscientiousness -0.11 -0.20 0.04 -0.26 0.18
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.28 0.35 0.13 0.39 -0.13
20. IPIP Openness -0.09 -0.02 0.04 0.03 0.21
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.28 0.41 0.20 0.45 -0.15
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.25 0.39 0.16 0.46 -0.05
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.22 0.34 0.10 0.36 -0.10
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.26 0.35 0.18 0.46 -0.05
25. VRIN 0.28 0.37 0.22 0.41 -0.08
Table A8. Continued
6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
6. COPE Emotion-Focused Coping --
7. COPE Avoidant Coping 0.27 --
8. DIS Tolerance -0.21 -0.07 --
9. DIS Avoidance 0.17 0.16 -0.25 --
10. DTS Tolerance -0.18 -0.25 0.21 -0.28 --
11. DTS Appraisal -0.24 -0.42 0.17 -0.22 0.68 --
12. DTS Absorbtion -0.26 -0.35 0.15 -0.20 0.77 0.74 --
13. DTS Regulation -0.22 -0.26 0.12 -0.28 0.57 0.59 0.55 --
14. Externalizing 0.11 0.40 -0.06 0.16 -0.30 -0.35 -0.38 -0.24
15. K10 0.10 0.44 -0.03 0.17 -0.44 -0.57 -0.53 -0.32
16. IPIP Extraversion 0.16 0.00 0.00 0.04 0.05 0.06 0.02 -0.05
17. IPIP Agreeableness 0.24 -0.19 0.01 -0.01 0.04 0.08 0.05 0.00
18. IPIP Conscientiousness 0.00 -0.32 0.08 -0.11 0.11 0.23 0.16 0.06
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.28 0.28 -0.21 0.06 -0.39 -0.55 -0.51 -0.26
20. IPIP Openness 0.04 -0.06 0.12 -0.01 0.14 0.11 0.04 0.12
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.04 0.39 -0.12 0.20 -0.26 -0.38 -0.31 -0.25
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.02 0.33 -0.07 0.12 -0.22 -0.30 -0.24 -0.14
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.06 0.35 -0.11 0.14 -0.20 -0.28 -0.27 -0.16
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.11 0.32 0.02 0.18 -0.24 -0.31 -0.30 -0.21
25. VRIN 0.08 0.35 -0.09 0.16 -0.26 -0.31 -0.26 -0.17
119
Table A8. Continued
14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21
14. Externalizing --
15. K10 0.42 --
16. IPIP Extraversion 0.02 -0.12 --
17. IPIP Agreeableness -0.10 -0.11 0.25 --
18. IPIP Conscientiousness -0.36 -0.25 0.10 0.20 --
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.21 0.51 -0.12 -0.03 -0.14 --
20. IPIP Openness 0.06 0.00 0.10 0.22 0.04 0.04 --
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.42 0.56 -0.17 -0.25 -0.37 0.31 -0.09 --
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.40 0.44 -0.32 -0.26 -0.24 0.27 -0.01 0.68
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.39 0.39 -0.07 -0.14 -0.38 0.24 -0.06 0.60
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.35 0.48 -0.07 -0.06 -0.26 0.24 0.00 0.61
25. VRIN 0.24 0.44 -0.08 -0.13 -0.16 0.31 -0.03 0.30
120
Table A8 Continued
22 23 24 25
22. WHODAS Getting Along --
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.56 --
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.49 0.55 --
25. VRIN 0.27 0.22 0.26 --
Note. Ns range from 454 to 483. ASI = Anxiety Sensitivity Index; ATQ =
Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire; DIS = Discomfort Intolerance Scale;
DTS = Distress Tolerance Scale; IPIP = International Personality Item Pool;
K10 = Kessler Psychological Distress Scale; WHODAS = World Health
Organization Disability Assessment; VRIN = Variable Response Inventory
121
122
1 2 3 4 5
1. ASI Physical --
2. ASI Mental 0.69 --
3. ASI Social 0.52 0.48 --
4. ATQ Frequency 0.46 0.54 0.39 --
5. COPE Active Coping -0.10 -0.15 0.07 -0.26 --
6. COPE Emotion-Focused Coping 0.15 0.06 0.06 0.02 0.38
7. COPE Avoidant Coping 0.31 0.45 0.25 0.49 0.00
8. DIS Tolerance -0.12 -0.08 0.10 -0.01 0.19
9. DIS Avoidance 0.24 0.21 0.14 0.11 0.05
10. DTS Tolerance -0.37 -0.39 -0.21 -0.55 0.19
11. DTS Appraisal -0.44 -0.54 -0.31 -0.56 0.20
12. DTS Absorbtion -0.41 -0.42 -0.29 -0.59 0.18
13. DTS Regulation -0.31 -0.37 -0.30 -0.39 0.00
14. Externalizing 0.29 0.35 0.32 0.50 -0.21
15. K10 0.48 0.54 0.32 0.73 -0.26
16. IPIP Extraversion -0.19 -0.12 -0.15 -0.27 0.23
17. IPIP Agreeableness -0.09 -0.21 -0.03 -0.22 0.38
18. IPIP Conscientiousness -0.24 -0.27 -0.16 -0.37 0.35
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.30 0.29 0.18 0.39 -0.22
20. IPIP Openness -0.18 -0.18 -0.01 -0.06 0.20
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.38 0.54 0.28 0.57 -0.27
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.34 0.46 0.23 0.56 -0.23
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.38 0.45 0.29 0.58 -0.26
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.28 0.41 0.28 0.52 -0.21
25. VRIN 0.23 0.29 0.18 0.34 -0.19
Table A9. Continued
6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
6. COPE Emotion-Focused Coping --
7. COPE Avoidant Coping 0.17 --
8. DIS Tolerance -0.19 -0.05 --
9. DIS Avoidance 0.14 0.06 -0.24 --
10. DTS Tolerance -0.14 -0.35 0.16 -0.27 --
11. DTS Appraisal -0.21 -0.46 0.17 -0.22 0.75 --
12. DTS Absorbtion -0.22 -0.39 0.14 -0.23 0.79 0.78 --
13. DTS Regulation -0.18 -0.27 0.08 -0.26 0.69 0.65 0.67 --
14. Externalizing 0.02 0.31 -0.03 0.05 -0.31 -0.34 -0.35 -0.29
15. K10 0.07 0.43 -0.10 0.19 -0.47 -0.54 -0.54 -0.33
16. IPIP Extraversion 0.17 -0.10 0.01 0.04 0.12 0.14 0.12 0.01
17. IPIP Agreeableness 0.32 -0.21 0.01 0.02 0.09 0.11 0.08 0.03
18. IPIP Conscientiousness 0.06 -0.33 0.09 -0.12 0.28 0.34 0.34 0.16
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.27 0.17 -0.23 0.15 -0.37 -0.49 -0.48 -0.28
20. IPIP Openness 0.06 -0.14 0.07 0.05 0.10 0.14 0.06 0.08
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm -0.06 0.42 -0.14 0.12 -0.39 -0.45 -0.40 -0.30
22. WHODAS Getting Along -0.08 0.35 -0.10 0.08 -0.35 -0.41 -0.35 -0.23
23. WHODAS Life Activities -0.02 0.35 -0.19 0.13 -0.41 -0.44 -0.42 -0.30
24. WHODAS Work/School -0.04 0.31 -0.08 0.16 -0.30 -0.34 -0.35 -0.27
25. VRIN -0.02 0.26 0.00 -0.03 -0.23 -0.28 -0.26 -0.21
123
Table A9. Continued
14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21
14. Externalizing --
15. K10 0.41 --
16. IPIP Extraversion -0.05 -0.24 --
17. IPIP Agreeableness -0.15 -0.24 0.32 --
18. IPIP Conscientiousness -0.40 -0.38 0.24 0.36 --
19. IPIP Neuroticism 0.23 0.50 -0.15 -0.02 -0.22 --
20. IPIP Openness -0.01 -0.13 0.06 0.24 0.09 -0.01 --
21. WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.46 0.58 -0.26 -0.29 -0.47 0.33 -0.15 --
22. WHODAS Getting Along 0.41 0.53 -0.37 -0.34 -0.37 0.30 -0.07 0.77
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.46 0.51 -0.25 -0.29 -0.48 0.30 -0.08 0.78
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.39 0.48 -0.16 -0.17 -0.40 0.28 -0.08 0.72
25. VRIN 0.28 0.40 -0.07 -0.19 -0.24 0.17 -0.07 0.23
124
Table A9. Continued
22 23 24 25
22. WHODAS Getting Along --
23. WHODAS Life Activities 0.71 --
24. WHODAS Work/School 0.60 0.68 --
25. VRIN 0.23 0.26 0.22 --
Note. Ns range from 318 to 340. ASI = Anxiety Sensitivity Index; ATQ =
Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire; DIS = Discomfort Intolerance Scale;
DTS = Distress Tolerance Scale; IPIP = International Personality Item Pool;
K10 = Kessler Psychological Distress Scale; WHODAS = World Health
Organization Disability Assessment; VRIN = Variable Response Inventory
125
126
FFMQ
FFMQ Obs FFMQ Des FFMQ Act Nonj
ASI Physical 0.15 -0.18 -0.38 -0.45
ASI Mental 0.08 -0.23 -0.46 -0.53
ASI Social 0.15 -0.03 -0.12 -0.35
ATQ Frequency 0.09 -0.29 -0.61 -0.50
COPE Active Coping 0.29 0.29 0.16 -0.02
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.13 -0.02 -0.24 -0.28
COPE Avoidant Coping 0.06 -0.32 -0.53 -0.44
DIS Tolerance 0.23 0.09 -0.10 -0.10
DIS Avoidance 0.19 0.15 0.01 -0.15
DTS Tolerance 0.11 0.23 0.17 0.20
DTS Appraisal 0.01 0.33 0.41 0.42
DTS Absorbtion 0.03 0.24 0.33 0.32
DTS Regulation -0.05 0.06 0.01 0.27
Externalizing 0.01 -0.26 -0.58 -0.37
K10 0.10 -0.29 -0.68 -0.53
