Understanding Road Rage: Implementation Plan For Promising Mitigation Measures
Understanding Road Rage: Implementation Plan For Promising Mitigation Measures
Understanding Road Rage: Implementation Plan For Promising Mitigation Measures
2009
OLP-457
October, 2009
Team Members Dr. Neelima Chakrabarty (PL) Arun Lal Srivastava Huma Tariq Vaijayanthee
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
I would like to express my sincere gratitude to Dr. S. Gangopadhyay, Director, Central Road Research Institute, New Delhi for giving me the opportunity to carry out my project work in Traffic Engineering and Road Safety Department, CRRI. I am very much thankful to Mr. A.N. Ramalingaiah , Head carry out my project in Traffic Engineering and Road Safety. It has been pleasant learning experience to work with Ms. S. Sundari Nanda (Additional Commissioner of Police, Licensing Branch, Defence Colony Building, New-Delhi-49). I take this opportunity to express my gratitude to her who took time out of her busy schedule to share her experiences with me regarding road rage causes and mitigation. I express my sincere thanks to Ms Chander Kanta (Inspector of Delhi Police, Licensing Branch) and Mr. Venu Gopal (Inspector of Delhi Police, Licensing Branch) for their support in collecting data. I would like to express my sincere thanks to all CRRI Staffs and all who have helped me directly or indirectly in completion of the project work. I can never forget the kind of confidence and encouragement given by my seniors in the duration of the project. (Traffic
Engineering & Road Safety) of CRRI, who has given me the opportunity to
TABLE OF CONTENTS Introduction Factors Influencing Road Rage Incidences of Global Road Rage Incidences Psychological Analysis of Road Rage Incidences
2.0 3.0.
29 29
4.
RESULTS AND INTERPRETATION OF FIELD OBSERVATION OF AGGRESSIVE DRIVING CASES Location- Wise Data Analysis Results and Interpretation Of Vehicle-Wise, Weather-Wise Analysis Of Road Rage Incidences
30-36
4.1 4.2.
4.3.
Administration, Analysis and Interpretation of Propensity of Aggressive Behaviour Rating Scale CONCLUSION OF THE RESEARCH FINDINGS
36-55
5.
55-56
5. 6.
Implementation Plan for Promising Mitigation Measures CRRI Contributions for the Mitigation of Road Rage
56-58 58-63
7.
References
64-70
Abstract: Road rage is a phenomenon which indicates that society is on edge. Increasingly, aggression and violence has appeared to drift further into mainstream Indian society. Stories of school shootings, workplace violence, violence on the airlines, and road rage abound. Road rage has recently been cited as equalling alcohol-impaired driving in the number of resultant motor vehicle accident related injuries and fatalities [Martinez, 1997 and Snyder, 1997]. The probability of becoming a victim on the roadways is significantly increased by the average exposure of the individual on the road. Presently Indian roads and driving conditions are a point of discussion moreover no comprehensive data is available to how many persons die or are injured due to road rage cases. In the last three years in the capital of India sudden provocation prompted people to kill or physically assault each other .In 2005-2006 it topped the list of murder motives. In 2007 it was the second highest cause in murder list. This paper discusses results of opinion survey of experts as well as of the drivers/ commuters, detailed analyses of the causes and timewise, vehicle-wise, location-wise aggressive behaviour among drivers. Paper further highlights action plan for mitigating measures for reducing aggressive behaviour among Indian drivers. Author Keywords: Aggression; Road rage; Violence. 1. INTRODUCTION A report from the World Health Organization and the World Bank (2004) on road traffic accidents and injuries estimated 1.2 million people are killed in road crashes each year and as many as 50 million are injured worldwide. There is substantial body of work on risk taking behaviour (Turner et al., 2004), socio-economic factors (McLeod et al., 2003) and driving history in relation to road traffic accidents. Turner and McClure reported that high incidence of motor vehicle crashes among young males was related to their risk taking behaviour. Apart from medical costs, damage to property and loss of productivity adds to the cost impact (Lin et al., 2003). A decade's worth of saving the Rs. 50,000 million estimated loss in traffic accidents every year could buy the country a 7,000 km-long, six-lane national highway at today's rates. That would cut
6
back on the Rs 300,000 million India loses every year due to road-related mishaps, freeing a sea of finance for the social development sector, including disaster management. According to the World Disasters Report 1998, The economic cost of road accidents is equivalent to approximately one per cent of the Gross National Product. Presently Indian roads and driving conditions are a point of discussion moreover no comprehensive data is available to how many persons die or are injured due to road rage cases. Road deaths in India registered a sharp 6.1% rise between 2006 and 2007. However, road safety experts say the real numbers could be higher since many of these accident cases are not even reported. There is no estimate of how many injured in road accidents die a few hours or days after the accident, points out Rohit Baluja, member of the UN Road Safety Collaboration and Commission of Global Road Safety Report representing Asia. The report, based on 2006 and 2007 statistics collected from 178 participating countries, said globally over 1.2 million people die in road accidents every year and 2025 million people suffer non-fatal injuries. The report pointed to speeding, drinking-driving and low use of helmets, seat belts and child restraints in vehicles as the main contributing factors. In 2004, road accident was the top ninth cause of death in 2004. In the last three years in the capital of India sudden provocation prompted people to kill or physically assault each other .In 2005-2006 it topped the list of murder motives. In 2007 it was the second highest cause in murder list. According to Delhi Police The most chilling examples are cases of violent road rage when people hit each other due to minor fights. According to doctors such erratic and potentially dangerous behaviour is a medical problem which is called Intermittent Explosive Disorder (IED) which is significantly different from foul temper and it affects up to one in 20 people more men than women. Doctor Rajesh Sagar, Associate Professor, Department of Psychiatry, AIIMS says People suffering from IED can go to extremes without understanding what they are doing. People do not realize the intensity of the problems until they commit a violent act. They can take it out in two different ways on others or on themselves (Bajpai, 2008). Some behavioural factors were significantly associated with having been involved in an accident: tailgating, getting angry with other drivers, aggressive behaviour, encounter with the police and being warned for poor riding. The accident involved rate associated with reporting yes for
each of behaviours was above 20% and was as high as 39% and 43% for aggressive behaviour and encounter with police, respectively . The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) distinguishes road rage from aggressive driving by classifying aggressive driving as a traffic offense and defining it as the operation of a motor vehicle in a manner which endangers or is likely to endanger people or property while road rage is classified as a criminal offense and is defined as an assault with a motor vehicle or other dangerous weapon by the operator or passenger(s) of one motor vehicle on the operator or passenger(s) of another motor vehicle or vehicles precipitated by an incident which occurred on a roadway(Martinez, 1997).Aggressive driving is an apparent cause which aggravates the road rage cases. The term Road Rage was first coined in 1988 (Fumento, 1998) and is defined in the 1997 Oxford English Dictionary as Violent Anger Caused by the Stress and Frustration of Driving in Heavy Traffic. Some researchers suggest this definition is not entirely accurate. For example, road rage has been described as a cultural habit of retaliation that occurs as a result of frustration and independent of heavy traffic (James & Nahl, 1998). Some scientists suggest the road rage as a mental disorder (Schmid, 1997). Elevated levels of anger may prompt aggressive and other risktaking behaviour, that can increase accident risk, and risk of other deleterious behaviour such as physical assault between drivers or argument with passengers (Deffenbacher, Oetting, & Lynch, 1994). Stradling & Parker, 1997 define aggressive driving by grouping driving behaviours into three categories: lapses, errors, and violations. Lapses are exemplified by such behaviours as pulling away from the curb in third gear, or switching on one control when trying to turn on another. Errors include failure to see a sign, misjudging a distance, etc. Violations involve intent and are related to aggressive driving such as disregarding the speed limit, running a red light, or tailgating. Violations aggravates the anger and frustration among the fellow road users or drivers which turns up into personal attacks on fellow drivers such as obscene gesturing, verbal insults, throwing objects, and, in extreme cases, physical assault. Such behaviours tend to be reciprocated quickly, and a relatively minor infraction can quickly escalate into a major altercation resulting in injury, property damage, or even death (Maiuro, 1998).According to the Additional Commissioner of Police Licensing, New-Delhi, Road Rage is momentary insanity arising out of irrational anger due to the emotional load of frustration which an individual is carrying. The point at which the road rage occurs is just a trigger, which brings forth the accumulated
8
tensions, pressure and frustrations in the mind of an individual to be forefront. In India, to date, there is no work published on risk taking behaviour and road rage incidence and related mitigation measures nor an objective record of driving with regard to road traffic accident. There is a need to develop an agreed conceptual framework for examining the above factors using appropriate parameters and measurable variables by systematic approach. This study attempts to provide evidence of several factors leading to road rage and how they lead to an accident. This can be used to control or modify the risk behaviours, build public health interventions like policy making on road safety measures, education and health. It might help devise methods to minimize or avoid consequences of road traffic accidents related to road rage. Road Rage Incidences Road rage stories coming from countries across the globe confirms its proliferation as a random, albeit widespread, form of potentially deadly interpersonal violence. Yet, despite its commonness, road rage presents a series of unique challenges to researchers, making it unlike most other forms of interpersonal violence. First, it usually involves expressed aggression between strangers. Second, it is related in some way to a driving incident. Third, it involves a perceived threat of "invasion" into one's space, and thus, one's identity. Fourth, it is a conflict phenomenon that has yet to secure a universally agreed upon definition. Fifth, there are, by some standards, insufficient quantifiable data documenting its existence. Sixth, theories about its causes abound, proposing an extremely broad range of probabilities. Seventh, the inability to target causes for the phenomenon presents major challenges to resolving it. 1.1. GLOBAL ROAD RAGE INCIDENCES This brief sampling of road rage stories coming from countries across the globe confirms its proliferation as a random, albeit widespread, form of potentially deadly interpersonal violence. Yet, despite its commonness, road rage presents a series of unique challenges to researchers, making it unlike most other forms of interpersonal violence.
