Provided by Brunel University Research Archive
Provided by Brunel University Research Archive
Provided by Brunel University Research Archive
uk
Provided by Brunel University Research Archive
1
Department of Gender Studies, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana.
2
The Kinsey Institute for Research in Sex, Gender, and Reproduction, Indiana University,
Bloomington, Indiana.
3
Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana.
4
To whom correspondence should be addressed at The Kinsey Institute for Research in Sex,
Gender, and Reproduction, Indiana University, Morrison Hall 313, 1165 East Third Street,
ABSTRACT
The aim of this study was to inform the development of a questionnaire to assess a woman‟s
tendency to respond with sexual excitation/inhibition in different situations. Nine focus groups,
involving 80 women (M age = 34.3 years; range, 18-84), were conducted. Women described a
wide range of physical (genital and non-genital), cognitive/emotional, and behavioral cues to
arousal. The relationship between sexual interest and sexual arousal was complex; sexual interest
was reported as sometimes preceding arousal, but at other times following it. Many women did
not clearly differentiate between arousal and interest. Qualitative data on the factors that women
perceived as “enhancers” and “inhibitors” of sexual arousal are presented, with a focus on the
following themes: feelings about one‟s body; concern about reputation; unwanted
partner; style of approach/initiation; and negative mood. The findings can help inform
INTRODUCTION
In most research on sexual arousal, there has been an assumption that lack of sexual
arousal is due to a lack of excitation. Inhibition of arousal has often been implicitly
acknowledged, but not studied, as a process separate from excitation. The concept of “inhibited
sexual desire” has been widely used in the clinical literature (American Psychiatric Association,
1980; Beck, 1994; Lief, 1977) but there has been little systematic study of this and no attempt to
distinguish between inhibited sexual desire and lack of desire. The newly developed “dual
control” model of sexual response postulates that, within the central nervous system, there are
separate and relatively independent excitatory and inhibitory systems (Bancroft, 1999; Bancroft
& Janssen, 2000). It is the balance between these two systems that determines whether sexual
arousal occurs in any particular situation. The model also postulates that individuals vary in their
propensity for both sexual excitation (SE) and sexual inhibition (SI).
The capacity to inhibit sexual response is seen as adaptive, as a means by which the
individual can avoid danger or other risks to well-being that might result from a sexual response
in a given situation. For some, however, the propensity for SI may be unduly high, resulting in
an impairment of the capacity for sexual function and, for others, the propensity for SI may be
low, increasing the likelihood of engaging in high-risk sexual behavior (Bancroft, 1999).
Research has been exploring this model and its possible relationship to both sexual risk-taking
(Bancroft, Janssen, Strong, Carnes, & Long, 2003; Bancroft et al., in press) and sexual
dysfunction in men (Bancroft & Janssen, 2000; 2001). A questionnaire (Sexual Inhibition
Scale/Sexual Excitation Scale, SIS/SES) has been developed to assess SI and SE in men and has
been demonstrated to have good psychometric properties (Janssen, Vorst, Finn, & Bancroft,
2002a; 2002b). Factor analyses yielded three factors–one excitation factor (SES) and two
Women‟s sexual arousal 4
inhibition factors, which have been labeled “inhibition due to threat of performance failure”
The SIS/SES has been adapted for women and used in a study of 1067 female college
students (Carpenter, Janssen, & Graham, 2003). The findings suggested that women had lower
SE scores and higher SI scores in comparison with men, with a fairly normal distribution,
showing variability in SE and SI scores. This study also found evidence for the convergent and
Our view, however, was that simply modifying this measure, originally developed for use
with men, might miss important aspects of SI and SE in women‟s experience or emphasize
genital response in a way women find less relevant or meaningful. There are a number of reasons
for believing that central inhibition in women may be fundamentally different in its underlying
mechanisms and scope of effects. It has been suggested that inhibition may be more important
for women than for men, particularly as it pertains to sexuality and reproduction (Bjorklund &
threatening, a possibly different time relationship between SI and sexual activity (e.g., SI may
occur much earlier in women), and consideration of exclusively female factors, such as the
menstrual cycle and pregnancy. For example, concerns about one‟s reputation may be a more
important SI factor for women‟s sexuality than for men‟s (Tiefer, 2001).
This qualitative study was the first stage of a project to develop a questionnaire, using a
variety of stimuli. Rather than relying on researchers‟ assumptions about what factors are
important to women‟s sexual arousal, we wanted to hear from women themselves about what
We used focus groups of women to explore the factors most relevant to SI and SE in
women. Morgan (1996) defined a focus group as “a research technique that collects data through
group interaction on a topic determined by the researcher” (p. 130). As surveys are inherently
limited by the questions they ask, focus groups can provide data on how respondents themselves
talk about the topics (Morgan, 1996) and thus have been recommended as a means to inform
questionnaire development for more than a decade (Morgan, 1997). Although focus groups have
been increasingly used in sexuality research (Byers, Zeller, & Byers, 2002), few published
accounts exist of using focus groups as a means to construct questionnaires. Our goal was to use
the information obtained from the focus groups to help us devise specific items for a
questionnaire. This article reports on the qualitative data from the nine focus groups we
conducted.