IPIP Extraversion 0.08 0.28 0.22 0.13
IPIP Agreeableness 0.35 0.40 0.25 -0.03
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.17 0.38 0.51 0.21
IPIP Neuroticism -0.01 -0.30 -0.46 -0.39
IPIP Openness 0.34 0.39 0.29 0.11
WHODAS
Understand/Comm -0.02 -0.33 -0.54 -0.40
WHODAS Getting Along -0.02 -0.34 -0.46 -0.41
WHODAS Life
Activities -0.09 -0.30 -0.42 -0.29
WHODAS Work/School -0.07 -0.26 -0.45 -0.30
VRIN -0.11 -0.23 -0.12 -0.15
128
FFMQ MEAQ
Nonr MEAQ BA DAv MEAQ Pr
ASI Physical 0.02 0.34 0.25 0.32
ASI Mental -0.01 0.37 0.34 0.37
ASI Social 0.10 0.37 0.34 0.19
ATQ Frequency -0.06 0.19 0.19 0.38
COPE Active Coping 0.53 0.10 0.08 -0.16
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.07 0.23 0.18 0.24
COPE Avoidant Coping 0.11 0.33 0.17 0.52
DIS Tolerance 0.21 -0.10 -0.10 -0.05
DIS Avoidance 0.12 0.29 0.30 0.09
DTS Tolerance 0.25 -0.34 -0.36 -0.24
DTS Appraisal 0.26 -0.35 -0.33 -0.33
DTS Absorbtion 0.26 -0.40 -0.34 -0.32
DTS Regulation 0.02 -0.38 -0.45 -0.12
Externalizing 0.06 0.18 0.09 0.37
K10 -0.12 0.28 0.23 0.41
IPIP Extraversion 0.16 -0.22 -0.08 -0.26
IPIP Agreeableness 0.14 0.00 0.11 -0.12
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.14 -0.08 0.04 -0.48
IPIP Neuroticism -0.39 0.25 0.22 0.31
IPIP Openness 0.05 -0.13 0.02 -0.19
WHODAS
Understand/Comm -0.07 0.25 0.16 0.38
WHODAS Getting Along -0.08 0.26 0.13 0.35
WHODAS Life
Activities -0.10 0.23 0.10 0.34
WHODAS Work/School -0.11 0.27 0.16 0.34
VRIN -0.06 0.09 -0.01 0.11
129
PHLMS
Aw PHLMS Ac WBSI VLQ I
ASI Physical 0.08 0.34 0.38 -0.02
ASI Mental 0.01 0.37 0.41 0.07
ASI Social 0.22 0.38 0.29 0.02
ATQ Frequency -0.03 0.31 0.43 -0.22
COPE Active Coping 0.40 0.18 -0.01 0.28
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.16 0.36 0.22 0.30
COPE Avoidant Coping -0.01 0.36 0.38 0.03
DIS Tolerance 0.25 0.10 0.06 0.00
DIS Avoidance 0.16 0.18 0.10 0.17
DTS Tolerance 0.13 -0.19 -0.24 0.04
DTS Appraisal 0.09 -0.29 -0.38 0.13
DTS Absorbtion 0.02 -0.28 -0.41 0.17
DTS Regulation -0.10 -0.33 -0.32 -0.02
Externalizing -0.09 0.19 0.31 -0.15
K10 -0.02 0.38 0.50 -0.19
IPIP Extraversion 0.06 -0.11 -0.20 0.31
IPIP Agreeableness 0.48 0.13 -0.05 0.36
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.27 -0.10 -0.23 0.21
IPIP Neuroticism -0.11 0.29 0.45 -0.16
IPIP Openness 0.32 -0.14 -0.10 0.10
WHODAS
Understand/Comm -0.16 0.24 0.34 0.02
WHODAS Getting Along -0.10 0.27 0.34 -0.11
WHODAS Life Activities -0.16 0.19 0.27 -0.03
WHODAS Work/School -0.08 0.21 0.29 -0.03
VRIN -0.09 0.07 0.11 -0.12
131
VLQ C
ASI Physical 0.00
ASI Mental 0.09
ASI Social -0.02
ATQ Frequency -0.23
COPE Active Coping 0.24
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.17
COPE Avoidant Coping 0.10
DIS Tolerance 0.01
DIS Avoidance 0.14
DTS Tolerance 0.03
DTS Appraisal 0.11
DTS Absorbtion 0.15
DTS Regulation 0.04
Externalizing -0.13
K10 -0.20
IPIP Extraversion 0.25
IPIP Agreeableness 0.19
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.20
IPIP Neuroticism -0.20
IPIP Openness 0.12
WHODAS Understand/Comm 0.00
WHODAS Getting Along -0.13
WHODAS Life Activities -0.07
WHODAS Work/School -0.06
VRIN -0.08
132
Note. Ns range from 273 to 342; Note. Ns range from 273 to 342; AAQ = Accepance and
Action Questionnaire 2; ATQ-b = Automatic Thought Questionnaire believability; CFQ
= Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire; EQ = Experiences Questionnaire decentering scale;
FFMQ Obs = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Observe; FFMQ des = Five Factor
Mindfulness Questionnaire Describe; FFMQ Act = Five Factor Mindfulness
Questionnaire Act with Awareness; FFMQ Nonj = Five Factor Mindfulness
Questionnaire Nonjudge; FFMQ Nonr = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire
Nonreact; MEAQ BA = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Behavioral
Avoidance; MEAQ DAv = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distress
Aversion; MEAQ Pr = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Procrastination;
MEAQ D/S = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distraction/Suppression;
MEAQ R/D = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Repression/Denial;
MEAQ DE = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distress Endurance;
MAAS = Mindful Attention Awareness Scale; PHLMS Aw = Phildelphia Mindfulness
Scale Awareness; PHLMS Ac = Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale Acceptance; WBSI =
White Bear Suppression Inventory; VLQ I = Valued Living Questionnaire Imporance;
VLQ C = Valued Living Questionnaire Consistency
133
FFMQ
FFMQ Obs FFMQ Des FFMQ Act Nonj
ASI Physical 0.20 -0.06 -0.24 -0.38
ASI Mental 0.16 -0.20 -0.38 -0.51
ASI Social 0.19 -0.04 -0.23 -0.34
ATQ Frequency 0.16 -0.28 -0.44 -0.53
COPE Active Coping 0.23 0.32 0.17 0.02
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.11 0.23 -0.05 -0.15
COPE Avoidant Coping 0.10 -0.23 -0.38 -0.42
DIS Tolerance 0.24 0.04 -0.08 -0.03
DIS Avoidance 0.10 0.06 -0.16 -0.15
DTS Tolerance -0.08 0.21 0.32 0.40
DTS Appraisal -0.12 0.28 0.40 0.59
DTS Absorbtion -0.12 0.22 0.41 0.46
DTS Regulation -0.11 0.09 0.27 0.35
Externalizing 0.10 -0.12 -0.43 -0.30
K10 0.18 -0.24 -0.49 -0.53
IPIP Extraversion 0.08 0.22 0.01 0.09
IPIP Agreeableness 0.21 0.30 0.03 0.03
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.05 0.22 0.39 0.15
IPIP Neuroticism 0.11 -0.20 -0.28 -0.45
IPIP Openness 0.25 0.19 0.00 0.00
WHODAS
Understand/Comm 0.03 -0.31 -0.41 -0.32
WHODAS Getting Along 0.04 -0.25 -0.21 -0.32
WHODAS Life Activities -0.01 -0.20 -0.31 -0.23
WHODAS Work/School 0.10 -0.20 -0.36 -0.30
VRIN 0.13 -0.17 -0.26 -0.35
135
FFMQ MEAQ
Nonr MEAQ BA DAv MEAQ Pr
ASI Physical 0.02 0.33 0.37 0.22
ASI Mental -0.04 0.23 0.35 0.21
ASI Social 0.05 0.17 0.18 0.12
ATQ Frequency -0.06 0.26 0.34 0.29
COPE Active Coping 0.26 -0.14 -0.08 -0.21
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping -0.07 0.08 0.15 0.04
COPE Avoidant Coping -0.04 0.27 0.29 0.34
DIS Tolerance 0.23 -0.19 -0.11 -0.06
DIS Avoidance 0.05 0.22 0.24 0.16
DTS Tolerance 0.23 -0.33 -0.47 -0.24
DTS Appraisal 0.28 -0.35 -0.51 -0.28
DTS Absorbtion 0.27 -0.31 -0.47 -0.30
DTS Regulation 0.08 -0.32 -0.52 -0.17
Externalizing -0.06 0.17 0.17 0.33
K10 -0.11 0.25 0.37 0.29
IPIP Extraversion 0.08 -0.25 0.01 -0.19
IPIP Agreeableness 0.07 -0.08 -0.11 -0.09
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.09 -0.18 -0.14 -0.44
IPIP Neuroticism -0.35 0.23 0.36 0.22
IPIP Openness 0.13 -0.23 -0.20 -0.07
WHODAS
Understand/Comm -0.09 0.26 0.23 0.28
WHODAS Getting Along -0.07 0.17 0.12 0.19
WHODAS Life Activities -0.03 0.18 0.12 0.27
WHODAS Work/School -0.06 0.16 0.20 0.33
VRIN -0.07 0.24 0.29 0.20
136
VLQ C
ASI Physical -0.11
ASI Mental -0.17
ASI Social -0.02
ATQ Frequency -0.24
COPE Active Coping 0.25
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.06
COPE Avoidant Coping -0.07
DIS Tolerance 0.06
DIS Avoidance -0.04
DTS Tolerance 0.18
DTS Appraisal 0.17
DTS Absorbtion 0.19
DTS Regulation 0.03
Externalizing -0.17
K10 -0.25
IPIP Extraversion 0.15
IPIP Agreeableness 0.08
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.11
IPIP Neuroticism -0.19
IPIP Openness 0.02
WHODAS Understand/Comm -0.16
WHODAS Getting Along -0.19
WHODAS Life Activities -0.20
WHODAS Work/School -0.23
VRIN -0.13
139
Note. Ns range from 454 to 482; AAQ = Accepance and Action Questionnaire 2; ATQ-b
= Automatic Thought Questionnaire believability; CFQ = Cognitive Fusion
Questionnaire; EQ = Experiences Questionnaire decentering scale; FFMQ Obs = Five
Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Observe; FFMQ des = Five Factor Mindfulness
Questionnaire Describe; FFMQ Act = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Act with
Awareness; FFMQ Nonj = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Nonjudge; FFMQ
