Gong 2011 Biolinguistics
Gong 2011 Biolinguistics
Gong 2011 Biolinguistics
net/publication/235998273
CITATIONS READS
2 133
2 authors, including:
Christophe Coupé
French National Centre for Scientific Research
71 PUBLICATIONS 521 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Christophe Coupé on 21 May 2014.
1. Overview
approaches adopted and adapted from disciplines other than linguistics. In this
review, we will go over these talks and present our opinions on future research
of linguistic complexity.
English or Korean learning (Lee et al. 2010) infants, as well as the acquisition data
of other languages.
Albert Bastardas-i-Boada (University of Barcelona) generalized a philoso-
phical, holistic view of language contact. He conceived of an ecosystem of lang-
uage, including brain/mind, social interaction, group, economics, media, and
political factors. All these dynamic factors co-produced and co-determined the
forms, usage, and evolution of language.
Unlike linguists, the anthropologist Thomas Schoenemann (Indiana
University) focused on complexity in the physical substrate of language (the
human brains) and of human behaviors. He advocated the theory of language-
brain coevolution (Deacon 1997), and suggested that an increasing complexity of
hominin conceptual understanding led to an increasing need for syntax and
grammar to fulfill efficient communications (Schoenemann 1999). He argued that
concepts were based upon networks connecting different brain regions, and that
the size of those regions across species was proportional to the degree of
elaboration of the functions they underlie. In the past, the increase in brain size
was correlated with the increase in degree of specialization of parts of the brain
(Schoenemann 2006).
The genetic linguist Jean-Marie Hombert (University of Lyon 2) focused on
the relation between population size, social complexity and language dispersal.
Based on genetic and demographic data, he suggested that Pygmy hunter-
gatherers and Bantu-speaking farmers in Central Africa shared a common
ancestry (Quintana-Murci et al. 2008, Berniell-Lee et al. 2009). This case study
illustrated that population size and hierarchy could be two important factors
within linguistic communities that helped develop linguistic complexity.
Apart from describing and circumscribing linguistic complexity, many talks tried
to propose general procedures or quantitative measures to evaluate different
aspects of linguistic complexity. Artificial intelligence expert Luc Steels
(University of Brussels & Sony CSL, Paris) presented a general procedure to
account for linguistic complexity. This procedure includes five steps: (i) describ-
ing a complex linguistic structure, (ii) identifying its function, (iii) reconstructing
processing and acquiring mechanisms for this structure, (iv) surveying its vari-
ations in languages, and (v) identifying its selective advantage. Such an approach
helped pinpoint the different factors that contributed to linguistic complexity. In
addition, Steels presented several simulation studies that explored the evolution
of complexity in semantics and syntax. These studies supported his recruitment
theory of language evolution (Steels 2009), stating that (i) strategies and structures
that could satisfy communicative needs, reduce cognitive efforts, and increase
social coherence could be adopted by language users and survive in languages,
and (ii) the emergence of linguistic complexity was a process of self-organization
of existing systems and of recruitment of new mechanisms in a cultural environ-
ment.
Statistical physicist Vittorio Loreto (Sapienza University of Rome & Insti-
tute for Scientific Interchange, Torino) pointed out that statistical physics served
Biolinguistics Forum 373
References