METHOD
Participants
effort was made to obtain age, race, ethnic group, educational, and relationship status diversity in
the sample. Thus, a range of recruitment strategies was used, including flyers and
and campus centers. Women interested in participating were screened by telephone and if
eligible, were mailed a demographic questionnaire. They were informed that the purpose of the
study was “to collect information on women‟s experience of sexual arousal and assess factors or
types of situations that promote or interfere with women‟s sexual interest or arousal.”1
__________________________________________________________________________
1
In this article, we are using the terms “sexual desire” and “sexual interest” interchangeably.
Women‟s sexual arousal 6
We made the decision to have groups that were fairly homogenous with respect to age
(18-24 years, 25-45 years, and 46 years and older), but mixed with regard to other demographic
factors, such as student status, and ethnic and racial background. As recommended by previous
researchers (Seal, Bogart, & Ehrhardt, 1998), we over-recruited to control for cancellations and
no-shows and scheduled 12 women for each group. To ensure diversity in these “mixed” groups,
no more than six women who were students or who described their race as “white” were
scheduled for any one group. In total, six “mixed” groups (two 18-24 year groups, Ns = 6 and
10; two 25-45 year groups, Ns = 9 and 9; two 46+ year groups, Ns = 10 and 9) were conducted.
In order to enhance the overall diversity of our sample, we also conducted two groups of
lesbian/bisexual women (one aged 18-24 years, N = 9, and one aged 25 years and older, N = 10)
and one group of African-American women (aged 18-35 years, N = 10). Our view was that these
“segmented” groups might facilitate discussion because minority participants might feel more
Participants were 80 women (mean age = 34.3 years; SD = 16.1; range = 18–84 years).
Table I contains demographic information on the sample. As can be seen, participants were
highly educated, but were quite diverse in terms of other demographics, such as employment,
-----------------------------
-----------------------------
Measures
Women‟s sexual arousal 7
Procedure
Two female moderators facilitated each of the focus group sessions. In each group, one of
the moderators was a Ph.D. level psychologist and the other was either an M.A. level researcher
All of the sessions were audiotaped and transcribed for analysis. In addition, moderators
made written notes during the sessions. No individual identifiers were collected to ensure the
The groups were held in a private room in a local public library, with the exception of the
two lesbian/bisexual groups that met in a conference room at The Kinsey Institute. The rationale
for this was that women attending a lesbian/bisexual group might feel more comfortable meeting
at The Kinsey Institute, rather than in a public venue, particularly if they had not “come out” as
lesbian or bisexual. Consent forms and background questionnaires were collected when each
woman arrived. Each session began with introductions by the moderators and the participants.
Name cards were provided for each participant; however, women were told that they could
choose not to use their real names and could use a pseudonym. Refreshments were provided. At
the end of the 2-hour session, women were thanked for their participation and received a $25
payment.
Study approval was obtained from the Indiana University Bloomington Campus
1) Description of the purpose of the study and the procedural rules of the focus group. Women
to share ideas to help us develop a better understanding of women‟s sexual arousal and its
components as well as the factors or types of situations that promote or interfere with
women‟s sexual interest and arousal. We will use the information to help us develop a
questionnaire.
Women were told that they could share information from their own experience, things they have
peers). Participants were also asked to honor the privacy of the other participants and not to share
any of the focus group discussion with others outside of the group.
a) Sexual arousal and its components. “How do women know when they are sexually
aroused? What cues are there? Is vaginal lubrication (“wetness”) a counterpart or parallel
to erection?”