Nonr = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Nonreact; MEAQ BA =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Behavioral Avoidance; MEAQ DAv =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distress Aversion; MEAQ Pr =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Procrastination; MEAQ D/S =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distraction/Suppression; MEAQ R/D =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Repression/Denial; MEAQ DE =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distress Endurance; MAAS = Mindful
Attention Awareness Scale; PHLMS Aw = Phildelphia Mindfulness Scale Awareness;
PHLMS Ac = Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale Acceptance; WBSI = White Bear
Suppression Inventory; VLQ I = Valued Living Questionnaire Imporance; VLQ C =
Valued Living Questionnaire Consistency
140
FFMQ
FFMQ Obs FFMQ Des FFMQ Act Nonj
ASI Physical 0.12 -0.19 -0.31 -0.42
ASI Mental 0.08 -0.27 -0.37 -0.50
ASI Social 0.21 -0.10 -0.32 -0.34
ATQ Frequency 0.17 -0.35 -0.43 -0.52
COPE Active Coping 0.23 0.37 0.16 0.13
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.13 0.28 -0.06 -0.12
COPE Avoidant Coping 0.01 -0.29 -0.33 -0.37
DIS Tolerance 0.19 0.04 0.00 0.10
DIS Avoidance 0.20 0.02 -0.21 -0.22
DTS Tolerance -0.10 0.28 0.33 0.46
DTS Appraisal -0.08 0.34 0.39 0.57
DTS Absorbtion -0.16 0.24 0.41 0.52
DTS Regulation -0.15 0.18 0.31 0.39
Externalizing 0.16 -0.18 -0.42 -0.24
K10 0.12 -0.39 -0.51 -0.54
IPIP Extraversion 0.14 0.32 0.07 0.13
IPIP Agreeableness 0.14 0.28 0.10 0.07
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.03 0.31 0.39 0.24
IPIP Neuroticism 0.02 -0.22 -0.32 -0.39
IPIP Openness 0.19 0.30 0.01 0.06
WHODAS
Understand/Comm -0.01 -0.36 -0.42 -0.34
WHODAS Getting Along 0.01 -0.34 -0.30 -0.36
WHODAS Life
Activities 0.04 -0.27 -0.40 -0.34
WHODAS Work/School 0.15 -0.23 -0.41 -0.31
VRIN 0.11 -0.20 -0.23 -0.31
142
FFMQ MEAQ
Nonr MEAQ BA DAv MEAQ Pr
ASI Physical -0.05 0.40 0.37 0.34
ASI Mental -0.08 0.37 0.41 0.29
ASI Social 0.09 0.26 0.30 0.29
ATQ Frequency -0.12 0.34 0.32 0.42
COPE Active Coping 0.32 -0.18 -0.14 -0.26
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping -0.12 0.08 0.17 -0.03
COPE Avoidant Coping -0.14 0.30 0.33 0.33
DIS Tolerance 0.18 -0.23 -0.22 -0.10
DIS Avoidance 0.08 0.25 0.27 0.22
DTS Tolerance 0.22 -0.30 -0.41 -0.32
DTS Appraisal 0.28 -0.37 -0.44 -0.32
DTS Absorbtion 0.25 -0.33 -0.41 -0.34
DTS Regulation 0.10 -0.32 -0.45 -0.28
Externalizing -0.01 0.21 0.17 0.36
K10 -0.12 0.34 0.37 0.40
IPIP Extraversion 0.07 -0.25 -0.04 -0.26
IPIP Agreeableness 0.03 -0.16 -0.09 -0.15
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.10 -0.20 -0.22 -0.49
IPIP Neuroticism -0.32 0.23 0.31 0.24
IPIP Openness 0.13 -0.17 -0.19 -0.02
WHODAS
Understand/Comm -0.15 0.33 0.30 0.40
WHODAS Getting Along -0.11 0.29 0.19 0.30
WHODAS Life Activities -0.14 0.28 0.23 0.41
WHODAS Work/School -0.11 0.23 0.27 0.44
VRIN -0.10 0.18 0.14 0.18
143
PHLMS
Aw PHLMS Ac WBSI VLQ I
ASI Physical 0.00 0.38 0.36 -0.13
ASI Mental -0.07 0.40 0.36 -0.13
ASI Social 0.16 0.38 0.36 -0.10
ATQ Frequency 0.00 0.47 0.44 -0.23
COPE Active Coping 0.26 -0.14 -0.09 0.24
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.14 0.13 0.11 0.19
COPE Avoidant Coping -0.14 0.27 0.29 -0.06
DIS Tolerance 0.12 -0.07 -0.08 0.09
DIS Avoidance 0.14 0.26 0.22 0.07
DTS Tolerance 0.01 -0.40 -0.39 0.18
DTS Appraisal 0.08 -0.47 -0.40 0.19
DTS Absorbtion -0.02 -0.47 -0.45 0.15
DTS Regulation -0.02 -0.40 -0.34 0.08
Externalizing -0.03 0.22 0.26 -0.33
K10 -0.06 0.46 0.45 -0.17
IPIP Extraversion 0.12 -0.12 -0.20 0.20
IPIP Agreeableness 0.27 0.03 0.01 0.23
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.17 -0.21 -0.24 0.22
IPIP Neuroticism -0.01 0.43 0.38 -0.04
IPIP Openness 0.23 0.02 0.06 0.06
WHODAS
Understand/Comm -0.20 0.33 0.38 -0.24
WHODAS Getting Along -0.19 0.28 0.30 -0.31
WHODAS Life Activities -0.14 0.29 0.36 -0.27
WHODAS Work/School -0.02 0.32 0.39 -0.14
VRIN 0.02 0.28 0.21 -0.12
145
VLQ C
ASI Physical -0.17
ASI Mental -0.19
ASI Social -0.15
ATQ Frequency -0.24
COPE Active Coping 0.22
COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.14
COPE Avoidant Coping -0.12
DIS Tolerance 0.03
DIS Avoidance 0.05
DTS Tolerance 0.17
DTS Appraisal 0.20
DTS Absorbtion 0.17
DTS Regulation 0.12
Externalizing -0.28
K10 -0.26
IPIP Extraversion 0.20
IPIP Agreeableness 0.14
IPIP Conscientiousness 0.19
IPIP Neuroticism -0.10
IPIP Openness 0.16
WHODAS Understand/Comm -0.28
WHODAS Getting Along -0.27
WHODAS Life Activities -0.25
WHODAS Work/School -0.25
VRIN -0.12
146
Note. Ns range from 318 to 340; AAQ = Accepance and Action Questionnaire 2; ATQ-b
= Automatic Thought Questionnaire believability; CFQ = Cognitive Fusion
Questionnaire; EQ = Experiences Questionnaire decentering scale; FFMQ Obs = Five
Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Observe; FFMQ des = Five Factor Mindfulness
Questionnaire Describe; FFMQ Act = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Act with
Awareness; FFMQ Nonj = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Nonjudge; FFMQ
Nonr = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Nonreact; MEAQ BA =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Behavioral Avoidance; MEAQ DAv =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distress Aversion; MEAQ Pr =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Procrastination; MEAQ D/S =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distraction/Suppression; MEAQ R/D =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Repression/Denial; MEAQ DE =
Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Distress Endurance; MAAS = Mindful
Attention Awareness Scale; PHLMS Aw = Phildelphia Mindfulness Scale Awareness;
PHLMS Ac = Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale Acceptance; WBSI = White Bear
Suppression Inventory; VLQ I = Valued Living Questionnaire Imporance; VLQ C =
Valued Living Questionnaire Consistency
Table A13. Student T1-T2 Correlations Among ACT scales
1. T1 2. T1 3. T1 4. T1 5. T1 6. T1 7. T1 8. T1 9. T1
1. T2 AAQ 0.74 0.61 0.65 -0.41 0.09 -0.34 -0.41 -0.57 -0.12
2. T2 ATQ Believability 0.58 0.71 0.50 -0.29 0.18 -0.30 -0.38 -0.45 -0.05
3. T2 CFQ 0.67 0.59 0.75 -0.43 0.14 -0.32 -0.45 -0.63 -0.22
4. T2 EQ decentering -0.33 -0.32 -0.42 0.63 0.20 0.38 0.21 0.27 0.42
5. T2 FFMQ Observe 0.15 0.17 0.14 0.17 0.69 0.20 -0.23 -0.19 0.28
6. T2 FFMQ Describe -0.27 -0.23 -0.24 0.29 0.19 0.71 0.21 0.19 0.26
7. T2 FFMQ Act with Awareness -0.43 -0.39 -0.43 0.21 -0.16 0.24 0.74 0.38 0.03
8. T2 FFMQ Nonjudge -0.49 -0.47 -0.57 0.24 -0.21 0.18 0.39 0.76 0.03
9. T2 FFMQ Nonreact -0.12 -0.10 -0.29 0.39 0.27 0.20 0.00 0.09 0.67
10. T2 MEAQ Behavior Avoidance 0.30 0.22 0.28 -0.15 0.08 -0.24 -0.32 -0.33 -0.15
11. T2 MEAQ Distress Aversion 0.36 0.31 0.35 -0.15 0.06 -0.17 -0.29 -0.33 -0.15
12. T2 MEAQ Procrastination 0.41 0.33 0.33 -0.18 0.11 -0.28 -0.51 -0.30 -0.05
13. T2 MEAQ Distraction/Suppression 0.28 0.20 0.32 -0.12 0.13 -0.09 -0.27 -0.34 -0.08
14. T2 MEAQ Repression/Denial 0.36 0.34 0.31 -0.17 -0.07 -0.48 -0.35 -0.33 -0.10
15. T2 MEAQ Distress Endurance -0.11 -0.13 -0.13 0.14 0.21 0.27 0.14 0.06 0.19
16. T2 MAAS -0.41 -0.34 -0.44 0.26 -0.07 0.32 0.59 0.40 0.03
17. T2 PHLMS Awareness 0.05 0.09 0.04 0.25 0.58 0.30 -0.09 -0.09 0.28
18. T2 PHLMS Acceptance 0.50 0.43 0.56 -0.16 0.26 -0.20 -0.42 -0.57 -0.06
19. T2 WBSI 0.52 0.42 0.58 -0.32 0.18 -0.21 -0.41 -0.54 -0.10
20. T2 VLQ Importance -0.17 -0.13 -0.09 0.19 0.02 0.14 0.11 0.13 -0.06
21. T2 VLQ Consistence -0.24 -0.16 -0.14 0.20 0.03 0.20 0.20 0.13 -0.01
147
Table A13. Continued
148
149
Note. Ns range from 337 to 340. Test-retest reliability coefficients are bolded.
