ENG File - Scientific Thinking
ENG File - Scientific Thinking
ENG File - Scientific Thinking
developing observational instruments, formulating and modifying theories, deducing consequences from theories, making
predictions from theories, testing theories, inducing regularities and invariants from data, discovering theoretical constructs, and
others.
—Simon, Langley, & Bradshaw, 1981, p. 2
Nearly 40 years after Einstein’s remarkably insightful statement, Francis Crick offered a similar perspective: that
great discoveries in science result not from extraordinary mental processes, but rather from rather common ones. The
greatness of the discovery lies in the thing discovered.
I think what needs to be emphasized about the discovery of the double helix is that the path to it was, scientifically speaking,
fairly commonplace. What was important was not the way it was discovered, but the object discovered—the structure of DNA
itself. (Crick , 1988, p. 67; emphasis added)
Under this view, scientific thinking involves the same general-purpose cognitive processes—such as induction,
deduction, analogy, problem solving, and causal reasoning—that humans apply in non- scientific domains. These
processes are covered in several different chapters of this handbook: Rips, Smith, & Medin, Chapter 11 on
induction; Evans, Chapter 8 on deduction; Holyoak, Chapter 13 on analogy; Bassok & Novick, Chapter 21 on
problem solving; and Cheng & Buehner, Chapter 12 on causality. One might question the claim that the highly
specialized procedures associated with doing science in the “real world” can be understood by investigating the
thinking processes used in laboratory studies of the sort described in this volume. However, when the focus is on major
scientific breakthroughs, rather than on the more routine, incremental progress in a field, the psychology of problem
solving provides a rich source of ideas about how such discoveries might occur. As Simon and his colleagues put it:
It is understandable, if ironic, that ‘normal’ science fits . . . the description of expert problem solving, while ‘revolutionary’
science fits the description of problem solving by novices. It is understandable because scientific activity, particularly at the
revolutionary end of the continuum, is concerned with the discovery of new truths, not with the application of truths that are
already well-known . . . it is basically a journey into unmapped terrain. Consequently, it is mainly characterized, as is novice
problem solving, by trial-and-error search. The search may be highly selective—but it reaches its goal only after many halts,
turnings, and back- trackings. (Simon, Langley, & Bradshaw,
1981, p. 5)
The research literature on scientific thinking can be roughly categorized according to the two types of scientific
thinking listed in the opening paragraph of this chapter: (1) One category focuses on thinking that directly involves
scientific content. Such research ranges from studies of young children reasoning about the sun-moon-earth system
(Vosniadou & Brewer, 1992) to college students reasoning about chemical equilibrium (Davenport, Yaron, Klahr, &
Koedinger, 2008), to research that investigates collaborative problem solving by world-class researchers in real-world
molecular biology labs (Dunbar,
1995). (2) The other category focuses on “general” cognitive processes, but it tends to do so by analyzing people’s
problem-solving behavior when they are presented with relatively complex situations that involve the integration and
coordination of several different types of processes, and that are designed to capture some essential features of
“real-world” science in the psychology laboratory (Bruner, Goodnow, & Austin, 1956; Klahr & Dunbar, 1988;
Mynatt, Doherty, & Tweney, 1977).
There are a number of overlapping research traditions that have been used to investigate scientific thinking. We
will cover both the history of research on scientific thinking and the different approaches that have been used,
highlighting common themes that have emerged over the past 50 years of research.