Artigo CognicaoENivelDeExperienciaDI
Artigo CognicaoENivelDeExperienciaDI
Artigo CognicaoENivelDeExperienciaDI
A
s professional practice in fields such as architecture and interior
design is increasingly regulated by licensure requirements, and as
an accredited disciplinary degree becomes more often a prerequisite
for licensure examinations, formal design education plays a greater role than
ever before in shaping how fledgling designers develop design expertise and
determining who will have the opportunity to gain such expertise. Famous de-
signers such as Frank Lloyd Wright and Ray and Charles Eames are just a
few examples of individuals who never completed formal design education,
and yet had tremendous impacts on twentieth-century design. Persons with
similar biographies could not practice as freely in many twenty-first-century
design professions.
This observation is not made to suggest that licensure should not be required
for professional design practice, nor that a design-related degree should not
be prerequisite to licensure. Instead, it is offered as a basis from which to
argue that as we constrain the range of sanctioned pathways available to in-
dividuals seeking to develop design expertise and professional status, we
Corresponding author:- should recognize that the experiences students have in formal design
kennsmit@indiana. programs (sometimes to the exclusion of other types of experiences) may
edu increasingly shape the development and future expression of design expertise.
www.elsevier.com/locate/destud
0142-694X Design Studies 36 (2015) 77e98
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2014.09.001 77
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
In light of this reality, the work reported here seeks to contribute to design
expertise literature by examining conditions students perceive to have been
either conducive to, or counter-productive to, their success in formal design
education.
It is anticipated that examining factors which impinge upon the early devel-
opment of design expertise as fostered in formal design education experiences
has implications in two complementary arenas. First, in the arena of scholar-
ship of teaching and learning, the identification and understanding of chal-
lenges faced by students may better equip teachers and curriculum
designers to develop experiences that will be of greatest benefit. Second, in
the arena of disciplinary self-study, the examination of perspectives held by
students coming into a field may provide insights into disciplinary character-
istics that have been forgotten or never recognized by individuals who have
been socialized into the discipline (Siegel & Stolterman, 2008), as well as clues
to future expressions of design expertise. These educational experiences are
likely to influence the nature of future expertise as it is collectively expressed
within a given discipline to an ever greater extent as shared professional de-
grees become compulsory for professional practice. Identifying key factors
which shape the early development of design expertise, in real time, may
help us forecast and pre-emptively evaluate key issues which will characterize
the futures of design disciplines.
1 Review of literature
Examining conditions which contribute to the development of early design
expertise in the context of formal design education can be informed by two
inter-related bodies of literature: (1) that which seeks to identify qualities of
design expertise, and (2) that which examines the contexts and conditions
under which such expertise is developed. Findings in these two bodies of liter-
ature can be further contextualized by a brief exploration of the more general
scholarship concerning the nature and acquisition of expertise across
disciplines.
In making this point, it is not the intention to suggest that any scholar explic-
itly claims that broader contextual factors, such as those concerning culture or
interpersonal dynamics, are unimportant in the development of design exper-
tise. However, it is useful to recognize that the conditions under which many
studies of design expertise are conducted (particularly the relatively artificial
environments and timelines necessitated in some cases by study controls and
timelines) may separate study subjects from both (1) the broader contextual
factors which typically factor into their professional practice, and (2) the fac-
tors in their personal biography which shaped their expertise. In strictly
controlled experimental studies, the factors left to observe are those internal
to the participants (such as differences in ways of thinking or acting made
visible by the study), or those resources and conditions made available in
the design of the study treatment (such as access to specific kinds of resources).
The unintended consequence of such work may be that some readers then
focus on the factors highlighted by study findings, while not considering fac-
tors that the experimental design has precluded from surfacing. Over-
reliance on the findings of such studies may artificially constrain our primary
discourse on expertise to the personal characteristics or attributes observable
in these experimental settings. Such discourse may ignore conditions of exper-
tise distributed in the environment, in communities of practice, or in personal
biography if not supplemented by a greater quantity of literature inclusive of
broader contextual factors. When broader considerations related to conditions
shaping the development of expertise are currently addressed, they typically
appear in the context of literature on design education, and less often as direct
contributions to discourse on the nature (as well as the acquisition) of design
expertise.
