Kucseraj 76072
Kucseraj 76072
Kucseraj 76072
by
2009
THE DISSERTATION COMMITTEE FOR JOHN VINCENT KUCSERA, JR.
CERTIFIES THAT THIS IS THE APPROVED VERSION OF THE FOLLOWING
DISSERTATION:
Committee:
Kristin Neff
Richard Reddick
Diane Schallert
Tiffany Whittaker
RACIAL MINDFULNESS: EXPLORING THE INFLUENCE OF
MINDFULNESS ON RACIAL BIASES
by
DISSERTATION
Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of
in Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
For my mother.
RACIAL MINDFULNESS: EXPLORING THE INFLUENCE OF
MINDFULNESS ON RACIAL BIASES
Publication No._____________
We disbelieve it; we deny it; we even disguise it; but racial prejudice continues to
permeate the United States. As a result, researchers labor to determine variables that can
reduce these attitudes and consequently, improve social behavior. Three confirmed
conditions that can reduce racial attitudes include: (a) awareness to racial biases, (b)
motivation for bias reduction, and (c) cognitive strategies for prejudice regulation.
However, racial awareness are usually nonexistent for White Americans, and when
introduced, racial awareness can cause negative outcomes, such as guilt or denial, that
can decrease motivation to reduce one’s prejudice levels. The construct and practices of
mindfulness may provide a solution to these limitations and help reduce racial prejudice
v
The present dissertation explored the initial steps of this racial mindfulness
mindfulness on their racial prejudice levels using structural equation modeling. Because
mindfulness can increase awareness to stimuli, mindfulness could meet the first prejudice
reduction condition (i.e., raise awareness to racial stimuli), and therefore, reduce racial
prejudice levels directly. In addition, mindfulness has been found to increase similar
variables that influences motivation to reduce racial prejudice levels, such as empathy
decrease their racial prejudice levels indirectly as well. Results from this study indicated
that mindfulness did not reduce racial prejudice levels directly or indirectly, although
there were some methodology limitations that could have obscured the results.
attenuate the negative affects that can arise when Whites first become aware of racial
biases, as mindfulness has been found to mitigate ego defensiveness and negative
article from White participants identified as holding a high and low degree of general
mindfulness were subject to content analysis. The results indicated that participants with
privilege and less negative reactions. The findings support the need to create and explore
vi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Delimitations .................................................................................................................. 7
Dissertation Overview.................................................................................................... 8
Chapter Summary......................................................................................................... 39
vii
Cognitive Regulatory Strategies.............................................................................. 45
Mindfulness .................................................................................................................. 51
Chapter Summary......................................................................................................... 67
Method ......................................................................................................................... 70
Participants .............................................................................................................. 70
Procedure ................................................................................................................. 71
Measures.................................................................................................................. 73
Results .......................................................................................................................... 87
viii
Path Coefficients and Effects .................................................................................. 97
Measures................................................................................................................ 108
Appendix D: Pilot Study for the Social Re(De)categorization Scale ............................. 145
ix
Appendix F: Symbolic Racism 2000 Scale (SR2K) ....................................................... 149
Vita.................................................................................................................................. 175
x
LIST OF TABLES
Table 4: Correlations, Means, Standard Deviations, and Alphas among Total Scales and
Subscales ........................................................................................................................... 89
Table 8: Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations among Measured Variables in the
Table 9: Model Fit Indices and Comparisons for Competing Measurement Models ....... 95
Table 10: Model Fit Indices and Comparisons for Competing Structural Models ........... 97
Table 11: Unstandardized and Standardized Path Coefficients for the Final Structural
Table 12: Standardized Effects for the Final Structural Equation Model ......................... 99
Table 13: Themes and Subthemes of the Content Analysis ........................................... 115
Table 14: Themes and Subthemes by Participants in the Low Mindfulness Group ....... 128
Table 15: Themes and Subthemes by Participants in the High Mindfulness Group ...... 129
xi
Table 16: Rotated Pattern and Structure Matrices for Responses to the Social
xii
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 1: Net Income and Net Worth of White and Black Families ................................ 12
xiii
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION
Within the last 40 years, great strides have been made in the United States (U.S.)
Institutional barriers for advancement of people of color are departing. The U.S. is
witnessing its first President of color. In every social sector, America appears closer to
having an egalitarian society where all races have an equal opportunity to achieve the
Yet, despite our progress, inequalities continue to exist between Whites and
people of color. Persisting racial differences are found in social domains of education
(Frankenberg, Lee, & Orfield, 2003), housing, employment (Quillian, 2006), health care
(Nazroo, 2003), and income (Isaacs, 2007). Researchers have found that a substantial
portion of these inequalities can be explained by past and present racial discrimination
(i.e., behavior), which can be explained by existing but hidden racial prejudice (i.e.,
reduce racial prejudice and consequently, improve social behavior. Researchers (e.g.,
Devine, 2001; Dovidio & Gaertner, 1999) indicate three conditions are necessary to
reduce racial attitudes. The first condition is an individual must be aware of and attentive
to racial stimuli, such as hidden prejudice and existing racial discrimination. The second
condition is the individual must be motivated to reduce or regulate such biases. This
desire can result from the drive to reduce negative feelings that occur when an individual
first becomes conscious of racial biases and experiences dissonance between these biases
1
and their egalitarian values or social norms (i.e., cognitive dissonance). In addition,
decategorization), and empathy are other variables that prior research has found to
motivate individuals to reduce racial prejudice levels. Finally, the third condition
necessary for prejudice reduction is an individual, once aware and motivated, must have
formidable task. First, most White Americans are unaware of hidden racial prejudice,
Whites rationalize that these differences stem from a choice or lack of motivation from
rather than racial prejudice or discrimination (Henkel, Dovidio, & Gaertner, 2006).
Further, when becoming aware of racial prejudice or discrimination, the negative feelings
of guilt and compunction that Whites may experience can cause many to deny or debunk
further that such biases exist (Quillian, 2006). For example, negative emotions arising
from cognitive dissonance have been found to prevent social action and prejudice
reduction (Pedersen, Walker, & Wise, 2005), as well as even increase Whites’ racial
prejudice levels (Branscombe, Schmitt, & Schiffhauer, 2007). Finally, even when White
Americans are (made) aware of racial biases and positively overcome such initial
negative emotions, the reduction in racial prejudice and discrimination are difficult to
2
cognitive resources required to continue consciousness and regulation of racial biases
(Devine, 1989).
These limitations (i.e., Whites are often unaware of racial biases, negative
outcomes can arise from Whites’ awareness to racial biases, and racial prejudice
reduction can be short-lived) combined with the social problem that inequalities and
racial biases continue to exist, create an arduous issue for researchers and educators to
tackle. Generally, scholars direct most of their efforts in exploring variables and
interventions that can increase Whites’ awareness and attention to racial stimuli, with the
always occur, and when it does, reduction may not last long. A variable that might
influence Whites’ attention and awareness to racial biases; increase their level of
stimuli with open receptivity or acceptance (Brown & Ryan, 2003). This mode of
processing varies naturally across individuals, coming easier to some than others. But like
most cognitive states, mindfulness may be enhanced by mental exercises, ancient and
new.
3
Although a rather simple concept, the benefits of mindfulness are considerable.
An increase in mindfulness has been found to increase connectedness with others and
empathy (Miller, 1995; Shapiro, Brown, & Biegel, 2007; Shapiro, Schwartz, & Bonner,
& Tori, 1997). A rise in mindfulness has also been found to lead to a greater perceptivity
and sensitivity to one’s environment, more openness to new information, creation of new
1997). Moreover, mindfulness has been found positively related to nonjudgment and self-
and preconception. Thich Nhat Hanh (1975), a notable Vietnamese Zen master, suggests
that mindfulness could affect an individual’s perception of reality, indirectly freeing one
from prejudice and stereotypes. Empirically, Langer, Bashner, and Chanowitz (1985)
supported Hanh’s assertion and found that teaching children mindfulness exercises
reduced their erroneous and indiscriminate prejudice towards individuals with physical
disabilities. In addition, Lillis and Hayes (2007) found that a racial prejudice session with
towards racial biases from pre to post and in comparison to a prejudice awareness session
4
Therefore, mindfulness may reduce one’s levels of racial prejudice indirectly
more importantly, acceptance to racial biases. To date, only one published study has
investigated mindfulness with racial prejudice (i.e., Lillis & Hayes, 2007), while a
handful of scholars propose this plausible connection (Langer & Moldoveanu, 2000; Orr,
2002; Riskin, 2004; Vacarr, 2003). In this dissertation, I explain and explore how White
students’ degree of mindfulness can influence their degree of racial prejudice directly and
racial group membership, and empathy. I also discuss and investigate how White
students’ mindfulness can decrease the negative outcomes that can arise from cognitive
dissonance and increase their acceptance towards racial biases, such as with the construct
of White privilege.
a. does mindfulness attenuate the negative effects that can arise from
cognitive dissonance?
5
b. does mindfulness influence acceptance towards the racial bias of
White privilege?
Structural equation modeling is then used to explore the structural model that White
students’ mindfulness can influence their racial prejudice directly and indirectly through
controlling for previous racial outgroup contact. It is hypothesized that participants with a
White person. From the study’s theoretical framework, it is expected that participants
with a higher degree of mindfulness exhibit greater acceptance to White privilege and
to address the limitations that Whites often encounter in racial interventions and
practices incorporated before, during, and after racial training) may serve as a solution.
6
However, explorative research is first needed. The studies within this dissertation serve as
initial steps for such research by exploring the effects of participants’ degree of general
mindfulness on their racial prejudice levels, and evaluating White participants’ reactions
The results will help determine the value of creating such a racial mindfulness
Delimitations
In general within this dissertation, the term race refers to populations that humans
have socially constructed for categorization purposes. With 94% physical variation lying
within and 6% genetic variation existing between so-called racial groups, the belief that
race refers to different human races of populations derived from genetic, ancestral, or
1998). I have also deliberately chosen to focus on the current oppression experienced by
people of color, although other marginalized populations in America could have been
selected such as, but not limited to, women, non-Christians, the Lesbian Gay Bisexual
Transgender and Queer/Questioning (LGBTQ) community, the young and the old, and
people with disabilities. Discrimination and prejudice based on the social construct of
race was chosen for three main reasons. First, the long history of racial discrimination
and prejudice has created a strong research base to build upon, such as supported
sound instruments. Additional motivation resulted from the current publicity and
immediacy of race that has been sparked by the 2008 U.S. presidential election. Finally,
7
and on a personal note, identifying as a White person with family members that I identify
awareness, attention, and acceptance of racial biases towards Blacks. The choice of
Whites stems from their position as an advantaged racial group, holding power, status,
and privilege that can better serve a greater good by influencing social change. In
addition, scholars continue to struggle with how to tackle White privilege and prejudice
in America (Manglitz, Johnson-Bailey, & Cervero, 2005) and mindfulness may serve as
an educational program for these concerns in the future. In relation to marginalized racial
groups, a focus has been placed on Whites’ prejudiced attitudes towards Blacks because
of the extensive history of discrimination between and the established research based
Dissertation Overview
consisting of the two studies explored in this dissertation. Chapters 5 and 6 present the
8
for future research.
9
CHAPTER 2: THE PROBLEM
the body politic and economic would have been a herculean task; but when to the
inherent difficulties of so delicate and nice a social operation were added the spite
and hate of conflict…when suspicion and cruelty were rife…the work of any
Racial Inequalities
If the prominent intellectual leader and political activist W.E.B. Du Bois was
alive today, I believe he would applaud the social uplifting of Blacks and other
marginalized groups in America over the last 40 years. However, I also think his
celebration would be cut short, as these racial groups, as a whole, have yet to reach an
inequalities between White and marginalized racial groups continue to exist in almost
every social sector. Examples of racial inequalities can be observed in domains of net
income and worth, home equity and ownership, and academic success and schooling.
and have more net worth than Black families. The median net income in 2001 of White
families was roughly $55,000 but only $34,000 for Black families (see Figure 1; Kaplan
& Valls, 2007). This difference has been found apparent for White compared to Black or
10
Latino males, even after controlling for education attainment, employment sector, human
capital, and institutional differences (Sidanious & Veniegas, 2000). Many studies have
also indicated that Whites have greater intergenerational and intragenerational transfer
(i.e., economic mobility) than marginalized racial groups (Corcoran 1995; Hertz 2005,
2006; Isaacs 2007; Kearney, 2006; McBrier & Wilson, 2004). Economic mobility refers
to the change in one’s economic situation over one’s lifetime (intragenerational) or from
mobility through a longitudinal study between Black and White families and found that
White children live in families with much higher income, are more likely to surpass
parental income, and more likely to move up the economic ladder than Black children; in
fact, Black children were more likely to slide down the economic ladder in comparison to
their parents.
Racial inequalities in net worth are even larger than differences in net income. As
presented in Figure 1 above, White families in 2004 had a median of $118,300 and an
average of $534,000 net worth, whereas Black families had a median of only $11,800 and
11
Figure 1. Net income and net worth comparisons of White and Black families.
Note: Net income figures from “Housing discrimination as a basis for Black reparations,” by J. Kaplan and
A.Valls, 2007, Public Affairs Quarterly, 21, p. 258. Net worth figures from Wolff, 2007, as cited from
“Foreclosed: State of the dream 2008,” by A. Rivera, B. Cotto-Escalera, D. Anisha, J. Huezo, and D.
Muhammad, 2008, Boston, MA: United for a Fair Economy. p. 30.
The consideration of home equity within net worth figures offers an explanation
for the large difference in net worth between White and marginalized racial groups. Black
and Latino families, on average, are far less likely to own a home than White families,
and if they do own, the home is valued less and appreciates at a lower rate than the
average home owned by a White family (Flippen 2004). The racial difference in home
equity, and as a result, net worth, is also predicted to grow larger within the next few
years due to the recent crash of the subprime (i.e., high interest) housing loan market.
According to Rivera et al. (2008), people of color were one of the best candidates for
these loans and are three times more likely to have subprime mortgage loans than Whites.
12
This finding suggests that Black and Latino families have and will continue to
foreclosures, and more spillover effects (e.g., crime, devaluation of neighborhoods) than
White families.
White Americans also tend to enjoy greater academic success and access to
quality education than Blacks and Latinos (Sidanious, & Veniegas, 2000). Data from the
and Latino students have math and reading skills similar to those of White middle-school
students (Education Trust, 2003a, 2003b). In addition, close to 50% of Black students and
nearly 40% of Latino students attend high schools in which graduation is not the norm
(Balfanz & Legters, 2004). These disparities can be explained by the differences in
Racial differences in educational quality are evident through the lack of racial
integration in schools and the resulting quality of schools attended by students of color.
According to the Civil Rights Project, the proportion of Black students in majority-White
schools has decreased to a level lower than 1968, resulting in an emergence of virtually
all minority campuses, called apartheid schools where “…enormous poverty, limited
resources, and social and health problems of many types are concentrated” (Frankenberg
13
These apartheid schools or districts have been found to contain lower teacher
quality (Peske & Haycock, 2006) and retention (Boyd, Grossman, Lankford, Loeb, &
Wyckoff, 2007); less access to innovative or challenging curricula (Barth, 2003); less
facility and capital improvements (Filardo, Vincent, Sung, & Stein, 2006); and receive
substantially less state and local money per student in comparison to majority White
schools and districts (Education Trust, 2003a). The consequence of these separate and
unequal schools on marginalized individuals and community was the foundation for
Brown vs. Board of Education (1954). Now, 55 years later, these negative effects may
still exist, as Jonathon Kozol (1991), a nonfiction writer and activist, highlighted:
Children [of color], of course, don't understand at first that they are being cheated.
They come to school with a degree of faith and optimism, and they often seem to
thrive during the first few years. It is sometimes not until the third grade that their
In short, racial inequalities between Whites and people of color exist in America
today. But what factors are influencing these inequalities? In the words of Du Bois
(1901), inequalities are attributed to the herculean task of socially uplifting once
considered lesser humans to an assured and self-sustaining position that is combined with
current unwilling neighbors, spite and hate, and suspicion and cruelty. That is, racial
inequalities are attributed to past and current effects of racial discrimination and
prejudice. Over a hundred years later, and despite forty years of improvement, research
14
Before we move to these findings, it is important to note that most White
Americans refute that racial discrimination and prejudice are attributing to racial
inequalities between Whites and people of color. Instead national surveys suggest that
racial groups as the main factor influencing their current inequalities (e.g., “If they
[people of color] only worked harder;” Schuman, Steeh, Bobo, & Krysan, 1997). Later in
this dissertation, however, we will soon see that this claim is itself a racial prejudiced
belief.
