Boutrel 2019
Boutrel 2019
Boutrel 2019
com/scientificreports
Alcohol use disorder, which includes the spectrum of drinking behaviors and consequences ranging from risky
use to heavy dependence, has been linked to a wide array of health and social problems1. Unhealthy alcohol use
accounts for an estimated 85,000 deaths at an economic cost of nearly $250 billion annually in the United States2.
In 2012, approximately 3.3 million deaths worldwide, representing 5.9% of all global deaths, and 139 million dis-
ability adjusted life years (5.1% of the global disease burden) were attributed to alcohol use3. At the clinical level,
alcohol use disorder, defined as an excessive ethanol seeking and taking with continued consumption despite
harmful consequences4, is associated with the occurrence of around 200 co-morbid diseases, the most nota-
ble amongst them being cardiovascular diseases, gastrointestinal diseases, various cancers and neuropsychiatric
disorders3, 5, 6. Meanwhile, risk drinking, defined as an average of 15 or more standard drinks per week, or 5 or
more on an occasion for men (8 and 4, respectively for women) can be clinically silent yet have adverse health
and social consequences7. This clinical observation emphasizes the need to identify other criteria than abuse or
dependence to better capture the mild to intermediate severity range of the disorder8.
Preclinical models of excessive alcohol intake have long been studied, not only for testing and developing
effective drugs to alleviate signs of alcohol addiction, but also for understanding the neurobiological substrates of
alcoholism9. Many strategies used the knockout of one specific gene10, 11, or studied alcohol-preferring strains12, 13,
and used continuous or intermittent access to alcohol to address behavioral responses to alcohol intoxication14–16.
With one core symptom of drug addiction being the high risk of relapse after a period of abstinence, the extinc-
tion/reinstatement model has long been used for assessing the neurobiological mechanisms underlying drug
seeking behaviors17–19 with the aim of screening effective drug treatments to prevent relapsing behaviors.
One limitation of this approach is the lack of consideration for individual vulnerability since all subjects are
treated with similar drug doses to assess the pharmacological impact of those drugs to prevent cue-, stress- or
priming-induced reinstatement of drug seeking behaviors20. Ethanol vapor model has been used for several
decades for inducing ethanol dependence21. Although the model has good predictive validity in deciphering
mechanisms and potential treatments for the human condition, it has rather poor face validity because animals
are intoxicated independently of their will22. While most studies still defend pharmacology-centered views and
1
Center for Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, Lausanne University Hospital, Lausanne,
Switzerland. 2King Abdullah University of Science and Technology (KAUST), Thuwal, Saudi Arabia. 3Brain Mind
Institute, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne, Switzerland. 4Division of Adolescent and
Child Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, Lausanne University Hospital, Lausanne, Switzerland. 5Toxicology and
Forensic Chemistry Unit, University Center of Legal Medicine, Lausanne, Geneva, Switzerland. Correspondence and
requests for materials should be addressed to B.B. (email: [email protected])
Figure 1. Flowchart for operant training for ethanol self-administration (Sacc: Saccharine concentration
expressed as percentage in weight/volume, E: Ethanol concentration expressed as percentage in weight/volume).
models that do not really capture the inter-individual vulnerability to lose control over alcohol consumption20, a
few studies depict the behavioral traits predicting alcohol drinking in outbred rats16, 23, 24, among which the impact
of social25–27 or traumatic stress28 have been shown to promote excessive alcohol drinking.
Overall, the scientific literature reports tremendous amounts of publications deciphering the neurobiologi-
cal mechanisms underpinning the brain adaptations responsible for alcohol dependence. However, the trigger-
ing mechanisms by which a healthy individual, prone to become alcoholic, slowly loses control over alcohol
consumption remains to be fully elucidated29. The inability to control drug taking in general and conditioned
responses in particular, is a complex brain disorder that affects the most vulnerable individuals and worsens with
recurring drug consumption. Further, understanding the heterogeneity in the behavioral characteristic of patients
with alcohol use disorders is warranted for developing personalized treatments30, notably in the light of the over-
all modest effect size for approved and experimental pharmacotherapies31.
Considering that treatments, on an average, produce limited improvements in ‘alcoholics’, one could expect
considerable clinical benefits in categorizing subpopulations of patients according to their biology32. Because
alcohol misuse is a complex neurobiological disorder, investigations based on one preclinical model operation-
alizing one diagnostic criterion are rather challenging. Among the number of unresolved methodological and
conceptual issues that need to be carefully considered and addressed to advance the field, neuroscience-based
clinical phenotypes could advance genetic studies of alcoholism etiology and treatment. Over the last decade,
converging evidence demonstrated that preclinical research could successfully integrate several dimensions of
addiction following the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual33. This research has identified specific phenotypes and
markers that underlie individual vulnerability to develop addiction-like behaviors in rats consuming cocaine34.
Not only this approach revealed a large spectrum of severity for cocaine abuse in rats, but it also allowed to better
depict the neurobiological adaptations specifically occurring in the brain of rats developing the behavioral hall-
marks of cocaine addiction35–42.