b) Sexual interest and sexual arousal. “How would you describe sexual interest? How is it
related to sexual arousal? Is there a clear demarcation between sexual interest and sexual
c) Factors that enhance or inhibit sexual arousal. (i) “What sorts of things enhance or
increase sexual arousal? (ii) What sorts of things prevent or stop women from being
The sequence of discussion topics was not rigidly fixed. In keeping with the primary goal
of the study, to gather information on factors that enhance/inhibit arousal, the majority of the
focus group session was spent on discussion of topic 2c. For these latter questions, the discussion
Women‟s sexual arousal 9
guide also contained a list of possible situations/factors that could be used as “prompts”;
however, as much as possible, moderators allowed the group members to generate ideas. The
aim was to have participants react to the ideas and statements of other group members. The
moderators tried to keep the conversation “on target” but guided the discussion only when
necessary and there was no attempt to try to control the way that the participants interacted (e.g.,
Data Analysis
All of the four authors were involved in analyzing the focus group transcripts. Our
method of analysis was drawn from Morgan (1997). In the first stage, transcriptions from each of
the nine focus groups were analyzed independently by two investigators, who listed recurrent
themes and specific quotes within each theme. Following this, all four investigators met to
compare the themes across investigators and groups. This was an interactive process that
involved repeated rereading of the transcripts. Discrepant themes were discussed until agreement
was reached and new themes were added to reflect as much of the data as possible. Next, themes
were organized into broad categories. Refinements were made to the coding scheme and the
labeling of themes, after discussion and consensus among the four researchers. The end result
was a coding scheme consisting of eight broad categories and within most of these, a number of
sub-categories (see Appendix). In the final stage of analyses, we applied the coding framework
to all of the data by annotating the transcripts with the numerical codes that indexed the
categories.
Although group comparison was not a focus of our study, we used a “grid” approach
(Knodel, 1993; Morgan, 1997) to provide a descriptive summary of the content of discussions.
On one axis of the grid were the coding categories/subcategories identified and, on the other, the
Women‟s sexual arousal 10
focus group session identifiers (e.g., 18-24 lesbian/bisexual group). The cells contained page
numbers of transcripts, where quotes that illustrated the particular theme were located. The grid
was a useful way to compare responses to each of the discussion questions across focus groups,
We did not rely on code counting per se, but rather on a more interpretative summary of the data.
RESULTS
The results are presented in three sections, corresponding to the three discussion topics
explored in the focus groups: (1) cues for sexual arousal; (2) the relationship between sexual
arousal and sexual interest; and, (3) factors that enhance or inhibit sexual arousal.
In all of the groups, women described a wide range of cues for sexual arousal, including
nervousness, heightened sense of awareness), and behavioral (e.g., sighing, moaning) indicators.
Genital changes described were sensations of tingling, warmth, fullness, swelling, and
lubrication, and non-genital physical changes included “butterflies” in the stomach, increased
heart rate, nipple hardening, increased skin sensitivity, changes in temperature, shortness of
breath, muscle tightness in stomach and legs, and flushing in the face and chest.
Although lubrication was reported as one of the cues of sexual arousal, women‟s
erection?” was a resounding “no”. Women observed that if a man experienced an erection in
sexual situation, this would be a signal that he was sexually aroused. A number of participants
reported that feeling aroused and being lubricated did not always co-occur, as illustrated by this
interaction:
Women‟s sexual arousal 11
P–2: There‟s times of the month too where there‟s just more lubrication because of whatever‟s
Moderator (M): So you can be aroused and not necessarily feel wet or vice versa?
P–3: Yeah, I find that wetness comes at a later stage of arousal for me. There‟s more of a
sequence thing. It comes later. It‟s not the first sign. [25-45 group]3
The fact that lubrication might be a later sign of arousal, and one that might not always be
P–2: And if there‟s a chance, I guess, to notice it too depending on what you‟re doing.
P–3: I don‟t think that‟s one of the first signals to me . . . I think there are a lot of things that you
In the discussions on the relationship between sexual interest and sexual arousal, a
number of women said that they did not clearly differentiate between sexual arousal and sexual
P: The arousal, the interest, they tend to, they blur . . . I‟m not even sure how to separate out one
P: Maybe I don‟t get interested very often but when I do, there‟s at least a little bit . . . a degree
of arousal. . . . For me if I‟m sexually interested in somebody, even a little bit, then I‟m a
__________________________________________________________________________
2
In all of the quotes involving more than one participant, P–1, P–2…P–N indicates a
P–1: I tend to think of arousal as more physical and interest more thoughtful and I don‟t think
P–2: For me, it can be either way actually. Some thought may come to my mind which arouses
me or I may feel aroused and then . . . it‟s hard to explain it. I may feel aroused and have an
interest in pursuing it. I think that‟s what I‟m trying to say. It‟s either way to me. [25-45
group]
Other women talked about sexual arousal occurring without any experience of sexual
interest:
P–1: I think there can be arousal without interest at all. You can be like, I don‟t know, riding on a
tractor or something.
Factors that affected SE and SI (see questions 2c) were classified into eight broad
categories. Each of these categories contained a number of sub-categories (for a list of the coding
categories, see Appendix). It is important to note that many of the factors in our coding scheme
were cited as “inhibitors” by some women and as “enhancers” by others or as both by the same
woman, depending on the specific situation being described. For example, negative mood states,
such as anxiety, were reported by some women as reducing their ability to become aroused and
by others as increasing it. Similarly, the possibility of being seen or heard while having sex was
variability, women also noted that context and timing were important in this regard; for example,
a particular style of approach by a partner might increase their arousal only if the partner was
Women‟s sexual arousal 13
someone they trusted, rather than a stranger, or a partner in whom they lacked trust. Factors or
situations were also seen as having variable effects on sexual arousal depending on whether they
one-night stand.