Racial Discrimination
I argue in this section that past and modern racial discrimination substantially
influences the racial inequalities existing today between Whites and people of color.
Racial discrimination is defined here as the differential treatment on the basis of race or
inadequately justified factors other than race that disadvantages a racial group (Blank,
Dabady, & Citro, 2004). This section presents only a sample of illustrations to show how
Past Discrimination
The U.S. clearly has had a troubled history of racial discrimination – if this term
can even be used to reference the inhumane treatment many marginalized racial groups
experienced. By the nineteenth century, the belief in Manifest Destiny1 and social
1
A term originating in the 19th century to justify the United States's westward expansion. Manifest Destiny
implied that it was divine destiny to spread democracy by colonizing land inhabited by indigenous people
in North America and expanding the United States into Mexican territory.
15
Darwinism (as well as supporting but falsified scientific evidence – see Gould, 1996),
allowed the White race to justify the systematic dislocation, segregation, annihilation, or
1998), African Americans (Franklin & Moss, 2000), and Mexican Americans (Acuña,
2007).
led to a cognitive shift of Americans’ dominant beliefs towards racial superiority and
justification for discrimination. Ironically, one person we could attribute for this
transformation is Adolf Hitler. The onset of World War II and Hitler’s “…reliance on
those schemas as illegitimate covers for hate-based injustice – a theme that was
reinforced by the civil rights movements of the 1960s and 1970s” (Hanson & Hanson,
2006, p. 442). To illustrate, how could White Americans at the time explain the
discriminatory treatment happening on their own soil while concurrently shun the
injustice occurring halfway across the world? To deal with this contradiction, Americans
began to distance themselves from notions of racial superiority and outright forms of
discrimination. Yet, turning off this behavior was not as simple as turning off a switch.
2001).
American history. One classic illustration was the discrimination practices within housing
loan programs created around the New Deal era, such as the Federal Housing Authority
16
(FHA loans) and the Veteran Administration (VA loans). These loan programs greatly
influenced decades of rapid home equity growth for Whites, leading to a significant
source of wealth accumulation (Kaplan & Valls, 2007). However, these programs
substantially discriminated against people of color. More than 98% of the $120 billion in
home loans issued between 1934 and 1962 went to White homebuyers (California
Newsreel, 2003) due to a variety of subtle racial discrimination practices within the FHA
and VA loan systems, such as indicating Black or mixed neighborhoods were uninsurable
and promoting the use of racial covenants (Jackson 1985; Kaplan & Valls, 2007;
Katznelson 2005).
Charles, 2003), restricted most people of color to live in urban cities, while the majority
of Whites moved to suburban areas. The effects of this move (often referenced as White
flight) influenced many urban businesses to transfer to suburbia as well, which greatly
reduced employment prospects for people of color. In addition, White flight helped
establish the Federal Highway Act that connected suburbs to cities, while at the same
(Kaplan & Valls, 2007). In any area with poverty and limited employment, other social
ills have been known to follow, such as crime and demoralization – both of which further
perpetuated the rippling effect of racial discrimination for people of color (Wilson, 1996).
As a result, this residential segregation has been referred to as the linchpin of racial
17
underfunded, segregated schools…inferior public services, businesses, and recreational
Past racial discrimination not only blatantly oppressed people of color but also
subtly denied them access to opportunities for wealth accumulation. Whites, on the other
hand, were silently privileged with such opportunities of higher wages, access to home
and business loans, higher quality schools, access to and tuition for college, closer
employment opportunities or the ability to purchase a car to reach employment, and many
other advantages, just from being White (Shapiro, 2005). In short, “American laws,
policies, practices, customs, and expectations quietly and situationally combined in the
last century to maintain, and even expand, the longstanding gap between Whites and
Current Discrimination
How can one measure such practices in this egalitarian era when discrimination is illegal
and often hidden or denied? How can discrimination be disentangled from other
differences across racial groups, arising from culture or past discrimination practices,
such as family size and stability, parenting style, education quality and importance,
18
Researchers measuring current discrimination use a variety of approaches to
address the following counterfactual: Would the observed outcome for an individual or
group be different had the individual or group been of a different race? Although a
multimethod approach is recommended, audit studies provide the best singular approach
to addressing this counterfactual, inferring that an adverse outcome is likely the result of
racial discrimination (Blank et al., 2004). Generally, a (racial) field audit study consists
(e.g., applying for a job) from paired testers with similar and matched characteristics
known to influence the outcome variable, such as same educational experience, but who
Most (racial) field audit studies have been employed to investigate the presence
seem appropriate given that residential segregation is referred to as the lynchpin of racial
inequalities and employment opportunities are assumed to provide “a way out” through
upward economic mobility. The U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development
(HUD) has conducted three of the largest audit studies in 1977 (Wienk, Reid, Herbig, &
Lee, 1979, as cited in Quillian, 2006), 1989 (Yinger, 1993), and 2000 (Turner, Ross,
Galster, & Yinger, 2002) to explore national housing discrimination for Black, Hispanic,
19
In the 2000 study (Turner et al., 2002), pairs of auditors were recruited that
consisted of a White and a person of color who were matched on gender and age, and
assigned similar socioeconomic characteristics, such as marital status, family size, and
income. These paired auditors were also given training on how to behave in front of a
randomly selected agent in order to be as identical as possible except for race. Turner et
al. explored four discriminatory practices (variables) in rental and sales markets for each
matched pair, and investigated a fifth practice for sales markets only: 1) whether the
auditor is told the unit is available and told of other units (availability), 2) whether the
auditor can look at the unit (inspection), 3) cost of the unit (cost), 4) the extent of
encouragement to rent the unit by the realtor (encouragement), and for sales markets, 5)
whether the auditor is steered towards neighborhoods that match the auditor’s race (racial
steering).
The results of the study indicate that Whites were significantly favored in terms of
In sale markets, Whites were significantly favored in all practices compared to Blacks,
cost and steering in comparison to Hispanics, and all practices except racial steering in
comparison to Asians. This national study indicates that marginalized racial groups in the
(Bendick, Jackson, Reinoso, 1994; Pager, 2003). Bertrand and Mullainathan (2004)
conducted one of the largest employment field tests by investigating callback rates to
4,890 resumes sent to over 1,300 job postings in Chicago and Boston newspapers. In this
20
study, the researchers used actual resumes, stripped them of identification information,
duplicated each resume, and categorized the pair (resume and corresponding copy) into a
high or low qualified resume group. Following, two resumes from each group were
assigned to a job posting and were randomly given stereotypical White-sounding names
(e.g., Greg Smith) and stereotypical Black-sounding names (e.g., Jamal Williams). The
results from over 1,300 job postings indicated that resumes with White-sounding names
received twice as many callbacks than the same resumes with Black-sounding names.
Further, the findings showed that low-quality resumes with White-sounding names
received a higher callback rate (10%) than high-quality resumes with Black-sounding
Sidanius and Pratto (1999) summarized more than 19 major employment audits in
five different nations (U.S.A., Germany, England, Canada, and Holland). Regardless of
the nation and with only a few exceptions, the results indicated a similar finding: a
groups. In addition, these studies also indicated that discrimination occurred at all stages
of the employment process for marginalized racial groups, such as fewer opportunities to
interview, a lower starting salary, and less likely to be directed towards jobs with greater
Results from these housing and employment audit tests, provides a sample of
This finding, coupled with the legacy effects of past inhumane and subtle discriminatory
treatment, presents a case that racial discrimination has created and continues to create
21
societal and individual advantages for White Americans, and disadvantages for everyone
else. The general catalyst for such racial discrimination is racial biases that exist within
people’s heads, that is, racial prejudice, which is the next area of discussion.
Racial Prejudice
that can result in a range of attitudes towards racial outgroups (Allport, 1954; Brewer,
1999; Fiske, 2005). In addition, this favorable, racial attitude can consist of an emotion, a
cognition (e.g., stereotype), or a combination of the two (Henkel et al., 2006). As a result,
the range of attitudes towards racial outgroups can vary from feelings of indifference to
beliefs. This in-group love, whether it leads to out-group hate or not, is believed to be the
attitudes are suggested to arise from normative cognitive processes, which are then
section focuses on the discussion and interplay between cognitive and motivational
Cognitive Processes
At its root, prejudice is believed to result from the normative and necessary
complex and stimulating world. As Gordon Allport once indicated, “The human mind
22
must think with the aid of categories…Once formed, categories are the basis for normal
prejudgment. We cannot possibly avoid this process. Orderly living depends on it” (1954,
p. 19).
provide detailed frameworks and explanations for this normative categorization process.
The first shared assumption is that the human mind is limited in capacity.
Depending on the specific theory, this limitation can occur at various points of processing
(e.g., selection or attention, Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; perception, Kohler, 1959;
interpretation, Anderson, 1984). A second shared postulation is that the human mind
develops and activates processes (e.g., selective attention, Gestalt effect, chunking,
schemata) that are based on stimuli categorization in order to compensate for limited
processing capabilities. A third premise is that this categorization process often becomes
automatic, again to free our limited cognitive capacity (Baumeister & Sommer, 1997); as
a result, the effect of automatic category activation can influence behavior without one’s
intention or awareness (e.g., Devine, 1989). In fact, “…most of a person’s everyday life
is determined not by their conscious intentions and deliberate choices but by mental
Chartrand, 1999, p. 462). A fourth assumption is that once categories (i.e., schemata,
templates, stereotypes) are formed, new external information is quickly assimilated (or
accreted) and accommodated (or tuned) to preexisting categories, allowing for effective
23
cognitive development and efficient thought processing (e.g., Rumbelhart & Norman,
1978). A final aspect across theories is that humans strive to remain in a state of category
equilibrium (e.g., Piaget, 1985). Therefore, external stimuli that contradict, or cannot be
assimilated or accommodated into one’s internal mental structures, are either created into
stimulated, fast-paced world. However, as with any benefit, there are tradeoffs. At the
& Corneille, 2005). Also, categorizations can become inflexible and undifferentiated due
to a lack of critical analysis, unless tied to one’s self-interest. As Allport (1954) indicated,
experience…life is just too short to have differentiated concepts about everything” (p.
groups. Broadly, individuals begin to categorize themselves with individuals who are
similar (ingroups) from individuals who are different (outgroups; Allport, 1954). Due to
consistency, and durability of group categories (Yzebyt & Corneille, 2005). As a result,
24
when an outgroup member is associated with a negative trait, aptitude, or behavior; these
simply because they are all alike. Combine this category-based assumption with a process
that is automatic, often subconscious, and generally inaccurate; and the result is a
Motivational Processes
favoritism of the ingroup over the outgroup. One motivational process influencing such
ingroup favoritism is the theoretical assumption that a person’s social identity is derived
from memberships in social groups (e.g., Social Identity Theory, Tajfel & Turner, 1979;
Self-Categorization Theory, Turner, 1985). Just as people are likely to believe and
support good things about themselves (i.e., hold a positive self-identity or boost their self-
esteem), they are likely to believe and support good things about their identified groups
(i.e., hold a positive social identity; Tajfel, 1978). This need for holding a positive view
of one’s own group causes people to enhance views of their ingroup while, at times,
derogating outgroups (e.g., ethnocentrism). In fact, numerous studies have found that
intergroup biases can result from participants categorizing themselves into groups based
on the most minimal and arbitrary factors – a phenomenon referred to as the minimal
threats, conflict, or competition between ingroups and outgroups. At the core of various
theories and models explaining different aspects of this process (e.g., Realistic Group
25
Conflict, LeVine & Campbell, 1972; Integrated Threat Theory of Prejudice, Stephan &
Stephan, 2000; Social Dominance Theory, Sidanius & Pratto, 1999; System Justification
Theory, Jost & Banaji, 1994; and Instrumental Model of Group Conflict, Esses, Jackson,
& Armstrong, 1998) lies the central assumption that ingroups will be favored and
outgroups, at times, will be discriminated against due to the perception of threat, conflict,
or competition (Esses, Jackson, Dovidio, & Hodson, 2005). The classic Robbers Cave
to two groups and brought them to a campsite (Robbers Cave State Park in Oklahoma).
The groups were first kept isolated from one another. After a handful of days, and as the
students began to become more aware of the other group’s presence, the researchers
initiated competition activities between the groups. These activities lasted a couple of
days, and for the remaining days, the researchers created a number of superordinate
scenarios that affected both groups, such as the camp’s drinking water supply running
dry. The researchers found that competition between groups produced intergroup biases
and discrimination, whereas cooperation and interdependence reduced such attitudes and
such as favoring one’s collective identity and perceiving group conflict or competition,
may give rise to not only ingroup love, but also outgroup hate (Brewer, 1999). These
processes are not only the cause and influence of racial prejudice, but also the unequal or
26
Forms and Measurement of Racial Prejudice
Most people think of racial prejudice in its most obvious and blatant form. Within
psychology, prejudice is seen as much more complex and multifaceted. From changing
social norms and legislature acts in the last 40 years, researchers have found that blatant
people can hold racial attitudes not only at the conscious level, but also at the
subconscious level.
more overt prejudice form. One example is the onset of racial microaggressions, referred
to as “…subtle, stunning, often automatic, and non-verbal exchanges, which are ‘put
downs’” (Pierce, Carew, Pierce-Gonzalez, & Willis, 1978, p. 66). Examples of racial
(suggesting that this is unusual), asking an Asian-American, “where were you born”
(suggesting that he or she is not American), stating “I’m not racist; I have several Black
friends” (indicating immunity to racism and solutions), asking a Black person “why do
a lower socioeconomic neighborhood to purchase a house (suggesting that they are not
affluent; Sue et al., 2007). Ironically, racial microaggressions have been found to
significantly increase more racial anger and frustration, and lower performance, and self-
27
esteem within people of color than more overt forms of discrimination (Solórzano, Ceja,
invisible nature of these prejudice forms, and their consequents such as microaggressions,
prevent people from realizing, tackling, and regulating their attitudes and behavior, as
well as their role in racial inequality. The next section provides a brief review of these
prejudice.
Before the Civil Rights era, racial prejudice was openly expressed and
laws; see Klarman, 2004). Researchers have referred to this openly expressed and
asked direct survey questions about the biological inferiority or treatment of marginalized
racial groups. For example, in 1942 and 1945, the National Opinion Research Center (as
cited in Schuman et al., 1997) found that 53% of Whites agreed that their race was
intellectually superior to Blacks and 55% of Whites agreed that Whites should obtain the
Near the end of the Civil Rights movement, legal discrimination was no longer
Fazio, 1997). These normative and legislature pressures led to a decline in blatant
28
prejudice on survey questionnaires (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1998). By the mid-1990s, a
survey exploring Whites’ support for equal treatment regardless of race received nearly
unanimous support. As a result, researchers who first analyzed racial attitude surveys
suggested that racial prejudice is on the decline to nonexistence in the U.S. (e.g., see
suggest that racial prejudice is persisting in our society (Gaertner & Dovidio, 2005;
Quillian, 2006). Therefore, many theorists have argued that racial prejudice has not
This evolved racial prejudice has been theorized and coined various names (see
Table 1, first three columns). Despite the variation, there are three overlapping
components across these theories. First, most Americans today possess normative racial
attitudes and hold (or at least follow) egalitarian values. A second overlapping theme is
that most Americans may not express these racial attitudes and behaviors blatantly due to
current legislation and egalitarian social norms, aside from a small share of traditional
racists. Finally, and as a result, most Americans will generally express their prejudice
indirectly or subtly as when racial beliefs or behavior can be justified on some other
29
Table 1
Subtle Conscious Racial Prejudice Theories
Name Primary citation Brief description Set
Reject blatant prejudice but view marginalized
McConahay
Modern racism racial groups as receiving unfair, preferential
(1986)
treatment.
View marginalized racial groups as not trying
hard enough to overcome difficulties they face
Racial Kinder and Sanders
and taking what they have not earned; Prejudice
resentment (1996)
is expressed in the language of American
individualism.
First set
Antipathy towards an out-group expressed by
Pettigrew and defending one’s in-group values, exaggerating
Subtle prejudice
Meertens (1995) out-group differences, and denying positive
emotional responses towards out-group members.
Reject blatant prejudice but express prejudice
indirectly by opposing policies that could help
Sears
Symbolic racism marginalized racial groups because it runs against
(1988)
learned morals and values. Similar to modern
racism.
Experience a conflict between positive and
Ambivalent Katz and Hass
negative emotions towards marginalized racial
racism (1988)
groups. Second set
Gaertner and Dovidio Believe in egalitarianism but have a personal
Aversive racism
(1986) aversion towards marginalized racial groups.