The recently revised DSM5 criteria defines alcohol misuse as a single disorder, with a broad spectrum from
mild to severe conditions, whereas former DSM-IV emphasized the distinction between alcohol abuse and alco-
hol dependence. The current consensus acknowledges that DSM5 criteria advantageously represent the clinical
experience33, 43. Taking cue from past observations inspired by clinical experience34, we report here a novel ani-
mal model addressing alcohol use disorder. In brief, three criteria of the DSM have been operationalized in rats:
(1) inability to cease drug seeking during signaled unavailability, (2) higher motivation for drug intake, and (3)
continued drug use despite adverse consequences. Rats identified as positive for all these criteria, defined as vul-
nerable, developed uncontrolled conditioned responses and enhanced alcohol use as compared to rats failing to
meet any of these criteria, accordingly named resilient.
Results
At the end of the baseline self-administration period (80 sessions during which animals were trained under a
fixed ratio 1, time out 4 sec to get 0.1 mL of 10% w/v ethanol, see Fig. 1), rats underwent a procedure for screening
evidence for addiction-like behavior35. A rat was considered positive for an addiction-like criterion when its score
was in the 66th to 99th percentile of the distribution. Hence, of the total 59 rats, we obtained 4 groups, 25 rats
with 0 criterion (42.4%), 14 rats with 1 criterion (23.7%), 13 rats with 2 criteria (22%) and 7 rats with 3 criteria
(11.9%) (Fig. 2A).
Persistence in lever pressing during the no-drug period. The mean number of lever presses for
each group were 5.07 ± 0.55 (0crit), 7.78 ± 0.92 (1crit), 11.28 ± 1.6 (2crit) and 16.04 ± 1.17 (3crit), respectively
(Fig. 2B). A one-way ANOVA showed a significant difference between groups (F3,55 = 18.345, p < 0.0001). Post
hoc Tukey’s tests revealed that, during the no-drug period, the 3crit rats exhibited higher lever presses compared
to all the other groups (vs 0crit p < 0.0001, vs 1crit p < 0.001, vs 2crit p < 0.048). The 2 criteria rats differed from
0 criteria rats (p < 0.001) but were comparable to 1 criteria rats (p = 0.092). The 0 and 1 criterion rats had similar
performances (p = 0.157).
Increased motivation for alcohol seeking and drinking in an effortful condition. The respec-
tive breaking points were 23.17 ± 1.76 (0crit), 34.19 ± 3.65 (1crit), 48.2 ± 2.24 (2crit) and 46.09 ± 2.90 (3crit)
(Fig. 2C). One-way ANOVA showed a statistically significant difference among the groups (F3,55 = 22.217,
p < 0.0001). Post hoc Tukey’s tests revealed that the 3crit rats displayed an increased motivation for ethanol
seeking compared to 0crit rats (p < 0.0001). Despite a large difference, the breaking point displayed by 3crit rats
failed to be statistically different from that of 1crit animals (p = 0.058), while 2crit and 3crit rats exhibited similar
performances (p = 0.96). The 2crit rats displayed a higher breaking point compared to 0crit and 1crit rats (vs 0crit
p < 0.001, vs 1 crit p = 0.003). Finally, even the 1crit rats exhibited a higher breaking point compared to the 0crit
rats (p = 0.008).
Figure 2. Identification of Addiction-like behaviors in rats. (A) Percentage of the total population (n = 59) of
rats positive for zero (0Crit), one (1Crit), two (2Crit), or three addiction-like criteria (3Crit). (B) Persistence
in drug seeking, as measured by number of lever presses (±SE) during the no-drug period. (C) Motivation
for the drug, as measured by the breaking point (±SE) during a progressive-ratio schedule of reinforcement.
(D) Resistance to punishment, as measured by the number of lever presses (±SE) when alcohol delivery was
associated with a mild electric foot shock (0.22 mA). (E) Average number of lever presses (±SE) for saccharine
0.2% (sessions 5–7). (F) Average number of lever presses (±SE) for ethanol 10% (sessions 24–27). (G) Average
number of lever presses (±SE) for ethanol 10% (sessions 44–47). (H) Average number of lever presses (±SE) for
ethanol 10% (sessions 77–80). *Significant as compared to 0crit, #Significant as compared to 0crit and 1crit, and
@
Significant as compared to 0crit, 1crit and 2crit, ^Significant as compared to 0crit rats during session 24–27.
Resistance to punishment. The mean number of lever presses for each group were 7.17 ± 0.5 (0crit),
11.93 ± 2.53 (1crit), 16.44 ± 2.33 (2crit) and 24.62 ± 3.99 (3crit rats) (Fig. 2D). One-way ANOVA showed a sta-
tistically significant difference among the groups (F3,55 = 12.53, p < 0.0001). Post hoc Tukey’s tests indicated that
2 and 3crit rats had similar resistance to punishment (p = 0.084) but 3crit rats accepted more shocks than 0crit
(p < 0.0001) and 1crit rats (p = 0.002). Whereas 2crit rats were not different from 1crit rats (p = 0.372), they
exhibited a clear-cut resistance to punishment as compared to the 0crit rats (p = 0.002). Finally, the 0crit and 1crit
rats manifested similar performance in resisting to punishment (p = 0.207).