Although the themes that emerged varied across groups, particularly different age groups,
certain themes were raised in all of the groups. These consistent themes included: feelings about
one‟s body; negative consequences of sexual activity (e.g., concern about reputation, pregnancy);
feeling desired and accepted by a sexual partner; feeling “used” by a sexual partner; and negative
mood.
Feeling comfortable and positive about one‟s body was frequently mentioned as a factor
that would facilitate sexual arousal. Statements such as this one were typical:
P: If I am feeling good about myself, I mean some days I feel like I‟m really okay. My hair is
just right and everything is working and it‟s much easier for me to feel aroused when I‟m
feeling really comfortable with myself . . . it‟s not as easy to feel aroused when I‟m not
Women also discussed feeling confident and having a positive self-image as enhancers of
arousal:
P–1: If I‟m feeling unattractive, like if I‟ve gained weight or something you know . . . but if I‟ve
lost 5 pounds . . . I‟m just like wanting to take my clothes off a lot . . .
P–2: If somebody told me that I had too big a butt or big thighs, I‟d be like “sorry”. [18-24 group]
Women‟s sexual arousal 14
P: It‟s important to me to be comfortable with my body and for the other person to be
comfortable with their body and for them to be comfortable with my body. [25-45 group]
P–3: Or just feeling not accepted in any way by your partner . . .[46+ group]
Many women, particularly in the younger age groups, said that concern about reputation
P–1: He‟s really cocky and thinks he can get anyone and like I don‟t want to fall for it. There‟s
so many consequences that we have to deal with like getting pregnant and things you just
P–2: Not to mention the whole school would know, well, not in college, but like in high school,
the whole school would know about it in a day and it‟d just be like . . . and then you regret
Women talked about a “double standard,” with fear about damaging one‟s reputation as
something that only women had to worry about. One participant voiced concerns about
P: Being single and you know, wanting to be sexual with another person and thinking “okay, am
I going to be too much?” or “am I going to be not enough?” or “what are they going to think
of me because I‟m doing these things?” and you know, my thinking doesn‟t go there with
him. It‟s not like, “Oh, is he a whore because he knows how to do this or that?” I‟m pretty
much more grateful that he knows but when it comes to me, there is this concern about, you
The following statement describes feelings of ambivalence: wanting to be openly sexual but at
P: I think that like if you go out . . . and you‟re really conservative and you don‟t do all that kind
of stuff and then you see a girl who is not like that and who is out, you know, having sex with
all these men and having fun. It‟s almost like you hate her but you want to be her at the same
time. You sit there and you‟re thinking, “she‟s a slut, I can‟t believe she‟s doing that.” At the
same time, it‟s like “whoa, she‟s getting lots of attention.” [18-24 group]
There were also some women who described feeling more sexually aroused in situations where
they felt that they were being “bad” by giving in to their sexual desire:
P: One thing that turns me on is sometimes just being bad, doing things I know I shouldn‟t do.. . .
Sometimes just being able to just do what you want and give in and not care what society or
anybody‟s thinking, that‟s exciting to me because you . . . can‟t be thinking about what your
friends would say if they knew or what your mom would think of you. [African-American
group]
One of the recurrent themes in the younger age groups was that many women felt the
need to “put on the brakes” to stop themselves from being aroused. Women talked about
knowing that they would be aroused in a given situation but not allowing themselves to “go
there,” for a wide range of reasons, including being in a current relationship, concerns about
reputation, lack of trust/safety issues, the person being an “inappropriate” partner, and concerns
about pregnancy. Women discussed this as being something that was their responsibility, rather
P: There‟s the typical whether or not to put on the brakes. It depends on who you‟re with, you
know, but you always have to make the decision because you know that the guy‟s not going
Women in the younger groups also talked about the ease with which they could switch on and
P: There‟s so much control . . . it‟s like you can almost [say] “yeah, I‟m interested but no, maybe
not, and then you completely forget about it if it‟s not really that interesting to you. It‟s
almost like you can turn it off and on if you want to. [18-24 group]
P–1: . . . with girls I think it‟s like you might have some inclinations and then you‟re like, “wait
a minute, you can‟t do that,” you‟re in a relationship or that guy‟s a loser . . . and all of a
sudden you just [think] “okay, fine, forget it, I can‟t. That‟s a bad idea,” and just walk away
from it. It‟s a lot easier for a girl to walk away from a situation.