AAQ = Accepance and Action Questionnaire 2; ATQ-b = Automatic Thought
Questionnaire believability; CFQ = Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire; EQ =
Experiences Questionnaire decentering scale; FFMQ Obs = Five Factor
Mindfulness Questionnaire Observe; FFMQ des = Five Factor Mindfulness
Questionnaire Describe; FFMQ Act = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire
Act with Awareness; FFMQ Nonj = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire
Nonjudge; FFMQ Nonr = Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire Nonreact;
MEAQ BA = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale Behavioral
Avoidance; MEAQ DAv = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale
Distress Aversion; MEAQ Pr = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale
Procrastination; MEAQ D/S = Multidimensional Experiential Avoidance Scale
Distraction/Suppression; MEAQ R/D = Multidimensional Experiential
Avoidance Scale Repression/Denial; MEAQ DE = Multidimensional
Experiential Avoidance Scale Distress Endurance; MAAS = Mindful Attention
Awareness Scale; PHLMS Aw = Phildelphia Mindfulness Scale Awareness;
PHLMS Ac = Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale Acceptance; WBSI = White Bear
Suppression Inventory; VLQ I = Valued Living Questionnaire Imporance; VLQ
C = Valued Living Questionnaire Consistency
151
1. T1 2. T1 3. T1 4. T1 5. T1
1. T2 ASI Physical 0.70 0.52 0.27 0.38 0.00
2. T2 ASI Mental 0.47 0.70 0.28 0.50 0.01
3. T2 ASI Social 0.36 0.43 0.49 0.41 0.01
4. T2 ATQ Frequency 0.33 0.50 0.20 0.76 -0.12
5. T2 COPE Active Coping -0.01 -0.08 0.05 -0.16 0.56
6. T2 COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.22 0.07 0.04 0.06 0.26
7. T2 COPE Avoidant Coping 0.20 0.37 0.08 0.44 0.05
8. T2 DIS Tolerance -0.10 -0.05 0.03 -0.01 0.11
9. T2 DIS Avoidance 0.32 0.20 0.09 0.11 0.12
10. T2 DTS Tolerance -0.33 -0.34 -0.15 -0.39 0.06
11. T2 DTS Appraisal -0.35 -0.45 -0.17 -0.44 0.09
12. T2 DTS Absorbtion -0.32 -0.37 -0.18 -0.44 0.05
13. T2 DTS Regulation -0.28 -0.32 -0.23 -0.29 -0.04
14. T2 Externalizing 0.12 0.24 0.11 0.41 -0.15
15. T2 K10 0.38 0.45 0.20 0.63 -0.16
16. T2 IPIP Extraversion -0.13 -0.09 -0.03 -0.21 0.16
17. T2 IPIP Agreeableness -0.01 -0.15 0.01 -0.15 0.21
18. T2 IPIP Conscientiousness -0.15 -0.22 0.03 -0.27 0.20
19. T2 IPIP Neuroticism 0.27 0.30 0.07 0.35 -0.18
20. T2 IPIP Openness -0.07 -0.05 0.01 -0.02 0.13
21. T2 WHODAS
Understand/Comm 0.25 0.44 0.15 0.47 -0.17
22. T2 WHODAS Getting Along 0.21 0.41 0.11 0.49 -0.12
23. T2 WHODAS Life Activities 0.28 0.45 0.14 0.50 -0.15
24. T2 WHODAS Work/School 0.17 0.33 0.15 0.46 -0.12
25. T2 VRIN 0.17 0.21 0.17 0.22 -0.08
152
6. T1 7. T1 8. T1 9. T1 10. T1
1. T2 ASI Physical 0.16 0.40 -0.03 0.30 -0.29
2. T2 ASI Mental 0.10 0.45 0.01 0.24 -0.30
3. T2 ASI Social 0.00 0.33 0.12 0.18 -0.24
4. T2 ATQ Frequency 0.04 0.51 0.10 0.17 -0.32
5. T2 COPE Active Coping 0.22 -0.10 0.10 0.01 0.11
6. T2 COPE Emotion-Focused
Coping 0.69 0.15 -0.22 0.15 -0.18
7. T2 COPE Avoidant Coping 0.11 0.59 0.02 0.11 -0.17
8. T2 DIS Tolerance -0.25 -0.16 0.76 -0.30 0.13
9. T2 DIS Avoidance 0.14 0.10 -0.27 0.54 -0.28
10. T2 DTS Tolerance -0.17 -0.34 0.06 -0.26 0.54
11. T2 DTS Appraisal -0.21 -0.38 0.09 -0.22 0.45
12. T2 DTS Absorbtion -0.24 -0.36 0.03 -0.20 0.50
13. T2 DTS Regulation -0.19 -0.29 0.02 -0.31 0.43
14. T2 Externalizing 0.00 0.29 0.06 0.12 -0.25
15. T2 K10 0.07 0.45 -0.01 0.25 -0.38
16. T2 IPIP Extraversion 0.18 -0.05 -0.02 0.02 0.03
17. T2 IPIP Agreeableness 0.28 -0.21 -0.06 -0.07 0.02
18. T2 IPIP Conscientiousness 0.03 -0.33 0.00 -0.14 0.10
19. T2 IPIP Neuroticism 0.23 0.17 -0.21 0.09 -0.36
20. T2 IPIP Openness 0.01 -0.08 0.07 -0.05 0.08
21. T2 WHODAS
Understand/Comm 0.00 0.42 -0.12 0.20 -0.26
22. T2 WHODAS Getting Along -0.02 0.40 -0.05 0.14 -0.19
23. T2 WHODAS Life Activities 0.01 0.44 -0.10 0.20 -0.28
24. T2 WHODAS Work/School 0.02 0.40 -0.07 0.18 -0.20
25. T2 VRIN -0.02 0.23 0.10 0.14 -0.17
153
Mechanical Turk
95% Upper
Factor Mean Random Bound
Raw Eigenvalue
number Eigenvalue Random
Eigenvalue
Student Time 1
95% Upper
Raw Mean Random
Factor number Bound Random
Eigenvalue Eigenvalue
Eigenvalue
Student Time 2
95% Upper
Factor Raw Mean Random Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random
Eigenvalue
159
Table A16 Continued
Note. Correlations >= 0.35 are highlighted. Scales used to define factor are underlined.
160
161
Mechanical Turk
Factor Factor Factor Factor
1 2 3 4
AAQ 0.83 0.06 0.09 0.02
ATQ-Believability 0.87 -0.02 0.15 -0.08
CFQ 0.78 0.34 0.22 0.11
EQ decentering -0.14 -0.69 0.18 -0.05
FFMQ Observe 0.29 -0.22 0.70 0.02
FFMQ Describe -0.25 -0.05 0.48 -0.09
FFMQ Act with Awareness -0.82 0.16 0.14 0.06
FFMQ Nonjudge -0.62 0.08 -0.17 -0.28
FFMQ Nonreact 0.16 -0.84 0.07 -0.13
MEAQ Behavioral
Avoidance 0.06 0.09 -0.06 0.77
MEAQ Distress Aversion -0.02 0.17 0.06 0.81
MEAQ Procrastination 0.37 0.08 -0.26 0.39
MEAQ
Distraction/Suppression -0.25 -0.19 0.00 0.84
MEAQ Repression/Denial 0.49 -0.31 -0.44 0.21
MEAQ Distress Endurance -0.14 -0.51 0.25 0.11
MAAS -0.58 -0.08 0.31 0.12
PHLMS Awareness 0.10 -0.18 0.77 0.17
PHLMS Acceptance 0.26 -0.07 0.11 0.68
WBSI 0.42 0.12 0.08 0.59
VLQ Importance -0.27 -0.13 0.04 0.30
VLQ Consistence -0.24 -0.17 0.02 0.16
163
Student Time 1
Factor Factor Factor Factor
1 2 3 4
AAQ 0.74 0.00 0.12 -0.01
ATQ-Believability 0.68 -0.07 0.14 -0.11
CFQ 0.90 0.07 -0.06 0.02
EQ decentering -0.55 -0.43 0.07 0.06
FFMQ Observe 0.24 -0.74 0.14 -0.08
FFMQ Describe -0.09 -0.41 -0.31 -0.06
FFMQ Act with Awareness -0.10 0.23 -0.75 -0.04
FFMQ Nonjudge -0.61 0.13 -0.13 -0.17
FFMQ Nonreact -0.32 -0.57 0.33 -0.11
MEAQ Behavioral
Avoidance -0.02 0.14 0.30 0.47
MEAQ Distress Aversion 0.22 0.08 0.08 0.58
MEAQ Procrastination -0.05 0.02 0.64 0.12
MEAQ
Distraction/Suppression -0.05 0.02 0.00 0.87
MEAQ Repression/Denial -0.04 0.24 0.56 0.25
MEAQ Distress Endurance -0.05 -0.48 -0.23 0.14
MAAS -0.17 0.09 -0.64 0.04
PHLMS Awareness 0.22 -0.70 -0.08 0.03
PHLMS Acceptance 0.52 -0.12 0.04 0.49
WBSI 0.57 -0.17 0.13 0.25
VLQ Importance -0.04 -0.13 -0.31 0.30
VLQ Consistence -0.22 -0.13 -0.22 0.22
164
Student Time 2
Factor Factor Factor Factor
1 2 3 4
AAQ 0.57 0.02 0.27 0.19
ATQ-Believability 0.56 -0.04 0.36 -0.04
CFQ 0.79 0.07 0.18 0.09
EQ decentering -0.42 -0.54 -0.07 -0.02
FFMQ Observe 0.21 -0.82 0.15 -0.04
FFMQ Describe -0.03 -0.46 -0.38 -0.18
FFMQ Act with Awareness -0.14 0.22 -0.56 -0.25
FFMQ Nonjudge -0.54 0.14 -0.16 -0.26
FFMQ Nonreact -0.39 -0.63 0.24 0.06
MEAQ Behavioral
Avoidance -0.02 0.07 0.12 0.73
MEAQ Distress Aversion 0.08 0.10 -0.07 0.76
MEAQ Procrastination -0.04 -0.01 0.44 0.53
MEAQ
Distraction/Suppression 0.08 -0.02 -0.24 0.84
MEAQ Repression/Denial -0.05 0.24 0.46 0.44
MEAQ Distress Endurance -0.09 -0.41 -0.33 0.13
MAAS -0.13 0.00 -0.61 -0.13
PHLMS Awareness 0.21 -0.78 -0.17 0.01
PHLMS Acceptance 0.45 -0.16 0.00 0.51
WBSI 0.43 -0.08 0.06 0.45
VLQ Importance -0.07 0.01 -0.51 0.28
VLQ Consistence -0.08 0.05 -0.51 0.17
Note. Correlations >= 0.35 are highlighted.