Certainly, the social context impacts upon the availability and prominence of
role models to whom a novice might look, the visibility of expert achievements
to which the student might aspire, and the availability of coaches capable of
nurturing expertise. Simonton (2003, 2006) emphasizes that the socio-
cultural milieu, including factors such as political stability, economics, and
the social value attached to different types of activities, plays a pivotal role
in shaping both opportunities and motivation for the development of exper-
tise. As summarized by Hunt (2006), ‘Becoming an expert in almost anything
requires literally years of work. People will do this only if they have some
initial success, enjoy the work, and are supported by the social climate. Exper-
tise is not solely a cognitive affair’ (p. 36).
In a related argument, Baker and Horton (2004) propose that social factors
also fundamentally shape the defining characteristics of expertise in different
areas. Specifically, as an activity gains in popularity and prestige, the pool
of competitors vying for recognition as experts in the field grows, and thus
tends to raise the level of performance required to distinguish oneself in the
field. With this in mind, it could be argued that socio-cultural factors not
only shape the likelihood of accessing opportunities to develop expertise,
but also function to influence how such expertise is ultimately defined. Thus,
while secondary factors related to social climate and context may not be
directly observable in future expressions of expertise, they function as modi-
fiers of the likely development of such expertise and constitute important con-
siderations in dialogue concerned with the perceived nature of expertise.
In fields of design where college degrees are increasingly pre-requisite for entry
into the profession, it is important to examine how socio-cultural influences
may influence the likelihood of students matriculating into and persisting
through such programs. Indeed, the findings in expertise literature which point
to the important motivational and support functions of social context seem to
be reinforced in literature on student success in degree programs. Astin (1993),
after his landmark study involving thousands of college students concluded,
‘the student’s peer group is the single most potent source of influence on
growth and development during the undergraduate years’ (p. 398). Similarly,
Pascarella and Terenzini (2005) found that peer influence impacted intellectual
development and was a significant factor in program persistence. Such findings
may significantly alter our future models of expertise development within
educational settings, requiring that socio-cultural and other motivational fac-
tors been considered alongside the discipline-specific knowledge and skills that
are often the primary features of such models (Alexander, 2003).
2 Method
2.1 Participants
Students enrolled in their last required interior design studio at a large, public,
Midwestern university were invited to participate in interviews regarding their
design education experiences. This final studio is taught during only one se-
mester each academic year, and students from three successive cohorts (a sepa-
rate cohort each year, for three consecutive years), were recruited for study
participation. Ultimately, 38 students participated in interviews. The invited
cohorts were overwhelmingly comprised of female students, and 36 of the 38
participants were women. Most participants were traditional college-age stu-
dents in their early to mid-twenties.
Students enrolled in the final required studio were targeted for participation in
this study because they were at a cross-road deemed particularly beneficial to
the gathering of student narratives. As a prerequisite to enrollment in this stu-
dio, each had completed nearly all of their undergraduate design coursework,
and most were on track to graduate with Bachelor’s degrees at the end of the
academic year in which they were taking the studio. It was assumed that their
undergraduate learning experiences would be relatively fresh in their minds
and not yet dimmed by the extensive passage of time, or altered by extensive
professional practice. Furthermore, by conducting the interviews near the
end of the educational experience, it was anticipated that students would be
able to describe their perceptions of personal trajectories, changes, and
challenges over the course of that period.
How has the program been similar to, or different from, what you expected?
What did you expect the program to be like when you began?
What were your incoming perceptions of interior design?
How do those perceptions compare to your current perceptions?
What parts of the program did you find most difficult? Why?
What project/projects did you feel you were most successful with?
What do you like about the finished project?
What do you think made this project so successful for you?
How did you approach this project?
What project do you believe was least successful for you? Why?
How do you know if a project is going well or not? (Or if it is successful or not?)
Do you ever get stuck and have a hard time moving forward when you are working on a design project?
If so, does it usually happen at a certain point in the design process? If so, how to you get unstuck?
What advice would you give an incoming student?
What (if anything) would you do differently if you were starting the program again?
What advice would you give to faculty? What worked well and has to stay?
What did not work well and has to be changed or improved?