1.1.1 Brief Case Histories of Road Rage Incidences In Mississauga, Canada a driver of a van was run over by a pickup truck during the truck driver's fit of road rage (Toronto Star, 17 June, 2000). In London, two drivers came to blows over who was going to use the car wash first at a service station in Lightwater (Sunday Telegraph, 02 July, 2000). In Israel, a driver was beaten to death after he cut off another driver in traffic (Los Angeles Times, 1 July 2000). In Jerusalem, July 1999 saw the stabbing death of a 36 yearold driver after he asked another driver not to scratch his car, and the knifing of a 50 year-old man by a 19 year-old when the elder driver honked his car horn at the younger driver (The Jerusalem Post, 21 June 2000). In Jerusalem, a 28 year-old man was charged in the beating death of a 46 year-old man whom he says cut him off on the road; the elder man was never allowed to emerge from his vehicle, he was beaten to death as he sat behind the steering wheelof his car (The Jerusalem Post, 21 June 2000). In Ireland, a car driver became angry after a bus overtook him on the road. Consequently, the car driver pulled in front of the bus, forced it to stop, and attacked it (The Irish Times, 23 May 2000). In New Zealand, a 35 year-old driver became irate when he felt he was being followed to closely by another vehicle, in turn, he assaulted the other driver through his open car window (The Southland Times, 31 May 2000). In Malaysia, a 30 year-old man who stabbed another man to death over an incident of road rage was sentenced to 10 years in prison (Deutsche Presse-Agentur, 4 July 2000). In China, a 44 year-old police officer who killed a 28 year-old man in a fit of road rage was sentenced to death (Agence France Presse, 3 July 2000). In India, a popular skating instructor used his vehicle to repeatedly run over, crush, and kill another man with whom he argued over an incident on the road. This incident was preceded by an angry motorist who killed a young girl and dumped her body in a drain and a young man on a scooter who "thrashed" a couple also riding a scooter because he was
10
enraged that they stopped at a yellow caution light instead of proceeding through it (India Today, 15 May 2000). Atlanta, Georgia a two-year old toddler was shot through the neck by an irate motorist engaged in an argument over a road incident with the toddler's father (The Atlanta Journal & Constitution, 30 June 2000). In Denver, Colorado a 51 year-old man used a .25 caliber semi-automatic pistol to kill a 32 year-old bicyclist who cut him off on the road (The Denver Post, 21 June 2000). In Detroit, Michigan a 34 year-old man said he was attacked with a club by another motorist after the two drivers stopped on the road to confront each other (The Detroit News, 24 May 2000). Near Cincinnati, Ohio a 29 year-old woman cut in front of a 24 year-old pregnant woman and slammed on her brakes in an irate gesture following her anger about the latter's driving. The mother-to-be lost control of her car in a violent accident in which her unborn child was killed (Newsweek, 02 June 1997). In Florida, a 41 year-old man who pulled into an exact change lane at a toll booth was shot and killed as he exited his truck to confront an irate driver behind him who was annoyed at the 41 year-old's delay in paying the toll (Car and Driver, September 1998). 1.2 FACTORS INFLUENCING ROAD RAGE Demographic variables (e.g. driver age, gender, experience etc.) are relevant but the focus has been on identifying components of driver personality that predict aggressive driving. 1.2.1 Personality Variables of the Drivers McGuire, 1956 contributes to the beginnings of personality trait research within the driving literature when he compared accident- and violationfree drivers with accident- and violation-incurring drivers. Using the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) Hathaway & McKinley, 1951 and the Rosenzweig Picture Frustration Study Rosenzweig et al., 1947, he found that the two groups produced significant differences on these standardized tests. The unsafe drivers scored significantly higher on the Psychopathic Deviate (Scale 4) and the Schizophrenic (Scale 8) scales of the MMPI. Likewise, the unsafe drivers produced significantly
11
higher scores on the Ego-Defensive and Need-Persistence Scales of the Rosenzweig. Neither groups' scores were in the clinical range for either test. Item analysis indicated that the unsafe drivers endorsed responses that were more aggressive, less conservative, less mature, less conscientious, and more antisocial in nature. Tailgating, honking horn showing anger, yelling or making angry gestures are the examples of personality factors which have been implicated in aggressive driving which include sensation seeking, impulsiveness, and trait driving anger (Dahlen et al.2005; Jonah, 1997; Schwebel et al, 2006). Sensation seeking factor contributes to the prediction of aggressive driving, risky driving, and losses of concentration, moving citations, and motor vehicle accidents beyond driving anger alone Dahlen and White (2006). Personality variables and the incidence of psychopathology in traffic accidents were investigated by Tsuang et al., 1985. They reviewed the literature in order to compare non psychiatric and psychiatric populations in an attempt to examine the roles of personality factors, Axis I disorders, suicide, life events, alcohol, and drugs in traffic accidents across populations. Tillman & Hobbs, 1949 had first studied the relationship of traffic accidents and personality traits and concluded, a man drives as he lives. Subsequent research often bore out this conclusion with some contradiction. In their review, Tsuang et al. found noticeable trends for associations between higher incidents of traffic accidents and certain personality characteristics. Such characteristics in non psychiatric populations included less control of hostility and anger, less tolerance of tension, less maturity, less conformity, more difficulty with authority, more hyperactivity, more belligerences, and a tendency to take risks. Higher accident rates are reported among criminal samples. The authors note the crossover of many reported personality characteristics with characteristics of borderline and antisocial personality disorders. The role of psychopathology in traffic accidents was found more discrepant in their review of the relevant literature. The authors report general agreement concerning the reports of higher incidence of traffic accidents in patients with diagnosed personality disorders and paranoid ideation. The reviewed studies differed widely on their findings of relationships between psychotic and neurotic disorders and the incidence of traffic accidents. The authors also note that risk-taking and hyperactivity (defined by the author as aggressiveness, impulsiveness, and disrespect for authority) are associated with higher incidence of traffic accidents. In addition to personality traits and the occurrence of Axis I disorders, state and trait aspects of affect and mood may have an effect on driving
12
behaviour. [Groeger, 1997] speculated that depressed individuals might be expected to perform more sluggishly at the wheel. He based this supposition on the body of research concerning mood and memory. In this research, mood has been seen to affect the ability to learn and retrieve information. Similarly, a driver experiencing high anxiety may commit more driving errors. A hostile mood may also affect driver performance and lead to aggressive driving behaviours. Overall, in investigating these questions, the author found that during a driving test, increases in hostility alone appeared to be related to deficits in performance. Specific driving behaviours, which are emitted in relation to any given mood state, require further investigation. This preliminary research points out the necessity of taking transient (state) and trait moods into account when conducting research into driving behaviours. Certain personality variables, traits, and characteristics may be associated with more driver stress, which may lead to increased aggression and, in turn, lead to a higher incidence of traffic accidents. More accidents certainly increase the risk of injury, property damage, and fatality. Results from the SUNY-Albany program have indicated a high rate of both Axis I and II psychopathology within a community sample (court-referred and self-referred) of aggressive drivers [Galovski & Blanchard, 2002 and Galovski & Blanchard, in press]. Using the clinician-administered, standardized Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I and II disorders [First et al., 1994 and First et al., 1995], it was found that 80% of the 30 participants met criteria for at least one Axis I disorder and 57% met criteria for at least one Axis II disorder. Notably, 33% of the population met criteria for intermittent explosive disorder (IED)the only Axis I disorder to currently hold anger as its hallmark feature. When compared to a control, nonaggressive driving population, it was found that the aggressive drivers (AD) exhibited significantly more overall Axis I and II psychopathology. With respect to specific disorders, the AD population was diagnosed significantly more frequently with IED, alcohol abuse, substance abuse, antisocial personality disorder, and borderline personality disorder. These results suggest that significant psychiatric distress exists in the aggressive driving population. Of course, we cannot submit each potential driver to a personality screening test. However, knowledge gained through these types of profiling studies does inform the type of interventions we use to correct aggressive driving behaviours. For instance, in discovering the elevated levels of IED in the Albany program, this was surmised that future interventions should particularly target
13
impulsivity. In the Albany program, certainly entrenched personality disorders were observed that may directly contribute to aggressive driving or at least predispose a person to drive aggressively. This was observed that aggressive driving was more of a poor coping skill, albeit a dangerous one, in response to stressful life events. Bringing this point to their attention and offering them alternative coping skills often went far in helping to remedy the problematic behaviour. Predisposing antisocial personality traits, as a response to environmental stress, the relative anonymity and quick getaway that the automobile provides and individual variables, such as age, gender, social status, and ethnicity, all have been identified as contributors to aggressive driving. Researchers have also explored psychological variables or characteristics of drivers in order to identify certain personality types or profiles that typify the aggressive driver. Personality types (e.g., the Type A or coronary-prone behaviour type) may exacerbate the experience of stress and contribute to aggressive driving behaviour. The tendency to drive aggressively thus may be inherent in a person much like a personality trait, or may be more transient and dependent on a given mood and the day's circumstances. For example, it may be the case that the typical aggressive driver is a generally hostile individual who perceives the world and its inhabitants negatively across situations. Or, on the other hand, the average aggressive driver may be a busy person in a hurry to complete a task. This latter person may emit aggressive behaviours subsequent to a frustrating stimuli or a thwarted goal-directed activity. Thus far, little is known about the persona we commonly refer to as the aggressive driver. Personality traits inherent in drivers might affect driving behaviour through a number of pathways. Matthews et al., 1991 review several pathways in which personality traits influence driving behaviour. Different personality traits have been shown to influence cognitive appraisals of events, levels of arousal, preferred level of stimulation, sensitivity to reward/threat cues, and more generalized stress syndromes. Conversely, it is possible that the individual's personality could be formed or altered based on driving experience (long-term exposure to traffic impedance or commute). Various approaches to differentiating and categorizing types of aggressive drivers in the literature have been employed. Larson, 1996a and Larson, 1996b categorizes aggressive drivers into five types. The speeder refers to the driver who races against the clock. This person's goal is to reach a given destination as quickly as possible and make good time. When these efforts are thwarted or obstructed, this driver quickly becomes angry. The second category of drivers is termed
14
the competitor. This driver attempts to bolster self-esteem by creating contests out of driving situations and attempting to beat other drivers in given situations (i.e., will attempt to race another driver at a neighbouring toll booth to get through first). Losing these battles increases the competitor's anger and aggressive driving behaviour. The third category involves the passive aggressor. This driver thwarts other drivers' attempts to pass, drive faster, merge, etc. This type of driver feels that giving in to another driver results in loss of status or selfesteem. Although this driver may not speed or tailgate, preventing others from achieving their goals increases anger in the latter and thus increases the hazard on the road just as effectively. The fourth type of driver is termed the narcissist. This driver sets rigid standards regarding proper driving behaviour and feels angry when infractions of these standards are observed in others. These infractions may include actual driving behaviour or characteristics of a given driver like sex, age, type of car, etc. Finally, the vigilante refers to the driver who is a self-appointed jury, judge, and enforcer of fellow drivers. This driver feels justified in punishing infractions of traffic laws and will engage in a spectrum of punishing behaviours, from shouting and swearing and obscene gesturing to cutting off drivers and even killing other drivers. Researchers have also taken a more empirical approach in identifying aggressive driver profiles. A study conducted by [Gulian et al., 1989] considered the role of locus of control and attentional styles as they relate to driver stress. These authors found a lack of impact of controllability of driving situations on driver stress. This finding contradicts previous research [Schaeffer et al., 1988] in other domains in which feeling in control of a situation modifies stress levels. The attentional styles of individuals was related to driver stress in this study in that cognitive failures (failures in perception, memory, and motor function) were associated with the dimensions of aggression in driving and dislike of driving. A follow-up study by Matthews et al., 1991 investigated more molecularly the relationship between individual differences in driver stress (as identified by their DBI; Gulian et al., 1989) and personality dimensions across four studies. The first study associated driver stress to three major personality dimensions: extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism (as identified by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, Eysenck & Eysenck, 1975). Neuroticism proved to be the strongest predictor of driver stress and was positively correlated with aggression in overall driver stress. Psychoticism was also positively correlated with aggression. These
15
findings suggest that personality traits, especially neuroticism, may play a role in predisposing an individual to driver stress that may result in aggressive driving. A follow-up study indicated that neuroticism is associated with relatively ineffectual coping strategies Dorn & Matthews, 1992. Subjects who ranked high in neuroticism reported significantly greater incidences of aggressive and confrontative strategies. The authors suggested that ineffective coping strategies might mediate the relationship seen between neuroticism and driver stress. Matthews et al., 1991 addressed more detailed individual differences in frustration and irritation on the road. The authors identify the driver's predisposition to hostility and aggression as possible antecedents to irritation and anger on the road. This irritation and anger frequently results in aggressive and hostile behaviours toward other drivers Turner et al., 1975. Results of study revealed that aggression were strongly correlated with hostility as measured by the BussDurkee Hostility Inventory Buss & Durkee, 1957. Thus, the physical or verbal act of aggression in driving was strongly related to a predisposition to hostility in general. Study is focused on the cognitive aspects of driving and the effects of the beliefs of an individual about driving competency and the ability to cope with driving situations. These authors theorize that believing oneself to be less competent in driving results in greater overall driver stress. Results are consistent with this hypothesis that believing oneself to be less competent on the road was associated with higher levels of reported driver stress. Some studies have found that conscientiousness is inversely related to motor vehicle accidents and moving violations (e.g. Arthur and Doverspike, 2001; Arther and Graziano, 1996). The relative neglect of positive personality factors overlooks a number of potentially useful variables which could inform prevention or intervention programs. 1.2.2 Environmental Factors Most drivers, at one time or another, experience heat, pollution, noise, and other driving-related stressors. Other investigators studied less interpersonal variables as contributors to aggressive driving. Aggression can be conceptualized as occurring within a larger context. Research into the context of the environment at large has included the interaction of environmental stress and driver aggression. Anger, which has been seen
16
to be related to aggression, has been defined in research and clinical settings as a form of strong arousal activated by a proximal event. There is a need to consider not only proximal cues to anger, but also to identify distal and contextual cues. In considering an anger-provoking situation, the context of the situation must be appraised (Novaco, 1993). Novaco 1993 suggests using three themes towards the assessment of anger in a given context. The first theme is termed embeddedness and refers to the understanding of the construct of anger as being connected across space and time by influencing variables that occur across settings. An example of such a variable might be the general economy. Financial stress resulting from a poor economy may impact interpersonal domestic relationships (arguments about spending), the ability to engage in extracurricular activities, higher gas prices, etc. Thus, anger may manifest itself in a particular situation such as driving, but may involve frustration from a more diffuse stressor such as economic concerns. Economic strain impacts the driving situation in a number of ways. For instance, financial difficulties prohibit an individual from purchasing a comfortable car. Job location frequently does not correspond to desirable housing in proximity. Thus, anger and aggression during commutes may be embedded within the larger transportation, residential, and economic contexts. The second theme is called interrelatedness and refers to the reciprocity in which anger influences other behavioural and environmental elements over time and vice versa. In this sense, anger is surrounded by previous occurrences and greatly influenced by past experiences. For example, rush-hour traffic and stress from work may contribute to interpersonal difficulties in the home. When the interpersonal difficulties are the sole focus of attention, the larger contextual contributors (work stress and rush-hour traffic) may be overlooked. [Novaco, 1993] describes the third theme as transformationality, which refers to dynamic change processes occurring with anger. For instance, through positive feedback cycles, angry encounters can produce escalation effects that, over time, transform behaviour patterns. For example, honking the horn in annoyance may be isolated incidents for some drivers, but may escalate to involve a family of annoyance behaviours that include more serious acts of aggression over time. Although these themes are primarily theoretical in nature, the author does draw upon empirical literature from a variety of areas of scientific inquiry to support his views (for a further review of this literature, see [Novaco, 1993]).