165
Mechanical Turk
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 27.94 2.20 2.30
2 5.48 2.11 2.18
3 3.77 2.04 2.10
4 2.76 1.98 2.03
5 2.22 1.93 1.98
6 1.80 1.88 1.93
7 1.55 1.83 1.87
8 1.47 1.79 1.83
9 1.25 1.75 1.79
10 1.17 1.71 1.75
11 1.12 1.67 1.71
12 1.06 1.64 1.68
13 1.01 1.61 1.64
14 0.97 1.57 1.61
15 0.93 1.54 1.57
167
Student Time 1
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 26.25 1.93 2.00
2 6.46 1.86 1.91
3 4.01 1.81 1.85
4 2.92 1.77 1.81
5 2.16 1.73 1.76
6 1.86 1.69 1.72
7 1.49 1.66 1.69
8 1.41 1.62 1.66
9 1.29 1.60 1.62
10 1.18 1.57 1.60
11 1.09 1.54 1.57
12 1.01 1.51 1.54
13 0.98 1.48 1.51
14 0.93 1.46 1.48
15 0.91 1.44 1.46
168
Table A21. Correlations Between Subfactor Loadings for 1st Scale-Level EFA factor
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
My painful experiences and memories make it difficult for me to live a
life that I would value. AAQ 0.41 0.33 0.11
I’m afraid of my feelings. AAQ 0.41 0.28 0.11
I worry about not being able to control my worries and feelings. AAQ 0.60 0.15 0.08
My painful memories prevent me from having a fulfilling life. AAQ 0.43 0.26 0.14
Emotions cause problems in my life. AAQ 0.52 0.19 0.02
It seems like most people are handling their lives better than I am. AAQ 0.37 0.28 0.11
Worries get in the way of my success. AAQ 0.49 0.15 0.10
I feel like I’m up against the world. ATQ-b 0.17 0.39 0.02
I’m no good. ATQ-b 0.02 0.72 -0.05
Why can’t I ever succeed? ATQ-b 0.12 0.61 0.02
No one understands me. ATQ-b 0.07 0.59 0.05
I’ve let people down. ATQ-b 0.06 0.62 0.01
I don’t think I can go on. ATQ-b 0.05 0.62 0.05
I wish I were a better person. ATQ-b 0.16 0.47 -0.05
I’m so weak. ATQ-b 0.03 0.70 0.02
My life’s not going the way I want it to. ATQ-b 0.21 0.38 0.06
I’m so disappointed in myself. ATQ-b 0.15 0.64 0.08
Nothing feels good anymore. ATQ-b 0.06 0.72 0.05
I can’t stand this anymore. ATQ-b 0.04 0.70 0.06
I can’t get started. ATQ-b 0.10 0.62 0.06
170
Table A22. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
What’s wrong with me? ATQ-b 0.11 0.68 -0.04
I wish I were somewhere else. ATQ-b 0.12 0.55 -0.05
I can’t get things together. ATQ-b 0.14 0.69 0.02
I hate myself. ATQ-b -0.05 0.84 -0.03
I’m worthless. ATQ-b -0.03 0.89 -0.04
Wish I could just disappear. ATQ-b 0.02 0.75 -0.06
What’s the matter with me? ATQ-b 0.08 0.71 -0.03
I’m a loser. ATQ-b 0.01 0.78 0.03
My life is a mess. ATQ-b 0.07 0.63 0.09
I’m a failure. ATQ-b 0.05 0.79 0.00
I’ll never make it. ATQ-b -0.02 0.81 0.06
I feel so hopeless. ATQ-b 0.05 0.76 0.01
Something has to change. ATQ-b 0.41 0.38 -0.16
There must be something wrong with me. ATQ-b 0.11 0.75 -0.04
My future is bleak. ATQ-b -0.04 0.78 0.07
It’s just not worth it. ATQ-b -0.05 0.80 -0.05
I can’t finish anything. ATQ-b 0.07 0.67 0.01
My thoughts cause me distress or emotional pain CFQ 0.56 0.26 0.02
I get so caught up in my thoughts that I am unable to do the
things that I most want to do CFQ 0.65 0.16 0.07
Even when I am having distressing thoughts, I know that
they may become less important eventually CFQ 0.41 -0.23 -0.29
171
Table A22. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I over-analyse situations to the point where it’s unhelpful to me CFQ 0.62 0.06 0.08
I struggle with my thoughts CFQ 0.67 0.06 0.15
Even when I’m having upsetting thoughts, I can see that those thoughts may
not be literally true CFQ 0.42 -0.19 -0.20
I get upset with myself for having certain thoughts CFQ 0.63 0.06 0.02
I need to control the thoughts that come into my head CFQ 0.69 -0.03 -0.11
I find it easy to view my thoughts from a different perspective CFQ 0.13 0.06 -0.33
I tend to get very entangled in my thoughts CFQ 0.70 0.00 0.09
I tend to react very strongly to my thoughts CFQ 0.50 0.16 -0.08
Its possible for me to have negative thoughts about myself and still know
that I am an OK person CFQ 0.43 -0.24 -0.02
It’s such a struggle to let go of upsetting thoughts even when I know that
letting go would be helpful CFQ 0.67 0.12 0.04
I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until some
time later. MAAS 0.14 -0.15 -0.52
I break or spill things because of carelessness, not paying attention, or
thinking of something else. MAAS 0.18 -0.14 -0.64
I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present. MAAS -0.12 -0.06 -0.62
I tend to walk quickly to get where I’m going without paying attention to
what I experience along the way. MAAS -0.13 0.13 -0.58
I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they
really grab my attention. MAAS 0.18 -0.02 -0.50
I forget a person’s name almost as soon as I’ve been told it for the first time. MAAS -0.04 0.13 -0.45
It seems I am “running on automatic” without much awareness of what I’m
doing. MAAS -0.10 0.04 -0.74
172
Table A22. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I rush through activities without being really attentive to them. MAAS -0.14 0.06 -0.76
I get so focused on the goal I want to achieve that I lose touch
with what I am doing right now to get there. MAAS -0.04 -0.03 -0.56
I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m
doing. MAAS -0.08 -0.01 -0.76
I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something
else at the same time. MAAS -0.22 0.16 -0.49
I drive places on “automatic pilot” and then wonder why I went
there. MAAS 0.18 -0.20 -0.65
I find myself preoccupied with the future or the past. MAAS -0.32 0.04 -0.33
I find myself doing things without paying attention. MAAS -0.12 0.14 -0.76
I snack without being aware that I’m eating. MAAS 0.13 -0.24 -0.63
When I do things, my mind wanders off and I’m easily distracted. FFMQ AwA -0.49 0.01 -0.35
I don’t pay attention to what I’m doing because I’m daydreaming,
worrying, or otherwise distracted FFMQ AwA -0.39 0.02 -0.38
I am easily distracted. FFMQ AwA -0.39 -0.03 -0.38
I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the
present. FFMQ AwA -0.41 -0.07 -0.34
It seems I am “running on automatic” without much awareness of
what I’m doing. FFMQ AwA -0.35 -0.02 -0.39
I rush through activities without being really attentive to them. FFMQ AwA -0.34 0.02 -0.37
I do jobs or tasks automatically without being aware of what I’m
doing. FFMQ AwA -0.21 -0.08 -0.39
I find myself doing things without paying attention. FFMQ AwA -0.33 -0.03 -0.45
173
Table A22. Continued.
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I criticize myself for having irrational or inappropriate emotions. FFMQ NJ -0.48 -0.06 0.00
I tell myself I shouldn’t be feeling the way I’m feeling. FFMQ NJ -0.40 -0.19 -0.01
I believe some of my thoughts are abnormal or bad and I shouldn’t think that
way. FFMQ NJ -0.45 -0.11 -0.07
I make judgments about whether my thoughts are good or bad. FFMQ NJ -0.34 -0.12 0.25
I tell myself that I shouldn’t be thinking the way I’m thinking. FFMQ NJ -0.51 -0.05 -0.03
I think some of my emotions are bad or inappropriate and I shouldn’t feel
them. FFMQ NJ -0.44 -0.17 0.01
When I have distressing thoughts or images, I judge myself as good or bad,
depending on what the thought/image is about FFMQ NJ -0.36 -0.16 0.17
I disapprove of myself when I have irrational ideas. FFMQ NJ -0.30 -0.18 0.01
Note. Underlined loadings indicate that the item is used to calculate the subfactor subscore.
174
Table A23. Student Time 1 Subfactors of ACT Factor 1 (Fusion/Inflexibility), Using Promax Rotation
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
My painful experiences and memories make it difficult for me to live a
life that I would value. AAQ 0.34 0.44 0.01
I’m afraid of my feelings. AAQ 0.60 0.19 -0.01
I worry about not being able to control my worries and feelings. AAQ 0.64 0.14 0.01
My painful memories prevent me from having a fulfilling life. AAQ 0.33 0.40 0.00
Emotions cause problems in my life. AAQ 0.56 0.25 -0.05
It seems like most people are handling their lives better than I am. AAQ 0.41 0.35 0.07
Worries get in the way of my success. AAQ 0.44 0.27 0.03
I feel like I’m up against the world. ATQ-b 0.20 0.50 0.02
I’m no good. ATQ-b 0.17 0.67 -0.05
Why can’t I ever succeed? ATQ-b 0.15 0.66 0.02
No one understands me. ATQ-b 0.33 0.43 0.01
I’ve let people down. ATQ-b 0.20 0.51 0.01
I don’t think I can go on. ATQ-b -0.13 0.79 0.03
I wish I were a better person. ATQ-b 0.16 0.57 0.04
I’m so weak. ATQ-b -0.01 0.66 0.07
My life’s not going the way I want it to. ATQ-b 0.06 0.63 0.02
I’m so disappointed in myself. ATQ-b 0.06 0.69 0.04
Nothing feels good anymore. ATQ-b 0.02 0.72 0.02
I can’t stand this anymore. ATQ-b 0.08 0.62 -0.01
I can’t get started. ATQ-b -0.09 0.63 0.15
175
Table A23. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
What’s wrong with me? ATQ-b 0.31 0.54 -0.06
I wish I were somewhere else. ATQ-b 0.19 0.46 -0.02
I can’t get things together. ATQ-b 0.08 0.65 0.07
I hate myself. ATQ-b -0.01 0.83 -0.06
I’m worthless. ATQ-b -0.08 0.86 -0.05
Wish I could just disappear. ATQ-b 0.02 0.75 -0.07
What’s the matter with me? ATQ-b 0.27 0.62 -0.10
I’m a loser. ATQ-b -0.09 0.72 0.01
My life is a mess. ATQ-b 0.05 0.65 0.03
I’m a failure. ATQ-b -0.10 0.86 -0.03
I’ll never make it. ATQ-b -0.20 0.82 0.07
I feel so hopeless. ATQ-b -0.04 0.79 0.04
Something has to change. ATQ-b 0.30 0.39 0.01
There must be something wrong with me. ATQ-b 0.22 0.67 -0.13
My future is bleak. ATQ-b -0.14 0.74 0.11
It’s just not worth it. ATQ-b -0.08 0.71 0.04
I can’t finish anything. ATQ-b -0.08 0.68 0.09
My thoughts cause me distress or emotional pain CFQ 0.60 0.25 -0.02
I get so caught up in my thoughts that I am unable to do the
things that I most want to do CFQ 0.41 0.32 0.15
Even when I am having distressing thoughts, I know that
they may become less important eventually CFQ 0.28 -0.11 -0.11
176
Table A23. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I over-analyse situations to the point where it’s unhelpful to me CFQ 0.60 0.02 0.05
I struggle with my thoughts CFQ 0.73 0.14 -0.04
Even when I’m having upsetting thoughts, I can see that those thoughts may not
be literally true CFQ 0.21 -0.19 0.02
I get upset with myself for having certain thoughts CFQ 0.77 0.01 -0.01
I need to control the thoughts that come into my head CFQ 0.75 -0.01 -0.03
I find it easy to view my thoughts from a different perspective CFQ 0.07 -0.06 -0.02
I tend to get very entangled in my thoughts CFQ 0.61 0.06 0.06
I tend to react very strongly to my thoughts CFQ 0.56 0.08 -0.02
Its possible for me to have negative thoughts about myself and still know that I
am an OK person CFQ 0.20 -0.12 -0.07
It’s such a struggle to let go of upsetting thoughts even when I know that letting
go would be helpful CFQ 0.64 -0.03 0.04
I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until some
time later. MAAS -0.05 0.01 -0.38
I break or spill things because of carelessness, not paying attention, or thinking
of something else. MAAS 0.02 0.02 -0.47
I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present. MAAS -0.12 -0.01 -0.58
I tend to walk quickly to get where I’m going without paying attention to what I
experience along the way. MAAS -0.04 0.04 -0.49
I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they really
grab my attention. MAAS 0.05 0.11 -0.52
I forget a person’s name almost as soon as I’ve been told it for the first time. MAAS 0.00 0.06 -0.34
It seems I am “running on automatic” without much awareness of what I’m
doing. MAAS 0.05 0.01 -0.73
177
Table A23. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I rush through activities without being really attentive to them. MAAS 0.12 -0.01 -0.80
I get so focused on the goal I want to achieve that I lose touch
with what I am doing right now to get there. MAAS 0.02 -0.04 -0.66
I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m
doing. MAAS 0.14 -0.05 -0.79
I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something
else at the same time. MAAS -0.19 0.08 -0.47
I drive places on “automatic pilot” and then wonder why I went
there. MAAS 0.09 -0.06 -0.58
I find myself preoccupied with the future or the past. MAAS -0.42 0.10 -0.36
I find myself doing things without paying attention. MAAS 0.00 0.08 -0.76
I snack without being aware that I’m eating. MAAS -0.06 0.04 -0.46
When I do things, my mind wanders off and I’m easily distracted. FFMQ AwA -0.09 0.01 -0.49
I don’t pay attention to what I’m doing because I’m daydreaming,
worrying, or otherwise distracted FFMQ AwA -0.02 -0.06 -0.55
I am easily distracted. FFMQ AwA -0.02 -0.01 -0.47
I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the
present. FFMQ AwA -0.14 -0.06 -0.54
It seems I am “running on automatic” without much awareness of
what I’m doing. FFMQ AwA -0.04 -0.14 -0.60
I rush through activities without being really attentive to them. FFMQ AwA -0.08 -0.04 -0.59
I do jobs or tasks automatically without being aware of what I’m
doing. FFMQ AwA 0.02 -0.02 -0.64
I find myself doing things without paying attention. FFMQ AwA -0.06 0.06 -0.69
178
Table A23. Continued.