Once the four groups of rats were identified, we analyzed a posteriori individual performance during the
baseline conditioning to address the evolution of alcohol seeking and taking over the course of the instrumental
conditioning (Fig. 2E,F,G,H). A Pearson’s Correlation analysis was done between the three addiction-like behav-
iors, the active lever presses towards the end of 0.2% saccharine training (when rats displayed the highest lever
pressing activity, Fig. 2E) and at three different time points of the ethanol operant conditioning (Fig. 2F,G,H).
Remarkably, the three criteria scores were all positively correlated with the average active lever presses at the end
of ethanol training only (session 77–80) (Table 1). It is important to note that whereas most of the rats maintained
constant lever pressing behavior, 0-crit rats displayed reduced conditioned responses over time, reflecting a grow-
ing disinterest for lever pressing (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Figure 1). Rats exhibited similar weight gain during
the 80 sessions of ethanol intake (Supplementary Figure 2A and B), which excludes a body size-induced bias
in alcohol consumption. Interestingly, while all rats exhibited similar (and rather high) levels of ethanol intake
after 25 sessions of ethanol operant conditioning (around 2 g/kg within 30 min), 2- and 3-crit rats consumed 1.5
times as much amount of alcohol after 80 sessions of ethanol self-administration (roughly 0.9 versus 0.6 g/kg,
Supplementary Figure 2C and D), confirming that the development of excessive conditioned responses to alcohol
requires a prolonged exposure to alcohol context.
Addiction score. The addiction score was calculated as the sum of the standardized scores of each of the
addiction-like criteria44. The addiction scores were −1.96 ± 0.2, −0.15 ± 0.25, 1.98 ± 0.26 and 3.64 ± 0.6, respec-
tively (Fig. 3A). A one-way ANOVA demonstrated a significant main effect for addiction score among the groups
(F3,55 = 71.33, p < 0.0001). Not only Tukey’s post-hoc tests revealed that all addiction scores statistically differed
one from each other, but a Pearson’s analysis revealed a positive correlation between the three addiction-like crite-
ria and the addiction score confirming that the addiction score is highly representative of the three addiction-like
criteria (Persistence in drug seeking during the no-drug period: r = 0.794; p < 0.001, Excessive motivation for
alcohol seeking: r = 0.818; p < 0.001, Resistance to punishment: r = 0.641; p < 0.001).
Average active lever Average active lever Average active lever Average active lever
presses for saccharine presses for ethanol presses for ethanol presses for ethanol
only (session 5–7) only (session 24–27) only (session 44–47) only (session 77–80)
Persistence in drug seeking
−0.180 0.267 0.208 0.378*
during the no-drug period
Excessive motivation
for alcohol seeking and 0.25 0.227 0.385* 0.649*
drinking
Resistance to punishment 0.373* −0.055 0.102 0.362*
Table 1. Correlational analysis between the three addiction-like criteria and active lever presses measured
during 4 key phases of the operant conditioning. *p < 0.05 Significant using Pearson’s correlational analysis (2
tailed).
Strikingly, the addiction score also correlated positively with active lever presses for ethanol towards the mid-
dle (session 44–47) (Pearson’s correlation: r = 0.305, p = 0.018) and end of the training (session 77–80) (Pearson’s
correlation: r = 0.609, p < 0.0001). But it did not correlate with active lever presses for saccharine or lever presses
for ethanol at the beginning of the training (session 24–27).
Factor analysis. A factor analysis was conducted for the three addiction-like criteria to determine
whether they loaded on the same underlying construct. The eigenvalue was kept as 1. Remarkably, all the
three addiction-like criteria loaded on one factor only (Persistence in drug seeking during the no-drug period:
r = 0.819, Excessive motivation for alcohol seeking: r = 0.818, Resistance to punishment: r = 0.641) accounting for
58% of the variance, further supporting that the three addiction-like criteria are measures of a single underlying
factor that represents persistent/compulsive drug use.
As can be seen above, 0crit and 1crit rats are rather comparable to each other, with negative addiction scores,
while 2crit and 3crit are quite similar, with both displaying positive addiction scores. Hence, we clubbed together
the data of criteria 0 and 1 rats (addiction trait negative or resilient = Group R) and the data of criteria 2 and 3 rats
(addiction trait positive or vulnerable = Group V).
Comparison of anxiety trait. To evaluate whether anxiety-like behavior could represent a predictive trait
for future addictive-like behavior, we analyzed a posteriori individual performance on the elevated plus maze,
measured right after the end of the saccharine fading procedure. The percent time spent in the open arm for
Resilient and Vulnerable rats was 30.36 ± 4.58 and 11.99 ± 2.34, respectively. A series of 8 rats observed on the
same day was lost due to technical failure. An unpaired T test showed that the Vulnerable rats spent significantly
lesser time in the open arm (t(49) = 2.65, p = 0.0106), suggesting an anxious profile, most likely representing a
predisposing characteristic for developing alcohol use disorder in rats (Fig. 3B).