17
The emphasis on the economic and environmental consequences of the increases in traffic and general congestion has left the cumulative emotional, behavioural, and physical consequences of increased travel relatively neglected. However, the relationship between the environmental context and driving stress has been investigated in a number of studies. Road design, intra -vehicular discomfort, such as noise and heat, air pollution, road quality, commuting time, overcrowding, and congestion, are some of the environmental influences related to driver stress Stokols & Novaco, 1981. These aversive conditions, which occur with some regularity during daily driving situations, are considered stressful by virtue of their impedance properties. Travel impedance is thus defined as behavioural constraints on movement and goal-directed activity, which is an aversive and frustrating condition. Routine exposure to such accumulated stress negatively impacts physical health, interpersonal relationships, and occupational functioning. In particular, travel impedance over time (as in daily commute) was related to stress reactions as measured by physiological arousal, negative mood, and performance deficits. Because of the relative paucity of research on aggressive driving per se, related research must be reviewed for a better understanding of the contextual dynamics involved. Research on driver stress has provided some insight into patterns of driving within a larger context. In addition, across a variety of fields of scientific inquiry, environmental stressors have shown a linear relationship to aggressive behaviour. As Rule & Nesdale, 1976 summarize, a number of environmental stressors to which humans are exposed have adverse effects on human behaviour. Specifically, stressors can lead to aggression. Such variables reviewed include noise, density (as in overpopulation and crowding), temperature, and humidity. Although the existence of these stressors does not necessarily directly cause aggressive behaviour, stress has been shown to contribute to increased aggression in a multitude of studies. Thus, stressors can be conceptualized as aggressive cues, which accumulate or combine with other ingredients to produce aggressive responses. For instance, environmental stressors can predispose an individual to react with excessive aggression to a perceived frustration or insult. The experience of driving in a car during rush hour twice a day, 5 days a week encapsulates all of these environmental stressors (noise, pollution, density, and temperature) along with the added effect of time. It is not improbable to conclude that these stressors contribute significantly to the variance of aggressive driving behaviour.
18
Gulian et al., 1989 investigated the association of driver stress and coping strategies. The authors postulated that driver stress be conceptualized in one of two seemingly interrelated ways. First, driver stress can be thought of as a measurable response to a specific driving situation. Second, driver stress can be considered, in a broader sense, to be a result of a more pervasive personality trait found in given individuals. Driver stress was seen to occur on two levels: situationally or as an accumulation of negative feelings and frustrations related to the driving experience. Each level exacerbates and reinforces the other in a reciprocal manner. Results of this investigation supported previous research indicating that general life situations and the environmental context influence driver stress. These authors found that the interpretation of other drivers' behaviours (including aggressive behaviour) was the number one cause of stress in their driving sample. Most importantly, these investigators found that 42% to 75% of responses to driver stress involved aggressive driving behaviours. In conclusion, the authors highlighted the need for intervention strategies aimed at reducing aggressive behaviours as responses to driver stress. The daily commute affords the researcher the ideal naturalistic site to investigate the relationship of driver stress and subsequent aggression. Previous research conceptualized the degree of commuting distress, as measured by self-reported distress, heart rate, and blood pressure, and performance deficits, to vary as a function of the degree of impedance in travel Novaco et al., 1979, Stokols & Novaco, 1981 and Stokols et al., 1978]. More recently, [Schaeffer et al., 1988] examined the effects of chronic low-level stress of daily commutes on psychological, behavioural, and physiological outcomes. These investigators operationalized travel impedance as average speed during commuting (lower speeds indicated higher impedance). The influence of control over driving situations among commuters was also examined through comparison of single drivers (high control) vs. carpool occupants (low control) and through the existence of primary and secondary routes (high control as a result of a choice) vs. primary routes only (low control as a result of no choice in route). Results indicated that higher levels of impedance were significantly associated with higher systolic and diastolic blood pressure, but not for heart rate or self-report measures of anxiety or hostility. Single and carpool drivers did not differ in their reported mood precommute, but significant differences did emerge on postcommute mood measures. Contrary to expectations, single drivers were significantly more hostile and anxious than carpool drivers. The authors postulated that single drivers might feel a greater
19
urgency to arrive to work on time. The authors also surmised that lack of social interaction on the commute might account for some of the resulting poor mood. Overall, results indicated that the high impedance work commute is associated with higher systolic and diastolic blood pressure and decreases in behavioural performance. Speed of travel and control over the car's environment also contribute to the stress of the commute. Control over the car's environment seemed to partially offset the stress of a high impedance commute. However, choice of routes and control over which route to take seemed to add stress to the commute. Therefore, locus of control as a factor in driver stress requires further investigation. Temperature has been investigated as a source of stress influencing hostility while driving. Increases in uncomfortable levels of heat are related to increases in aggression [Baron & Ransberger, 1978]. [Kenrick & McFarlane, 1986] studied the effect of high temperature on interpersonal hostility while driving. Conducting their study during the months of April through August in Phoenix, AZ, these authors arranged for a confederate to block the intersection for 12 seconds. Similar to previously mentioned studies, aggression was measured by horn latency, horn duration, and total number of beeps. Subjects included any drivers who pulled up behind the confederate at the intersection with their windows down (assuming no air-conditioning). The horn measurements were correlated to continuous temperature readings provided by the University's Department of Geography. Temperatures ranged from 88 to 116 F during the course of the study. Results indicated a significant, linear relationship between temperature and aggression. Stress and negative mood appear to occur frequently in driving situations. As previously noted, today people are spending large amounts of time in the car on both a daily and an annual basis. The cumulative effect of stress from driving can clearly be considered substantial. Thus, everyday life stressors coupled with common stressful driving situations can create a climate ripe for aggressive driving. This accumulation of stress, along with related aggressive driving behaviours [Schaeffer et al., 1988], may be contributing to aggressive driving and possibly increasing the risk of motor vehicle accidents. 1.2.3 Social Factors A number of factors have been identified as causing or contributing to these driving behaviours known as aggressive driving. In the relevant literature, the social significance of the automobile, as well as social and
20
interpersonal factors involved in driving, have been somewhat neglected in favor of individual factors (i.e., reaction time) and the manipulation of various stimuli [Knapper & Cropley, 1981]. Thus, while a wide breadth of literature on the mechanics of driving exists, a relative paucity of research is found on the personological variables contributing to anger on the roads and the resulting aggressive driving behaviours. A number of these variables, such as perceived social class (status), gender, ethnicity, perception of aggression, and age, have been seen to play a significant and often overlooked role in the development, maintenance, and exacerbation of aggressive driving behaviours. In a remark on humans' fascination with the automobile, [Marsh & Collett, 1986] note the many drawbacks to the automobile. The average speed of many commuters is slower than the horse and buggy. Cities are blanketed with dense covers of smog from the pollutants spewed out by automobiles. Roadways cut through beautiful countryside and consume huge portions of cities at the expense of peoples' homes and businesses, and as well as account for thousands of injuries and deaths. Yet, despite the negative consequences of the automobiles, people choose to spend hours in traffic jams instead of alternative transportation, spend thousands of dollars on their automobiles, and cope with the plethora of everyday hassles and expenses of owning a car. The answer to this seeming contradiction, as put forth by these authors, does not lie in the political or economic arenas, but instead in the psychology of the car. One important feature of this humancar relationship is the social status that the car symbolizes. [Doob & Gross, 1968] examined the role of social status as associated with cars and aggressive driving behaviours. They hypothesized that high status implies the ability to exercise sanctions. Thus, the fear of retaliation from a high status individual would prevent a low status individual from engaging in aggressive behaviours when faced with a higher status individual. In order to investigate this hypothesis, the researchers stopped either old (low status) or new (high status) vehicles at an intersection, effectively blocking subsequent cars' passage. Horn latency (time it took for blocked vehicle to sound horn), frequency (number of beeps), and duration (length of beep) were measured. The make, model, and year of the blocked car were recorded along with characteristics of the occupants of the blocked car including sex, estimated age, number of occupants, and number of subsequent cars behind the blocked car. Thirty-eight subjects comprised the low status condition with 36 in the high status condition. In the low status condition,
21