Note: From “The Symbolic Racism 2000 Scale,” by P. J. Henry and D. O. Sears, 2002, Political
Psychology, 23, pp. 253-283.
Subtle racial prejudice theories can be categorized into two different sets (Table 1,
last column). The first set includes symbolic racism, modern racism, racial resentment,
and subtle prejudice theories. These theories conceptualize prejudice each a little
differently but with the shared assumption that: Whites harbor negative feelings or
stereotypes towards marginalized racial groups, they express this prejudice indirectly or
secretly by opposing public or social policies for preferential treatment, and justify their
negative attitudes are due to marginalized racial groups (or public policies developed for
these groups) violating or will possibly violate traditional American values (Henry &
30
Sears, 2002). For example, Whites may view affirmative action programs as unwarranted
opportunity. They may view these programs as unfair impositions to the just and fair
society of America and may question, “Why should they [marginalized racial groups] get
special treatment?”
The major consequence of these theories is that subtle racial attitudes may
influence Whites to discriminate and block public policies that could improve racial
inequality and discrimination. Many researchers have found this set of theories helps
explain why Whites oppose affirmative action, welfare spending, tax-reduction policies
(Kinder & Sanders, 1996; Sears, 1988), why Whites oppose race-targeted government
polices more than policies for the poor (Bobo & Kluegel, 1993) and why Black
candidates for political office become so controversial (Kinder & Sears, 1981).
The second set of subtle racial prejudice theories, aversive and ambivalent racism,
share some of the same features as the first set, but involve more mixed emotions and are
more common attitudes of liberal and educated Whites. Sociologists Gaertner and
Dovidio (1986) described their aversive racism as the following. Whites will tend to
suppress or avoid expressing their racial biases because of their egalitarian self-image.
Consequently, they will tend to experience anxiety and discomfort during interracial
situations and try to avoid these situations or at least from “appearing racist.” These
individuals will regularly engage in forms of aversion and manifest their feelings in
31
The description for the theory of ambivalent racism is somewhat similar to
aversive racism. Katz and Hass (1988) suggested that Whites have both pro-Black
feelings rooted in egalitarianism and sympathy, and anti-Black feelings due to dissonance
with American (i.e., White) values. Both positive and negative feelings towards Blacks
exist simultaneously within individuals, can create tension and discomfort, and can help
explain the ambivalence for individuals endorsing egalitarian values but failing to support
victims of discrimination or the “underdog” (e.g., “Yes, there should be more college
scholarships for Blacks”). However, in situations that run against American values such
American values may actually represent White values; Devos & Banaji, 2005)
ambivalent racists might be negative toward Blacks (e.g., “But this should not come as an
subtle racial theories—they can lead to resisting public policies designed to increase
racial equality (Katz & Hass, 1988). In addition, researchers have found that
prevent intergroup contact or integration. One classic example by Gaertner and Dovidio
(1977) investigated how many times White participants suggested they would help either
a White or a Black victim in two different scenarios: where the White participant was the
only witness, or where the White participant was part of a group of White witnesses. The
32
researchers found that White participants would help both victims regardless of race
when they were the only witness. However, when the White participant was part of a
group of witnesses, the researchers found that White participants helped Black victims
38% of the time and White victims 75% of the time. Gaertner and Dovidio speculated
that these White participants in this study rationalized a reason not to help based on some
other factor than race (i.e., “There are other witnesses that can help”). These researchers
have also found more recent and similar results with experiments investigating hiring and
criminal justice decisions (for a review see Gaertner & Dovidio, 2005). Aversive racism
has also been posited as some of the reasoning behind the controversy of the delayed
response and racial accusations during the Hurricane Katrina aftermath (Henkel et al.,
2006).
Generally, both sets of subtle racial prejudice theories are measured by subtle
self-report scales. For example, the Subtle Prejudice Scale (Pettigrew & Meertens, 1995)
are not lazy, but they are not ambitious either; pp. 59-60). Another example is the
Symbolic Racism Scale (Henry & Sears, 2002) that is composed of four specific
components: Blacks fail to progress due to their unwillingness to work hard, Blacks are
demanding too much, denial that racial discrimination currently exists, and the sense that
33
Subconscious Prejudice
found that conscious attitudes are only partly responsible for discriminatory behavior.
or awareness—and greatly influence judgment and action, even for individuals who
renounce or score low on subtle prejudice measures (e.g., Devine, 1989, 2001; Dovidio,
Kawakami, Johnson, Johnson, & Howard, 1997; Fazio & Olson, 2003).
The central tenet within subconscious prejudice research is that all individuals
group member; Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams, 1995; Wittenbrink, Judd & Park,
feelings, evaluations, or stereotypes in the same way an encounter with a pitbull may
Patricia Devine (1989) was one of the first researchers to investigate the
measure subconscious attitudes, Devine used a priming method that subliminally primed
subjects by being quickly shown a word or image before beginning a task. Devine found
34
that subjects who were primed stereotypical words related to Blacks interpreted a hostile
vignette much more aggressive than subjects primed with nonracial terms, even those
subjects who scored low on subtle conscious prejudice measures. Other studies—using
subconscious racial attitudes using the computerized Implicit Association Test (IAT).
discrimination of the two tasks combined, and then the discrimination of the two tasks
combined in reverse. In their study, the initial discrimination was to distinguish common
White first names with Black first names, the attribute dimension was to distinguish
pleasant versus unpleasant words, and then the combination and reverse combination of
the two (e.g., White names with pleasant words and Black names with unpleasant words,
and then White names with unpleasant words and Black names with pleasant words). The
researchers found that almost all of the 26 White subjects were faster at matching
pleasant words with their own race and Blacks with unpleasant words rather than the
reverse order.
Correll, Park, Judd, & Wittenbrink (2002) also measured subconscious prejudice
using latency response time but with a video shooting game that they developed. The
game showed 10 Black and 10 White target images appearing in the game four times,
twice as a target with a gun, and twice as a target without a gun. In the first study, 40
35
undergraduate (39 White, 1 Latino) students were told to decide to shoot a person holding
a gun (by hitting a key on the keyboard) and not to shoot the person without a gun
(hitting another keyboard key). The researchers found that participants, on average,
decided to shoot Blacks who were armed more quickly than armed Whites, and decided
not to shoot unarmed Whites more quickly than unarmed Blacks; the differences were
statistically significant. In the second study, the researchers explored the error rates
undergraduates (42 White, 1 Latino, 1 Asian). The results indicated that participants, on
average, significantly and mistakenly decided to shoot an unarmed target more often if he
was Black, and decided not to shoot an armed target more often if he was White.
From these results and numerous other studies, there are some basic postulations
to be different than conscious racial attitudes. Fazio and Olson (2003) found that most
studies investigating the relation between these two attitudes find low and insignificant
individuals (Devine, 1989; Greenwald et al., 1998). For example, Nosek, Banaji, &
Greenwald (2002) found that 70 to 90% of 200,000 Whites sampled (recruited from the
media and assessed through the IAT website) were found to have subconscious prejudice
via the IAT towards Blacks. Third, subconscious prejudice can manifest into
36
For instance, in Devine’s 1989 study, the researcher found, on average, that both
high level and low level subtle conscious prejudiced individuals (as measured by the
Modern Racism Scale) held subconscious racial stereotypes of Blacks. However, only
those with low subtle conscious prejudice were able to control and block these
subconscious stereotypes from manifesting into discrimination. That is, when asked to
list as many alternate labels for, and all of their thoughts in reference to, “Black
Americans,” low subtle conscious prejudiced individuals listed similar negative labels of
Black Americans as high subtle prejudiced individuals. (This result confirms Devine’s
previous finding that both high and low subtle conscious prejudice individuals, on
average, hold subconscious racial stereotypes.) On the other hand, with writing their
thoughts about Blacks, low subtle conscious prejudice individuals listed more positive
than negative thoughts and this was significantly different than high subtle conscious
prejudiced individuals who listed more negative than positive thoughts. Therefore,
Devine concluded that subconscious prejudice can lead to discrimination, but if a person
has the motivation or desire (low subtle conscious prejudice), and the cognitive attention
and ability to control these automatic thoughts, this could substantially decrease
subconscious stereotypes from manifesting into behavior. However, she noted that having
the attention and ability to control these automatic thoughts can be a very tedious,
For example, Correll and colleagues (2002) found in their videogame shooting
experiment that subtle prejudice scores (as measured by the Modern Racism Scale) were
not significantly related to participants’ shooter bias, because, they reasoned, high and
37
low subtle prejudiced individuals had to make decisions under pressure. Therefore, low
prejudiced individuals (although they had the motivation and desire) did not have the
Dovidio, Kawakami, and Gaertner (2000) found that even when individuals have
the desire to monitor and control these automatic attitudes (i.e., have low levels of subtle
conscious prejudice) and the opportunity (e.g., after White students took a subconscious
a Black confederate), subconscious prejudice still leaked out in more subtle behaviors,
such as abnormal eye contact and body language (as observed by the confederates and
triangulated by outside coders via taped videos of the conversations). These researchers
implied that this could explain why intergroup situations are often awkward and
unmotivated to control these attitudes (i.e., have higher levels of subtle conscious
prejudice) or when decisions are made under pressure (i.e., when individuals are
attitudes can also manifest in nonverbal discriminatory behavior even when the above
criteria is met.
last 40 years, racial discrimination continues to exist. To help explain this phenomenon,
researchers have developed various theories suggesting that prejudice is now more
38
subtlety expressed, and that most Americans hold racial attitudes at the subconscious
level that can influence behavior subconsciously. As a result from these attitudes, Whites
may deny the existence of racial discrimination, rarely support government programs for
factors other than race such as Latinos or Blacks not working hard enough (Quillian,
2006). In addition, Whites may also endorse cultural stereotypes, develop some negative
feelings toward other groups (e.g., fear, disgust, discomfort), avoid intergroup contact,
and cultivate more positive feelings of their own group (Henkel et al., 2006). Therefore, it
appears that although current racial attitudes are less obvious, the consequences are still
visible.
Chapter Summary
chapter indicates that past and current racial discrimination continues to influence these
inequalities. Due to the rise in egalitarian beliefs and social norms within the last century,
racial discrimination and prejudice has not decreased but rather altered to a subtler form.
In addition, research has found that prejudice exists at the subconscious level, with
variables that reduce these subtle and subconscious attitudes may provide the best hope
for an instrument of social regeneration – a challenging task explored in the next chapter.
39
CHAPTER 3: THE SOLUTION
identified many psychological and intergroup processes that can improve racial attitudes,
and therefore, discrimination. Recent models of prejudice reduction (e.g., Amodio &
Devine, 2005; Dasgupta & Greenwald, 2001; Devine, Plant, Amodio, Harmon-Jones, &
Vance, 2002; Dovidio & Gaertner, 1999) indicate that three general conditions are
needed to improve racial attitudes. The first condition is an individual must be conscious
of racial biases. Once conscious, the second condition is the individual must become
motivated to change or regulate such biases. This desire can result from a variety of
processes, such as resolving the cognitive dissonance one may experience between
egalitarian values and racial biases, extending or reducing the benefits of social ingroup
favoritism, or experiencing empathy. The last condition needed to reduce racial prejudice
and discrimination is the individual must have cognitive regulatory strategies to continue
Racial Consciousness
heightened sensitivity to limited stimuli for varying lengths of time (Brown & Ryan,
40
specifically focused towards internal racial stimuli (e.g., racial prejudice – Rowe,
Bennett, & Atkinson, 1994) or racial external stimuli (e.g., racial group members or
indirect medium is contact with racial outgroup members. Racial intergroup contact has
been found to increase awareness and attention to racial stimuli, which then influences
mediating variables of racial prejudice reduction (discussed shortly), such as empathy and
social categorization of racial groups, and finally, as a result, reduces racial prejudice
levels (see review by Dovidio, Gaertner, & Kawakami, 2003). For example, Pettigrew
and Tropp (2006) meta-analyzed over 713 research samples and found that in 94% of the
studies reviewed, intergroup contact was associated with lower levels of racial prejudice2.
awareness and attention can influence a decrease in racial attitudes and therefore, less
2
In this meta-analysis, effect sizes were greater for studies that structured intergroup contact under Gordon
Allport’s (1954) optimal conditions: equal status between groups, common goals, intergroup cooperation,
and the support of authorities. However, it should be noted that Pettigrew and Tropp found that these
conditions were not essential (i.e., seventy-five percent out of the 670 studies found effective in reducing
racial prejudice were not structured in line with Allport’s conditions, although effect sizes were smaller as
well).
41
Motivation Variables
Cognitive Dissonance
An increase in awareness and attention to external and internal racial biases can
most Americans today endorse or follow egalitarian values, raising awareness of and
attention to conscious racial biases can create cognitive dissonance within an individual.
When one becomes aware of this inconsistency, it can arouse many negative feelings
such as guilt or shame, which in turn “…motivate[s] the development of more favorable
racial attitudes and produce more favorable intergroup behaviors (even nonverbal
behaviors) several months later” (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1999, p. 102). As a result, this
dissonance can influence people to inhibit or reprocess their own negative attitudes
(Pedersen et al., 2005), as well as promote positive behavior towards racial outgroups.
attitudes. The negative emotions that arise from becoming conscious of automatic
stereotypical behaviors can motivate individuals who hold egalitarian values, or at least
adhere to egalitarian social norms, to consciously inhibit negative responses, and with
practice, can eventually eliminate negative stereotype activation (Devine & Monteith,
1993). For example, when low-prejudice individuals become aware of their failure to
control subconscious prejudice, the dissonance they experience will create control cues
for future situations; these cues can then influence individuals to carefully consider and
prevent future automatic prejudices and behavior from manifesting (Monteith, Ashburn-
42
Nardo, Voils, & Czopp, 2002). However, Devine (1989) noted that awareness of and
consciously inhibiting prejudice once conscious, is difficult to carry through, much like a
bad habit that requires much attention, effort, and time (i.e., cognitive resources).
Social Re(De)Categorization
(i.e., social decategorization). An increase in either of these variables can transform one’s
extend the benefits of ingroup favoritism to former outgroup members. As a result, social
recategorization has been found to reduce racial attitudes and discriminatory behavior
towards racial outgroups and members (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1999; Gaertner & Dovidio,
2000).
43
Similarly, an increase in attention and awareness to racial group members can
influence people to decategorize race membership. That is, people begin to perceive
of racial groups. Therefore, this social decategorization process can either reduce the
salience of ingroup identity, which can reduce ingroup favoritism; or reduce outgroup
categorization, which can reduce racial attitudes towards the racial outgroup by
Empathy
reduce racial prejudice through a variable that is different but related to cognitive
towards racial outgroups. Empathy generally refers to the ability of taking the perspective
of another person (Stephan & Finlay, 1999). The greater the awareness and attention to
racial outgroups and racial discrimination, the greater the possibility of perceiving the
world from a racial outgroup’s perspective. Stephen and Finlay reviewed studies
investigating the effects of empathy on ingroups’ attitudes and behavior towards a variety
of marginalized outgroups from people who identify as gay to prisoners. Their review of
the literature indicated that empathy improves attitudes and behavior towards a variety of
Some explanations of how empathy can reduce racial attitudes and behavior
include the following. Viewing the world from a racial outgroup member’s perspective
may increase the possibility of seeing racial discrimination directed towards this member,
44
which then may induce cognitive dissonance with one’s egalitarian beliefs. Empathy
could also influence one’s perception of social categorization. For instance, perspective
taking can increase perceiving similarities between ingroups and outgroups, which can
reduce feelings of threat or competition with racial outgroups, or broaden one’s ingroup
categorization and therefore, extend one’s ingroup favoritism towards former outgroups.
Further, empathy can produce a greater concern of welfare for racial outgroup members,
which therefore, influences more positive beliefs and affect towards members of these
groups.
racial consciousness on her or his racial attitudes and behavior. In other words, the
greater one’s racial consciousness, the greater the possibility of influencing cognitive
dissonance with one’s egalitarian beliefs, seeing similarities across or differences within
racial groups, and experiencing empathy, all of which have been found to reduce racial
prejudice. However, previous researchers have found that consciousness and motivation
are not the only conditions needed to reduce racial biases. To continue consciousness and
Reducing racial prejudice is like breaking a bad habit (Devine, 1989). To break
such a tendency, awareness and attention, as well as motivation, are of course required.