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I criticize myself for having irrational or inappropriate emotions. FFMQ NJ -0.59 -0.08 -0.02
I tell myself I shouldn’t be feeling the way I’m feeling. FFMQ NJ -0.64 -0.02 -0.01
I believe some of my thoughts are abnormal or bad and I shouldn’t think that
way. FFMQ NJ -0.62 -0.06 -0.07
I make judgments about whether my thoughts are good or bad. FFMQ NJ -0.59 0.07 -0.04
I tell myself that I shouldn’t be thinking the way I’m thinking. FFMQ NJ -0.68 0.02 -0.03
I think some of my emotions are bad or inappropriate and I shouldn’t feel
them. FFMQ NJ -0.63 -0.03 -0.09
When I have distressing thoughts or images, I judge myself as good or bad,
depending on what the thought/image is about FFMQ NJ -0.54 0.01 -0.13
I disapprove of myself when I have irrational ideas. FFMQ NJ -0.50 -0.04 -0.12
Note. Underlined loadings indicate that the item is used to calculate the subfactor subscore
179
Table A24. Student Time 2 Subfactors of ACT Factor 1 (Fusion/Inflexibility), Using Promax Rotation
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
My painful experiences and memories make it difficult for me to live a
life that I would value. AAQ 0.36 0.34 0.10
I’m afraid of my feelings. AAQ 0.44 0.23 0.16
I worry about not being able to control my worries and feelings. AAQ 0.62 0.17 0.03
My painful memories prevent me from having a fulfilling life. AAQ 0.36 0.40 0.06
Emotions cause problems in my life. AAQ 0.58 0.17 0.03
It seems like most people are handling their lives better than I am. AAQ 0.50 0.32 0.04
Worries get in the way of my success. AAQ 0.52 0.19 0.07
I feel like I’m up against the world. ATQ-b 0.06 0.62 0.05
I’m no good. ATQ-b 0.13 0.68 0.02
Why can’t I ever succeed? ATQ-b 0.21 0.58 0.03
No one understands me. ATQ-b 0.21 0.57 0.03
I’ve let people down. ATQ-b 0.22 0.59 -0.07
I don’t think I can go on. ATQ-b -0.23 0.89 0.01
I wish I were a better person. ATQ-b 0.17 0.60 0.00
I’m so weak. ATQ-b -0.01 0.76 0.03
My life’s not going the way I want it to. ATQ-b 0.11 0.64 0.02
I’m so disappointed in myself. ATQ-b 0.06 0.68 0.07
Nothing feels good anymore. ATQ-b -0.07 0.90 -0.05
I can’t stand this anymore. ATQ-b 0.06 0.77 0.00
I can’t get started. ATQ-b 0.05 0.71 0.06
180
Table A24. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
What’s wrong with me? ATQ-b 0.25 0.69 -0.04
I wish I were somewhere else. ATQ-b 0.17 0.49 0.09
I can’t get things together. ATQ-b 0.12 0.72 0.01
I hate myself. ATQ-b -0.07 0.89 -0.04
I’m worthless. ATQ-b -0.03 0.86 -0.04
Wish I could just disappear. ATQ-b -0.11 0.89 0.01
What’s the matter with me? ATQ-b 0.23 0.77 -0.11
I’m a loser. ATQ-b 0.01 0.77 -0.05
My life is a mess. ATQ-b 0.06 0.72 0.01
I’m a failure. ATQ-b 0.00 0.83 -0.03
I’ll never make it. ATQ-b 0.02 0.78 -0.01
I feel so hopeless. ATQ-b -0.01 0.86 -0.07
Something has to change. ATQ-b 0.38 0.47 -0.07
There must be something wrong with me. ATQ-b 0.15 0.78 -0.10
My future is bleak. ATQ-b -0.06 0.82 0.03
It’s just not worth it. ATQ-b -0.01 0.79 0.01
I can’t finish anything. ATQ-b -0.01 0.72 0.04
My thoughts cause me distress or emotional pain CFQ 0.73 0.21 -0.03
I get so caught up in my thoughts that I am unable to do the
things that I most want to do CFQ 0.60 0.26 0.06
Even when I am having distressing thoughts, I know that
they may become less important eventually CFQ 0.46 -0.23 -0.22
181
Table A24. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I over-analyse situations to the point where it’s unhelpful to me CFQ 0.77 -0.09 0.08
I struggle with my thoughts CFQ 0.71 0.18 0.02
Even when I’m having upsetting thoughts, I can see that those thoughts may
not be literally true CFQ 0.44 -0.22 -0.21
I get upset with myself for having certain thoughts CFQ 0.73 0.11 -0.01
I need to control the thoughts that come into my head CFQ 0.71 0.09 -0.03
I find it easy to view my thoughts from a different perspective CFQ 0.32 -0.09 -0.20
I tend to get very entangled in my thoughts CFQ 0.71 0.07 0.05
I tend to react very strongly to my thoughts CFQ 0.58 0.11 0.02
Its possible for me to have negative thoughts about myself and still know
that I am an OK person CFQ 0.38 -0.31 -0.20
It’s such a struggle to let go of upsetting thoughts even when I know that
letting go would be helpful CFQ 0.68 0.06 0.02
I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until some
time later. MAAS 0.07 -0.19 -0.38
I break or spill things because of carelessness, not paying attention, or
thinking of something else. MAAS 0.20 -0.15 -0.51
I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the present. MAAS 0.02 -0.12 -0.58
I tend to walk quickly to get where I’m going without paying attention to
what I experience along the way. MAAS -0.08 0.05 -0.61
I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they
really grab my attention. MAAS 0.04 -0.02 -0.46
I forget a person’s name almost as soon as I’ve been told it for the first time. MAAS 0.02 -0.06 -0.37
It seems I am “running on automatic” without much awareness of what I’m
doing. MAAS 0.07 0.01 -0.81
182
Table A24. Continued
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I rush through activities without being really attentive to them. MAAS 0.02 0.07 -0.85
I get so focused on the goal I want to achieve that I lose touch
with what I am doing right now to get there. MAAS -0.04 0.07 -0.72
I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m
doing. MAAS 0.11 -0.01 -0.86
I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something
else at the same time. MAAS -0.13 0.12 -0.54
I drive places on “automatic pilot” and then wonder why I went
there. MAAS 0.13 -0.07 -0.65
I find myself preoccupied with the future or the past. MAAS -0.28 -0.02 -0.38
I find myself doing things without paying attention. MAAS -0.03 0.11 -0.84
I snack without being aware that I’m eating. MAAS -0.03 0.06 -0.54
When I do things, my mind wanders off and I’m easily distracted. FFMQ AwA -0.23 0.04 -0.43
I don’t pay attention to what I’m doing because I’m daydreaming,
worrying, or otherwise distracted FFMQ AwA -0.14 -0.08 -0.51
I am easily distracted. FFMQ AwA -0.09 0.06 -0.52
I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s happening in the
present. FFMQ AwA -0.15 -0.09 -0.47
It seems I am “running on automatic” without much awareness of
what I’m doing. FFMQ AwA -0.07 0.02 -0.67
I rush through activities without being really attentive to them. FFMQ AwA -0.20 0.08 -0.57
I do jobs or tasks automatically without being aware of what I’m
doing. FFMQ AwA -0.08 0.14 -0.68
I find myself doing things without paying attention. FFMQ AwA -0.12 0.10 -0.66
183
Table A24. Continued.
Int
Item Scale Inflexibility Belief Detachment
I criticize myself for having irrational or inappropriate emotions. FFMQ NJ -0.59 -0.09 -0.08
I tell myself I shouldn’t be feeling the way I’m feeling. FFMQ NJ -0.61 0.02 -0.10
I believe some of my thoughts are abnormal or bad and I shouldn’t think that
way. FFMQ NJ -0.63 -0.02 -0.09
I make judgments about whether my thoughts are good or bad. FFMQ NJ -0.59 0.13 0.01
I tell myself that I shouldn’t be thinking the way I’m thinking. FFMQ NJ -0.63 0.08 -0.11
I think some of my emotions are bad or inappropriate and I shouldn’t feel
them. FFMQ NJ -0.58 -0.01 -0.14
When I have distressing thoughts or images, I judge myself as good or bad,
depending on what the thought/image is about FFMQ NJ -0.51 -0.01 -0.17
I disapprove of myself when I have irrational ideas. FFMQ NJ -0.52 0.00 -0.13
Note. Underlined loadings indicate that the item is used to calculate the subfactor subscore
184
185
Mechanical Turk
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 12.88 1.94 2.03
2 4.35 1.85 1.92
3 3.65 1.78 1.84
4 2.59 1.72 1.77
5 2.04 1.67 1.72
6 1.62 1.62 1.66
7 1.40 1.58 1.63
8 1.23 1.54 1.58
9 1.19 1.50 1.54
10 1.09 1.46 1.50
11 1.06 1.43 1.46
12 0.98 1.39 1.42
13 0.96 1.36 1.39
14 0.92 1.32 1.36
15 0.91 1.29 1.32
186
Student Time 1
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 9.69 1.73 1.80
2 4.71 1.66 1.72
3 3.08 1.61 1.66
4 3.05 1.57 1.61
5 2.07 1.53 1.56
6 1.60 1.49 1.53
7 1.49 1.46 1.49
8 1.36 1.43 1.46
9 1.16 1.39 1.43
10 1.09 1.37 1.40
11 1.07 1.34 1.36
12 1.01 1.31 1.34
13 0.96 1.29 1.31
14 0.94 1.26 1.29
15 0.91 1.24 1.26
187
Student Time 2
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 13.53 1.89 1.98
2 5.11 1.80 1.86
3 3.45 1.74 1.79
4 3.04 1.68 1.73
5 1.95 1.64 1.68
6 1.58 1.59 1.64
7 1.40 1.55 1.59
8 1.21 1.51 1.55
9 1.16 1.47 1.51
10 1.01 1.44 1.47
11 0.97 1.40 1.43
12 0.93 1.37 1.40
13 0.88 1.34 1.37
14 0.86 1.31 1.34
15 0.81 1.28 1.31
Note. 500 simulations using a significance level of 95%. Non-normal
random datasets used (based on raw data). Principal components
analysis used.