84% of the subjects honked at least once compared to 50% in the high status condition. Subjects also waited longer on average to honk at the high status car that was blocking their passage. Status thus appeared to have an inhibitory effect on aggression in that subjects tended to exhibit less patience and more aggression (as measured by honking) toward a low status vehicle. Replicating the [Doob & Gross, 1968] study, [Deaux, 1971] investigated the sex of the frustrator in addition to the previously investigated status and sex of the frustrated subject. Similar trends for status were found as compared to Doob and Gross' study. However, these trends did not reach statistical significance. Sex of the frustrating driver was found to be the most influential variable. Fifty-two percent of all drivers honked at the frustrating car when driven by a male as opposed to 71% of drivers honking at a car driven by a female who engaged in identical frustrating behaviour. Differences in the sex of the frustrated subject emitting the behaviour of honking were not found to be statistically significant. The lack of sex differences in aggressive driving is further supported by the results of the survey by [Turner et al., 1975] mentioned previously. These authors had found that, overall, aggressive driving and provocation on the roads did not differ significantly between men and women. However, trends indicated that women engaged in more covert and less detectable forms of aggression (losing temper, feeling as if they could kill the other driver, feeling annoyed at lack of turn signalling, and swearing out loud) than men. Conversely, men tended to engage in more overtly aggressive behaviours that could be detected by other drivers (flashing headlights in anger, making rude gestures, giving chase to another driver, and retaliating in some way to another aggressive driver). Further replications of this study yielded different results. [Chase & Mills, 1973] found that newer cars produced more honking (opposite effect for status) and found no significant effect for sex. Their study was conducted 10 years later and in a different region of the country possibly accounting for some of the differences in the results. [Hankes-Drielsma, 1974] replicated the original study in Canada and measured an ethnicity factor by prominently displaying a clear symbol of ethnic identification. Honking was found to have significantly shorter latencies in the vehicle that did not display an ethnic symbol. [Turner et al., 1975] used similar methodology in investigating the role of a prominently displayed symbol of aggression (a gun in the rack located in plain view of the subject's car) as well as a variable of dehumanization
22
or anonymity (visibility of the driver). The investigators arranged to have a truck, with a visible gun prominently displayed in a gun rack paired with an aggressive or nonaggressive bumper sticker, block the intersection. The mutual visibility of both the subject and the confederate was varied by either pulling a curtain over the rear window to effectively block the view of the driver or by leaving the curtain open. Results indicated that the male subjects were more likely to honk at an anonymous individual supporting the hypothesis that inhibitions towards aggression are lowered when the victim is dehumanized (through anonymity). When the gun was paired with the aggressive bumper sticker, horn-honking responses were modified. In the case of the visible gun paired with the aggressive bumper sticker and an anonymous driver, more rapid horn-honking ensued. These authors surmise that aggressive stimuli may provoke aggressive responses, or that the stimuli may serve as a retrieval cue and cause subjects to recall unrelated incidents of aggression. [Ellison et al., 1995] also found that anonymity of drivers blocking traffic resulted in shorter honking latencies, longer honk duration, and more frequent honks as compared to visible drivers. The greater degrees of aggression observed in the conditions of the anonymous driver supports [Milgram, 1965] findings that a subject is more likely to aggress against an anonymous victim than against a visible victim. Age was found to have a negative relationship with driver stress and aggression in several studies. [Matthews et al., 1991] found that age was negatively correlated with several dimensions of driver stress. Older drivers generally reported lower overall levels of stress. Younger drivers reported a higher rate of aggression and more negative reactions about being overtaken and overtaking other cars. These findings replicated previous research conducted by the same investigators reporting that younger individuals report more daily stress in driving during commuting. As a result of more stress, younger drivers were seen to use more inefficient coping strategies (especially aggressive driving behaviours) as compared to their older driving counterparts [Gulian et al., 1989 and Gulian et al., 1990]. Driving style of the drivers exhibits the cultural background. In a cultured road site with a police patrolling car in sight, a driver refrains from acting on the aggressive disposition but a road site with no police in sight, same driver is likely to honk at the fellow drivers and then crosses the intersection even if the light has changed to red. Tillman & Hobbs (1949) found that high degree of socio-pathology i.e. greater involvement with courts, police, jail, collection agencies are correlated with high-crash rate
23
taxi drivers as compared to crash-free taxi drivers. They found that crashinvolved drivers exhibited more aggressive behaviour, than crash-free drivers. 1.2.4 Road Conditions: One common source of frustration and aggression is congestion. Driver frustration in congested road conditions, uneven road surfaces and unrepaired/broken road conditions may lead to an increase in aggressive driving and a common cause of road rage. Given this approach, to reduce or eliminate aggressive driving by treating its causes rather than its carriers (especially when driver surveys suggest that aggressive driving characterizes the majority of the driving population) the first task is to identify the source of the frustration. It is a fair assumption that the frustrated goals in driving are efficient mobility and pleasure. Automobile manufacturers and highway designers certainly believe these are relevant goals since they are promoted in advertisements, in cars people buy, and in routes people choose to drive. Yet, with more registered vehicles than licensed drivers, and when most drivers want to use the roads at similar times, one has to observe how both goals are frustrated. The situational sources of the frustration are the same factors that cause congestion: blocked path of travel by other cars, red signal lights, insufficient number of lanes for the volume, discourteous drivers, and the tendency of many people to organize their workrecreation cycles around the same times and similar routes. The driving aggression studies are not conclusive with respect to the effect of gender. In a meta-analysis of 143 studies that examined gender differences in aggressive behaviour, Hyde (1984) found women are less likely to exhibit extreme aggression, but possibly just as likely to exhibit more subtle aggression, and exhibit aggression in more subtle verbal terms. Doob & Gross (1968) found that male drivers were quicker than female drivers to honk at vehicles stopped at the green light, but did not report differences in the likelihood of honking. Deaux (1971) obtained that drivers of both genders honked more at stopped female drivers than male drivers. On the other hand Hauber (1980) observed that both male and female drivers were more aggressive towards crossing male pedestrians than towards female pedestrians. Lawson (1991)
24
reported that crash-involved red light running drivers were more likely to be males. If a better understanding of roadway and environmental factors taken care of that induce irritation and contribute to aggressive driving then road rage cases may be reduced. 1.2.5 The impact of the automobile Globally, the advent of the automobile in the 20th century has dramatically altered the occupational, social, interpersonal, economic, and environmental realms of individual countries to varying degrees. Global explosion of vehicle ownership and usage over the years has been the main cause of roadway congestion, bringing with it more motor vehicle accidents resulting in economic loss, injury, and death. As early as 1940, traffic statistics indicated that death rates increase with auto registration in a linear fashion [Ross, 1940] Fatalities and injuries resulting from automobile crashes have decreased from 1970 to 1996 in the United States [BTS USDOT, 1998] with the improvements made in transportation safety and public awareness. Such improvements include structural improvements in the highways and secondary roads, technological advances in the safety features of the automobiles themselves, legislation in safety (seat belt laws), and social reform (such as in the area of drunk driving). Marsh & Collett, 1986 explored aggressive driving behaviours correlated with the automobile. Driver or owner recognizes his automobile as an outlet for social and individual expression. Car sports, car magazines, drive-in movies and restaurants, colours, shapes, sizes, and models across the last century are reflective of economic, political, societal values, and trends at any given historical cross-section of time. As such, not only is the car a symbol of status, but it is an extension of our personalities and values. As much as a car is a reflection of individuality, it also is an outlet for frustration and aggression. In this respect, the car can be used as a weapon. Because the majority of the adult population owns a car, the automobile is not often considered in a dangerous sense. Deaths on the roadways are termed accidents. People do not think of arming themselves when getting in their vehicles. Without the association of aggression and the car, people tend minimize the potential ramifications of driving aggressively.
25
1.2.6 Age and Aggressive Driving The relationship between age and aggressive driving seems to be more consistent. Retting & Williams (1996) found that driving through the red light is more common among young drivers. Lawson (1991) in his analysis of red light running crashes, found that the red light violators were more likely to be under 35 years old than the non-violators. Hauber (1980) reported that younger drivers are more likely than older drivers to honk at pedestrians. The two aggressive behaviours that we studied were running red lights (instrumental aggression) and horn honking at stopped vehicles that obstructed traffic movement at signalized intersections during the green phase (hostile or instrumental aggression). Despite the prominence of the aggressive driving issue in the media, there are still very few empirical studies that directly assessed variables that may affect aggressive driving, and fewer still that used these two behaviours that we studied; namely driving through red lights, and horn honking at drivers blocking traffic at signalized intersections. A recent study on driving through red lights by Retting & Williams (1996) compared the characteristics of 462 drivers who violated the red light signal with over 900 drivers who did not violate the red light at the same intersection. They found that drivers who drove through the red light were younger, were less likely to use seat belts, and had worse driving records than the drivers who did not drive through the red light. These results are consistent with the numerous studies of crash analyses that find that young male drivers are over-involved in crashes relative to women and older men (e.g. Cerrelli, 1995 and NHTSA, 1996). Horn honking was first used as an observable dependent measure of aggressive driving behaviour in a field study published by Doob and Gross almost 30 years ago (1968). In their experimental paradigm the experimenter's car was stopped at a signalized intersection, and remained stopped when the light changed to green. They then studied the hornhonking behaviours of the drivers stopped behind the experimenter's car. They rationalized that this is a realistic situation for observing frustrationaggression, since it provides a clearly identifiable frustrator and a fairly typical response for the blocked driver (p. 213). Also they noted that horn honking has both an instrumental value (since it may quicken the movement of the blocking car) and an emotional tension-reducing value (since it is an unpleasant stimulus to the driver of the blocking car).