P–2: You can just shut it off like you said. [18-24 group]
Unwanted Pregnancy/Contraception
Fears about unwanted pregnancy were described as having a very negative impact on
sexual arousal, particularly if one‟s partner did not share these concerns:
P: Unwanted pregnancy is a big turn off and if you‟re with a partner who seems unconcerned
about that, then it really feels like a danger. It feels like a hazard, you know, I mean more
than just if you‟re with a steady partner and you‟re both concerned about it. [46+ group]
Women also discussed how a partner‟s shared concern about contraception could serve to buffer
P: Contraceptives are definitely at least a . . . barrier to arousal at times. The more that my
partner is comfortable with the use of whatever we‟re using, that really helps . . . and it‟s less
very arousing:
Women‟s sexual arousal 17
P: It is very arousing to me to have someone verbally and physically appreciate my body. [25-45
group]
P: I like it when they caress not only like your body parts that get sexually aroused but just like
your arms and because it feels like he‟s encompassing you and appreciating your whole body.
[African-American group]
Many women talked about how their arousal was increased with partners who seemed particularly
interested in them as individual women, rather than someone that they just wanted to have sex
P: When they‟re attracted to you and it‟s like they just have to touch you and can‟t do enough
This situation was contrasted with that of feeling used by a partner, which was described as a
powerful turn-off:
P: I experienced too many times waking up facing away from him and ah, I‟m not trying to be
graphic, but just rubbing himself on me as though I could have just been any tree in the forest
. . . that would not only kill any ability that I could have ever found in the morning to be
aroused but ah . . . probably for days. There‟s a difference between caressing and using
As well as feeling desired, a recurrent theme was the importance of feeling “accepted” by
a partner; for example, a partner who was accepting of one‟s responses during sexual activity
P: If I have permission to make sound, that is much more arousing. Listening to my partner‟s
sound and my sound and having the permission is much more arousing than feeling like you
The converse of this was feeling inhibited sexually when a partner did not approve of a woman‟s
P: Even with my second husband, and we were together 16 years, he was not accepting of my
sexual responses. . . . I make a lot of noise or [with] my favorite way to orgasm, he felt left
out. . . . That was just the beginning of just really shutting down. [46+ group]
Women also discussed the importance of a partner feeling comfortable with their sexual past, as
P–1: I can‟t imagine being turned on by someone who would be morally condemning my past.
More general criticism by a partner was also said to have a negative effect on sexual arousal:
P–1: . . . guys being cruel and saying, ah, any criticism or something negative, that‟s just a huge
turn-off.
P–3: Yeah.
P–4: It doesn‟t even have to be . . . like even if it‟s something that happened earlier that day. [25-
45 group]
but the importance to their own arousal of how a partner approached them was a key theme:
P: I want to say his “game” . . . you know, how the man approached you, how did he get me to
talk to him longer than like, five minutes? How did he get me to be interested in him and the
women:
Women‟s sexual arousal 19
P-1: It could be because I was raised Catholic and everybody jokes to me, comes up behind me,
you know “I‟m not responsible” then, and he comes up behind me and puts his arms around
my waist and it‟s like, well “it‟s not my fault.” If they‟re going to take me from behind, it‟s
not my fault.
A potential turn-off was a partner who was too “polite” or who asked for sex:
P: If somebody asked me to do something. I hate that. Like, “will you go down on me?” and stuff
and like blatantly ask me . . . It will eventually get there, they don‟t have to ask me, but like
Although being able to communicate about sex with a partner was often seen as positive,
particularly in the older age groups, a partner verbally “asking” for sex was widely regarded as a
turn-off:
P–1: My husband, as long as we‟ve met . . . he‟s just a very polite young man and he just would,
you know, while we are in the throes of sexual passion, he would just say “May I have sex?”
or something like that, and I wish [he] wouldn‟t ask. That‟s a turn-off.
P–3: Even now . . . he‟ll say something like . . . “Well, tonight can we have sex?” or something
like that, and I‟m like “Why don‟t you just come and you know, kiss me and like that.”
P–5: Exactly.
Many women mentioned that they were less aroused when partners did not spend long
enough on foreplay, were not enthusiastic during sexual interaction, or were not attentive to their
sexual needs. In particular, making genital contact too fast, or what some of the younger
P–1: When they grab, that turns me off. Yeah, I mean if they sort of grab for bits of you . . .
P–3: Yeah.
P–4: I get really turned off if people make genital contact too fast.
P–5: Yeah.
P–7: Right . . . it‟s very disappointing, you know . . . it‟s like, let‟s build this up, let‟s really
In addition to making genital contact too quickly, sexual activity that ended quickly because a
P–1: Sometimes they get this like stare and their tongue is kind of half out and, you know, and
you‟re like “Whoa, are you there?” The next thing you know, they‟re done.