Evaluation of alcohol consumption vs. saccharine fading in a two-choice paradigm. The rats
were provided a choice between 10% w/v ethanol and fading concentrations of saccharine starting from 0.2%
w/v saccharin to finally water. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted with the two groups as
the between subject factor, and the ethanol/saccharine fading procedure as the within subject factor. The anal-
ysis revealed a significant group effect (F8,48 = 4.047, p = 0.001), a significant effect of the choice procedure
(F6,48 = 93.77, p < 0.0001) and a significant effect of the group x choice interaction (F186,48=1.61, p=0.0267). Post
hoc analyses revealed that the Vulnerable rats significantly preferred ethanol compared to Resilient ones for sac-
charine concentrations lower than 0.0125% (Fig. 3C).
Finally, confirming that the addiction score is highly representative, a Pearson’s correlation analysis showed a
linear positive correlation between the addiction score and 10% w/v ethanol vs water choice, which was statisti-
cally significant (r = 0.331, p = 0.037).
Evaluation of blood alcohol levels elimination in vulnerable versus resilient rats. In a last set of
experiments, we measured blood alcohol levels (BAL) in a subset of vulnerable and resilient rats, at 15, 30, 60 and
180 min after ethanol gavage (1 g/kg). A two-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant decrease in
BAL over time (F3,57 = 24, p < 0.0001), but no difference between groups (F1,17 = 0.2, p = 0.66) (Fig. 4).
Discussion
In 1990, H.A. Skinner emphasized that “Most adults in North America are either light drinkers or abstainers, so
alcohol does not cause them problems. However, a small but often highly visible minority - approximately 5% of
the adult population - show major symptoms of alcohol dependence. Between these extremes, there is a sizable
group of about 20% of the population, particularly young men, who are drinking at risk levels and have encoun-
tered some problems related to their alcohol use”45. Later, R. Saitz confirmed this observation and underscored
that “The prevalence of unhealthy use is 7 to 20 percent or more among outpatients, 30 to 40 percent among
patients in emergency departments, and 50 percent among patients with trauma”1. These two observations high-
lighted the broad spectrum of alcohol consumption levels, and posed the question about “problem drinkers” in
the general population who are not manifesting major symptoms of alcohol dependence but are drinking at levels
that increase risks for medical and psychosocial consequences45.
Figure 3. Identification of addiction-vulnerable and addiction-resilient rats. (A) The four experimental groups
were linearly ranked along the addiction score (±SE), with negative addiction scores for 0crit and 1crit rats,
and positive addictions scores for 2crit and 3crit rats, identifying two groups of rats, the addiction resilient
(Group R, n = 39) and the addiction vulnerable (Group V, n = 20). (B) Anxiety-related behavior, as measured
by the percentage of time (±SE) spent in the open arms during a 5-min exposure to an elevated plus-maze in
resilient (n = 35) and vulnerable (n = 16) animals. (C) Comparison of alcohol versus saccharine consumption
in a two-choice paradigm. Data represent the number of lever presses for ethanol 10% in percent of total lever
presses (ethanol + saccharine). The concentration of saccharine solution was reduced (0.2 0.1, 0.05. 0.025,
0.0125, 0.0062, 0%w/v) until rats were given the choice between alcohol and tap water (Group R: n = 32, Group
V: n = 10). *Significant as compared to 0crit, #Significant as compared to 0crit and 1crit, and @Significant as
compared to 0crit, 1crit and 2crit, &Significant as compared to group R.
Understanding aspects of addiction neuroscience requires appropriate preclinical models that should be
highly representative of the clinical understanding of addiction disease. Most of the animal models used for
the past decades have resulted in incremental growth in understanding the pharmacological basis of drug
i ntox i c at i on 4 6 – 5 0 . Howe ve r, t h e c onst r u c t of d r u g s e l f - a d m i n i st r at i on i s d i f fe re nt f rom
t hat of d r u g a d d i c t i on , s i mply b e c aus e mo st of t he re c re at i ona l d r u g us e rs ke e p c on -
trol and do not encounter any signif icant problem. While it has long been thought that
re p e ate d e x p o su re to d r u g s of abu s e s h ou l d l e a d to a d d i c t i on - l i ke ph e n ot y p e s 4 8 , 5 0 , 5 1 ,
converging evidences now point out to individual vulnerabilities towards developing addiction-like state34, 52.
In line with these recent reports, our preclinical data extend clinical findings showing that addiction-like
trait development not only requires prolonged exposure to alcohol but also depends on individual
vulnerabilities.
Figure 4. Ethanol elimination rates measured in resilient (Group R, n = 12) and vulnerable (Group V, n = 7)
animals 15, 30, 60 and 180 min after ethanol administration by oral gavage (1 g/kg). Data represent blood
alcohol levels (±SE) expressed in g/kg.