26
The effects of several independent variables on horn honking have been studied by different researchers. In all of these studies the experimenter's car was stopped at a red signal. When the light changed to green a timer was started and it was stopped when either the driver behind honked the horn or 12 s had elapsed. Special care was taken in these studies to conduct all trials during non-rush hours. In the first study of the kind, Doob & Gross (1968) showed that drivers more readily honked at a lowstatus car (old Rambler sedan) than at a high-status car (new Chrysler Imperial). In light Sunday traffic the mean delay (for male drivers) was 6.8 s for the former and 8.5 s for the latter. Also males were quicker to honk than women by 12 s. Deaux (1971) partially replicated Doobs and Gross findings and also showed that drivers more readily honked at female drivers than at male drivers, supportingwhat they termedthe damn female driver stereotype. Turner, Layton & Simons (1976), measured honking behaviour on Saturdays and found a greater likelihood of honking at a blocking pickup truck when the curtain behind the driver was drawn than when it was removed, suggesting that drivers (all males) are more likely to honk when their anonymity is assured than when it is not. Finally, in the most recent study using this measure, Ellison et al., (1995) observed honking tendencies by drivers of convertible cars and 44 vehicles with the top up (providing anonymity) and with the top down (with no anonymity). They found that the time to honk was significantly shorter when the driver behind the stopped car was with the top up (6.36.5 s) than when he/she were in an open top (9.09.6 s). They did not find any differences between men and women. In all of the above studies, care was taken to conduct the research in non-rush hours. Thus, the very common situation of congestion and (possibly) time pressure was not a factor. The effects of congestion, rush hours, or crowding on driving behaviour, to the best of my knowledge, have not been studied empirically. However there is ample evidence from social psychology and urban anthropology that crowding and congestion lead to increase in violence. The urban plight of most large cities is a testimonial to that. A related issue, that to the best of my knowledge also has not been tested directly, is that people drive more aggressively when under pressure than when they are not under pressure [though in a related study, McMurray (1971) found that people just prior to divorce have a higher crash rate than either much before or after it]. In the traffic scene, time pressure, congestion, and alienation (or anonymity) are often confounded; occurring at the same times and in the same places. Thus we are probably more likely to observe some aggressive driving in
27
crowded cities during the rush hours than in small towns during the weekends. Some direct evidence for this was obtained by Hennessy & Wiesenthal (1997). In their study, drivers were queried about their mood and behaviours through cellular phones in congested and noncongested traffic. Using structured questionnaires, they found that state stress (as distinguished from trait stress) was much higher in high congestion than in low congestion, and aggressive behaviours (including swearing, purposeful tailgating, and horn honking) were twice as frequent in the high congestion than in the low congestion. 1.2.7. Aggressive Behaviour (Horn Honking) At Stopped Cars At Signalized Intersections The effect of both situational variables (traffic signal duration, and time pressure) and individual differences (age and sex of drivers) significantly effect on aggressive behaviour (i.e. horn honking) when waiting behind a car when the traffic signal light changes to green. The duration of the traffic signal green and red phases are directly related to the expected time delays, and affect aggressive behaviour. Time pressure by the day time of travel, or driving in weekday rush-hour traffic is more stressful and generate more aggression than weekend driving (Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1997 ). Ellison et al. (1995) ; Doob & Gross (1968) obtained moderate correlations between honking delays, number of honks, and duration of honks, and also noted that some drivers revved their engines instead of honking. 1.3 The Need of This Project This has been observed that relatively low level of attention given the study of road rage, particularly in comparison to the scholarly attention given to other types of violence such as street crime, domestic abuse, and school violence. Keeping in view of the above present project was initiated to defines, quantifies, explains, and implements remedial measures for the road rage phenomenon.
28
2. OBJECTIVE(S) OF THE STUDY: The major objectives of the present project were: I. II. III. IV. V. Time-Wise, Vehicle Wise and incidence wise observation of road rage cases in Delhi Analysis of PADS scale across different age groups, professional groups, gender and driving exposures On the basis of observations and results of rating scales identify the various factors leading to road rage. Documentation of records. Implementation Plan for preventing road rage.
3.
METHODOLOGY
This study was conducted on drivers randomly across age group, qualification, profession and driving exposures with the help of experienced traffic police personnel. Various aggressive driving behaviour parameters were measured by the administration of propensity of aggressive driving scale (PADS). Following steps were adopted in the present study: Interview with the Additional CP Licensing, Delhi regarding opinion about road rage Field observation of Road Rage incidences Data collection by administering PAD Scale on drivers Data compilation and analysis Report writing Planning and Implementation of Mitigation measures This study was conducted on drivers randomly across age group, qualification, profession and driving exposures with the help of experienced traffic police personnel. Various aggressive driving behaviour parameters were measured by the administration of propensity of aggressive driving scale (PADS).Due to time and manpower constraint the observation area surrounding Delhi and NCR was kept limited.
29
4. RESULTS AND INTERPRETATION OF TIME OBSERVATION OF AGGRESSIVE DRIVING CASES Field observations were carried out in three phases 4.1 4.2 4.3
WISE
FIELD
Time wise field observation and recording of Road Rage incidences and Aggressive Driving cases Results and interpretation of vehicle-wise, weather-wise analysis of road rage incidences in Delhi and some NCR areas Administering Propensity of Aggressive Driving Rating Scale on Drivers of the selected sample.
4.1 Time Wise Field Observation and Recording of Road Rage Incidences and Aggressive Driving Cases Time wise field observation and recording of Aggressive Driving cases were made on NH-8 (near Okhla Bus Depot Phase-II), New-Delhi team members recorded time wise various aggressive driving and road rage incidences for duration of five hours i.e. from 9:15 a.m. to 5 p.m. The main objective of selecting this NH-8 road stretch was to compare aggressive behaviour and risk taking practices of drivers on off peak hours and peak hours. Apart from these several complains were also received from road users about aggressive driving practices shown by drivers of different vehicles on this particular road stretch. Observations were made on the basis of number of traffic violations and risk taking behaviours (as mentioned below) shown by the drivers of light motor vehicles i.e. car/jeep, three wheelers and two wheelers and heavy motor vehicle drivers: 4.1.1 4.1.2 4.1.3. 4.1.4. Number Number Number Number of of of of Red Light Jumping cases tailgating cases aggressive Honking Horn Wrong Side Overtaking
4.1.1
Overall 503 (whole vehicle population) number of red light jumping cases were observed out of which drivers of two wheelers & light motor vehicles were observed maximum violating traffic signals by jumping red lights
30
(total 239,48% and total 207,41% ) as compare to the drivers of other vehicles.
Table 1: Field Observation of Risk Taking Behaviour (Red Light Jumping Cases) Red Light Jumping Time 9:15 -10:15 am 10:30 -11:30 am 1:30 -2:30 pm 3:00 4:00 pm 4:00 5:00 pm Total
Car/Jeep 44 38 32 40 53 207
% 9 8 6 8 11 41
T.W*. 52 42 37 47 61 239
% 10 8 7 9 12 48
TH.W.* 7 10 6 7 9 39
%. 1 2 1 1 2 8
H.M.V*. 0 3 6 7 2 18
% 0 1 1 1 0 4
*T.W. =Two Wheelers, TH.W. =Three-Wheelers, H.M.V. =Heavy Motor Vehicles Maximum numbers of red light jumping cases were observed during the evening peak hours i.e. between 4.00-5.00 pm while minimum numbers of such cases were observed during afternoon i.e. between 1.30-2:30 p.m. (table-2). 4.1.2 Observation of Behaviour of Tailgating
During the period of 9:15 a.m. to 5:00 p.m. total 52 cases of tailgating were observed .One of the interesting observation was the drivers who were high speeding e.g. car/jeep and two wheelers drivers they minimum tailgated as their tendency were to jump red light, high speeding and overtaking the front vehicles. In such cases out of the total 52 tailgating cases only 4% car/jeep drivers and none of the two wheeler drivers tailgated during this time interval. Comparatively slow moving three wheelers drivers (85%) and heavy motor vehicle drivers (12%) were observed tailgating other vehicle maintaining minimum space with the front vehicle. Also the maximum numbers of tailgating cases were observed during the peak hours between 1:30 p.m. -2:30 p.m. while minimum numbers of cases were
31
observed during evening peak hours between 3:00 p.m. -4.00 p.m. (table-3). Table 2: Field Observation of Risk Taking Behaviour (Tailgating) Tailgating Time 9:15 -10:15 am 10:30 -11:30 am 1:30 -2:30 pm 3:00 4:00 pm 4:00 5:00 pm Total Car/Jeep 0 1 1 0 0 2 % 0 2 2 0 0 4 T.W. 0 0 0 0 0 0 % 0 0 0 0 0 0 THREE W. 10 10 11 5 8 44 % 19 19 21 10 15 85 H.M.V. 1 2 3 0 0 6 % 2 4 6 0 0 12
4.1.3
Overall 107 vehicles were observed aggressively honking horns out of which drivers of heavy motor vehicles were found observed maximum honking aggressive horns as 37% of the total vehicle population followed by drivers of two vehicles and car/jeep as 30% and 26%. Table 3: Field Observation of Risk Taking Behaviour (Honking Horn Cases) Aggressive Honking horn Time 9:15 -10:15 am 10:30 -11:30 am 1:30 -2:30 pm 3:00 4:00 pm 4:00 5:00 pm Total Car/Jeep 3 6 4 4 9 26 % 3 6 4 4 8 24 T.W. 6 6 2 9 7 30 % 6 6 2 8 7 28 THREE W. 1 2 2 2 4 11 % 1 2 2 2 4 10 H.M.V. 7 5 8 7 13 40 % 7 5 7 7 12 37
Maximum number cases related to aggressive honking horn were observed during the evening peak hours between 4:00-5:00 p.m. while minimum numbers of cases were observed between 1:30-2:30 p.m.
32
4.1.4
Wrong side overtaking were maximum observed in two wheelers i.e. 43% followed by drivers of car/jeep i.e. 23% .It is interesting to observe that high speeding drivers i.e. two wheeler riders and car/jeep drivers were found maximum overtaking from wrong side. Table 4: Field Observation of Wrong side overtaking Wrong side overtaking Time 9:15 -10:15 am 10:30 -11:30 am 1:30 -2:30 pm 3.00 - 4.00 pm 4.00 - 5.00 pm Total Car/Jeep 7 3 4 7 2 23 % 9 4 5 9 3 29 T.W. 7 3 10 2 12 34 % 9 4 13 3 15 43 THREE W. 1 2 1 2 1 7 % 1 3 1 3 1 9 H.M.V. 3 1 4 3 4 15 % 4 1 5 4 5 19
Three wheelers were found minimum overtaking (9%) from wrong side as compared to other vehicles. Maximum numbers of cases were observed during were observed during between 1:30- 2:30 p.m. and 4 p.m. -5p.m and minimum cases were observed between 10:30 a.m. to 11:30 a.m. 4.2 RESULTS AND INTERPRETATION OF VEHICLE-WISE, WEATHER-WISE ANALYSIS OF ROAD RAGE INCIDENCES
Field surveys related to the observation of various road rage incidences were carried out by the research team (which was comprised of police personnel and scientists from the Central Road Research Institute) on Delhi roads for eight months i.e. from October 2008 to May 2009. Out of the total 100 incidences, 24 major cases where some types of physical assaults or aggressive gestures / postures were involved by the aggressive drivers (which are highlighted below) were analysed.
33
4.2.1
Enforcement
and
Perception
of
Places of observations and number of incidences occurred at different place of Delhi and NCR areas are shown below (fig.1a,b).This has been observed that those areas where more road rage incidences have been observed, had either lack of proper road capacity leading traffic congestion, lack of working traffic signals or presence of enforcement. According to WHO report only 29% countries had managed to reduce traffic speed in urban areas and only 10% have been effective in managing it. Apparently, weather was clear by the research team. On the basis of this survey this may be assumed road rage incidences are not always related to weather conditions except some exceptional cases.
Fig. 1 a : Places of Observed Road Rage Incidences
Nehru Place
4 4 4 4 13
4 4 8 8
21 13 8
Modi Mill Flyover Mathura Road Dankaur Road Noida Chanakya Puri Khelgaon Road
34
% Incidences
4.2.2 Type of Vehicles Involved in Road Rage incidences During observation it was found that 42% car drivers were involved with fellow car drivers, while car drivers were either involved with two wheelers 33%, 4% with tractors and 4% with buses,4% with three wheelers and 8% with trucks. This shows that car drivers were maximum involved in all types of road rage cases.