However, if an individual does not possess cognitive resources to develop and practice
45
regulatory strategies to continue one’s recognition or intention, the habit will likely
prejudice, can manifest even when an individual is highly racial conscious and motivated
(internally or externally) to overcome racial biases (e.g., Blair and Banaji, 1996;
Kawakami, Dovidio, Moll, Hermsen, & Russin, 2000). However, other studies have
found that some highly racial conscious and motivated individuals can regulate racial
biases, even when cognitive constraints are high, due to developing and practicing
The discussion so far appears to paint a rather optimistic picture: Higher racial
consciousness can result in motivational processes that can reduce racial prejudice if
sufficient and continual cognitive resources are present. However, most White Americans
do not hold a high degree of racial consciousness. In addition, increasing Whites’ low
of, attention to, and regulation of racial biases is particularly challenging. These
limitations, which are explained in further detail below, help explain why racial
discrimination continues to exist in our society between Whites and people of color, as
46
well as why scholars continue to struggle with how to tackle racial prejudice in America
Most White Americans do not hold, or continue to hold, a high degree of racial
consciousness due to a variety of reasons. One main reason is that “[i]n addressing race,
areas of our lives, [W]hiteness is privileged, normalized, defied, and raceless” (Johnson,
1999, p. 1). Moreover, whiteness cannot see itself except through the reflection of what it
sees itself as not. In other words, Whites are not aware and attentive to race simply
because they do not need to (i.e., because of their White privilege, defined as the
unearned advantages and immunity granted to or enjoyed by Whites just from being
White). For instance, “Whites do not look at the world through a filter of racial
awareness, even though Whites are, of course, members of a race. The power to ignore
race, when White is the race, is a privilege, a societal advantage” (Wildman & Davis,
1997, pp. 317-318). However, when this social advantage or privilege is threatened or
compromised, Whites increase their racial consciousness, at least in respect to their own
racial identity. A prime example of such an occurrence is when Whites feel victims of
polls indicate that between half and three-fourths of Whites surveyed believe that, as a
racial group, they are routinely discriminated against from such policies (Pincus, 2002).
47
A second reason Whites tend to have, and may continue to have, a low degree of
racial consciousnesses is due to limited contact with racial outgroups. For instance,
Whites in primary and secondary school, on national average, attend public schools with
a student body that is 80% White (Frankenberg et al., 2003). In addition, current racially
segregated neighborhoods and real estate steering, discussed in chapter 2, further prevent
racial outgroup contact. And even if there is diversity within an environment (e.g.,
prefer to interact with people who look, and culturally act and talk like themselves
(Moody, 2001).
Because most Whites are not highly aware or attentive to racial biases, scholars
However, one main limitation with this approach is that when Whites experience an
increase in racial consciousness, they will likely experience self-esteem or ego threats,
and negative or unwanted emotions (e.g., guilt, anger), which can increase resistance to
that people may experience when they realize that a rejected decision might have been
better than their chosen decision (Brehm, 1956, as cited in Gawronski, Strack, &
Bodenhausen, 2008). To reduce this threat or feeling, people will tend to emphasize or
search for positive reasons of their chosen decision and negative reasons of the rejected
(but better) decision (Gawronski et al.). Thus, those who have decided (or simply believe)
48
that they are not prejudiced or that discrimination does not exist may try to support their
often difficult, stressful, uncomfortable, unpleasant, and perhaps coercive” (p. 23).
association” when first becoming aware of racial prejudice or discrimination, which can
These feelings are uncomfortable and can lead White students to resist learning
about race and racism. And who can blame them? If learning about racism means
seeing oneself as an oppressor, one of the bad guys, then of course there will be
consciousness can even increase Whites’ racial prejudice levels. Branscombe et al.
(2007) randomly assigned 189 White undergraduates to one of three conditions. In the
two experimental groups, students were asked to write about ways they have been either
control group, students were asked to write about their general life experiences. After the
researchers found that those who thought and wrote about White privilege expressed
49
Difficulty with Developing and Practicing Cognitive Regulatory Strategies
cognitive resources to develop and practice regulatory strategies (Devine & Monteith,
1993; Kawakami et al., 2000). Reasons for this regulation difficulty lie in cognitive
person’s everyday life is determined not by their conscious intentions and deliberate
guidance” (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999, p. 462). Automaticity frees our mental capacity
from tasks that no longer require attention in order to direct our energy and focus towards
process, especially for individuals who may have low racial consciousness and low self-
interest. However, numerous studies from Devine, Monteith, and colleagues (see Devine
et al., 2002) found that even Whites who hold a high degree of racial consciousness, may
still express racial biases due to the difficulty of regulating racial biases; but the degree of
50
In sum, an increase in racial awareness and attention can reduce racial prejudice
through a variety of motivational variables when cognitive resources are and continue to
emotions, which can resist exploration or increase levels of racial attitudes and behavior.
difficult task even for high racially conscious individuals. Therefore, a process is needed
to raise Whites’ degree of racial awareness and attention, while at the same time, reduce
that can offer a solution is an individual’s degree of mindfulness towards race (i.e., racial
Mindfulness
describes the concept similarly. During consciousness, stimuli is brought into awareness
and held in focal attention briefly, if at all, before an emotional or cognitive reaction is
made. These reactions are filtered often in a discriminate nature usually in reference to
accommodated into existing cognitive schemas (Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007). The
51
judgments, often automatically, to stimuli brought into attention and awareness (Bargh &
establishment and maintenance of order upon events and experience of relevance to the
self, and the facilitation of goal pursuit and attainment” (Brown et al., p. 212). The main
limitation with this mode of processing, however, is that stimuli are rarely observed
and awareness to stimuli, but with open receptivity, preventing the overlay of
it is, rather than viewing the world through conceptual filters, categories, and biases. In
this way, mindfulness is defined as attention to and awareness of present events and
experiences with open receptivity (Brown & Ryan, 2003). In addition, mindfulness can
human capacity, varying naturally in degree of stability and frequency within individuals
enhanced by exercises, ancient and new. Two different practices that can improve the
(accredited to cognitive psychologist Ellen Langer). Both paths have shown that an
52
increase in mindfulness can lead to many physical, psychological, and interpersonal
Mindfulness Meditation
…examining who we are, with questioning our view of the world and our place
in it, and with cultivating some appreciation for the fullness of each moment we
are alive; most of all, it has to do with being in touch. (Kabat-Zinn, 1994, p. 3)
The meditation generally begins with the practitioner reconnecting the mind and
the body, usually by focusing on one’s breathing. The practitioner then tries not to
constrict attention, but observe and welcome any emotional, mental, or physical
noninterpretation. The breath is continually used as an anchor to return attention when the
mind wanders or when the mind starts to attach, judge, or interpret an experience. This
process of focused breathing and allowing thoughts and feelings to come and go as they
certain concept is trained upon, or brought into focused awareness such as lovingkindness
our hurts, and our attachments, and a way to find sympathy and compassion with others,
53
and an area for discovering creative new options” (p. 264). Anticipating these benefits,
Western clinicians and psychologists have introduced mindfulness into many treatment
Cognitive Therapy (MBCT; Teasdale et al., 2000), Dialectal Behavior Therapy (Linehan,
1993), and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson,
1999) to name a few. By and large, mindfulness interventions have been found to reduce
and eating disorders, as well as enhance participants’ overall sense of well-being (for
participants that serve as a control group while the experimental group receives the
intervention. The control group later receives the treatment after a waiting period, usually
when the study is completed). Prior to treatment, the researchers found no significant
differences on outcome measures between the two groups. However, after the
intervention, analyses indicated that the intervention group, compared to the control
state anxiety, trait anxiety; and significantly increased on overall empathy and sense of
spirituality.
54
In addition, Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, and Walach (2004) meta-analyzed and
aggregated effect sizes for 20 mindfulness studies (N=1605 in total) that included both
researchers found significant medium effect sizes for mental and physical variables in
Aside from clinical settings, mindfulness meditation has also been incorporated
into education, and with beneficial effects. Jack Miller from the University of Toronto
has implemented this path of mindfulness within his holistic graduate courses to over
1000 students since 1988 (Miller & Nozawa, 2002). In these courses, students are
required to practice mindfulness meditation and record their experience through reflective
journaling. From the narrative descriptions within these journals, as well as evaluative
feedback from students, Miller (1995) found reoccurring themes; mindfulness meditation
increased the students’ self-efficacy, awareness, connectedness with others, and personal
well-being (e.g., reduction in stress). In a follow-up study (Miller & Nozawa), one
student wrote:
And that place of gentleness, and presence, and mindfulness, breathing and really
living in some healthy way of connecting, well, it connects you with yourself, but
it also connects you to those around you. I mean there’s a sense of common soul.
There’s just a sense that we are all just one. (p. 189)
55
beginning to introduce this meditation practice to provide a variety of benefits within
Straying away from the traditional and reflective style of mindfulness meditation,
professor at Harvard. Langer (1989) found that a mindfulness mode of processing arises
with stimuli that are novel or atypical; therefore, an individual can increase one’s
Langer and Moldoveanu (2000), “It does not matter whether what is noticed is important
or trivial, as long as it is new to the viewer. Actively drawing these distinctions keeps us
mindfulness, “mindlessness,” as she poetically describes as, “When the lights are on, but
no one is home” (p. 9). According to Langer (2000), when we are mindless we are
single or rigid perspectives, unaware of other ways of knowing, our behavior is routine
and predetermined, and we our oblivious to noticing new things. However, when
mindful, we experience all the opposite: We are more sensitive to the environment and to
the present, we have a greater openness to new information, we create new or broader
56
In her book, Mindfulness, Langer (1989) summarizes numerous research studies
In one experimental study, Langer and Perlmuter (1988, as cited in Langer 1989)
investigated the effects of mindfulness intervention with nursing home patients. The first
(lowest mindful) group was asked to monitor and evaluate their daily activities for a
week. The second (low mindful) group was directed to monitor different behaviors each
day. The third (high mindful) group was asked to do the same as the second group but
also to list three alternatives they could have selected but did not for each behavior listed.
The final (highest mindful) group performed the same as the third group but also chose
which activities to monitor. At the end of the week, the investigators found that the more
mindful the participants were, the less depressed and the more confident, dependent,
Joss and Langer (1986, as described in Langer, Hatem, Joss, & Howell, 1989)
three different conditions/groups. All three groups read a similar topic passage and were
given a preceding test measuring retention and creativity. However, the passage for each
group was written a little differently for each condition. The first group of students read a
passage written in absolute terms (mindless). The second group read a passage with
conditional (high mindful) terms such as “could be” or “possibly”. And the third group
57
read a passage written in absolute terms but introduced in a conditional (low mindful)
manner (e.g., “As one possible model…”). The investigators found that the students in
the mindless and low mindful groups were less creative in their responses and were
unaware when a case in the passage was fabricated in comparison to students in the high
mindful group.
towards the journey of mindfulness, the destination is generally the same: an increase in
which has yet to be extensively explored, is the reduction of racial prejudice and
discrimination.
Racial Mindfulness
racial stimuli (e.g., racial prejudice) or racial external stimuli (e.g., racial group members
Although research has yet to extensively investigate the relation of mindfulness with such
racial concepts as prejudice and discrimination, statements from scholars, results from
two published studies, and theoretical frames from the racial prejudice and mindfulness
58
Advocates for Racial Mindfulness
learning and anti-oppression, educators can utilize mindfulness practices to enhance the
efficacy of anti-oppressive pedagogy. She asserts that mindfulness has the potential to
address dualistic thinking and foster change both cognitively and affectively.
In the peace education literature, Leonard Riskin (2004), Director of the Center
Law, stated that mindfulness could help peace negotiators in several ways. To wit:
aware of certain deep assumptions, involving those based on ethnicity or culture” (p.86).
In addition, Barbara Vacarr (2003) posited that mindfulness training may help
White teachers in developing the ability to respond with more empathy, less judgment,
and greater awareness of White privilege to tense diversity moments in the classroom. In
direct relation to prejudice, although not racial prejudice in particular, Hanh (1975)
freeing one from prejudice and stereotypes. Results from two published studies also
59
Mindfulness and Prejudice Studies
Langer et al. (1985) hypothesized that if students were taught to be more mindful
or make more distinctions with people who have physical disabilities, students may be
more differentiated, less prejudiced, and see that abilities and disabilities are dependent
on context (e.g., “She may not be able to do X, but could do Y”) and not global
conditions (e.g., “She is disabled”). The researchers conducted the study with 47 sixth-
grade students during class time for five days (the fifth day was left for administering the
dependent measures). On two of the four days, half the students were shown slides of
people with unnoticeable physical disabilities. For the other two days, the other half of
students was presented with slides of people with noticeable physical disabilities.
Students in both the unnoticeable and noticeable groups were randomly split by
mindfulness treatment. Students in each group received identical looking booklets that
either allowed several different answers to one question (high mindfulness; e.g., “How
can this woman in the wheelchair drive a car?”) or allowed one answer for each question
(low mindfulness; e.g., “Can this woman in the wheelchair drive this car?”). This
group who were shown slides of people with noticeable physical disabilities were more
condescending, expressed less superficial preference, and were less likely to avoid these
individuals in hypothetical scenarios (e.g., were more likely to choose a blind person for
60
“pin the tail on the donkey” in comparison to low mindfulness groups who were more
likely to choose a person with unnoticeable physical disabilities). That is, teaching
children active distinction making (i.e., mindfulness) in this study reduced children’s
Langer & Moldoveanu (2000) later concluded that mindfulness could generalize
When we do not stop drawing distinctions between people at some arbitrary point
(e.g., skin color or accent), and we keep on drawing distinctions (down to feeding
habits, music they listen to, or any of thousands of issues), then we may discover
that most stereotypes that we have formed are not rooted in fact, but in choice. (p.
6)
In relation to racial prejudice, only one published study to date has explored
attitudes. Lillis and Hayes (2007) explored two classroom approaches to increase racial
judgment, and reinforcement of positive actions that are consistent with one’s egalitarian
values.
61
Using a counterbalanced within-group design, results from pre- to post- and pre-
to follow-up indicate that the mindfulness session increased participants’ awareness and
thought control and diffusion, and intentions of positive action. In comparison to the
greater results on all outcomes. As a result, mindfulness not only helped participants’
racial consciousness, but also increased their acceptance towards racial biases and served
Mindfulness directed towards race can reduce racial prejudice in three ways. First,
and similar to racial consciousness, racial mindfulness can increase one’s attention and
awareness of internal and external racial stimuli, in which the consciousness can motivate
experiencing empathy). Second, racial mindfulness can raise attention and awareness to
racial biases in an accepting and nonjudgmental fashion. This process could therefore
reduce self-invoked, negative emotions and their consequential effects, often experienced
from Whites when first coming to terms with racial biases. Finally, racial mindfulness
can decrease racial prejudice by reducing cognitive constraints for one to develop and
62
(meditation or active distinction making) that one can continue to use in the future and is
context independent.
and experiences with open receptivity (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Therefore, racial
mindfulness may influence Whites to become more aware of their conscious racial
groups. From racial mindfulness, for example, Whites may begin to realize that American
values are perhaps White values (Devos & Banaji, 2005) and an equal opportunity
Mindfulness has been described as the cognitive process of de-automatization, which can
subconsciously from habitual conditions (Deikman, 2000; Langer, 1989; Salomon &
attitudes.
Mindfulness has many benefits including awareness to internal and external stimuli,
multiple perspectives, empathy, and compassion for others (Baer, 2003; Langer, 1989,
63
2000; Miller, 1995; Miller & Nozawa, 2002; Shapiro et al., 1998). When applied to racial
biases, these mindfulness benefits appear similar to the motivational variables that can
reduce racial prejudice, discussed earlier in this section. That is, when applied to racial
stimuli, the mindfulness benefit of increased awareness could increase the opportunity of
benefits of creating new or broader mental categories and connectedness with others
could help social re(de)categorizing racial group memberships (Lillis & Hayes, 2007);
and the mindfulness benefits of empathy and compassion for others seems directly related
Acceptance
can also affect one’s level of acceptance towards racial biases. Mindfulness brings
nonjudgmental fashion. Therefore, racial mindfulness can potentially prevent many of the
negative or unwanted emotions (e.g., guilt, compunction) evoked within individuals who
first become aware of racial biases that could cause avoidance to or even a rise in racial
prejudice.
individuals’ self-esteem and ego defense mechanisms. The experimental group consisted
of two combined cohorts (n1=221, n2=216, combined mean age=18.27). The control was
64
recruited to match the demographics and social factors of the experimental group
between groups. Using multivariate statistics, and controlling for pre-test scores, the
participant’s overall self-esteem and reduced subconscious ego defense mechanisms such
Others have supported this result, indicating that the open receptivity aspect of
mindfulness attenuates cognitive or ego defensiveness (Hodgins & Knee, 2002), and
negative emotions, which can arise when one’s self-esteem or image is threatened
(Heppner et al., 2008). Therefore, racial mindfulness may decrease individuals’ (e.g.,
Whites’) ego-defensiveness and negative feelings that can arise when they become aware
of racial biases. This postulation is supported by Lillis and Hayes (2007) – the only
published study to date that has explored the effects of a racial mindfulness intervention –
where they found that racial mindfulness not only decreased participants’ racial prejudice
Finally, racial mindfulness can reduce cognitive constraints for one to develop
and practice cognitive strategies to regulate racial biases, as well as provide two
regulatory strategies (i.e., meditation or active distinction making) that one can adopt.