P–2: Yeah, and they don‟t want to do anything, they just kind of roll over. [18-24 group]
In contrast, the opposite situation was described where women became more aroused by partners
P: I‟m a lot slower than my husband in terms of reaching arousal and if he takes the time and
gives . . . and you know, goes that slow, that‟s very arousing to me where if I feel I have to go fast
P–1: Something that really puts the brakes on for me is if I can detect that the person that I‟m
having intercourse with is in it more for himself and it‟s not a fair balance. Like if I feel like
Women‟s sexual arousal 21
he‟s the one receiving more and I‟m not getting an equal amount of pleasure then that just
halts everything. Like if I perform oral and I want it too and he says “No, I don‟t want to do
that.”
P–2: That‟s interesting because . . . I definitely feel happy just performing oral sex and feeling
like I‟m giving that and that makes me feel really good and he can give it another way.
P–3: But he is giving it to you in a different way so you‟re still kind of balancing.
P–1: I felt . . . we started out sort of mutually initiating our sexual encounter and then I felt like I
P–3: Yes. And I was really put off and just got very angry and it was over. Get out of here,
Negative Mood
Discussions about the effects of negative mood on sexual arousal suggested that the
relationship between mood and sexuality is complex. Effects varied as a function of the
particular mood (e.g., anxiety, depression, or anger), as well as the reasons for the negative
mood, and other contextual factors. A number of participants talked about experiencing
P–2: Actually, sex is the greatest stress relief so I‟d much rather attack somebody when I‟m
Some women made a distinction between being less interested in sex with a partner, but being
P–1: When feeling really anxious, I would probably not be at all interested in sex, but . . .
conceivably interested in masturbation just as a distraction, as a relaxant, but not wanting to have
to think about someone else and take care of their needs too.
Depression and anger were more often described as having a negative effect on sexuality.
P: If you‟re very upset with your intended sexual partner, if you‟re very upset with him about
something, there‟s no way that you are going to be aroused. [46+ group]
Women talked about wanting physical affection but not sex when they were feeling depressed;
however, there were also women who talked about negative mood having little effect on their
sexual arousal:
P: I‟m about to go to sleep and I realize you‟re laying next to me, then the arousal comes no
matter if I was angry or I was happy before I got in the bed. … If I‟m already initially attracted to
you and we already have that type of relationship and we lay down next to each other, it‟s just
something about that person lying next to me, that arousal will come instantly no matter what
DISCUSSION
Three general topics related to women‟s sexual arousal were explored using focus
groups: (1) cues to sexual arousal; (2) the distinction/overlap of sexual interest and sexual
arousal; and (3) factors related to excitation and inhibition of sexual arousal. These findings add
support for the use of focus group methodology to obtain information on sensitive topics (Seal et
al., 1998; Wilkinson, 1999). Our experience was that women expressed a wide range of thoughts
and opinions during the group discussions and also reported that the experience was both
positive and educational. Clearly, our sample was self-selected and thus likely comprised of
women who felt comfortable discussing sexuality. These limitations apply to most research on
Women‟s sexual arousal 23
sexuality and other sensitive topics. A strength of the study is that such qualitative data give
voice to women‟s views on the process of sexual arousal that can help inform future research and
clinical work.
Arousal has traditionally been considered synonymous with lubrication (Bartlik &
Goldberg, 2000), and the current DSM-IV definition of Female Sexual Arousal Disorder is the
Association, 2000). Our data on how women “recognize” that they are sexually aroused support
the view that a wide range of physical (both genital and non-genital), psychological, and
behavioral changes characterize women‟s sexual arousal (Basson, 2002). Lubrication is only one
of the physiological changes that women experience when they are sexually aroused, and not a
necessary condition for women to report that they are sexually aroused. Other researchers have
responses (Basson, 2002; Everaerd, Laan, Both, & Van der Velde, 2000). Interestingly, however,
the importance of non-genital somatic changes to women‟s experience of sexual arousal has been
little studied. In contrast, there is a large body of research on the relation between genital
response and subjective arousal (Everaerd et al., 2000), showing lower concordance between
genital response and subjective reports of arousal in women, compared with men. It is possible
that women‟s ratings of arousal are more influenced by their state of general arousal, rather than
genital response. An early study by Levi (1969), which compared urinary adrenaline and
noradrenaline excretion in men and women after exposure to erotic films, is consistent with this
possibility. Catecholamine changes in women, but not in men, were positively and significantly
Consistent with previous studies (Ellison, 2000; Frank, Anderson, & Rubenstein, 1978;
The Working Group for a New View of Women‟s Sexual Problems, 2001), our qualitative data
support the observation that women do not usually separate sexual “interest” from “arousal.”