By adapting a preclinical model implementing the operational definitions used in the DSM criteria35, we
focused on an individualized approach which is of paramount importance considering that all individuals have
varied risks of developing addiction. The three addiction-like criteria (see supplementary information for fur-
ther discussion on data selection) can be considered as representatives of different items in DSM 5; persistent
or uncontrolled alcohol seeking (item 2), excessive motivation (item 3 and 4), and resistance to adverse conse-
quences (item 6, 7, 8 and 9). We performed a factor analysis to assess whether one factor could explain the variety
of results on these 3 different tests; in other words, we verified whether the variables were inter-related. Strikingly,
we demonstrated that the three addiction-like criteria loaded on one factor only, revealing that addiction-like
criteria are measures of a single underlying/latent construct which we could consider being perseverative/com-
pulsive drug use. It could also refer to the concept of “loss of control-prone phenotype”44 although the neuro-
biological and psychological underpinnings of this deserves further studies to be confirmed in this model. It is
important to stress out that our procedure aims at identifying rats with excessive conditioned responses, and we
pose the question of knowing whether these rats are at risk of losing control over alcohol intake. Although all rats
exhibited rather high levels of ethanol consumption initially (around 2 g/kg shortly after the end of the saccharine
fading procedure), they stabilized their intake in the range of 0–6 to 0.9 g/kg afterwards. Importantly, the operant
conditioning lasted for more than 3 months, but daily sessions were limited to 30 min only, which is not sufficient
for triggering a binge drinking behavior that brings blood alcohol levels to 80 mg/dl and above15, 53. Our working
hypothesis did not aim at evaluating the development of addiction criteria over time. Consequently, we only meas-
ured the addiction-like behaviors after 3 months of operant conditioning considering that, according to a recent
report, intermittent access to alcohol led to the development of quinine-resistant intake after 3 months only54.
Although we have not tested continued intake despite adulteration of alcohol with the bitter tastant qui-
nine, we make the reasonable assumption, although it as to be tested, that our vulnerable rats would display
quinine-resistant alcohol intake, in contrast to resilient rats, given recent evidence suggesting the similarities
between quinine- and footshock-resistant alcohol consumption24, 55.
One criticism on this model could raise the question about a potential selection bias given that all cri-
teria are associated with lever pressing behavior. In this perspective, the underlying construct measured
here would not be nothing more than lever pressing capacity. In other words, rats that are inherently good
at pressing the lever would be selected, and these three parameters computed by us would not truly model
addiction-like trait, but motor performance. Therefore, we conducted a correlation analysis between the
three addiction-like criteria and active lever presses measured during 4 key phases of the operant condition-
ing. Remarkably, whereas rats’ behavior during saccharin training or during the initial sessions of alcohol
conditioning could not predict future addiction score, the Pearson’s correlation analysis clearly demonstrated
that higher alcohol responses predicted the severity of the addiction score after a long history of alcohol
consumption only. It is important to note that only 0-crit rats (the larger proportion of the sample) displayed
reduced conditioned responses over time, reflecting a growing disinterest for lever pressing that is not sur-
prising with aging.
The addiction score calculated as the sum of the standardized scores of each of the addiction-like criteria in
our rats resembles the addiction severity index (ASI) in humans56. The scores for each group were significantly
different from each other, and were linearly increasing from 0crit to 3crit rats. Noteworthy, the scores of 0crit and
1crit rats were negative and those of 2crit and 3crit rats were positive, further supporting our claim of clubbing
them together and naming them addiction trait negative (addiction resilient) and addiction trait positive (addic-
tion vulnerable), respectively.
Again, because one possible confounding interpretation on rats’ ability to perform highly during the screen-
ing for addiction-like criteria could have been the long lasting operant conditioning, with rats exhibiting higher
levers pressing capacities ultimately displaying the highest addiction scores, rats were given a choice between
10% w/v ethanol and decreasing concentrations of saccharine. The addiction vulnerable group showed a higher
preference for alcohol as compared to the addiction resilient group. In this perspective, whatever the amount of
effort displayed by rats, resilient animals never exhibited a significant preference for the alcohol lever, whereas
vulnerable rats started displaying a preference for ethanol with the lowest saccharine concentrations. This fur-
ther strengthens the discriminating ability of our model for identifying rats having higher propensity to develop
addiction-like traits.
We also analyzed whether the anxiety trait tested at the beginning of the experiment was observed in rats
with increased propensity to develop addiction-like traits. There was a higher incidence of anxiety trait in the
vulnerable group as compared to the resilient one. Not only we confirm that high anxiety plays an important role
in ethanol self-administration34, 57, but we also corroborate human epidemiological studies showing that anxiety
and risk of alcohol abuse are co-morbid conditions58.
Finally, we have shown that both groups of rats have similar alcohol elimination rates, suggesting that most
likely, this animal model of vulnerability to lose control over alcohol consumption does not depend on the phar-
macokinetic of ethanol and its metabolism.