188
Table A26. Correlations Between Subfactor Loadings for 2nd Scale-Level EFA Factor
189
Table A27. Continued
190
Table A27. Continued
191
I want MEAQ DE 0.02 0.07 0.73 -0.04
Table A27. Continued
192
Table A28. Student Time 1 Subfactors of ACT Factor 2 (Awareness), Using Promax Rotation
193
Table A28. Continued
194
Table A28. Continued
195
I want MEAQ DE -0.02 -0.11 0.74 0.02
Table A28. Continued
196
Table A29. Student Time 2 Subfactors of ACT Factor 2 (Awareness), Using Promax Rotation
197
Table A29. Continued
198
Table A29. Continued
199
I want MEAQ DE 0.06 -0.01 0.75 0.06
Table A29. Continued
200
201
Mechanical Turk
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 12.94 1.77 1.86
2 3.29 1.69 1.75
3 2.01 1.62 1.67
4 1.84 1.56 1.61
5 1.33 1.51 1.55
6 1.02 1.46 1.50
7 1.00 1.42 1.46
8 0.97 1.38 1.41
9 0.93 1.34 1.37
10 0.90 1.30 1.33
11 0.85 1.27 1.30
12 0.83 1.23 1.26
13 0.78 1.20 1.23
14 0.73 1.17 1.19
15 0.69 1.13 1.16
202
Student Time 1
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 11.63 1.60 1.67
2 3.39 1.53 1.59
3 2.51 1.48 1.53
4 1.91 1.44 1.48
5 1.39 1.40 1.44
6 1.18 1.37 1.40
7 1.15 1.33 1.36
8 1.01 1.30 1.33
9 0.95 1.27 1.30
10 0.90 1.24 1.27
11 0.84 1.22 1.24
12 0.80 1.19 1.21
13 0.78 1.16 1.19
14 0.74 1.14 1.16
15 0.69 1.11 1.13
203
Student Time 2
Factor Raw Mean Random 95% Upper Bound
number Eigenvalue Eigenvalue Random Eigenvalue
1 13.76 1.73 1.82
2 3.34 1.65 1.71
3 2.33 1.58 1.64
4 1.88 1.53 1.57
5 1.15 1.48 1.52
6 1.10 1.44 1.48
7 1.01 1.40 1.44
8 0.97 1.36 1.39
9 0.85 1.32 1.36
10 0.81 1.29 1.33
11 0.76 1.25 1.28
12 0.75 1.22 1.25
13 0.72 1.19 1.22
14 0.72 1.16 1.19
15 0.66 1.13 1.16
Note. 500 simulations using a significance level of 95%. Non-normal
random datasets used (based on raw data). Principal components
analysis used.
204
Table A31. Correlations Between Subfactor Loadings for 3rd Scale-Level EFA Factor
205
Table A32. Continued
206
Table A32. Continued
207
Table A33. Student Time 1 Subfactors of ACT Factor 3 (Avoidance), Using Promax Rotation
208
Table A33. Continued
209
Table A33. Continued
210
Table A34. Student Time 2 Subfactors of ACT Factor 3 (Avoidance), Using Promax Rotation
211
Table A34. Continued
212
Table A34. Continued
213
Table A35. Correlations Among Subfactor Sum Scores for Mechanical Turk Sample
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Inflexibility --
2. Internalizing Belief 0.73 --
3. Detachment -0.51 -0.47 --
4. Perspective Taking -0.20 -0.16 0.27 --
5. Expressive Awareness -0.34 -0.32 0.42 0.18 --
6. Committed Action -0.13 -0.20 0.31 0.52 0.26 --
7. Physical Awareness 0.22 0.11 0.25 0.27 0.22 0.37 --
8. Physical Avoidance 0.43 0.25 -0.16 -0.08 -0.21 -0.02 0.12 --
9. Pain Aversion 0.30 0.19 -0.02 -0.08 -0.07 -0.09 0.08 0.59 --
10. Distraction 0.07 -0.07 0.16 0.34 0.01 0.45 0.29 0.40 0.31 --
11. Mental Avoidance 0.65 0.43 -0.25 -0.08 -0.26 0.01 0.28 0.50 0.42 0.37 --
214
Table A36. Correlations Among Subfactor Sum Scores for the Student Sample
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. T1 Inflexibility --
2. T1 Internalizing Belief 0.66 --
3. T1 Detachment -0.45 -0.34 --
4. T1 Perspective Taking -0.39 -0.25 0.16 --
5. T1 Expressive Awareness -0.33 -0.28 0.35 0.30 --
6. T1 Committed Action -0.06 -0.15 0.16 0.32 0.27 --
7. T1 Physical Awareness 0.27 0.16 -0.10 0.17 0.13 0.27 --
8. T1 Physical Avoidance 0.33 0.27 -0.25 -0.19 -0.28 -0.28 0.03 --
9. T1 Pain Aversion 0.34 0.28 -0.18 -0.15 -0.20 -0.18 0.08 0.51 --
10. T1 Distraction 0.24 0.07 -0.20 -0.07 -0.11 0.17 0.08 0.37 0.31 --
11. T1 Mental Avoidance 0.71 0.48 -0.40 -0.36 -0.31 -0.05 0.27 0.34 0.38 0.41 --
12. T2 Inflexibility 0.83 0.58 -0.47 -0.32 -0.33 -0.07 0.22 0.30 0.33 0.25 0.61
13. T2 Internalizing Belief 0.54 0.70 -0.36 -0.23 -0.35 -0.21 0.16 0.22 0.23 0.03 0.39
14. T2 Detachment -0.45 -0.32 0.72 0.21 0.38 0.20 -0.04 -0.23 -0.17 -0.11 -0.31
15. T2 Perspective Taking -0.28 -0.25 0.13 0.66 0.26 0.31 0.21 -0.21 -0.18 -0.01 -0.30
16. T2 Expressive Awareness -0.24 -0.27 0.29 0.26 0.70 0.33 0.15 -0.23 -0.14 0.00 -0.20
17. T2 Committed Action -0.03 -0.13 0.10 0.15 0.25 0.60 0.23 -0.19 -0.19 0.11 -0.03
18. T2 Physical Awareness 0.25 0.17 -0.14 0.19 0.15 0.25 0.75 0.00 0.05 0.10 0.21
19. T2 Physical Avoidance 0.32 0.23 -0.26 -0.18 -0.29 -0.24 0.06 0.63 0.34 0.29 0.33
20. T2 Pain Aversion 0.23 0.25 -0.15 -0.09 -0.19 -0.23 0.05 0.42 0.64 0.22 0.27
21. T2 Distraction 0.34 0.17 -0.18 -0.14 -0.13 0.11 0.13 0.36 0.29 0.63 0.37
22. T2 Mental Avoidance 0.70 0.49 -0.35 -0.25 -0.30 0.02 0.25 0.35 0.35 0.41 0.73
215
Table A36. Continued
12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22
12. T2 Inflexibility --
13. T2 Internalizing Belief 0.62 --
14. T2 Detachment -0.48 -0.35 --
15. T2 Perspective Taking -0.29 -0.29 0.19 --
16. T2 Expressive Awareness -0.30 -0.34 0.39 0.32 --
17. T2 Committed Action -0.05 -0.22 0.23 0.36 0.38 --
18. T2 Physical Awareness 0.26 0.15 -0.06 0.31 0.22 0.28 --
19. T2 Physical Avoidance 0.43 0.28 -0.27 -0.15 -0.27 -0.14 0.14 --
20. T2 Pain Aversion 0.29 0.25 -0.15 -0.14 -0.17 -0.20 0.06 0.56 --
21. T2 Distraction 0.42 0.12 -0.16 -0.04 -0.05 0.27 0.19 0.50 0.35 --
22. T2 Mental Avoidance 0.73 0.43 -0.35 -0.23 -0.23 -0.04 0.28 0.40 0.34 0.47 --
Note. Test-retest reliabilities are bolded.
216
Table A37. Factor Analysis of Lower-Order Factor Sum Scores Using Promax Rotation
217
Table A38. Mechanical Turk Correlations Between Subfactor Sum Scores and Other Study Variables
218
Table A38. Continued
219
Table A39. Student Time 1 Correlations Between Subfactor Sum Scores and Other Study Variables
220
Table A39. Continued
221
Table A40. Student Time 2 Correlations Between Subfactor Sum Scores and Other Study Variables
222
Table A40. Continued
223
Table A41. Fit Indices for Hierarchical and Correlated ACT Models
224
Table A42. Student Correlations Between ACT Subfactors and Psychopathology Measures Across Timepoints
225
Table A43. Mechanical Turk Partial Correlations Between ACT Subfactors and External Measures Controlling for Subscales of the
ASI, DIS, DTS, and COPE
226
Table A44. Student Time 1 Partial Correlations Between ACT Subfactors and External Measures Controlling for Subscales of the
ASI, DIS, DTS, and COPE
227
Table A45. Student Time 2 Partial Correlations Between ACT Subfactors and External Measures Controlling for Subscales of the
ASI, DIS, DTS, and COPE
228
229
APPENDIX B
FIGURES
233
Present Moment
Awareness
Acceptance Values
Psychological
Flexibility
Self as Context
234
Depressive
Awareness Avoidance
Inflexibility
235
Internalizing P erspective Expressive Committed P hysical P hysical P ain Mental
Inflexibility Detachment Distraction
Belief Taking Awareness Action Awareness Avoidance Aversion Avoidance
236
237
APPENDIX C
STUDY MEASURES
238
Below you will find a list of statements. Please rate how true each statement is for you.
1, never true | 2, very seldom true | 3, seldom true | 4, sometimes true | 5, frequently true | 6,
almost always true | 7, always true
1) My painful experiences and memories make it difficult for me to live a life that I would value.
6) It seems like most people are handling their lives better than I am.
Use the scale below to determine the one phrase that best represents the extent to which you
agree with each item. If any of the items concern something that is not part of your experience
(e.g., "It scares me when I feel shaky" for someone who has never trembled or had the "shakes"),
answer on the basis of how you think you might feel if you had such an experience. Otherwise,
2) When I cannot keep my mind on a task, I worry that I might be going crazy.