Beck and Bozman (1991), in a study of college-aged males and females, suggested that
nonprofessionals do not draw the same distinction between sexual desire and arousal as
researchers do. Beck and Bozman also reported significant correlations between desire and
arousal, which led them to suggest that sexual desire and arousal may be “two facets of the same
process within the sexual response” (p. 454). In contrast with males, studies of clinical samples
of women have also reported a considerable overlap between the dimensions of sexual desire and
arousal in women (Rosen et al., 2000). Some researchers have theorized that sexual “desire”
may reflect early arousal processes (Everaerd et al., 2000). Yet, desire and arousal continue to be
defined, and studied, as independent constructs, perhaps primarily to maintain current diagnostic
We designed the study to explore the concepts of SE and SI, with the goal of the
respond with SE/SI. We were concerned that the existing questionnaire (SIS/SES) (Janssen et al.,
2002a), developed for use with men, might not adequately assess factors relevant to women. A
broad range of themes emerged in the focus groups, only a subset of which are presented in
detail in this paper. Our data support the ideas put forward by The Working Group for a New
View of Women‟s Sexual Problems (2001). Inhibition often arises from relational and socio-
cultural factors as well as physical and psychological problems. Many of the themes reflected
factors that may be of particular relevance to women, and ones that are not well represented in
the current SIS/SES scales. For example, the second SIS/SES factor, SIS2 (labeled “threat of
Women‟s sexual arousal 25
performance consequences”), has 11 items that mainly relate to external threats, such as the
possibility of pregnancy or sexually transmitted disease. There are no items that cover threats
that arise from the sexual relationship or partner behavior (Janssen et al., 2002a). Yet, many
women in our study cited concerns such as being used, criticized, or rejected by partners as
We were not surprised that some of our factors (e.g., anxiety) were cited as “inhibitors”
important that the response categories and item wording allow for the fact that a given situation
e.g., the possibility of being seen or heard while having sex can be a strong “turn-on” for some
women, and a definite “turn-off” for others. Whether SE and SI are best conceptualized as
orthogonal vs. bipolar factors remains to be established. Our findings do suggest that the
concepts of sexual inhibition and excitation were meaningful and “made sense” to women. Also,
research using the male-based SIS/SES questionnaire, with both male and female samples, has
Research with adolescents (Fine, 1988; Tolman, 2002) has provided evidence of teenage
girls‟ need to avoid becoming sexually aroused in situations where the “costs” are too high. Our
qualitative data suggest that this is something that may be important beyond adolescence. Many
women in the younger 18-24 groups talked about the need to stop themselves from becoming
aroused, or to “put on the brakes,” because of concerns about reputation, lack of trust in partner,
safety issues, etc. Younger women also seemed more likely to cite partner-related themes as
important influences on their sexual arousal. For example, grooming, dress, and personality were
frequent topics of discussion in the younger age groups, whereas themes in our “self” category
Women‟s sexual arousal 26
(e.g. mood, physical state) were more often raised in the older groups. Research on
Although the primary purpose of our study was to gather data that would inform
questionnaire development, qualitative data such as these can also be useful in generating
hypotheses for further study as well as informing our concepts of sexual arousal processes. Our
data support a growing concern that current models of sexual arousal and dysfunction may be too
genitally focused, make distinctions between interest and arousal that do not reflect the
experiences of many women, and minimize the numerous factors that can affect arousal. In
conclusion, we would agree with Heiman (2001), who argued for “systematically gathered
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was funded (in part) by a grant from the Lilly Centre for Women‟s Health.
We thank Leah Davis, Carol McCord, Sarah Upchurch, and Kari Burns for help with moderating
the focus groups, and Danielle Wiser for assistance with data analysis.
Women‟s sexual arousal 28
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
Bancroft, J. (1999). Central inhibition of sexual response in the male: A theoretical perspective.
Bancroft, J., & Janssen, E. (2000). The dual control model of male sexual response: A theoretical
Bancroft, J., & Janssen, E. (2001). Psychogenic erectile dysfunction in the era of
Bancroft, J., Janssen, E., Strong, D., Carnes, L., & Long J. S. (2003). Sexual risk taking in gay
men: The relevance of sexual arousability, mood, and sensation seeking. Archives of
Bancroft, J., Janssen, E, Carnes, L, Strong, D. A., Goodrich, D., & Long, J. S. (in press). Sexual
risk taking in young heterosexual men: The relevance of personality factors. Journal of
Sex Research.
Bartlik, B., & Goldberg, J. (2000). Female sexual arousal disorder. In S. R. Leiblum & R. C.
Rosen, (Eds). Principles and practice of sex therapy, (3rd ed., pp. 85-117). New York:
Guilford Press.