However, the current set of experiments present a few limitations that need to be addressed soon. First, we
claim that our model most likely identifies animals at risk of losing control over alcohol consumption, possibly
triggering escalated intake over time. Therefore, it is important to measure alcohol intake in vulnerable and resil-
ient rats following continuous and intermittent alcohol access in home cages14, 16, 24, 53. Second, with impulsivity
being demonstrated to predict a switch to compulsive drug and palatable food taking36, 59, assessing whether
impulsive trait correlates with the alcohol addiction criteria is critical. Third, assessing whether the vulnerability
to relapse correlates with the alcohol addiction criteria is a necessary step for confirming the face validity of our
model. Finally, evaluating the effects of medication treatments known to reduce alcohol craving in each group is
compulsory for addressing the predictive validity of this model.
In conclusion, we have collected a series of observations demonstrating that only a minority of rats would
exhibit an addiction-like behavior for alcohol. By adapting to rodents a few criteria used for screening drug addic-
tion according to the DSM35, we contribute to recognize that “addiction is a progressive disorder and to some
extent iodiosyncrasic”34. Since the success rate of current treatments for alcohol use disorders is at best average,
we consider this endeavor is an original development of current methodologies, allowing preclinical studies to
investigate biobehavioral predispositions to lose control over alcohol consumption using a singular translational
approach.
Animals. Male Wistar rats (n = 59) were bred in-house in the institute’s animal facility (breeders ordered
from Charles River, France), they were approximately 7 weeks old and weighed 200–250 grams at the begin-
ning of the experiment. They were kept in reversed 12-h light/dark cycle (lights off at 8.30 am) and housed in
controlled temperature and humidity conditions. All experiments were conducted in accordance with the Swiss
National Institutional Guidelines on Animal Experimentation, and approved by the Swiss Cantonal Veterinary
Office Committee for Animal Experimentation (#1999 to Dr. Benjamin Boutrel).
Screening for addiction-like behavior. Between test sessions aiming at scoring addiction-like behaviors
(3 daily consecutive sessions each time, Supplementary Figure 3), rats underwent 2 consecutive sessions of basic
training during which they were trained again under the same baseline conditions.
Statistical analysis. Data are expressed as mean ± standard error (SE). Shapiro Wilk test was used to
ascertain the normality distribution of the data sets. Parametrical data were analyzed by one way- and two-way
ANOVAs followed by Tukey’s tests and Bonferroni correction, respectively. Factor analysis was conducted for the
three addiction-like criteria to determine if they loaded on the same underlying construct. Pearson’s correlational
analysis was used to determine the correlation between the addiction score, the three addiction parameters, 0.2%
saccharine training and ethanol training at different time points of training. Unpaired T test was used to analyze
the anxiety data. The level of significance was set at 0.05, and Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS
Statistics 23.
References
1. Saitz, R. Clinical practice. Unhealthy alcohol use. The New England journal of medicine 352, 596–607, doi:10.1056/NEJMcp042262
(2005).
2. Sacks, J. J., Gonzales, K. R., Bouchery, E. E., Tomedi, L. E. & Brewer, R. D. 2010 National and State Costs of Excessive Alcohol
Consumption. Am J Prev Med 49, e73–79, doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2015.05.031 (2015).
3. World Health Organization Global status report on alcohol and health 2014. WHO Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data.
Geneva: WHO Press (2014).
4. Koob, G. F. Neurobiological substrates for the dark side of compulsivity in addiction. Neuropharmacology 56(Suppl 1), 18–31,
doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.043 (2009).
5. Rehm, J. et al. The relationship of average volume of alcohol consumption and patterns of drinking to burden of disease: an overview.
Addiction 98, 1209–1228 (2003).
6. Dawson, D. A., Li, T. K. & Grant, B. F. A prospective study of risk drinking: at risk for what? Drug and alcohol dependence 95, 62–72,
doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.12.007 (2008).
7. Friedmann, P. D. Alcohol use in adults. The New England journal of medicine 368, 1655–1656, doi:10.1056/NEJMc1302445 (2013).
8. Saha, T. D., Chou, S. P. & Grant, B. F. Toward an alcohol use disorder continuum using item response theory: results from the
National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychological medicine 36, 931–941, doi:10.1017/
S003329170600746X (2006).
9. Tabakoff, B. & Hoffman, P. L. Animal models in alcohol research. Alcohol Res Health 24, 77–84 (2000).
10. Reich, T. et al. Genome-wide search for genes affecting the risk for alcohol dependence. Am J Med Genet 81, 207–215 (1998).
11. Crabbe, J. C. Progress With Nonhuman Animal Models of Addiction. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 77, 696–699 (2016).
12. Bell, R. L. et al. A Genetic Animal Model of Alcoholism for Screening Medications to Treat Addiction. International review of
neurobiology 126, 179–261, doi:10.1016/bs.irn.2016.02.017 (2016).
13. Bell, R. L. et al. Animal models for medications development targeting alcohol abuse using selectively bred rat lines: neurobiological
and pharmacological validity. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior 103, 119–155, doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2012.07.007 (2012).