Racial mindfulness can reduce cognitive constraints due to the de-categorical and de-
65
As previously mentioned, cognitive psychology indicates that mindful processing
entails drawing new categories or distinctions (Langer, 1989) or freeing oneself from
such categories (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Therefore, mindfulness directed towards race
may reduce the normative categorization process that has been theorized as the onset of
racial prejudice (Allport, 1954). In addition, mindfulness has been referred to as cognitive
experiences a shift away from automaticity, and more towards monitoring mental
processes (Deikman, 2000; Salomon & Globerson, 1987). Therefore, if mindfulness was
directed towards racial stimuli, the de-automatization process can reduce cognitive
constraints for one to develop and practice cognitive strategies to regulate racial biases.
For example, mindfulness may create more time for an individual to consciously decide
how to respond, rather than responding subconsciously and automatically to racial biases.
In addition, the two paths to increase mindfulness (i.e., meditation and actively
drawing distinctions) can serve as cognitive strategies that an individual can adopt to
regulate racial biases. One of the limitations of racial consciousness interventions is that
the effects are often dependent on the program or context (e.g., intergroup contact).
However, the paths of mindfulness (i.e., meditation and actively drawing distinctions) are
non-contextual practices that an individual can continually use to regulate racial biases.
In sum, research has yet to extensively investigate mindfulness with such racial
66
from two published studies, and theoretical frames from the racial prejudice and
mindfulness.
Chapter Summary
prejudice researchers suggest three general conditions are needed to decrease racial
one’s racial biases. However, most White Americans do not hold a high degree of racial
consciousness, and if increased, Whites may experience negative outcomes. Further, due
cognitively taxing and difficult for many. Mindfulness directed towards race may provide
a solution to these limitations and reduce racial prejudice levels. However, racial
the next chapter explores the effects of mindfulness on White students’ conscious and
67
CHAPTER 4: RESEARCH DESIGN
Within the last 40 years, great strides have been made in America in relation to
racial equality, yet inequalities continue to exist between Whites and people of color.
prejudice, I have illustrated that racial attitudes continue to affect our society today. From
argued that mindfulness directed towards racial stimuli, such as racial prejudice,
discrimination, or inequalities, may reduce racial attitudes and overcome limitations often
associated with current prejudice reduction models. The present research investigates the
extent to White college students’ degree of mindfulness can influence their degree of
racial prejudice directly and indirectly through motivational mediating variables, as well
empathy?
68
a. does mindfulness attenuate the negative effects that can arise from
cognitive dissonance?
consisting of two studies. The first research question is empirically investigated in Study
equation modeling (SEM) is then used to explore the theoretical framework of whether
participants’ degree of mindfulness decreases their level of racial prejudices directly and
decategorization, and empathy, while controlling for previous racial outgroup contact.
content analysis on White participants’ written reactions to an article that describes White
69
CHAPTER 5: STUDY A
and subconscious racial prejudice towards Blacks using SEM. I considered using an
experimental design, which would consist of creating and exploring the effects of a racial
Method
Participants
The study consisted of 341 undergraduate college students selected from the
using a stratified random sampling procedure. Two strata were used for selection: (a)
students who racially identify as White, and (b) a balance of students who self-identify as
male (n=164, 48%) and female (n=177, 52%). Participants consisted of first-year (n=17,
5%), second-year (n=38, 11%), third-year (n=102, 30%), fourth-year (n=150, 44%), fifth-
year (seniors; n=24, 7%), and graduate level students (n=10, 3%). Of the 327 participants
who provided the optional department name of their major or degree, the responses show
a wide representation ranging from the Business school to “Undeclared,” with Liberal
Arts (n=98, 30%), Communication (n=65, 20%), and Business (n=55, 16%)
70
Procedure
Students were randomly and equally selected into four different groups (Group A,
B, C, and D). All groups received an introductory message of the study via email that
included a SurveyMonkey Web address where electronic measures of the study can be
accessed. The Web addresses in the email message, however, were different for each
group, as the ordering of the electronic measures varied by group membership due to
detail shortly.
Once at the SurveyMonkey Web site, all groups first observed a consent form
explaining the potential risks and benefits of participation with limited knowledge about
the intention of the study in order to prevent participant bias, and the possibility of being
randomly selected for a follow-up (Study B). One risk for participation was subject
identification. The departmental subject pool requires students to provide their University
reduce this risk, the consent form informed students that their number will be used for
this research credit purpose only and how identification will be protected (i.e., each
UTEID will be given a random 5-digit number, only the 5-digit number will be connected
to responses, a separate list connecting UTEIDs with random 5-digit numbers will be
password protected and accessed by the primary researcher only, and each entry on this
71
After agreeing to participate and entering their UTEID, all groups then responded
to a set of demographic items, a qualifying item for participation in the follow-up study,
and the following scales: (a) previous racial outgroup contact, (b) mindfulness, (c)
empathy, and (d) social re(de)categorization measures, which are described in the next
section.
Group D, on the other hand, completed the conscious (subtle) prejudice scale first and
then the subconscious measure. Within each of these pairings for the subconscious
measure, one of the groups (e.g., Group A and Group C) receives a chronological block
order of the measure where the other group (e.g., Group B and Group D) receives a
reversed (counterbalanced) block order (this measure and block orders are described in
Table 2
Administration Order of Measures
Order Measures (All Groups)
72
Measures
Because of the indirect and direct effects of racial intergroup contact on racial
prejudice, data on this variable was important to collect and later to control across
participants in this study. A 7-item scale adapted from Chang (20023; see Appendix A)
collected participants’ degree of previous racial outgroup contact. With these items,
percentage of people who were/are White in each of the following groups: high school
classmates, the neighborhood where they grew up, current close friends, current
neighbors, immediate and non-immediate family members, and romantic partners. All
item responses are reverse scored so a high total score indicates a high degree of previous
Mindfulness
The Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2006; see
overall factor. The five facets are: observing, describing, acting with awareness,
responses on the observing facet were initially excluded in this study, as Baer et al.
(2006) found that this facet failed to fit the hierarchical mindfulness model with their
3
There were no reliability or validity data reported for scores on this scale in Chang (2002).
73
observing facet) and a five-facet hierarchical model occurred during the model
The full scale consists of 39-items (31-items minus the observe subscale) on a 5-
point Likert scale (1 = “Never or very rarely true”, 5 = “Very often or always true”),
where a combined higher score reflects a higher degree of overall mindfulness. Sample
items include “When I’m walking, I deliberately notice the sensations of my body
When I do things, my mind wanders off and I’m easily distracted (acting with
(nonjudging);” and “I perceive my feelings and emotions without having to react to them
(non-reacting).” The FFMQ has sound psychometric properties, and scores on the scale
have been found to be valid and reliable with college student samples. For instance,
internally consistency was .75 to .91 for scores determining the five facets of mindfulness
Empathy
Three subscales from the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1980;
measure four aspects of empathy: empathetic concern, perspective taking, fantasy, and
personal distress (reverse coded) – as well as an overall empathy factor (Bäckström &
Björklund, 2007; Cliffordson, 2002). In this study, participants took only the empathetic
74
well”). The fantasy subscale, which measures the tendency to identify with fictional
characters in movies and other situations, was not included in this study, as it was not
deemed necessary. Sample items of the three subscales include: “I would describe myself
The IRI is widely used for an empathy measure and has been found negatively
related to conscious (subtle) racial prejudice (Bäckström & Björklund, 2007). Scores on
the IRI have been found valid and consistent internally and over time with college student
samples (e.g., α=.72 for empathetic concern, α=.78 for perspective-taking, and α=.78 for
personal distress subscales, Davis [N=579]; and α=.82 for a hierarchical empathy factor,
Social Re(De)Categorization
recategorization and decategorization levels of group membership. The 9-item scale was
adapted from Gaertner, Rust, Dovidio, Bachman, and Anastasio (1994), which appears to
be the only published study with a scale or items attempting to measure such constructs4.
In this prior study, the researchers used four intergroup contact items to assess students’
perceptions of social categorization of the student body on campus. Two items assessed
4
Gaertner et al. (1994) developed four items from modifying the four highest loading items of the equal
status factor of the School Interracial Climate Scale derived from the development and validation study of
this scale (Green, Adams, & Turner, 1988).
75
social decategorization (i.e., “At school, it usually feels as though we belong to different
groups [reverse scored]; At school, it usually feels as though we are individuals and not
members of a particular group”), and the other two items were designed to measure
students’ social recategorization of students on campus (i.e., “Despite the different groups
at school, there is frequently the sense that we are all just one group; Although there are
different groups of students at this school, it feels as though we are all playing on the
same team”). Incorporating modifications of these items and using them as a guide, five
additional items were created, each with a 5-point Likert response scale (1= “Strongly
The total 9-item, projected 2-factor, scale was then explored with a pilot study
sample to determine factor structure and reliability of scores (Appendix D). The results
indicated a 6-item, 2-factor scale. However, the reliability of scores on the Social
Decategorization subscale, as well as the pattern and structure coefficients of two of the
subscale items, were not as high as expected. Therefore, two items that were projected to
measure social decategorization but dropped during the factor analysis procedure in the
pilot study were modified and investigated here with Study A participants. In addition,
the one dropped item projected to measure social recategorization was also modified and
explored (in an attempt to create an over-identified measurement model during the next
stage of data analysis). Therefore, participants in this study completed a 9-item scale that
consisted of the validated six items and three modified items from the pilot study results
(Appendix E). Higher scores on these subscales indicate a higher degree of social
76
Conscious (Subtle) Racial Prejudice
The Symbolic Racism 2000 Scale (SR2K; Henry & Sears, 2002) collected
participants’ current level of conscious (subtle) racial prejudice. The SR2K scale is an 8-
item measure with both Likert and non-Likert response scales (see Appendix F), where a
example item is as follows: “Irish, Italian, Jewish, and many other minorities overcame
prejudice and worked their way up. Blacks should do the same.” The SR2K has
students, adults, and other racial groups besides Whites. Scores on the scale have been
found to be adequately reliable (e.g., internal consistency was .79, N=702; Henry &
Sears).
participants’ current level of subconscious prejudice. The IAT has quickly become the
most widely used subconscious measure in psychology (Fazio & Olson 2003; Quillian,
2006) with over 200 research papers on subconscious attitudes of all sorts (e.g., religion,
weight, age; see http://projectimplicit.net/nosek/iat/). Scores on the IAT have been found
validity (see Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2007), as well as adequate reliability (e.g.,
alphas for internally consistency ranged from .7 to .9; Greenwald & Nosek, 2001).
target concepts (e.g., "Blacks" versus "Whites") with positive versus negative evaluations
77
(e.g., "pleasant words" versus "unpleasant words"), in a seven-block sequence (some of
the blocks are used for practice to acquaint participants with the IAT). A participant
taking the computerized IAT learns to use two keys, one on the left and one on the right,
For example, in Block 1, a participant learns to press the left-key each time a
White face appears, and the right-key each time a Black face appears (see Table 3). Then
in Block 2, the participant learns to respond with the same two keys to pleasant words
Blocks 3 and 4, both target faces and evaluation words are presented in a random
sequence, and the participant is still asked to perform the responses previously learned
(e.g., White faces and pleasant words = left-key). In Block 5, the initial assignment of
keys to target concepts is reversed, so that the left- key is now assigned to Black faces
and the right-key assigned to White faces. Finally, in Blocks 6 and 7, target faces and
evaluation words are again presented in a random sequence, but now with the reversed
assignment from Block 5 (e.g., White faces and unpleasant words = left-key).
Table 3
Block Sequences of the IAT
Block Trials Left-key response Right key response
B1 20 White face Black face
B2 20 Pleasant words Unpleasant words
B3 20 White face + Pleasant words Black face + Unpleasant words
B4 40 White face + Pleasant words Black face + Unpleasant words
B5 40 Black Face White face
B6 20 Black face + Pleasant words White face + Unpleasant words
B7 40 Black face + Pleasant words White face + Unpleasant words
78
A comparison of average latency between Blocks 3 and 4, and Blocks 6 and 7 are
the critical stages to reveal the association strengths between faces and attributes.
Participants who possess stronger and positive associations with Whites compared to
Blacks will have a faster response time (and less errors) in Blocks 3 and 4 than Blocks 6
and 7. In most studies, half the sample completes the task in the above chronological
block order, and the other half competes the task with a counterbalanced block order of 5,
(1) Use data from Blocks 3, 4, 6, and 7; (2) eliminate trials with latencies >
10,000 ms; (3) eliminate subjects whom more than 10% of trials have latencies <
300 ms; (4) compute one standard deviation for all trials in Blocks 3 and 6, and
another standard deviation for all trials in Blocks 4 and 7; (5) compute means for
trials in each of the four blocks (3, 4, 6, 7); (6) compute two difference scores
(one between Blocks 3 and 6 and the other between Blocks 4 and 7) subtracting
what is intended to represent the high (positive) end of the measure from the
block containing associations representing the low end (7) divide each difference
score by its associates standard deviation from Step 4; and (8) average the two
From this procedure, the IAT score (a D score) ranges from -2 to +2. Break points for
scores include .15 (slight prejudice), .35 (moderate prejudice), and .65 or above (strong
79
prejudice towards Blacks and a higher negative score indicates a greater degree of
Data Analysis
First, I ran diagnostics using the SPSS program to determine that the assumptions
of SEM have been met and to explore scale reliability. Following, using maximum-
likelihood estimation via the Amos program, I employed latent variable SEM to examine
the direct and indirect effects of mindfulness on racial prejudice, while controlling for
Model Development
of measures to help create the initial measurement model. Following, the initial
structural model was specified. Finally, competing a priori models were compared to the
For the initial measurement model, presented in Figure 2, observed variables were
obtained from (a) the total score for subconscious prejudice, (b) scale items for social
multi-item measures. It was necessary to item parcel in order to reduce the number of
parameters estimated in the structural model. In addition, because the purpose of the
study is to explore the relations between latent variables and not the relations among
items comprising the measured variables, parceling was warranted (Little, Cunningham,
80
Figure 2. Initial measurement model with observed variables and latent factors.
Note: M= Mindfulness, PRC = Prior Racial Outgroup Contact, SRC = Social Recategorization, SDC =
Social Decategorization, CP = Conscious (Subtle) Prejudice, SP = Subconscious Prejudice, E = Empathy,
and P = Item Parcels
contact and conscious [subtle] prejudice measures), parcels were established for each
scale by first fitting a factor solution to each set of items and then averaging the items
with the highest and lowest coefficients to form the first indicator, averaging the items
with the next highest and lowest coefficients to form the second indicator, and so on. For
approach, responses were first fitted to a factor solution for each set of subscale items.
81
Following, the subscale items with the highest coefficients were averaged together to
form the first parcel (e.g., items with the highest coefficients for IRI subscales of
empathetic concern, perspective taking, and personal distress were averaged together),
subscale items with the second highest coefficients were averaged together, subscale
items with the lowest coefficients were averaged together, and then subscale items with
the second lowest coefficients were averaged. Therefore, the four parcels for each
multidimensional measure reflect all of the dimensions present within the set of items.