Basson, R. (2002). Are our definitions of women‟s desire, arousal and sexual pain disorder too
Women‟s sexual arousal 29
broad and our definition of orgasmic disorder too narrow? Journal of Sex and Marital
Beck, J. G. (1994). Hypoactive sexual desire disorder: An overview. Journal of Consulting and
Beck, J. G., Bozman, A. W., & Qualtrough, T. (1991). The experience of sexual desire:
Bjorklund, D. F., & Kipp, K. (1996). Parental investment theory and gender differences in the
Byers, P.Y., Zeller, R. A., & Byers, B. D. (2001). Focus group methods. In M. W. Wiederman &
B. E. Whitley (Eds.), Handbook for conducting research on human sexuality (pp. 173-
Carpenter, D. L., Janssen, E., & Graham, C. A. (2003). Gender differences in the factor structure
Fine, M. (1988). Sexuality, schooling, and adolescent females: The missing discourse of desire.
Frank, E., Anderson, C., & Rubenstein, D. (1978). Frequency of sexual dysfunction in “normal”
Everaerd, W., Laan, E., Both, S., & van der Velde, J. (2000). Female sexuality. In L. Szuchman
Heiman, J. R. (2001). Sexual desire in human relationships. In W. Everaerd, E. Laan, & S. Both.
(Eds.), Sexual appetite, desire and motivation: Energetics of the sexual system (pp. 117-
Women‟s sexual arousal 30
Janssen, E., Vorst, H., Finn, P., & Bancroft, J. (2002a). The Sexual Inhibition (SIS) and Sexual
Excitation (SES) Scales: I. Measuring sexual inhibition and excitation proneness in men.
Janssen, E., Vorst, H., Finn, P., & Bancroft, J. (2002b). The Sexual Inhibition (SIS) and Sexual
Knodel, J. (1993). The design and analysis of focus group studies: A practical approach. In D. L.
Morgan (Ed.), Successful focus groups: Advancing the state of the art (pp. 35-50).
visual sexual stimulation in human females and males. Psychosomatic Medicine, 31, 251-
268.
Lief, H. I. (1977). What‟s new in sex research? Inhibited sexual desire. Medical Aspects of
Morgan, D. L. (1997). Focus groups as qualitative research (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Rosen, R., Brown, C., Heiman, J., Leiblum, S., Meston, C., Shabsigh, D., et al. (2000). The
Female Sexual Function Index (FSFI): A multidimensional self–report instrument for the
assessment of female sexual function. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 26, 191–208.
Seal, D. W., Bogart, L. M., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (1998). Small group dynamics: The utility of focus
Practice, 2, 253-266.
The Working Group for a New View of Women‟s Sexual Problems. (2001). A new view of
women‟s sexual problems. In E. Kaschak & L. Tiefer (Eds.), A new view of women’s
Tiefer, L. (2001). General discussion. In J. Bancroft (Ed.), The role of theory in sex research (pp.
Tolman, D. (2002). Dilemma of desire: Teenage girls and sexuality. Cambridge MA: Harvard
University Press.
Wilkinson, S. (1999). Focus groups. A feminist method. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 23,
221-244.
Women‟s sexual arousal 32
Table I.
n %
Race/ethnic group
Asian 2 (2.5)
Black 14 (17.5)
Hispanic 2 (2.5)
White 57 (71.3)
Other 5 (6.3)
Marital status
Married 18 (22.5)
Separated/divorced 15 (18.8)
Widowed 3 (3.8)
Sexual orientation
Heterosexual 50 (62.5)
Bisexual 8 (10.0)
Lesbian 19 (23.8)
Uncertain 3 (3.8)
Religion
Protestant 21 (26.3)
Catholic 12 (15.0)
Women‟s sexual arousal 33
Jewish 3 (3.8)
Other 29 (36.3)
None 15 (18.8)
Employed
Full-time 24 (30.0)
Part-time 38 (47.5)
or university
Yes 77 (96.3)
No 3 (3.8)
Relationship status
SD 8.6
Range .67-54
Women‟s sexual arousal 34
APPENDIX
Self
Psychological state
Mood/emotional state
Physical state
Emotional „openness‟/vulnerability
Partner
Psychological characteristics
Comfort with body; personality; comfort with own or partner‟s sexual past;
Attractiveness; smell
Appropriateness
Style of approach/initiation
Relationship dynamics/interaction
Relationship quality
Relationship stage/phase
Partner attractiveness
Partner skill
Partner inexperience
Partner enthusiasm
Partner acceptance
Partner attentiveness
Variation
Equality
Communication
Risk
Power dynamics
Timing
Women‟s sexual arousal 36
Imagery/fantasy/thoughts
Interruptions/physical
Comfort/discomfort
Masturbation
Setting
Romantic or sexual
Liberating
External stimuli
Internal stimuli
Fantasy/imagery
Hormones
Fertility
Contraception
STDs/HIV