14. Simms, J. A. et al. Intermittent access to 20% ethanol induces high ethanol consumption in Long-Evans and Wistar rats. Alcoholism,
clinical and experimental research 32, 1816–1823, doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00753.x (2008).
15. Sabino, V., Kwak, J., Rice, K. C. & Cottone, P. Pharmacological characterization of the 20% alcohol intermittent access model in
Sardinian alcohol-preferring rats: a model of binge-like drinking. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 37, 635–643,
doi:10.1111/acer.12008 (2013).
16. Spoelder, M. et al. Individual Variation in Alcohol Intake Predicts Reinforcement, Motivation, and Compulsive Alcohol Use in Rats.
Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 39, 2427–2437, doi:10.1111/acer.12891 (2015).
17. Le, A. & Shaham, Y. Neurobiology of relapse to alcohol in rats. Pharmacology & therapeutics 94, 137–156 (2002).
18. Shaham, Y., Shalev, U., Lu, L., De Wit, H. & Stewart, J. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major
findings. Psychopharmacology 168, 3–20, doi:10.1007/s00213-002-1224-x (2003).
19. Mantsch, J. R., Baker, D. A., Funk, D., Le, A. D. & Shaham, Y. Stress-Induced Reinstatement of Drug Seeking: 20 Years of Progress.
Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 335–356, doi:10.1038/
npp.2015.142 (2016).
20. Spanagel, R. Alcohol addiction research: from animal models to clinics. Best Pract Res Clin Gastroenterol 17, 507–518 (2003).
21. Gilpin, N. W., Richardson, H. N., Cole, M. & Koob, G. F. Vapor inhalation of alcohol in rats. Current protocols in neuroscience/editorial
board, Jacqueline N. Crawley… [et al.] Chapter 9, Unit 9 29, doi:10.1002/0471142301.ns0929s44 (2008).
22. Vendruscolo, L. F. & Roberts, A. J. Operant alcohol self-administration in dependent rats: focus on the vapor model. Alcohol 48,
277–286, doi:10.1016/j.alcohol.2013.08.006 (2014).
23. Hayton, S. J., Mahoney, M. K. & Olmstead, M. C. Behavioral traits predicting alcohol drinking in outbred rats: an investigation of
anxiety, novelty seeking, and cognitive flexibility. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 36, 594–603,
doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01668.x (2012).
24. Hopf, F. W. & Lesscher, H. M. Rodent models for compulsive alcohol intake. Alcohol 48, 253–264, doi:10.1016/j.alcohol.2014.03.001
(2014).
25. Wolffgramm, J. & Heyne, A. From controlled drug intake to loss of control: the irreversible development of drug addiction in the rat.
Behavioural brain research 70, 77–94 (1995).
26. Wolffgramm, J. & Heyne, A. Social behavior, dominance, and social deprivation of rats determine drug choice. Pharmacology,
biochemistry, and behavior 38, 389–399 (1991).
27. Wolffgramm, J. An ethopharmacological approach to the development of drug addiction. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews 15,
515–519 (1991).
28. Edwards, S. et al. Traumatic stress reactivity promotes excessive alcohol drinking and alters the balance of prefrontal cortex-
amygdala activity. Translational psychiatry 3, e296, doi:10.1038/tp.2013.70 (2013).
29. Crabbe, J. C., Bell, R. L. & Ehlers, C. L. Human and laboratory rodent low response to alcohol: is better consilience possible?
Addiction biology 15, 125–144, doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00191.x (2010).
30. Litten, R. Z. et al. Heterogeneity of alcohol use disorder: understanding mechanisms to advance personalized treatment. Alcoholism,
clinical and experimental research 39, 579–584, doi:10.1111/acer.12669 (2015).
31. Ray, L. A., Courtney, K. E., Bujarski, S. & Squeglia, L. M. Pharmacogenetics of alcoholism: a clinical neuroscience perspective.
Pharmacogenomics 13, 129–132, doi:10.2217/pgs.11.173 (2012).
32. Heilig, M., Goldman, D., Berrettini, W. & O’Brien, C. P. Pharmacogenetic approaches to the treatment of alcohol addiction. Nature
reviews. Neuroscience 12, 670–684, doi:10.1038/nrn3110 (2011).
33. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.) American Psychiatric Publishing,
Arlington, VA (2013).
34. Belin, D., Belin-Rauscent, A., Everitt, B. J. & Dalley, J. W. In search of predictive endophenotypes in addiction: insights from
preclinical research. Genes, brain, and behavior 15, 74–88, doi:10.1111/gbb.12265 (2016).
35. Deroche-Gamonet, V., Belin, D. & Piazza, P. V. Evidence for addiction-like behavior in the rat. Science 305, 1014–1017, doi:10.1126/
science.1099020 (2004).
36. Belin, D., Mar, A. C., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking.
Science 320, 1352–1355, doi:10.1126/science.1158136 (2008).
37. Belin, D., Balado, E., Piazza, P. V. & Deroche-Gamonet, V. Pattern of intake and drug craving predict the development of cocaine
addiction-like behavior in rats. Biological psychiatry 65, 863–868, doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.031 (2009).