4 8 4
Car vs. car Bus vs. Car 42 Two Wheelers vs. Car Car vs. Three Wheeler Truck vs. Car
33 4
35
4.2.3 Type of Traffic Rule Violence involved During Road Rage Incidences Road rage is just a trigger which brings forth all frustrations deposited inside the human mind for any minor incidence which may be any form of violation of traffic rules from either of the parties i.e. victims or aggressor , resulting into physical harm, fatalities and mental assaults. From the survey it was observed that 38% road rage incidences were triggered due to high speeding accompanied with wrong inside overtaking and 25% due to tailgating by any of the parties (victim or aggressor). Other types of violence involved during road rage incidences were red light jumping 13%, hitting from the back 13%,high speeding with overloaded vehicle 4%,aggressive honking horn 4% and driving under influence of alcohol 4%.
Toxicated Driving Red Light Jumping Tailgeting Back Hitted High speeding and over loading High speeding and wrong side overataking Honking Horn
4
13 25 13 4 38 4
10
15
20 Percentage
25
30
35
40
4.3. THE PROPENSITY OF AGGRESSIVE DRIVING SCALE (PADS): This scale was developed by Jason P. DePasquale, E. Scott Geller, Steven W. Clarke and Lawrence C. Littleton in 1999 . This scale is designed to identify individuals with the greatest tendency to become angry while driving and subsequently engage in hostile driving behaviours or acts of
36
road rage. Investigation revealed that the PADS is having adequate testretest reliability (.91) and validity. In addition, the PADS is having significant and positive correlations with the BussDurkee Hostility Index (BDHI; r=.40) and the trait subscale of the StateTrait Anger Scale (STAS; r=.40). The PADS had a significant correlation with Eysenck's Impulsivity Scale (r=.28). The PAD scale consists of 19 situations in different driving scenario; each driving scenario has four options. Each option has been divided according to the level of intensity of aggressiveness pertaining to the given situations. In this scale level= (0) stands for non aggressive responses shown by respondents, level= (1) for somewhat aggressiveness and level= (2) for very aggressiveness and level = (3) for extreme aggressive behaviour i.e. physical assault and chasing the other fellow driver or other road user.
4.3.1. INTERPRETATION OF RESULTS OF PAD SCALE 4.3.1.1 Sample Size and characteristics: In the present study 191 drivers from Delhi were randomly selected across different age groups, professions, qualification and driving experiences. In the present sample population 81% were males and 19% were females, age wise categorisation shows that 3% were from below 18 years 36% were from 19 - 25 years, 30% were from 26-35 years, 15% were from 36-45 years, 9% were from 46-55, 7% were 56 &above. Qualification wise categorisation of sample consisted of 36% graduates,30% post graduates, 14% and 17% were qualified up to higher secondary and secondary level and 3% were only qualified up to primary level. According to the driving experiences 49% were having up to 5 years, 26% were having 5-10 years, 11% were having 10-15 years, 7% were having 15-20 years and 7% were having more than 20 years of driving experience. Profession wise in the present sample 21% were students, 3% were engineers, 8% were business men, 57% were service men, 1% and 10% were housewives and from other types of profession
37
LEVELS
ON
NINETEEN
The respondents task was to select those situation which they had faced in their real life while driving and they had to choose the option which they nearly did while facing those situations. His/her behaviour was rated according to the responses he/she had chosen. 4.3.3 LEVEL OF AGGRESSION SHOWN BY THE RESPONDENTS OF DIFFERENT AGE GROUP Overall responses of the respondents in all situations of PADS scale were compiled and analysed to observe the age-wise effect of aggression over different situations. On the basis of the data overall maximum aggressive responses were shown by the respondents of 19 to 25 years age group who has shown 12.9% extreme aggressive responses, 23.7% aggressive responses followed by the below 18 years age groups whose parents had provided them their vehicles to drive and some respondents were having motorbikes. These age groups were observed as most vulnerable groups for involving in road rage cases resulting to fatalities and accidents.
Table 6: Level of Aggressiveness Shown By the Respondents of Different Age Groups Age of the respondents Below 18 19-25 26-35 36-45 46-55 56 & above No Somewhat Extreme Aggression Aggression Aggression Aggression 52.6 23.1 16.8 7.5 38.2 25.2 23.7 12.9 60.7 19.7 15.4 4.2 54.9 20.1 17 8 56.1 30.8 11.5 1.6 63.2 22.9 9.8 4.1
38
Table-7: Analysis Of Variance According To the Different Age Groups Sum of Square s df 36.002 120.76 3 156.76 4 43.174 209.39 9 252.57 4 4.665 90.497 95.162 6.827 105.96 9 112.79 6 13.486 219.28 6 184 190 6 183 189 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 2.248 1.192 1.886 .085 1.138 .576 1.976 .071 .778 .492 1.581 .155 7.196 1.144 6.289 .000 Mean Square 6.000 .656
Different Situations Situatio n1 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n2 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n3 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n4 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n5 Between Groups Within
F 9.142
Sig. .000
39
Groups Total Situatio n6 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n7 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n8 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n9 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 10 Between Groups Within Groups Total
8 232.77 5 44.908 156.04 5 200.95 3 12.376 76.303 88.679 5.856 65.767 71.623 5.905 66.084 71.990 17.707 159.59 2 177.29 8 190 6 183 189 6 183 189 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 190 2.951 .867 3.402 .003 .984 .359 2.740 .014 .976 .357 2.731 .015 2.063 .417 4.947 .000 7.485 .853 8.778 .000
40
Situatio n 11
6.775 36.104 42.880 32.460 168.80 3 201.26 3 11.881 77.041 88.921 24.791 470.38 2 495.17 3 18.510 86.820 105.33 0 27.282
6 184 190 6 183 189 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 190 6
1.129 .196
5.755
.000
Situatio n 12
5.410 .922
5.865
.000
Situatio n 13
1.980 .419
4.729
.000
Situatio n 14
4.132 2.556
1.616
.145
Situatio n 15
3.085 .472
6.538
.000
Situatio n 16
Between Groups
4.547
2.915
.010
41
Within Groups Total Situatio n 17 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 18 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 19 Between Groups Within Groups Total
287.01 1 314.29 3 .377 22.866 23.242 13.305 201.87 9 215.18 3 10.326 74.271 84.597
1.560
.063 .125
.502
.806
2.217 1.097
2.021
.065
1.721 .404
4.264
.000
4.3.4. LEVEL OF AGGRESSION SHOWN BY THE RESPONDENTS OF DIFFERENT GENDER Female drivers showed more aggression as compared to the male drivers as female drivers have shown 10.2 percent extreme aggression level as opposed to 6.4 percent of male drivers. Table-8: Level of Aggressiveness Shown By the Respondents of Different Gender Groups Gender Male No Somewhat Extreme Aggression Aggression Aggression Aggression 55.7 24 13.8 6.4
42
Female
47.3
24.6
17.7
10.2
Table-9: Analysis Of Variance According To the Different Gender Sum of Square s df 6.270 150.49 4 156.76 4 1.732 250.84 2 252.57 4 .182 94.981 95.162 2.923 109.87 3 112.79 2 188 190 2 187 189 2 188 190 2 188 190 1.461 .584 2.501 .085 .091 .505 .180 .836 .866 1.341 .645 .526 Mean Square 3.135 .801 Signific ance level .022
Different Situations Situatio n1 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n2 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n3 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n4 Between Groups Within Groups Total
F 3.916
43
6 Situatio n5 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n6 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n7 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n8 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n9 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 10 Between Groups 3.704 229.07 1 232.77 5 3.644 197.30 9 200.95 3 1.095 87.584 88.679 1.339 70.284 71.623 .372 71.618 71.990 1.607 2 188 190 2 187 189 2 187 189 2 188 190 2 188 190 2 .803 .860 .425 .186 .381 .488 .615 .669 .374 1.790 .170 .548 .468 1.169 .313 1.822 1.055 1.727 .181 1.852 1.218 1.520 .221
44
Within Groups Total Situatio n 11 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 12 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 13 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 14 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 15 Between Groups Within Groups
175.69 2 177.29 8 .491 42.388 42.880 2.211 199.05 2 201.26 3 2.115 86.807 88.921 .496 494.67 7 495.17 3 3.237 102.09 3
188 190 2 188 190 2 187 189 2 188 190 2 188 190 2 188
.935
.246 .225
1.090
.338
1.106 1.064
1.039
.356
1.057 .462
2.290
.104
.248 2.631
.094
.910
1.619 .543
2.981
.053
45
Total Situatio n 16 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 17 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 18 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 19 Between Groups Within Groups Total
105.33 0 5.460 308.83 3 314.29 3 .452 22.790 23.242 .779 214.40 4 215.18 3 1.378 83.219 84.597
190 2 188 190 2 187 189 2 188 190 2 188 190 .689 .443 1.556 .214 .390 1.140 .342 .711 .226 .122 1.854 .159 2.730 1.643 1.662 .193
As with most human behaviour, there is a stated and unstated, a conscious and unconscious motivation for most traffic disputes. Today aggressor could be male or female, young (usually), or old, educated or uneducated, rich or poor. Violent traffic disputes are rarely the result of a single incident but the cumulative result of a series of stressors.
46
4.3.5 LEVEL OF AGGRESSION SHOWN BY THE RESPONDENTS OF DIFFERENT DRIVING EXPERIENCES Almost similar results have been observed in the data analysis according to driving experiences .Respondents of category one i.e. the drivers having driving experiences uoto 5 years have shown maximum aggression level while the other groups has almost same level of aggressiveness. This observation of research data proves that the novice drivers have maximum chances for involving in road rage incidences. Table-10: Level of Aggressiveness Shown By the Respondents of Different Driving Experiences Driving No Somewhat Extreme Experiences Aggression Aggression Aggression Aggression Upto 5 48.1 24 17.9 10 5 to 10 58.2 23.5 13 5.3 10 to 15 61.3 23.7 10.9 4.1 15 to 20 63.1 20.5 10.7 5.7 Above 20 58.5 28.9 8.6 4 Table-11: Analysis Of Variance According To The Different Driving Experience Sum of Square s 10.709 5 146.05 185 6 156.76 190 4 3.816 5 .763 1.352 .565 .727 Mean Square F 2.142 .789 2.713
Different Situations Situation 1 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 2 Between Groups Within Groups Total
df
Sig. .022
47
4 Situation 3 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 4 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 5 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 6 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 7 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 8 Between 1.132 5 .226 .508 .446 .816
1.230 .576
2.133
.063
106.64 185 7 112.79 190 6 30.710 5 202.06 185 5 232.77 190 5 6.621 5
6.142 1.092
5.623
.000
1.324 1.056
1.254
.286
1.189 .450
2.643
.025
.672
1.821
.111
48
Groups Within Groups Total Situation 9 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 10 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 11 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 12 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 13 Between Groups Within Groups 68.264 185 71.623 190 2.468 5 .494 .376 1.313 .260 .369
.547 .944
.580
.715
.426 .220
1.933
.091
1.976 1.040
1.900
.096
1.236 .447
2.764
.020
82.741 185
49
Total Situation 14 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 15 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 16 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 17 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situation 18 Between Groups Within Groups Total
88.921 190 14.194 5 480.97 185 9 495.17 190 3 11.779 5 93.550 185 105.33 190 0 19.048 5 295.24 185 5 314.29 190 3 .232 5 .046 .125 .370 .868 3.810 1.596 2.387 .040 2.356 .506 4.659 .000 2.839 2.600 1.092 .366
.694 1.144
.606
.695
50
Situation 19
5.076
1.015 .430
2.362
.042
4.3.6 Level of Aggression Shown By the Respondents of Different Profession Respondents of category one i.e. students have shown maximum aggression level while the other groups has almost same level of aggressiveness. This observation of research data shows that the drivers of having new driving experiences have maximum chances for involving in road rage incidences as compared to the other professional categories. Table 12: Level of Aggressiveness Shown By the Respondents of Different Profession Profession No Somewhat Extreme Aggression Aggression Aggression Aggression 40.6 25.7 20.8 12.9 50 23.6 18.4 8 64.5 57.6 42.1 58.1 19.6 24.8 21 20.7 11.5 11.9 31.5 14.4 4.4 5.7 5.4 6.8
Table 13: Analysis Of Variance According To the Different Profession Sum of Square s df Mean Square
Different Situations
Sig.