For the initial structural model, findings from prior research (discussed in chapter
3) were first included in the model. Empathy (Stephan & Finlay, 1999), social
recategorization (Dovidio & Gaertner, 1999; Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000), and
decategorization (Gaertner & Dovidio, 2005) have been found to directly reduce racial
prejudice levels. In addition, prior racial outgroup contact has been found to directly
decrease participant’s conscious (subtle) prejudice and subconscious prejudice levels; this
variable has also been found to increase participants’ empathy and social categorization
levels, which indirectly explains some of the effect racial outgroup contact has on one’s
researchers have shown that this variable can increase empathy (Shapiro et al., 1998) and
After these prior research findings were included, predicted paths were then
drawn in the structural model. First, mindfulness was hypothesized to initially occur
before and therefore, affect prejudice (and not the other way around), which can be
82
theorists (e.g., Brown et al., 2007; Langer, 1989), mindfulness is a mode of cognitive
processing that involves attention of and awareness to stimuli before the overlay of
appears to have time precedence over (occurs before) inflexible, categorical, and
automatic cognitive processes (i.e., racial prejudice), at least initially. Second, because
mindfulness has been found to increase empathy and interconnectedness (or social
re[de]categorization), and these variables, on their own, have been found to decrease
racial prejudice levels, mindfulness was hypothesized to decrease racial prejudice levels
indirectly. Third, mindfulness was theorized to reduce racial prejudice levels directly
based from the following: (a) statements from scholars postulating the effect of
mindfulness on racial prejudice (e.g., Orr, 2002); (b) results from two published studies
(Langer et al., 1985; Lillis & Hayes, 2007); (c) the finding that mindfulness increases two
decrease racial prejudice levels; and (d) how mindfulness can influence other variables
that have been found to decrease racial prejudice, such as an increase in one’s awareness
and attention to stimuli with open receptivity (e.g., acceptance to cognitive dissonance;
Brown & Ryan, 2003), a reduction in cognitive automaticity, and an increase in mental
and generally occurs before racial prejudice development, and can decrease racial
prejudice levels both indirectly and directly. Figure 3 depicts this initial structural model
83
Figure 3. Initial structural model with supported and predicted effects. Based from prior research, red
arrows indicate a path of increase and black arrows indicate a path of decrease. Blue arrows indicate
predicted paths of decrease. For ease of presentation, the measurement model is excluded.
Note: M= Mindfulness, PRC = Prior Racial Outgroup Contact, SRC = Social Recategorization, SDC =
Social Decategorization, CP = Conscious (Subtle) Prejudice, SP = Subconscious Prejudice, and E =
Empathy.
Model Comparison
After this initial model was constructed, competing a priori models were
compared to determine the best fitting model. Once the best fitting model was selected,
the following goodness of fit indices were used to determine model fit: chi-square
statistic (χ2), Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), root mean
square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual
84
(SRMR). For the GFI and CFI, values above .90 indicated an adequate fit and values
above .95 indicated a good fit to the data (Keith, 2006). Values below .05 for RMSEA
and .08 for SRMR indices indicated a good fitting model to the data (Hu & Bentler, 1998,
1999). Values of the RMSEA served as the primary focus of model fit, as this index is
designed to assess the approximate fit of a model and therefore, suggested as a more
To compare rival nested models, the chi-square difference test or change in chi-
square (Δχ2; Keith, 2006) was used to determine the best fitting and most parsimonious
model. A statistically significant change in chi-square suggests that the more constrained
model should be rejected. In addition, because the chi-square can be sensitive to sample
size (Keith), the Akaike Information Criteria (AIC) and Bayesian Information Criterion
(BIC) were also investigated. To compare competing non-nested models, the AIC and
BIC were used. Models with lower AIC and BIC values were preferred.
Two competing measurement models were specified and compared to the initial
measurement model of this study. The first competing model consists of incorporating
the Social Re(De)categorization items that were modified during a pilot study with this
scale. Therefore in this alternative model, the Social Recategorization factor has four
indicators (three validated items plus one modified item) and the Social Decategorization
factor consists of five indicators (three validated items plus two modified items). The
second measurement model consists of including the observing facet from the
mindfulness measure. Therefore, item parcels for the hierarchical mindfulness factor
85
After the best fitting measurement model was determined, three competing a
priori structural models were specified and compared to the initial structural model of this
study. The first competing structural model explored the correlation between social
recategorization and social decategorization factors, as this was predicted during scale
conscious (subtle) and subconscious racial prejudice. Most studies investigating the
relation between these two attitudes find correlations below 0.2 (Fazio & Olson, 2003).
However, some studies have found a larger and significant correlation (e.g., Kawakami,
Dion, & Dovidio, 1998; McConnell & Liebold, 2001). Therefore, the first competing
model included a correlation of the conscious (subtle) and subconscious measures within
(subtle) and subconscious prejudice were excluded. Because this study is not measuring
attention to general stimuli with open receptivity (mindfulness) to include a direct focus
on racial stimuli (racial mindfulness) may be too distal. However, the effects of
86
appear similar to the variables of social recategorization, social decategorization, and
Results
data assumptions and conducting reliability analyses of scores on scales and subscales.
Following, I developed and compared the measurement and structural models against
alternative a priori models. Finally, using the best fitting structural equation model, I
explored the indirect and direct effects of mindfulness on racial prejudice, as well as
other effects.
resulted in the following findings. One item from the Prior Racial Outgroup Contact
Scale (i.e., Item 5: percentage of immediate family members that were White) was not
normally distributed and therefore, dropped from further data analysis. In addition, there
were 10 participants who had more than 10% of their IAT trials with latencies under 300
milliseconds, which could reflect guesswork. Therefore, these cases were eliminated,
which reduced the total sample size to 331 subjects. Besides these two cases, all other
data assumptions were satisfied. Scale reliability was then assessed via estimates of the
Correlations, means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s coefficient alphas for each
scale and subscale are presented in Table 4. The means suggest that participants, on
87
average, have limited previous racial outgroup contact, low conscious (subtle) prejudice,
88
Table 4
Correlations, Means, Standard Deviations, and Alphas among Total Scales and Subscales
Scale PRC SRC1 SRC SDC2 SDC M3 M M_O M_D M_A M_NJ M_NR E E_PT E_EC E_PD CP SP
PRC 1.00
SRC1 .07 1.00
SRC .10 .94** 1.00
SDC2 .07 .31** .39** 1.00
SDC .05 .32** .41** .94** 1.00
M3 -.15** .01 .07 .20** .22** 1.00
M -.14* .04 .10 .25** .27** .95** 1.00
M_O .01 .09 .11* .19** .22** .02 .32** 1.00
M_D -.09 .01 .08 .17** .18** .68** .68** .12* 1.00
M_A -.10 -.01 .02 .13* .12* .71** .68** .00 .30** 1.00
M_NJ -.12* .01 .06 .10 .13* .76** .66** -.20** .28** .43** 1.00
M_NR -.11 .01 .04 .15** .17** .56** .57** .15** .21** .14* .36** 1.00
E .10 .20** .26** .21** .21** -.05 .00 .17** .04 -.05 -.02 -.15** 1.00
E_PT .04 .21** .30** .30** .31** .20** .25** .20** .16** .12* .15** .12* .73** 1.00
E_EC .07 .17** .25** .19** .20** .03 .08 .16** .10 .02 .04 -.11 .83** .48** 1.00
E_PD .11* -.02 -.08 -.14** -.16** -.43** -.42** -.05 -.25** -.30** -.29** -.36** .38** -.15** .08 1.00
CP -.24** -.08 -.10 -.12* -.10 .12* .11* -.02 .08 .07 .10 .07 -.17** -.09 -.11* -.14** 1.00
SP .01 -.03 -.03 -.11* -.09 .04 .03 -.02 .01 -.01 .09 .04 .00 -.02 -.05 .08 0.10 1.00
Mean 1.85 3.26 3.33 3.27 3.26 3.34 3.31 3.19 3.49 3.31 3.47 3.12 3.35 3.59 3.81 2.64 2.27 .49
SD .76 .74 .66 .72 .49 .44 .37 .56 .67 .66 .75 .51 .39 .62 .63 .49 .51 .35
α .81 .67 .64 .66 .45 .89 .86 .71 .89 .87 .9 .73 .81 .8 .81 .79 .79 .81
Note: PRC = Prior Racial Outgroup Contact, SRC = Social Recategorization, SDC = Social Decategorization, M= Mindfulness, M_O = Observe facet, M_D =
Describe facet, M_A= Act with Awareness facet, M_NJ = Nonjudge facet, M_NR = Non-React facet, E = Empathy, E_PT = Perspective-Taking E_EC =
Empathetic Concern, E_PD = Personal Distress, CP = Conscious (Subtle) Prejudice, and SP = Subconscious Prejudice
1
Values represent only SRC items 3, 5, and 8.
2
Values represent only SDC items 4, 6, 9.
3
Values represent all mindfulness items except observing subscale items.
* p<.05; **p<.01
89
Model Development and Model Comparison
Measurement Model
observed score for the subconscious prejudice measure, subscale items for the social
racial outgroup contact and conscious [subtle] prejudice measures), parcels were
established for each scale by first fitting a factor solution to each set of items using
exploratory factor analysis and then averaging the items with the highest and lowest
coefficients to form the first indicator, averaging the items with the next highest and
lowest coefficients to form the second indicator, and so on. Table 5 depicts the factor
structure of these unidimensional measures and how each parcel was created.
Table 5
Exploratory Factor Analysis Results and Item-to-Construct Parceling of Unidimensional Measures
Factor/Item Eigenvalue Variance Coefficients Item Parcels
CP 3.24 40.46%
SR2K Item 1 .734 CP Parcel 1
SR2K Item 2 .612 CP Parcel 3
SR2K Item 3 .324 CP Parcel 1
SR2K Item 4 .588 CP Parcel 4
SR2K Item 5 .469 CP Parcel 2
SR2K Item 6 .557 CP Parcel 4
SR2K Item 7 .627 CP Parcel 2
SR2K Item 8 .552 CP Parcel 3
Note: PRC = Prior Racial Outgroup Contact Scale, CP = Conscious (Subtle) Prejudice, and SR2K =
Symbolic Racism 2000 Scale.
90
For multi-dimensional measures (i.e., mindfulness and empathy subscales),
Using this approach, responses were first fitted to a factor solution for each set of
subscale items. Next, because subscales of these measures consist of multiple items,
items with pattern and structural coefficients below .40 were deleted. Following, the
subscale items with the highest coefficients were averaged together to form the first
parcel, subscale items with the second highest coefficients were averaged together,
subscale items with the lowest coefficients were averaged together, and then subscale
items with the second lowest coefficients were averaged. Table 6 and 7, respectively,
presents the factor structure of the mindfulness and empathy measures used in this study
Created item parcels were then investigated and data assumptions from the
individual item analysis remained satisfied. The initial measurement model was then
for the observed variables used in the initial model are presented in Table 8.
91
Table 6
Exploratory Factor Analysis Results and Item-to-Construct Parceling of the Mindfulness Measure
Pattern Structure
Factor/Item Eigenvalue Variance Item Parcels
Coefficients Coefficients
NJ 7.83 23.73%
FFMQ Item 3 .650 .667 M Parcel 4
FFMQ Item 10 .633 .666 M Parcel 3
FFMQ Item 14 .717 .765
FFMQ Item 17 .715 .683
FFMQ Item 25 .779 .811 M Parcel 2
FFMQ Item 30 .827 .825 M Parcel 1
FFMQ Item 35 .686 .711
FFMQ Item 39 .672 .688
D 3.86 11.71%
FFMQ Item 2 .762 .761 M Parcel 1
FFMQ Item 7 .733 .735
FFMQ Item 12 .714 .745
FFMQ Item 16 .700 .729
FFMQ Item 22 .509 .591 M Parcel 4
FFMQ Item 27 .657 .649
FFMQ Item 32 .631 .636 M Parcel 3
FFMQ Item 37 .750 .765 M Parcel 2
A 2.81 8.51%
FFMQ Item 5 .765 .712 M Parcel 2
FFMQ Item 8 .705 .743
FFMQ Item 13 .807 .788 M Parcel 1
FFMQ Item 18 .580 .672
FFMQ Item 23 .519 .591 M Parcel 3
FFMQ Item 28 .597 .657
FFMQ Item 34 .535 .586 M Parcel 4
FFMQ Item 38 .684 .715
NR 2.46 7.47%
FFMQ Item 19 . 542 .560
FFMQ Item 21 .427 .457 M Parcel 3
FFMQ Item 24 .596 .603 M Parcel 2
FFMQ Item 29 .738 .723 M Parcel 1
FFMQ Item 33 .497 .506 M Parcel 4
O1 1.85 5.62%
FFMQ Item 15 .561 .598 M Parcel 1
FFMQ Item 20 .537 .556 M Parcel 2
FFMQ Item 26 .499 .519 M Parcel 4
FFMQ Item 31 .481 .506 M Parcel 3
92
Table 7
Exploratory Factor Analysis Results and Item-to-Construct Parceling of the Empathy Measure
Pattern Structure
Factor/Item Eigenvalue Variance Item Parcels
Coefficients Coefficients
EC 4.89 25.71%
IRI Item 1 .702 .730 E Parcel 2
IRI Item 3 .458 .421 E Parcel 3
IRI Item 6 .475 .512 E Parcel 4
IRI Item 10 .755 .728 E Parcel 1
IRI Item 13 .593 .661
IRI Item 15 .646 .704
IRI Item 17 .512 .598
PD 3.25 17.10% .
IRI Item 4 .624 .623
IRI Item 7 .495 .512 E Parcel 3
IRI Item 12 .606 .601 E Parcel 4
IRI Item 14 .631 .646
IRI Item 18 .817 .812 E Parcel 1
IRI Item 20 .662 .649 E Parcel 2
PT 1.62 8.52%
IRI Item 2 .404 .529 E Parcel 3
IRI Item 5 .570 .595 E Parcel 4
IRI Item 8 .580 .645
IRI Item 16 .605 .659
IRI Item 19 .799 .723 E Parcel 1
IRI Item 21 .669 .662 E Parcel 2
93
Table 8
Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations among Measured Variables in the Initial Measurement Model
PRC PRC PRC SRC SRC SRC SDC SDC SDC M M M M E E E E CP CP CP CP
Variable SP
_P1 _P2 _P3 _3 _5 _8 _4 _6 _9 _P1 _P2 _P3 _P4 _P1 _P2 _P3 _P4 _P1 _P2 _P3 _P4
PRC_P1 1.00
PRC_P2 .65** 1.00
PRC_P3 .64** .60** 1.00
SRC_3 .06 .13* .02 1.00
SRC_5 -.05 .01 -.08 .57** 1.00
SRC_8 .11* .12* .09 .28** .35** 1.00
SDC_4 .10 .07 .08 .15** .21** .10 1.00
SDC_6 -.01 .00 -.02 .18** .12* .06 .35** 1.00
SDC_9 .09 .08 .03 .29** .32** .27** .50** .33** 1.00
M_P1 -.09 -.12* -.14** -.03 -.02 -.03 .13* .11 .07 1.00
-
M_P2 .15** -.14** -.16** .02 .05 .01 .15** .09 .13* .71** 1.00
M_P3 -.10 -.05 -.14** .10 .05 .06 .22** .15** .20** .56** .59** 1.00
-
M_P4 .13** -.16** -.09 -.02 -.02 -.09 .13* .08 .04 .56** .55** .56** 1.00
E_P1 .04 .05 .04 .02 .02 .01 .03 -.15** .03 -.08 -.02 .02 .07 1.00
E_P2 .01 .04 -.02 -.01 .09 .05 .11* -.02 .10 .04 .10 .14** .03 .46** 1.00
E_P3 -.06 -.04 -.09 -.08 -.11* -.02 .03 .05 -.01 .21** .25** .19** .20** .15** .13* 1.00
E_P4 -.07 -.04 -.04 .04 .03 -.02 -.03 -.03 -.02 .10 .18** .12* .13* .18** .19** .25** 1.00
-
CP_P1 .21** -.15** -.10 -.06 -.12* -.07 -.16** -.14** -.18** .10 .07 .03 .11 -.05 -.02 .06 .18** 1.00
-
CP_P2 .16** -.18** -.14** .00 .00 -.01 .00 .04 -.03 .09 .09 .07 .18** -.03 .01 .12* .16** .45** 1.00
-
CP_P3 .15** -.18** -.15** -.06 -.08 -.13* -.10 .02 -.16** .05 .05 .01 .11* -.04 -.05 .04 .13* .59** .41** 1.00
-
CP_P4 .16** -.22** -.18** .00 -.01 -.04 -.05 -.07 -.10 .08 .10 .06 .13* .01 .00 .15** .11* .52** .63** .42** 1.00
SP -.01 .05 -.01 .03 -.03 -.06 -.07 -.12* -.06 .07 .03 .02 .06 -.07 -.08 -.08 -.02 .17** .06 .01 .08 1.00
Mean 2.03 1.85 1.68 3.25 3.36 3.16 3.22 2.99 3.59 3.32 3.11 3.36 3.25 3.45 3.47 3.03 2.98 2.05 2.42 2.33 2.28 .49
SD .86 .85 .92 .98 .93 .95 1.05 .86 .85 .54 .55 .53 .52 .49 .47 .48 .55 .59 .67 .69 .62 .35
Note: PRC = Prior Racial Outgroup Contact, SRC = Social Recategorization, SDC = Social Decategorization, M= Mindfulness, E = Empathy, CP = Conscious
(Subtle) Prejudice, SP = Subconscious Prejudice, and P = Item Parcels.