38. Belin, D., Berson, N., Balado, E., Piazza, P. V. & Deroche-Gamonet, V. High-novelty-preference rats are predisposed to compulsive
cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 36,
569–579, doi:10.1038/npp.2010.188 (2011).
39. Belin, D. & Deroche-Gamonet, V. Responses to novelty and vulnerability to cocaine addiction: contribution of a multi-symptomatic
animal model. Cold Spring Harbor perspectives in medicine 2, doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a011940 (2012).
40. Kasanetz, F. et al. Transition to addiction is associated with a persistent impairment in synaptic plasticity. Science 328, 1709–1712,
doi:10.1126/science.1187801 (2010).
41. Kasanetz, F. et al. Prefrontal synaptic markers of cocaine addiction-like behavior in rats. Molecular psychiatry 18, 729–737,
doi:10.1038/mp.2012.59 (2013).
42. Cannella, N. et al. In vivo structural imaging in rats reveals neuroanatomical correlates of behavioral sub-dimensions of cocaine
addiction. Addiction biology. doi:10.1111/adb.12500 (2017).
43. Grant, B. F. et al. Epidemiology of DSM-5 Alcohol Use Disorder: Results From the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and
Related Conditions III. JAMA psychiatry 72, 757–766, doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584 (2015).
44. Deroche-Gamonet, V. & Piazza, P. V. Psychobiology of cocaine addiction: Contribution of a multi-symptomatic animal model of loss
of control. Neuropharmacology 76(Pt B), 437–449, doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.014 (2014).
45. Skinner, H. A. Spectrum of drinkers and intervention opportunities. CMAJ 143, 1054–1059 (1990).
46. Hyman, S. E. & Malenka, R. C. Addiction and the brain: the neurobiology of compulsion and its persistence. Nature reviews.
Neuroscience 2, 695–703, doi:10.1038/35094560 (2001).
47. Lüscher, C. & Malenka, R. C. Drug-evoked synaptic plasticity in addiction: from molecular changes to circuit remodeling. Neuron
69, 650–663, doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.017 (2011).
48. Nestler, E. J. & Aghajanian, G. K. Molecular and cellular basis of addiction. Science 278, 58–63 (1997).
49. Koob, G. F. & LeMoal, M. Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science 278, 52–58 (1997).
50. Kauer, J. A. & Malenka, R. C. Synaptic plasticity and addiction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 844–858 (2007).
51. Robinson, T. E. & Berridge, K. C. The Neural Basis of Drug Craving - an Incentive-Sensitization Theory of Addiction. Brain research
reviews 18, 247–291 (1993).
52. Enoch, M. A. Pharmacogenomics of alcohol response and addiction. Am J Pharmacogenomics 3, 217–232 (2003).
53. Wise, R. A. Voluntary ethanol intake in rats following exposure to ethanol on various schedules. Psychopharmacologia 29, 203–210
(1973).
54. Hopf, F. W., Chang, S. J., Sparta, D. R., Bowers, M. S. & Bonci, A. Motivation for alcohol becomes resistant to quinine adulteration
after 3 to 4 months of intermittent alcohol self-administration. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 34, 1565–1573,
doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01241.x (2010).
55. Seif, T. et al. Cortical activation of accumbens hyperpolarization-active NMDARs mediates aversion-resistant alcohol intake. Nature
neuroscience 16, 1094–1100, doi:10.1038/nn.3445 (2013).
56. Cacciola, J. S., Alterman, A. I., Habing, B. & McLellan, A. T. Recent status scores for version 6 of the Addiction Severity Index (ASI-
6). Addiction 106, 1588–1602, doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03482.x (2011).
57. Spanagel, R. et al. Anxiety: a potential predictor of vulnerability to the initiation of ethanol self-administration in rats.
Psychopharmacology 122, 369–373 (1995).
58. Hasin, D. S., Stinson, F. S., Ogburn, E. & Grant, B. F. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and
dependence in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Archives of
general psychiatry 64, 830–842, doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.830 (2007).
59. Velazquez-Sanchez, C. et al. High trait impulsivity predicts food addiction-like behavior in the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology:
official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 2463–2472, doi:10.1038/npp.2014.98 (2014).
60. Richardson, N. R. & Roberts, D. C. Progressive ratio schedules in drug self-administration studies in rats: a method to evaluate
reinforcing efficacy. Journal of neuroscience methods 66, 1–11 (1996).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank the technical assistance of Sara Bernasconi, Clarisse Meneghel and Vanessa Pierroz. The
financial support of the Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, Lausanne
University Hospital, Switzerland is gratefully acknowledged.
Author Contributions
K.J. and B.B. designed and performed behavioral experiments, analyzed data. K.J. performed the statistical
analyses, and B.B. wrote the manuscript. P.J.M. and O.H. assisted with experimental design and discussed the
results. M.A. performed blood alcohol analyses. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Additional Information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at doi:10.1038/s41598-017-09801-1
Competing Interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.