51
Situatio n1
13.703 143.06 1 156.76 4 15.553 237.02 1 252.57 4 6.255 88.908 95.162 10.042 102.75 4 112.79 6 18.136 214.63 9 232.77 5 28.998
6 184 190 6 183 189 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 190 6
2.284 .778
2.937
.009
Situatio n2
2.592 1.295
2.001
.068
Situatio n3
1.042 .483
2.157
.049
Situatio n4
1.674 .558
2.997
.008
Situatio n5
3.023 1.167
2.591
.020
Situatio n6
Between Groups
4.833
5.143
.000
52
Within Groups Total Situatio n7 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n8 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n9 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 10 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 11 Between Groups Within Groups Total
171.95 5 200.95 3 4.562 84.117 88.679 6.743 64.880 71.623 8.995 62.994 71.990 6.970 170.32 8 177.29 8 4.866 38.014 42.880
183 189 6 183 189 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 190
.940
.760 .460
1.654
.135
1.124 .353
3.187
.005
1.499 .342
4.379
.000
1.162 .926
1.255
.280
.811 .207
3.925
.001
53
Situatio n 12
25.874 175.38 9 201.26 3 10.165 78.757 88.921 23.306 471.86 7 495.17 3 10.080 95.250 105.33 0 19.344 294.94 9 314.29 3 .505
6 183 189 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 190 6 184 190 6
4.312 .958
4.499
.000
Situatio n 13
1.694 .428
3.958
.001
Situatio n 14
3.884 2.564
1.515
.175
Situatio n 15
1.680 .518
3.245
.005
Situatio n 16
3.224 1.603
2.011
.066
Situatio n 17
Between Groups
.084
.677
.668
54
Within Groups Total Situatio n 18 Between Groups Within Groups Total Situatio n 19 Between Groups Within Groups Total
.124
1.325 1.126
1.177
.321
.540 .442
1.221
.298
4.3.7 Level of Aggression Shown By the Respondents of Different Qualification Overall, there was no significant difference up to .05 level in ANOVA has been found as drivers qualified up to post graduate levels were found showing maximum aggressive responses followed by drivers qualified up to primary and secondary levels. Table 14: Level of Aggressiveness Shown By the Respondents of Different Qualification Qualification No Somewhat Extreme Aggression Aggression Aggression Aggression 34.2 31.5 68.4 58.2 50.3 32.8 31.7 19.5 23.5 24.6 21.2 26.3 9.4 12.5 16.2 11.8 10.5 2.7 5.8 8.9
55
Post Graduate
40.6
25.6
16
17.8
56
5. CONCLUSION OF THE RESEARCH FINDINGS Some common characteristics of all these road rage cases are It usually involves expressed aggression between strangers. It is related in some way to a driving incident. It involves a perceived threat of "invasion" into one's space, and thus, one's identity. It is a conflict phenomenon that has yet to secure a universally agreed upon definition. There are, by some standards, insufficient quantifiable data documenting its existence. Theories about its causes abound, proposing an extremely broad range of probabilities. The inability to target causes for the phenomenon presents major challenges to resolving it. The table 15 reveals that somewhat aggressiveness was shown by the 35.1% respondents when driver of another vehicle came suddenly in front of the respondents vehicle and the respondents had to put an emergency brake. Extreme aggressiveness was shown by 68.6% respondents when a fellow driver occupied their parking space in a fully congested parking lot. Somewhat aggressiveness had been shown by the 36.6% respondents towards those drivers who had the general tendency to play music at high volume while driving. About 39% respondents had shown aggressiveness towards those drivers who had the general tendency of frequently switching over lanes and preventing fellow drivers from passing efficiently. Respondents had shown aggressive attitudes towards risk taking pedestrians who runs across the road ,dont use zebra crossings and come in front of the busy traffic and bump with them, about 34% of the respondents had come across with such type of situation. Some drivers have the bad habits of eating and drinking on the road side and throwing the waste paper, bottles and other things out of their windows while driving. 96.4% drivers had come across with such type of incidences where the waste paper, bottles etc. thrown out by the drivers whom they were following and these got struck on their windshields. Facing such situations 4.2% respondents responded extreme aggressively which led to following of the faulty drivers vehicle and fighting with him,
57
39.3% respondents gave bad gestures to the faulty drivers and shouted towards the faulty drivers. Table 15: Analysis of Aggression Levels on Nineteen Different Situations Behaviour No aggressive behaviour Somewhat aggressive behaviour Aggressive behaviour Extreme aggressive behaviour Total S1* S2 S3 S4 S5 S6 S7 S8 S9 S10
47.1 55.5 0
14.7 16.8
25.7 0
0 18.8
0 10.5
13.1 8.4
4.2 39.3
7.3 100
19.9 100
10.5 100
14.1 100
0 100
0 100
0 100
0 100
Behaviour S11
S12
S13
S14
S15
S16
S17
S18
S19
No aggressive behaviour 66 52.3 Somewhat aggressive behaviour 34 21.5 Aggressive behaviour 0 16.2 Extreme aggressive behaviour 0 9.9 Total 100 100 *S1 to S19=situation 1
3.1
3.6
3.1
75.9
96.9
53.4
30.9
28.8
52.9 67.5 0
0 3.1
18.3 17.3
53.4 15.7
11 100
0 100
Respondents had also found victimised by the fellow drivers like blocking their space, bad gestures etc. for which 46.6% respondents responded aggressively (varying from somewhat 18.3% to extreme aggressiveness 11%) towards the aggressors. Generally drivers in India driver at night with high beams which in case directly fall on the eyes of the drivers
58
produce glare effect and cause temporary impairment of vision due to the bleaching effect of Rods. About 69% respondents responded aggressively towards the drivers who were driving with high beam at night time. 6. CRRI CONTRIBUTIONS FOR THE MITIGATION OF ROAD RAGE Research team participated in various activities to mitigate the road rage. Some of them as following: THE MITIGATIONS MEASURES BY CRRI PLANNED ARE DIVIDED INTO FOLLOWING MAIN AREAS The mitigations measures are broadly divided into following main areas:
1. EDUCATION AND ENFORCEMENT a) Research team has planned to develop an educational film on ROAD RAGE to highlight the causes and ways to control road rage. b) Working with the Enforcement people CRRI research team working as a coordinator to talk road rage victims, help in various research activities to find effective interventions of aggressive driving and road rage. Enforcement efforts have been accompanied by public information campaigns. CRRI Cooperative programs with Delhi police in various aspects were found to be effective. To understand the mechanisms underlying young drivers' risk-taking behaviour in CRRI participated in Road Safety and Anti Aggression campaign.. The goal such campaign was to bring about a change in the students and social climate so that normative behaviour of children is constructive. Various schools participated in the campaign. c) CRRI participated in the First Aggression Management Workshop organized by the Delhi Police under chairmanship of Additional Commissioner of Police, Licensing Branch of Delhi Police on 15th and 17 th April, 2009 in which CRRI participated as one of the coordinator of the core group along with the other highly expertise persons. d) In the workshop NAVCHENTA website www.navchetna.com was launched. The prior objective of launching such website was to inculcating safety attitude and non aggressive behaviour among youngsters through different processes e.g.
59
a) CRRI research team is interacting with the different experts for solving kids problems b) CRRI research team is helping in research for identifying causes and mitigation of aggressive behaviour through this network. 2. BEHAVIOUR SCANNING AND MODIFICATION AGGRESSION MANAGEMENT WORKSHOP THROUGH
CRRI participated in aggression management workshop organized in Lady Irwin College by TrafficZam.com, website www.trafficzam.com for imparting aggression management practices on the road. CRRI highlighted various causes of road rage incidences and highlighted to avoid such conditions through safe and calm attitude. CRRI also provided practical simulation practices among the students to show the after effect of road rage and stress on human health. This type of intervention is useful for those drivers who do not have any record of long time driving offences. Students and young drivers were taught to: a) Control the Level of frustration and how to Avoid Becoming a Victim of road rage b) Reducing Stress and Fatigue on the Road c) Regular maintenance of the Car d) Take journeys in easy stages e) Knowledge about road signs and road rules f) Knowledge regarding First Aid 3. STEPS TAKEN AND PLANNED FOR FURTHER MITIGATING ROAD RAGE CRRI had also planned for a collaborative project work on indentifying aggressive attitude among college students riding two wheelers with Dr Seema Melhotra, Associate Professor, Dept of Mental Health & Social Psychology, NIMHANS, Bangalore. This project is under review in CSIR.
60
61
Letter from Additional Commissioner of Police, Licensing Branch of Delhi Police inviting CRRI for the participation in the Anti Road Rage and Anti Gun Violence Campaign
62
--- On Wed, 3/12/08, seema mehrotra <[email protected]> wrote: From: seema mehrotra <[email protected]> Subject: Re: your inquiry regarding Research for Aggressive Driving and Road Rage behaviour among two wheeler users To: "Neelima Chakrabarty" <[email protected]> Cc: "manoj sharma" <[email protected]> Date: Wednesday, 3 December, 2008, 8:46 AM
Dear Dr Neelima: It was a pleasure to hear from you. My apologies for the delay in responding. The proposal I mentioned has been developed by three of us in the department (namely myself, Dr Paulomi and Dr Manoj). It aims to examine aggressive driving in a student sample and an intervention component in the second phase. 1. Please let us know whether you would like to join us as co-investigator on this project. In that case, we could collect data at Bangalore and you could collect data in Delhi . Subsequently, we could collate the results. 2. Due to practical constraints, it is not feasible to come to Delhi , specifically for this purpose. We will also not be able to mobilize any funds for travel. 3. Regarding the project that you mentioned, we would be happy to join as co investigators at some point if you so desire and if it adds value in some way to the work you are already in the process of carrying out . We look forward to the possibility of a fruitful and mutually satisfying collaboration,
Thanking you Sincerely Dr Seema Mehrotra Associate Professor Dept of Mental Health & Social Psychology NIMHANS, Bangalore 63
Fax and e-mail received from Dr.Seema Malhotra, Associate Professor, Dept of Mental Health & Social Psychology, NIMHANS, Bangalore
64
7. References 1. AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. (1997). Aggressive driving: three studies. Washington, DC:AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. 2. Bajpai ,Ravi Anger cause of most murders in city, article th published in Hindustan Times 4 August,2008. 3. Baron & Ransberger, 1978. R.A. Baron and V.M. Ransberger, Ambient temperature and the occurrence of collective violence: the long hot summer revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36 4 (1978), pp. 351360. 4. BTS USDOT, 1998. Bureau of Transportation Statistics (BTS): U.S. Department of Transportation (USDOT) (1998). Transportation Statistics Annual Report 1998 (pp. 77106) [On-line]. Available: http://www.bts.gov. 5. Buss & Durkee, 1957. A.H. Buss and A. Durkee, An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal of Counseling Psychology 21 (1957), pp. 343349. 6. Chase & Mills, 1973. L.J. Chase and N.H. Mills, Status of frustrators as a facilitator of aggression: a brief note. Journal of Psychology 84 (1973), pp. 225226 7. Cohen, W. and Tharp, M., 1997. Road rage. US News Online June; (http://www.drivers.com/issues/roadrage/2driv.htm). 8. Connell, D., & Joint, M. (1997). Driver Aggression. In AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, Aggressive driving: three studies. Washington, DC: AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. 9. Deaux, 1971. K.K. Deaux, Honking at the intersection: a replication and extension. Journal of Social Psychology 84 (1971), pp. 159 160. View Record in Scopus | Cited By in Scopus (10) 10. Doob & Gross, 1968. A.N. Doob and A.E. Gross, Status of frustration as an inhibitor of horn-honking responses. Journal of Social Psychology 76 (1968), pp. 213218.