9) When I notice my heart is beating rapidly, I worry that I might have a heart attack.
ASI Continued
3, 4, 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 14
2, 12, 15, 16
1, 5, 13
241
Instructions: Listed below are a variety of thoughts that pop into people's heads. Please read each
thought and indicate how frequently, if at all, the thought occurred to you OVER THE LAST
WEEK. After rating each thought's frequency, please indicate how strongly, if at all, you tend
Please rate how frequently you experienced this thought over the last week.
Please indicate how strongly, if at all, you tend to believe that thought, when it occurs.
2) I'm no good.
8) I'm so weak.
ATQ Continued
Below you will find a list of statements. Please rate how true each statement is for you.
1, never true | 2, very seldom true | 3, seldom true | 4, sometimes true | 5, frequently true | 6,
2) I get so caught up in my thoughts that I am unable to do the things that I most want to do
3) Even when I am having distressing thoughts, I know that they may become less important
eventually
6) Even when I'm having upsetting thoughts, I can see that those thoughts may not be literally
true
12) It's possible for me to have negative thoughts about myself and still know that I am an OK
person
13) It's such a struggle to let go of upsetting thoughts even when I know that letting go would be
helpful
244
COPE
We are interested in how people respond when they confront difficult or stressful events in their
lives. There are lots of ways to try and deal with stress. This questionnaire asks you to indicate
what you generally do and feel, when YOU experience stressful events. Obviously different
events bring out somewhat different responses, but think about what you USUALLY do when
you are under a lot of stress. Please indicate the response that most reflects how you deal with
stressful events.
1, I usually don't do this at all | 2, I usually do this a little bit | 3, I usually do this a medium
10) I focus on dealing with this problem, and if necessary let other things slide a little
12) I try hard to prevent other things from interfering with my efforts at dealing with this
14) I hold off doing anything about it until the situation permits
15) I make sure not to make matters worse by acting too soon
245
COPE Continued
17) I ask people who have had similar experiences what they did
20) I talk to someone who could do something concrete about the problem
30) I accept that this has happened and that it can't be changed
39) I feel a lot of emotional distress and I find myself expressing those feelings a lot
COPE Continued
47) I admit to myself that I can't deal with it and quit trying
48) I reduce the amount of effort I'm putting into solving the problem
49) I turn to work or other substitute activities to take my mind off things
Instructions: Below are statements about how some people feel and behave. For each statement
below, indicate the number which best describes the degree to which the statement applies to
you.
3 Moderately Like Me
6 Extremely Like Me
4) When I begin to feel physically uncomfortable, I quickly take steps to relieve the discomfort
1, 2
3, 4, 5
248
Directions: Think of times that you feel distressed or upset. Select the response that best
1, strongly agree | 2, mildly agree | 3, agree and disagree equally | 4, mildly disagree | 5, strongly
disagree
2) When I feel distressed or upset, all I can think about is how bad I feel.
9) Other people seem to be able to tolerate feeling distressed or upset better than I can.
15) When I feel distressed or upset, I cannot help but concentrate on how bad the distress
actually feels.
249
DTS Continued
1, 3, 5
2, 4, 15
8, 13, 14
250
Instructions: We are interested in your recent experiences. Below is a list of things that people
sometimes experience. Please indicate how much you currently have experiences similar to those
described.
4) I notice all sorts of little things and details in the world around me.
13) I think over and over again about what others have said to me
15) I can observe unpleasant feelings without being drawn into them
16) I have the sense that I am fully aware of what is going on around me and inside me
19) I think about the ways in which I am different from other people
EQ continued
EQ decentering items:
For each question, please select the response that best applies to you.
11) I have taken money from someone's purse or wallet without asking.
Please rate each of the following statements using the scale provided. Indicate the response that
best describes your own opinion of what is generally true for you.
1, never or very rarely true | 2, rarely true | 3, sometimes true | 4, often true | 5, very often or
always true
6) When I take a shower or bath, I stay alert to the sensations of water on my body.
8) I don't pay attention to what I'm doing because I'm daydreaming, worrying, or otherwise
distracted
11) I notice how foods and drinks affect my thoughts, bodily sensations, and emotions.
12) It's hard for me to find the words to describe what I'm thinking.
14) I believe some of my thoughts are abnormal or bad and I shouldn't think that way.
15) I pay attention to sensations, such as the wind in my hair or sun on my face.
16) I have trouble thinking of the right words to express how I feel about things
FFMQ Continued
19) When I have distressing thoughts or images, I step back and am aware of the
20) I pay attention to sounds, such as clocks ticking, birds chirping, or cars passing.
22) When I have a sensation in my body, it's difficult for me to describe it because I can't find the
right words.
23) It seems I am running on automatic without much awareness of what I'm doing.
24) When I have distressing thoughts or images, I feel calm soon after.
25) I tell myself that I shouldn't be thinking the way I'm thinking.
27) Even when I'm feeling terribly upset, I can find a way to put it into words.
29) When I have distressing thoughts or images I am able just to notice them without reacting.
30) I think some of my emotions are bad or inappropriate and I shouldn't feel them.
31) I notice visual elements in art or nature, such as colors, shapes, textures, or patterns of light
and shadow.
33) When I have distressing thoughts or images, I just notice them and let them go.
34) I do jobs or tasks automatically without being aware of what I'm doing.
35) When I have distressing thoughts or images, I judge myself as good or bad, depending what
37) I can usually describe how I feel at the moment in considerable detail.
FFMQ Continued
Please indicate the extent to which you agree with each of the following statements.
3) When something upsetting comes up, I try very hard to stop thinking about it
9) When negative thoughts come up, I try to fill my head with something else
12) Even when I feel uncomfortable, I don't give up working toward things I value
17) When I have something important to do I find myself doing a lot of other things instead
18) I am willing to put up with pain and discomfort to get what I want
20) I work hard to avoid situations that might bring up unpleasant thoughts and feelings in me
257
MEAQ Continued
22) When upsetting memories come up, I try to focus on other things
26) I prefer to stick to what I am comfortable with, rather than try new activities
33) When unpleasant memories come to me, I try to put them out of my mind
34) In this day and age people should not have to suffer
40) When a negative thought comes up, I immediately try to think of something else
43) I don't let pain and discomfort stop me from getting what I want
MEAQ Continued
47) Why do today what you can put off until tomorrow
49) Some people have told me that I "hide my head in the sand"
57) I don't let gloomy thoughts stop me from doing what I want
59) I'm quick to leave any situation that makes me feel uneasy
62) When working on something important, I won't quit even if things get difficult
259
MEAQ Continued
2, 7, 13, 19, 25, 31, 34, 38, 44, 50, 54, 58, 61
4, 10, 16, 21, 23R, 28, 35, 41, 46, 49, 52, 56, 60
These questions concern how you have been feeling over the past 30 days. Indicate the response
1, None of the time | 2, A little of the time | 3, Some of the time | 4, Most of the time | 5, All of
the time
1) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel tired out for no good reason?
2) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel nervous?
3) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel so nervous that nothing could calm you
down?
4) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel hopeless?
5) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel restless or fidgety?
6) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel so restless you could not sit still?
7) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel depressed?
8) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel that everything was an effort?
9) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel so sad that nothing could cheer you up?
10) During the last 30 days, about how often did you feel worthless?
261
Below is a collection of statements about your everyday experience. Using the scale provided,
please indicate how frequently or infrequently you currently have each experience. Please
answer according to what really reflects your experience rather than what you think your
1) I could be experiencing some emotion and not be conscious of it until some time later.
2) I break or spill things because of carelessness, not paying attention, or thinking of something
else.
4) I tend to walk quickly to get where I'm going without paying attention to what I experience
5) I tend not to notice feelings of physical tension or discomfort until they really grab my
attention.
6) I forget a person's name almost as soon as I've been told it for the first time.
9) I get so focused on the goal I want to achieve that I lose touch with what I am doing right now
to get there.
10) I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I'm doing.
11) I find myself listening to someone with one ear, doing something else at the same time.
12) I drive places on automatic pilot and then wonder why I went there.
MAAS Continued
Instructions: On the following pages, there are phrases describing people's behaviors. Please use
the provided rating scale to describe how accurately each statement describes you. Describe
yourself as you generally are now, not as you wish to be in the future. Describe yourself as you
honestly see yourself, in relation to other people you know of the same sex as you are, and
IPIP Continued
Please indicate how often you experienced each of the following statements within the past
week.
3) When talking with other people, I am aware of their facial and body expressions.
9) When I walk outside, I am aware of smells or how the air feels against my face.
11) When someone asks how I am feeling, I can identify my emotions easily.
15) I notice changes inside my body, like my heart beating faster or my muscles getting tense.
16) If there is something I don't want to think about, I'll try many things to get it out of my mind.
19) When talking with other people, I am aware of the emotions I am experiencing.
20) When I have a bad memory, I try to distract myself to make it go away.
266
PHLMS Continued
This survey is about thoughts. There are no right or wrong answers, so please respond honestly
to each of the items below. Be sure to answer every item by indicating the best response.
14) There are many thoughts that I have that I don't tell anyone.
15) Sometimes I stay busy just to keep thoughts from intruding on my mind.
268
This questionnaire asks about difficulties due to health conditions. Health conditions include
diseases or illnesses, other health problems that may be short or long lasting, injuries, mental or
emotional problems, and problems with alcohol or drugs. Think back over the last 30 days and
answer these questions thinking about how much difficulty you had doing the following
activities. In the last 30 days, how much difficulty did you have in:
4) Learning a new task, for example, learning how to get to a new place?
8) Maintaining a friendship?
14) Getting all the household work done that you needed to do?
18) Getting all the work done that you need to do?
WHODAS Continued
1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6
7, 8, 9, 10, 11
Below are domains of life that are valued by some people. We are concerned with your
subjective experience of your quality of life in each of these domains. One aspect of quality of
life involves the importance one puts on the different domains of living. Rate the importance of
each domain (by indicating a number) on a scale of 1-10. 1 means that domain is not at all
important and 10 means that domain is very important. Not everyone will value all of these
domains, or value all domains the same. Rate each domain according to your own personal sense
10 extremely important
2) Marriage/couples/intimate relations
3) Parenting
4) Friendships/social relations
5) Employment
6) Education/training
7) Recreation
8) Spirituality
9) Citizenship/Community Life
VLQ Continued
In this section, we would like you to give a rating of how consistent your actions are with each
value. Everyone does better in some domains than others. We are NOT asking about your ideal
in each domain. We want to know how you think you have been doing during the past week.
Rate each item (by indicating a number) on a scale of 1-10. 1 means that your actions have been
fully inconsistent with your value and 10 means that your actions have been fully consistent with
10 extremely consistent
2) Marriage/couples/intimate relations
3) Parenting
4) Friendships/social relations
5) Employment
6) Education/training
7) Recreation
8) Spirituality
9) Citizenship/Community Life