* p<.05; **p<.01
94
The initial measurement model was then compared to two competing models to
determine the best fitting a priori model. The first competing model was nested with the
initial model and included additional Social Re(De)categorization Scale items that were
modified in a prior pilot study. The second alternative model was non-nested with the
initial model and consisted of the inclusion of the Observe FFMQ subscale during
mindfulness item-to-construct parceling. Model fit indices explored across the three a
priori models were the change in chi-square (for the nested model only), AIC, BIC, and
the RMSEA – to provide an idea of within model fit. Table 9 presents the comparative
Table 9
Model Fit Indices and Comparisons for Competing Measurement Models
RMSEA
Model χ2 df Δχ2 Δdf p AIC BIC
(90% CI)
.048
Initial measurement model 335.19 189 . 463.19 706.53
(.040-.057)
Including modified SDC and .057
530.97 255 195.78 66 .000 670.97 937.12
SRC items model (.050-.064)
Including M_O subscale items .050
342.75 189 . 470.75 714.09
model (.041-.058)
Note: SDC = Social Decategorization, SRC = Social Recategorization, and M_O = Mindfulness Observe
facet
A comparison of the change in chi-square, AIC, and BIC indicated that the initial
measurement model is a better fitting model to the data than the other two competing
models. Further exploration of model fit for this measurement model produced the
following values: GFI = .92, CFI = .93, and SRMR = .058. Therefore, these indices, and
especially the RMSEA index, indicate that the initial measurement model provides a
Using this initial measurement model, predicted paths were drawn to create the
initial structural model. Next, the initial structural model was compared to three, a priori,
alternative models to determine the best fitting structural equation model. The first
competing model explored the possibility that the Social Recategorization and Social
well. The final alternative model explored the prediction that mindfulness reduces
conscious (subtle) and subconscious prejudice indirectly rather than directly by deleting
the direct paths from the Mindfulness variable to the prejudice variables. All three of
these competing structural models nest with the initial structural model, and therefore,
exploring the following indices will determine the best fitting structural model: change in
chi-square, AIC, and BIC. In addition, investigating the RMSEA index for each model
will present an idea of within model fit. Table 10 presents the model comparison
findings.
96
Table 10
Model Fit Indices and Comparisons for Competing Structural Models
RMSEA
Model χ2 df Δχ2 Δdf p AIC BIC
(90% CI)
.054
Initial structural model 378.42 193 498.42 726.54
(.046-.062)
.048
Correlated SDC and SRC model 338.66 192 39.76 1 .000 460.66 692.59
(.040-.056)
.054
Correlated CP and SP model 375.45 192 2.97 1 .085 497.45 729.38
(.046-.062)
.054
Indirect mindfulness only model 386.06 195 7.65 2 .022 502.06 722.58
(.046-.062)
Note: SDC = Social Decategorization, SRC = Social Recategorization, CP = Conscious (Subtle) Prejudice,
and SP = Subconscious Prejudice
A comparison of the change in chi-square, AIC, and BIC indicated that the first
latent variables) is the best fitting model to the data than the initial model and the other
two competing models. Further exploration of model fit for this model produced the
following values: GFI = .92, CFI = .93, and SRMR = .058. Therefore, these indices, and
the RMSEA index, indicate that this structural model is the final structural equation
The primary focus of this study was to explore the direct and indirect effects of
mindfulness on conscious (subtle) and subconscious racial prejudice. Using the final
structural equation model for this study, Table 10 presents the unstandardized path
coefficients (b) with their standard error and critical ranges, and standardized path
coefficients (). Following, Table 11 depicts the standardized direct, indirect, and total
97
Table 11
Unstandardized and Standardized Path Coefficients for the Final Structural Equation Model
Path b SE CR p
M ---> SRC .050 .133 .380 .704 .027
---> SDC .528 .154 3.430 .000* .268
---> E .32 .067 1.963 .050 .154
---> CP .192 .095 2.010 .045 .144
---> SP .127 .065 1.960 .050 .131
Note: M = Mindfulness, PRC = Prior Racial Outgroup Contact, SRC = Social Recategorization,
SDC = Social Decategorization, CP = Conscious (Subtle) Prejudice, and SP = Subconscious Prejudice
98
Table 12
Standardized Effects for the Final Structural Equation Model
Effect Variable SRC SDC E CP SP
Direct M .027 .268 .154 .144 .131
PRC .027 .171 .047 -.240 .054
SRC .000 .000 .000 .018 .067
SDC .000 .000 .000 -.206 -.177
E .000 .000 .000 .024 -.127
Note: M = Mindfulness, PRC = Prior Racial Outgroup Contact, SRC = Social Recategorization, SDC =
Social Decategorization, CP = Conscious (Subtle) Prejudice, and SP = Subconscious Prejudice.
The path coefficients and effects contained within this model suggest a variety of
prejudice. That is, given the adequacy of the model, for each standard deviation increase
.27 of a standard deviation; in addition, for each standard deviation increase in degree of
subconscious prejudice will decrease by .21 and .18 of a standard deviation, respectively,
with all things being equal. However, because of the positive effects (although not
99
statistically significant) from mindfulness to both conscious (and subconscious
prejudice (, as well as the similar positive but nonsignificant direct paths from
mediating effect of social decategorization was not large enough for mindfulness to have
Two other predictions that were attained from the final model relate to prior racial
outgroup contact. This latent variable followed a similar path to mindfulness of having a
as large as the Mindfulness variable. However, and different from mindfulness, the
construct of Prior Racial Outgroup Contact had a negative effect on participants’ degree
social recategorization also did not have negative effects on participants’ racial prejudice
levels. Finally, prior racial outgroup contact did have negative effects on participants’
The results of the path coefficients and effects contained within the final structural
equation model for this study indicate that after controlling for prior racial outgroup
increasing awareness and attention to racial biases, and indirectly through motivational,
indicated that mindfulness does not appear to affect prejudice levels directly or indirectly.
The overall finding that mindfulness did not directly decrease racial prejudice
levels are contrary to prediction, but perhaps not surprising. Although, general
Langer 1989, 1997), an increase in attention and awareness to racial stimuli, such as
racial discrimination or one’s own racial prejudices (i.e., racial mindfulness), may be too
state mindfulness and directly educating individual regarding racial biases (in comparison
to a control intervention) is the next step in this program of research, and should provide
However, the findings that general mindfulness did not increase racial prejudice
levels indirectly were more unexpected. According to the final structural equation model
101
prejudice levels, mindfulness, overall, did not indirectly affect prejudice levels. One
possible reason is how participants’ degree of empathy was measured in this study.
The findings that general mindfulness did not increase empathy, and empathy did
not decrease at least conscious (subtle) prejudice are quite contrary to prior research. The
deemed as important as this subscale was created for this study and therefore, may not be
confirmed construct validity, such as the Social Connectedness Scale (Lee & Robbins,
empathy level (e.g., Miller, 1995; Shapiro et al., 1998, 2007). Similarly, empathy has
often been found to decrease an individual’s conscious (subtle) prejudice levels (Stephan
& Finlay, 1999; Finlay & Stephan, 2000). Upon further investigation, the believed
reason for the nonsignificant effect from mindfulness to empathy was due to one of the
empathy subscales (i.e., personal distress), significantly correlating (but negative and
correlating with the other empathy subscale, empathetic concern (r = .08; see Table 4).
These subscales were predicted to correlate highly and determine a hierarchical empathy
factor. To investigate, I dropped this subscale, reran the empathy item parceling process,
and incorporated revised empathy item parcels into the initial measurement model and
102
From this process, mindfulness did not improve the initial measurement model fit,
but did significantly increase participants’ degree of empathy (.18) in the final
structural equation model. In addition, the path from empathy to conscious (subtle)
from empathy to conscious (subtle) prejudice was negative, the coefficient was still small
and nonsignificant. Therefore, other path coefficients in the final structural equation
model did not change much. For example, mindfulness still did not directly, indirectly,
One possible reason why empathy did not significantly decrease conscious
(subtle) prejudice, and therefore, why mindfulness did not decrease this prejudice
variable indirectly through empathy and social decategorization with or without the
personal distress subscale items included in the model, is because some prior researchers
have indicated that empathy is more of an emotion and therefore, has an negative effect
on the affective dimension of conscious (subtle) prejudice rather than the cognitive
dimension (Esses & Dovidio, 2002; Tropp & Pettigrew, 2005). The conscious (subtle)
prejudice scale used in this study was more measuring students’ cognitive dimension of
conscious (subtle) prejudice (e.g., “How much of the racial tension that exists in the
United States today do you think Blacks are responsible for creating?) and therefore,
provides reasoning why these predictions were not found. In future research, an affective
measure, such as a feeling thermometer (i.e., an imaginary scale ranging from 0o [very
cold] to 100 o [very warm] that prompts participants to indicate their feelings to a certain
103
person or group; Campbell, 1971), will be used to more closely investigate the indirect
The results found from this study must be interpreted in light of several
limitations. The first limitation is that most of the data collected in this study were self-
report. Social desirability and other issues, such as shared method variance, could have
biased the results. For example, the mean score for the conscious (subtle) prejudice, self-
report measure was relatively low in comparison to the mean score of the subconscious
prejudice, implicit measure, which was moderately high, although these instruments were
this study. A second limitation is that the data were obtained from students at only one
predominantly White institution (PWI). Incorporating students from other PWIs may
have improved the study, at least the generalization of findings. The third limitation also
relates to the sample. Because a large sample size is often needed in SEM, there were not
enough participants in this study to randomly split the total sample and cross-validate the
final structural model, which would have increased the validity of the findings. A fourth
limitation is that subconscious prejudice was determined in the models by only a single
observed indicator. Although reliability estimates were calculated for this measure, this
construct could still contain invalidity and unreliability, which could have influenced the
results. Similar to invalidity and unreliability issues, the fifth limitation in this study was
the use of item parceling for some of the latent variables. However, because the purpose
of the study was to explore the relations between latent variables and not the relations
among items comprising the measured variables, parceling was deemed appropriate. The
104
final limitation relates to SEM, as well as other statistical methods (e.g., multiple
regression and path analysis), is that the final structural equation model could have
omitted common variables or causes, which could have affected the results of the study.
However, it was believed that the randomly stratified and balanced sample, as well as
incorporating and measuring prior racial outgroup contact, subsumed all of the common
105
CHAPTER 6: STUDY B
increasing White students’ awareness of and attention to racial biases, such as White
privilege, does mindfulness attenuate the negative effects that can arise from cognitive
expected that participants with a higher degree of mindfulness would exhibit greater
acceptance to racial biases and less negative reactions resulting from post-decisional
cognitive dissonance.
Method
Participants
The study consisted of 40 students who were selected one week later from the
sample in Study A, using a stratified random sampling procedure. The strata used were
participants who responded “No” to the following qualifying item asked during Study A,
“Have you ever read an article by Peggy McIntosh (1989) entitled, ‘Unpacking the
students who met the first stratum, the 20 students with the highest overall mindfulness
scores ( =3.96) and the 20 students with the lowest mindfulness scores ( =2.87) from
Study A were recruited to participate in this study. The high mindful group consisted of
first-year (n=2, 10%), second-year (n=2, 10%), third-year (n=10, 50%), and fourth-year
106
level students (n=6, 30%), with a little more than half self-identifying as female (n=12,
60%). The low mindful group consisted of first-year (n=5, 25%), second-year (n=3,
15%), third-year (n=6, 30%), fourth-year (n=5, 25%), and fifth-year (senior) level
students (n=1, 5%), with close to half self-identifying as female (n=9, 45%).
Procedure
Participants received an introductory message about the study via email that
included a SurveyMonkey Web address where electronic activities of the study were
housed. Once at the SurveyMonkey Web site, all groups first observed a consent form
that explained the potential risks and benefits of participation with limited knowledge
about the intention of the study to prevent bias. After reading the consent form and
Following, and adapted from a similar procedure in Ancis and Szymanski (2001),
participants observed, read, and reacted to an article listing numerous benefits a White
woman has experienced due to her skin color in contrast to people of color. Before the
article was presented, the general directions read, “Please read the list below. Once
finished, identify 3 to 5 (or more) of the conditions that the author describes as relating to
White privilege, and within the blank template following, please provide reactions to the
conditions chosen.”
107
Measures
The White Privilege Scale (WPS; Swim & Miller, 1999; Appendix G) measured
privilege. The WPS consists of five items based on McIntosh’s (1989) White privilege
“Strongly Agree”) their degree of belief in White privilege. Sample items include: “My
skin color is an asset to me in my everyday life;” and “White people have certain
advantages that minorities do not have in this society.” Prior analyses indicated that items
reveal a single factor structure and scores are internally consistent (α=.72, N=102; Swim
& Miller).
After responding to the WPS, participants read and openly reacted to Peggy
McIntosh’s (1989) article, “Unpacking the Invisible Knapsack” (Appendix H). This
with White privilege such as (a) accurate, positive, and ample representation of
her race in the media, academic institutions, and grade school materials; (b) being
able to associate with members of her own race most of the time; (c) easily
finding products and services associated with her race and cultural traditions; (d)
108
seeking medical assistance, shopping in a store, using credit cards or checks, and
interacting with other White people; (e) engaging in behavior (e.g., talking with a
without it being attributed to her race; and (f) ignoring or devaluing the cultural
Data Analysis
In this study, I explored four assumptions based on findings from prior research.
The first assumption relates to the finding that most White individuals are unaware of
their Whiteness or White privilege, as it has been normalized and invisible (Wildman &
Davis, 1997). Therefore, it was assumed in this study that White participants with high
mindfulness would not significantly differ from participants with low mindfulness on the
White privilege measure, as both groups of participants will have relatively low scores.
(and if applicable, mean differences) of scores on the WPS between participants with
high and low mindfulness scores. Participants with a high White privilege score (i.e.,
mean of 3.5 or above based on the 5-point Likert scale of the White privilege measure)
were dropped from further data analysis, as these respondents are not likely to experience
post-decisional cognitive dissonance from reading the article (i.e., they already were
109
they realize that a rejected decision might have been better than their chosen decision
The second and third assumption relate to the prior research findings that many
White individuals experience cognitive dissonance when first becoming aware of racial
al., 2007; Pedersen et al., 2005). Gawronski et al. (2008) provided an explanation for this
dissonance, the person will tend to emphasize or search for positive reasons of their
chosen decision and negative reasons of the rejected (but better) decision to reduce this
threat or feeling. As a result, it was assumed in this study that participants would
experience some degree of post-decisional cognitive dissonance after they indicate their
initial decision (a low score on the White privilege measure) and then observe evidence
for their rejected decision (read the White privilege article). In addition, to reduce their
dissonance, it was assumed that participants would provide support for their low score on
the White privilege measure (or lack of awareness and agreement to White privilege) and
The last assumption is related to the open receptive nature of mindfulness and its
associated benefits. Prior research has determined that when one’s self-esteem or image
attenuate ego defensiveness and negative emotions (Emavardhana & Tori, 1997; Heppner
et al., 2008; Hodgins & Knee, 2002). Therefore, it was assumed in this study that
110
mindfulness would mediate participants’ ego-defensiveness and negative emotions
participants with high mindfulness scores would likely indicate greater acceptance and
less negative emotions in their reactions to the White privilege article in comparison to
themes and patterns related to White privilege emerge from the data, constant
comparative methodology (Lincoln & Guba, 1985) was the coding process used in this
validity) of the results (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). I achieved conformability by analyst
transferability by providing “thick descriptions” of the data and describing the context for
the reader in the following section. In addition, the second researcher and I achieved
5
The researcher is a doctoral graduate from the Department of Educational Psychology at The University
of Texas at Austin. The researcher self-identifies as White and has years of experience with qualitative
research, such as discourse analysis.
6
The auditor is a master’s graduate from the Department of Educational Administration at The University
of Texas at Austin. The auditor self-identifies as a person of color and has years of experience with social
justice and qualitative research.
111
purpose of the study, review the raw data, coding process and notes, and resulting themes
different stages. In stage one, another researcher and I first read and reread,
pages were analyzed with responses ranging from 2 lines to 2.5 pages with an average of
15 lines or .5 a page. The number of White privilege conditions the students’ responded
to ranged from zero to six with an average of three conditions. After reading the data, we
met to discuss general patterns and themes observed emerging from the first 5 responses.
categories and created an outline of categories from the data (Miles & Huberman, 1994),
Next, stage two consisted of reviewing and open coding the next 10 responses
independently using the initial list of categories. The second researcher and I met again to
discuss the coding scheme and the findings for these responses. From this process, we
revised our coding structure (i.e., combining two categories into one, and adding two new
categories).
Stage three then consisted of reviewing the rest of the 25 responses independently
last responses, which resulted in returning to the original data. These disagreements were
discussed and resolved without dropping the statement or code from the analysis.