65
11. Doob, A.N. and Gross, A.E., 1968. Status of frustrator as an inhibitor of horn-honking responses. Journal of Social Psychology 76, pp. 213218 12. Dorn & Matthews, 1992. L. Dorn and G. Matthews, Two further studies of personality correlates of driver stress. Personality and Individual Differences 13 (1992), pp. 949952. 13. Ellison et al., 1995. P.A. Ellison, J.M. Govern, H.L. Petri and M.H. Figler, Anonymity and aggressive driving behavior: a field study. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 10 1 (1995), pp. 265272. 14. Ellison, P.A., Govern, J.M., Petri, H.L. and Figler, M.H., 1995. Anonymity and aggressive driving behaviour: a field study. Journal of Social Behaviour and Personality 10, pp. 265272 15. Ellison-Potter, P. A. (1997). The effects of anonymity, aggressive stimuli, and trait anger on aggressive driving behaviour: a laboratory simulation. Ph.D. Dissertation, Colorado State University. 16. H.J. Eysenck and S.B.G. Eysenck, Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. , Hodder & Stoughton, London (1975). 17. Federal Highway Administration, 1998. Federal Highway Administration: Office of Highway Information Management. 1996 Highway Statistics (1998) (http://www.fhwa.dot.gov/ohim/1996/). 18. First et al., 1994. M.B. First, R.L. Spitzer, M. Gibbon and J.B.W. Williams, Structured Clinical Interview for Axis II Personality Disorders, version 2.0 Draft . , Biometrics Research Department, New York (1994, July). 19. First et al., 1995. M.B. First, R.L. Spitzer, M. Gibbon and J.B.W. Williams, Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis-I DisordersPatient Edition (SCID-I/P) (version 2.0). , Biometrics Research Department, New York (1995). 20. Fumento, M., 1998. Road rage is merely media mayhem. http://junkscience.com/sep98/roadrage.html 21. Galovski & Blanchard, 2002. T.E. Galovski and E.B. Blanchard, Intermittent explosive disorder and other psychiatric comorbidity
66
among 22.
court-referred
and
self-referred
aggressive
drivers.
Behaviour, Research, and Therapy 40 6 (2002), pp. 641651. Groeger, 1997. J.A. Groeger, Mood and driving: is there an and transport psychology: theory and application, effect of affect?. In: J.A. Rothengatter and E. Carbonell, Editors, Traffic 23. Pergamon, Amsterdam (1997), pp. 335342. Gulian et al., 1989. E. Gulian, G. Matthews, A.I. Glendon, D.R. Davies and L.M. Debney, Dimensions of driver stress. Ergonomics 32 6 (1989), pp. 585602. 24. Gulian et al., 1990. E. Gulian, A.I. Glendon, G. Matthews, D.R. Davies and L.M. Debney, The stress of driving: a diary study. Work and Stress 4 (1990), pp. 716. 25. Hankes-Drielsma, and 1974. M.P. identity. Hankes-Drielsma, , Queen's Driver aggression 26. line] http://www.aaafoundation.org/resources/index.cfm?button=agdrte xt). 27. Jonah, B.A., 1997. Sensation seeking and risky driving: a review and synthesis of the literature. Accident Analysis and Prevention 29, pp. 651665 28. Kenrick & McFarlane, 1986. D.T. Kenrick and S.W. McFarlane, Ambient temperature and horn-honking: a field study of the heat/aggression relationship. Environment and Behavior 18 (1986), pp. 179181. 29. Knapper & Cropley, 1981. C.K. Knapper and A.J. Cropley, Social and interpersonal factors in driving. In: G.M. Stephenson and J.M. Davies, Editors, Progress in Applied Social Psychology vol. 1, Wiley, London (1981), pp. 191220. frustrator University,
Department of Psychology, Kingston, Canada (1974). Joint, 1997. M. Joint, Road rage. In: AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, Editor, Aggressive driving: three studies (1997) [On-
67
30.
Larson, Editor, Steering clear of highway madness, BookPartners, Wilsonville, OR (1996), pp. 2528. 31. Larson, 1996b. J.A. Larson, Steering clear of highway madness: 32. a driver's guide to curbing stress and strain. ,
BookPartners, Wilsonville, OR (1996). Maiuro, 1998. R. Maiuro, Recovery: rage on the road [Online] 1998, Summer); http://www.icbc.com/Library/recovery/volume9/Number2/RageOn TheRoad/index.html/) 33. 34. Marsh & Collett, 1986. P. Marsh and P. Collett, Driving Martinez, 1997. Martinez, R. (1997, July 17). The statement passion: the psychology of the car. , Faber & Faber, Boston (1986). of the Honorable Recardo Martinez, M.D., Administrator, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration before the Sub-Committee on Surface Transportation, Committee on Transportation and Infrastructure, U.S. House of Representatives. 35. Martinez, R. (1997). Statement of the Honorable Ricardo Martinez, M. D., Administrator, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, before the U. S. House of Representatives, Committee on Transportation and Infrastructure, Subcommittee on Surface Transportation. Washington, DC, July 17. 36. Matthews et al., 1991. G. Matthews, L. Dorn and A.I. Glendon, Personality correlates of driver stress. Personality and Individual Differences 12 (1991), pp. 535549. 37. McGuire, 1956. F.L. McGuire, Psychological comparison of automobile drivers. U.S. Armed Forces Medical Journal 7 (1956), pp. 17411748. 38. McMurray, L., 1971. Emotional stress and driving performance: the effects of divorce. Behavioural Research in Highway Safety 1, pp. 100114 39. Milgram, 1965. S. Milgram, Some conditions of obedience and disobedience to authority. Human Relations 18 (1965), pp. 5776. Full Text via CrossRef
68
40. NHTSA. (1996). Traffic Safety Facts. (Report No. DOT SH 808 471). Washington DC: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, U. S. Department of Transportation. 41. Novaco et al., 1979. R.W. Novaco, D. Stokols, J. Campbell and J. Stokols, Transportation, stress and community psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology 7 4 (1979), pp. 361 380. 42. 43. 44. Novaco, 1993. R.W. Novaco, Clinicians ought to view anger Ross, 1940. H.L. Ross, Traffic accidents: a product of social Rule & Nesdale, 1976. B.G. Rule and A.R. Nesdale, contextually. Behaviour Change 10 4 (1993), pp. 208218. psychological conditions. Social Forces 18 (1940), pp. 569576. Environmental stressors, emotional arousal, and aggression. In: I.G. Sarason and C.D. Speilberger, Editors, Stress and anxiety vol. 3, Halsted, New York (1976), pp. 87103. 45. S.G. Stradling and D. Parker, 1997 Extending the theory of planned behaviour: the role of personal norm, instrumental beliefs, and affective beliefs in predicting driving violations. In: J.A. Rothengatter and E. Carbonell, Editors, Traffic and transport psychology: theory and application, Pergamon, Amsterdam (1997), pp. 367374. 46. Schaeffer et al., 1988. M.H. Schaeffer, S.W. Street, J.E. Singer and A. Baum, Effects of control on the stress reactions of commuters. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 18 11 (1988), pp. 944957. 47. Schwartz et al., 1981. G.E. Schwartz, D.A. Weinberger and J.A. Singer, Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine 43 (1981), pp. 343364. 48. Snyder, 1997. Snyder, D.S. (1997, July 17). Statement of David S. Snyder, Assistant General Counsel, American Insurance Association, representing advocates for highway and auto safety before the House Committee on Transportation and Infrastrusture, Surface Transportation Sub-Committee.
69
49.
R.E. Lushene, STAI manual for the statetrait inventory. , Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA (1970). 50. Spielberger, 1979. C.D. Spielberger, StateTrait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) professional manual. , Psychological Assessment Resources, Odessa, FL (1979). 51. Spielberger, C.D., Jacobs, G., Russell, S. and Crane, R.S., 1983. Assessment of anger: the StateTrait Anger Scale. In: Butcher, J.N. and Spielberger, C.D., Editors, 1983. Advances in personality assessment vol. 2, Lawrence Erlbaum, London, pp. 161189. 52. Stokols & Novaco, 1981. D. Stokols and R.W. Novaco, Transportation and well-being: an ecological perspective. In: I. Altman, J.F. Wohlevill and P.B. Everett, Editors, Transportation and well-being, Plenum, New York (1981), pp. 85130. 53. Stokols et al., 1978. D. Stokols, R.W. Novaco, J. Stokols and J. Campbell, Traffic congestion, Type A behavior, and stress. Journal of Applied Psychology 63 4 (1978), pp. 467480. 54. Stradling & Parker, 1997. S.G. Stradling and D. Parker, Extending the theory of planned behaviour: the role of personal norm, instrumental beliefs, and affective beliefs in predicting driving violations. In: J.A. Rothengatter and E. Carbonell, Editors, Traffic and transport psychology: theory and application, Pergamon, Amsterdam (1997), pp. 367374. 55. Tara E. Galovski , and Edward B. Blanchard Road rage: a domain for psychological intervention?Center for Stress and Anxiety Disorders, State University of New York at Albany, 1535 Western Avenue, Albany, NY 12203, USA 56. Tillman & Hobbs, 1949. W.A. Tillman and G.E. Hobbs, The accident-prone automobile driver: a study of the psychiatric and social background. American Journal of Psychiatry 106 (1949), pp. 321331.
70
57.
traffic accidents. American Journal of Psychiatry 142 (1985), pp. 538546. 58. C.W. Turner, J.F. Layton and L. Stanley-Simons, Naturalistic studies of aggressive behavior: aggressive stimuli, victim visibility, and horn-honking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31 6 (1975), pp. 10981107. 59. USDOT-NHSTA, 1998. U.S. Department of Transportation (USDOT), National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHSTA). (1998). Motor vehicle traffic crashes as a leading cause of death in the U.S., 1994. In Bureau of Transportation Statistics (BTS): U.S. Department of Transportation (USDOT) (Eds.), Transportation Statistics Annual Report 1998 (Technical report DOT H5 808 687) [On-line]. Available: http://www.bts.gov.
71