Category Description
Interpersonal This category deals with interpersonal relationships interviewees developed with
relationships peer students, instructors, and persons outside the program, particularly with
reference to the degree to which these relationships were seen as supportive or
non-supportive. These relationships were described as directly influencing the
effectiveness of their design education experiences with respect to their
developing expertise, as well as impacting their resilience and ability to persist
in the program.
Personal knowledge, This category deals with cognitive and skill changes that interviewees described
skills, and attributes occurring within themselves. These changes included acquiring content
knowledge, developing deeper understandings of design process and facility in
moving through such a process, mastering technical and craft skills needed in
completing projects, and shifting personal attitudes and expectations.
Pedagogical culture This category deals with the culture which students described as existing within
the program and, most often, within studios. Issues included studio expectations,
traditions, and policies, as well as larger issues pertaining to the nature and
management of the program’s curricular plan.
Resources This category deals with both knowledge resources and physical resources.
These resources were described as essential with respect to their availability and
were identified as major sources of frustration when they proved to be unreliable.
promise in their given area, and were subsequently provided even more social
support. It appears that the effort necessary to move beyond initial introduc-
tion to a field, to persist through difficult developmental phases, and to endure
the tedium which accompanies some aspects of expertise development was
often facilitated by ‘enormously supportive social contexts’ (Sosniak, 2003,
as quoted in Sosniak, 2006, p. 289). Sosniak (2006) summarized the role
such support had played as follows.
While these questions did not elicit the patterns that had been hoped, consid-
eration of the interview accounts as wholes did surface more generalized issues
that proved to be particularly challenging for most respondents with respect to
personal development in their ways of thinking and acting. Specifically, narra-
tives revealed significant difficulties learning how to manage design processes
and developing more nuanced, personal understandings of the nature of
design.
Table 4 Personal knowledge, skills, attributes: Sub-themes and representative interview excerpts
3.3.2 Feedback
Prominent features of typical design studios include both formative feedback
(often provided through informal desk critiques) and summative feedback
(often provided through final presentations and formal grades). Both types
of feedback proved difficult and were seen as major impediments by a sizable
portion of interviewees. Many expressed confusion regarding the purpose of
desk critiques. Having been required to present their work-in-progress to in-
structors, some could not understand why instructors had not corrected all
of their errors by the time the final design was developed. These interviewees
explained how surprised they had then been when they received lower grades
than had been hoped, having assumed there was an implicit agreement that in-
structors should guide students and mold interim work so that by the time the
final design is submitted, it is awarded an A letter-grade.
Another major issue in the area of physical resources was access to and
perceived suitability of studio facilities. Due to security concerns, students
did not have 24-h access to the design studios and many felt strongly the spaces
needed to be accessible longer hours. This was usually not, as one might
expect, because they needed access to specialized tools or resources at all hours
of the night, but often because they needed a dedicated space in which they
While patterns emerging in other contexts might not be appropriate for whole-
sale adoption in discourse regarding design expertise, the findings in this study
suggest that a broader range of considerations (like those being addressed in
areas such as athletics and music expertise) may be useful in trying to account
for conditions impinging upon ways we develop, exercise, and ultimately
define design expertise. Models which prompt consideration of a broader
range of variables may be useful tools for expanding dialogue regarding the
conditions that need to be attended if our efforts to educate today’s novices
are to beneficially foster tomorrow’s expressions of design expertise, as well
as for understanding how the conditions we attend to in the educational expe-
riences of today’s novices may shape tomorrow’s definitions of such expertise.
References
AIAS. (2010). Studio culture. Available online at. http://www.aias.org/website/ar-
ticle.asp?id¼77 Retrieved November 7, 2013.
Alexander, P. A. (2003). The development of expertise: the journey from acclima-
tion to proficiency. Educational Researcher, 32(8), 10e14.
Anthony, K. H. (1991). Design juries on trial: The renaissance of the design studio.
Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Astin, A. W. (1993). What matters in college? Four critical years revisited. San
Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.
Austerlitz, N., & Aravot, I. (2007). Emotions of architecture students: a new
perspective for the design studio. In A. M. Salama, & N. Wilkinson (Eds.),
Design studio pedagogy: Horizons for the future (pp. 233e245). Gateshead,
UK: The Urban International Press.