112
In stage four, once the open coding was complete, we met and began to group
similar categories together and explore other patterns and themes across categories. Upon
completion of the content analysis and coding procedure, stage five consisted of
independently reviewing the final themes and subthemes of coding for each response to
assess coding accuracy. Finally, stage six consisted of exploring themes and subthemes
The auditor reviewed all six stages of this constant comparative process, as well
as the original data and disagreements encountered during coding. The auditor supported
much of the coding process but provided two recommendations, which were incorporated
as revisions. The first suggestion included revising the language for one of our themes
and the second consisted of revising coding for one response. Because this second
suggestion was also one of the code/statements that was initially disagreed upon, the
Results
team identified themes and subthemes resulting from participants’ responses and coding
process. Finally, I explored themes and subthemes in relation to participants who were
total score above 3.5, indicating a high level of awareness and agreement to the construct
113
of White privilege. Therefore, these participants were included in the first three stages of
the content analysis process in order to aid open coding, but then deleted from subsequent
stages. The mean score of the 15 low-mindful participants was 2.50 with a standard
deviation of .684, and 2.32 with a standard deviation of .604 for the 15 high-mindful
participants. For the total 30 participants, internal consistency was .74 for the scores on
the White privilege measure. The new demographics of the high-mindful group
consisted of first-year (n=2, 13%), second-year (n=2, 13%), third-year (n=7, 47%), and
fourth-year level students (n=4, 27%), with still, a little more than half self-identifying as
female (n=8, 53%). The low mindful group now consisted of first-year (n=3, 20%),
second-year (n=3, 20%), third-year (n=6, 40%), fourth-year level students (n=3, 20%),
analysis process led to 4 general themes with 11 corresponding subthemes (see Table 13).
The general themes represent varying levels of awareness and agreement to the construct
of White privilege from none (Theme 1: Unawareness and/or denial to White privilege)
(Theme 3: Moderate awareness and agreement to White privilege) and finally to high
response was coded into only one general theme and more than likely, was coded into
114
(e.g., first-year, second-year, etc.) and gender, did not appear to vary across the four
general themes.
Table 13
Themes and Subthemes of the Content Analysis
Themes
The mean White privilege score (based on the WPS) for participants whose
responses were categorized into Theme 1, Theme 2, Theme 3, and Theme 4 were 2.39,
2.26, 2.77, and 1.8, respectively, indicating a general level of disagreement to the
construct of White privilege across categories. As a result, the category themes and
corresponding subthemes within each category can exemplify how these individuals dealt
115
disagreement to White privilege and then reading 47 conditions of how White privilege
privilege, racial discrimination, or racial prejudice did not exist for all of these
participants. For example, 7 participants identified with McIntosh’s condition #44 (“I
can easily find academic courses and institutions, which give attention only to people of
my race”) and expressed unawareness and disagreement, such as one student’s reaction:
limited its attention to only Caucasians. I have never been to or heard of any such
place, nor have I heard of any student groups or scholarships explicitly reserved
think about race affecting my day to day activities. I can agree with most all of the
statements here because I don't believe race has a huge affect on my life.”
There were also four other common (but not majority) subthemes found across
White privilege. These subthemes overlapped and included: presenting counter examples
116
Subtheme 1A: Presenting counter examples. Of the 12 participants, 7 expressed
conditions in the article. The exception example cited by 3 participants was U.S.
In my opinion these statements are racist and generalizing to all White people.
White people are a majority yes, but it doesn't mean that there are no multiracial
indicating that White people are treated in a “derogatory fashion” by people of color, or
being looked over for a job if “if the company has yet to meet its quota for minority
most of the time, pretty well assured that I will not be followed or harassed”), one student
responded, “This is absolutely not true. White people are followed and harassed just as
their responses. A range of expressions were used within and across responses that
117
“I hate how you put ‘I’ in front of every sentence as to say that it relates to me in
any way.”
“The rest [of the conditions] are absurd but I will humor the survey.”
language, such as, “I honestly don't care and don't feel they need to be griping about more
rights” and “Minorities continue to cry and whine” or possibly stereotypical language, “I
don't see how this [McIntosh’s condition 17] can even be related to race unless it is a
certain type of food, though I don't wish to list those foods at this time.”
Subtheme 1C: Using strong and certain language. Another subtheme found
across a third of the participants was a use of strong and certain language when
believe it would be almost impossible to find a class that serves only to whites; fairly
certain that Equifax doesn't look at your profile, see ‘African American’, and then lower
subtheme found with a third of participants who were classified as lacking awareness or
denying White privilege was attributing differential treatment to factors other than race.
These participants might be exhibiting a degree of subtle prejudice (i.e., expressing their
118
prejudice indirectly or subtly as when racial beliefs or behavior can be justified on some
other factor than race). For example, some other nonracial factors attributing to
differential treatment were gender (“I believe that my being a woman has more to do with
inequality than my race”), personality (“I do not associate race with work ethic … That is
a general personality trait”), and behavior (“It is because people live on credit and don't
participants appeared to show a degree of awareness to at least one out of minimal three
McIntosh’s White privilege conditions they, on average, referenced, and would either
For example, a participant identified McIntosh’s first condition (“I can, if I wish,
arrange to be in the company of people of my race most of the time”) and provided the
am involved in are mainly made up of White men and women so it's not very hard
to be in the company of people of my race. Like I said, I don't really see this as a
"privilege." It's just easier to find White people than it is to find other races.
However, for the another conditions cited, this participant indicated a degree of
25 (“If a traffic cop pulls me over or if the IRS audits my tax return, I can be sure I
119
haven't been singled out because of my race”): “Again, I have no idea what it feels like to
be singled out because of my race so I see this as a privilege. I can imagine it would be
In addition with this broad theme, participant responses differed from participants
in the first theme by not expressing anger or defensiveness in their responses. Moreover,
the language used in participant responses in this group was more flexible, which differed
drastically from those 4 students in the first theme that used strong and certain language.
There were also three other common (but not majority), overlapping subthemes
attributing differential treatment to nonracial factors (e.g., Whites being the majority),
numerous counter examples. Two of these subthemes were somewhat similar to two
subthemes in Theme 1. The main difference, however, was that participants in this
general theme indicated a degree of awareness to at least one of the minimal three White
Theme 1. In addition, other differences between these subthemes exist and are minor,
differs between Theme 1 and Theme 2 by degree of attribution from full to partial
attribution.
120
Subtheme 2A: Partially attributing differential treatment to nonracial factors. Of
the 11 participants categorized in this broad theme, 8 were found to partially attribute
differential treatment to factors other than race. These participants’ appeared to indicate
agreement to differential treatment but would somewhat justify that at least one of the
treatments was based on such nonracial factors as money, personality trait, achievement,
For example, one of these participants cited three of McIntosh’s conditions and
indicated:
I feel these three describe White privilege in America today. Although number
three is really a privilege of any one who has enough money to support
themselves and their family should be able to live in an area which they can
afford and want to live, I think that all people should be able to do well in a
I think racial attitudes are more based on personality and personal achievement
than solely race. Some people of color do fall into the stereotypes that are
culturally placed on them, but a large number have started to break through these
121
are exhibited in the following responses: “More and more people are beginning to value a
cultured view of the world and ignorance is something that I feel is not looked on
kindly;” “Number 24, I feel is highly unlikely in today's time. Speaking to the person in
charge now does not necessarily mean that you will be speaking to a white person. We
have taken long strides;” and “Today I feel that race has nothing to do with that factor.”
evidence: “I am never asked to speak for all the people of my racial group. This is true,
but no one should be asked to speak for all of the people in one racial group;” and “We
are all people and doing well should give credit to the person not the color of their skin.”
participant responses was the use of counter examples to help support the subthemes or
the overall theme found for this group. For instance, 2 participants cited President
In today's society, the "White privilege", in many cases, works against White
people, especially in the legal sense. If a White person is on trial against a person
of race, many times the jury will decide in favor of the person of race, believing
that White person persecuted the person of race b/c there does exist a notion of
"White privilege".
122
Theme 3: Moderate Awareness and Agreement to White Privilege
awareness and acceptance to the construct of White privilege. Overall, these participants
privilege conditions. Or, participants exhibited awareness and agreement to at least one
condition of the minimal three selected, but indicated the negative effects caused by the
and agreement to only one White privilege condition (I can do well at something without
Americans or Latinos, are often seen in this light, by both Whites and members of
their own race. Often times you hear people say things like, "I am a proud,
successful black woman"; however, I doubt many White people would make a
similar statement. It is hard to see the fine line between being a leader or success
story for a group of people without having one's success perhaps treated
There were also two other common (but not majority), overlapping subthemes
treatment to nonracial factors. This last subtheme is similar to the subtheme found from
123
Subtheme 3A: Expanding on or providing additional examples of White privilege.
told about our national heritage or about ‘civilization,’ I am shown that people of my
…when studying American history, our national heritage and our civilization, the
focus was mostly on the White race and their domination and cultivation of the
Americas. Granted, studies were done on all kinds of other cultures that were not
Caucasian, but they were done in short periods of time, while the majority of the
time was spent on our White forefathers discovering and colonizing our country.
condition(s) is from a participant who agreed to condition 22 (“I can remain oblivious of
the language and customs of persons of color who constitute the world's majority without
…I see how the author is coming from a very Americanized point of view of
difficult time because they are expected to learn and speak "White people"
124
Subtheme 3B: Partially attributing differential treatment to nonracial factors. The
last subtheme included 3 of the 6 participant responses and refers to the partial attribution
of differential treatment to factors other than race for the White privilege condition(s)
would partially rationalize that the treatment was based on such nonracial factors as
Whites being the majority, gender, or being uncultured. For example, in disagreement to
condition 6 (“I can turn on the television or open to the front page of the paper and see
people, so it's not unlikely to turn on the television and see someone of my race
on the screen. Of course, the media industry has been improving over the past few
decades.
degree of awareness and acceptance to the construct of White privilege. This participant
privilege conditions referenced, as well as exhibited the following two subthemes found
often in the response: indicating the negative effects and expanding on or providing
Subtheme 4A: Indicating the negative effects of White privilege. The participant
indicated many negative effects of White privilege throughout her or his response. For
125
example, in relation to the agreement of condition 7 (“When I am told about our national
heritage or about ‘civilization,’ I am shown that people of my color made it what it is”),
I think this is an important point because children need positive role models to
look up to. If schools or other contexts concentrate on white history leaders, then
a child of a racial minority is learning that people of their race did not contribute
Another example is in relation to condition 22 (“I can remain oblivious of the language
and customs of persons of color who constitute the world's majority without feeling in
my culture any penalty for such oblivion”), where the participant responded, “…our lack
of knowledge of other cultures is regrettable because it makes the people of the United
The final subtheme found often throughout this participant’s response is substantially
response to condition 22, where the participant provided an additional example of failing
When our Secretary of State Hilary Clinton went recently to Russia, she
committed cultural and language blunders when she gave him a "reset" button,
which was translated into the Russian word for 'overload'. The Russian
representative had to explain to her (in English) her mistake. This reflects poorly
126
on the United States that we could not even find a translator, while the Russian
In relation to condition 35 (“I can take a job with an affirmative action employer without
having my co-workers on the job suspect that I got it because of my race”), this
White co-workers assume that their new employee's race was a deciding factor.
Policies like this, although they may increase the amount of minority employees
in that workplace, send the message that these minority groups can't be successful
without help.
mindfulness would exhibit greater acceptance to racial biases and less negative reactions
differences between themes and subthemes of low and high mindfulness participants. The
number of participants in low and high mindfulness groups by themes and subthemes are
7
I considered using non-parametric tests, such as a chi-square test, to explore if these differences between
low mindful and high mindful were statistically significant. However, one of the general assumptions for
these types of tests is that cell sizes are 5 or larger, which is not the case for Themes 3 and 4.
127
Table 14
Themes and Subthemes by Participants in the Low Mindfulness Group
Low
Total
Themes mindful
n
n (%)
128
Table 15
Themes and Subthemes by Participants in the High Mindfulness Group
High
Total
Themes mindful
n
n (%)
In terms of the four general themes, it appears that participants with a low degree
of mindfulness are associated more with a lack or low degree of awareness and
agreement to White privilege (58% and 55%, respectively), where participants with a
moderate to high degree of mindfulness are associated with a higher degree of awareness
and agreement to White privilege (77% and 100%, respectively). Within Theme 1,
participants with low mindfulness are associated with more anger or defensive reactions
to, what is assumed to be, post-decisional cognitive dissonance (57%) than participants
129
with high mindfulness (20%). Therefore, the results of Study B appear to indicate that
biases and less negative reactions resulting from post-decisional cognitive dissonance.
Discussion
degree of acceptance towards the racial bias of White privilege, and decrease negative
responses to an article that lists 47 White privilege conditions from 15 participants with
the lowest overall mindfulness scores and 15 participants with the highest mindfulness
scores in Study A, and who all initially reported in Study B a low agreement to the
awareness and acceptance to the construct of White privilege emerged. The findings
exhibit greater acceptance to the racial construct of White privilege and less negative
reactions resulting from post-decisional cognitive dissonance. This finding was similar to
the results from the racial mindfulness study by Lillis and Hayes (2007), where
biases.
There are some limitations to this study. The other White coder and I could have
influenced our interpretations of the data due to our race. In addition, I could have biased
understanding of the purpose of the study. These possibilities were the reason for the use
130
of a qualified research team including an auditor who identifies as a person of color, as
credibility. However, the study still needs to be replicated. Social desirability also could
have affected participants’ responses. For example, one participant provided the
following two statements, which could indicate that this person was concerned with
social desirability when responding: “To preface this, I'd like to say that I was raised in a
household that typically condemned other races privately but was open to them in public”
and “I don't see how this can even be related to race unless it is a certain type of food,
though I don't wish to list those foods at this time.” Another limitation is that McIntosh’s
list of conditions was based on her own observations from 1989. An updated version of
the White privilege list was considered but not implemented. Due to the number of
respondents who used President Barack Obama as a counter example to White privilege,
131
CHAPTER 7: GENERAL DISCUSSION
Overview
continue to exist in almost every social sector. Such examples of racial inequalities can
be observed in domains of net income and net worth, home equity and ownership, and
academic success and schooling. Researchers have determined that a substantial portion
Due to the rise in egalitarian beliefs and social norms, present racial prejudice and
discrimination has not decreased within the last century, but rather they have altered to
subtler and subconscious forms; in addition, these present forms have been found to have
racial prejudice is believed to be the primary force behind racial discriminatory behavior,
and creating interventions to reduce conscious (subtle) and subconscious racial prejudice.
one’s racial attitudes: (a) consciousness of racial biases, (b) motivation to reduce them,
and (c) cognitive strategies for prejudice regulation. However, most White Americans do
not hold a high degree of racial consciousness; therefore, interventions and educational
programs are needed. In addition, when Whites’ racial consciousness is increased, many
guilt, which can influence Whites to avoid or increase their racial attitudes. Moreover,
132
due to the natural of prejudice, developing and practicing cognitive strategies to
continually regulate and reduce prejudice is cognitively taxing, difficult for many, and
intergroup contact.
The construct of mindfulness may provide a solution to these limitations and help
reduce both conscious (subtle) and subconscious racial prejudice. Mindfulness is defined
this construct may reduce racial prejudice directly due to its inherent nature of increasing
attention and awareness to stimuli (i.e., perhaps increasing racial consciousness). More
towards her or his racial prejudice, again, due to its definitional nature. Also, there is
prior research that has showed mindfulness can decrease ego defense activation.
Furthermore, mindfulness may reduce racial prejudice indirectly due to the work of
researchers who have found that mindfulness can influence, or is associated with, similar
motivational variables that reduces racial prejudice levels, such as social recategorization
(i.e., racial mindfulness), introducing the construct and mindfulness practices should
substantially reduce racial prejudice levels both directly and indirectly. Although this is
the ultimate goal of this program of research, this dissertation explored the initial and
investigating the effects of general mindfulness on racial prejudice, and then the effects
133
of general mindfulness on accepting racial biases, such as White privilege. These
exploratory steps will not only help create the theoretical framework and support for a
racial mindfulness intervention, but can also guide the framework and intervention if any
findings result.
Therefore, the first exploratory step, Study A, investigated the extent to White
students’ degree of general mindfulness can influence their degree of racial prejudice
towards Blacks directly through its conceptual nature of influencing attention and
awareness to internal and external stimuli. In addition, Study A explored the extent to
White students’ degree of general mindfulness can influence their degree of prejudice
determine the direct and indirect effect of mindfulness on racial prejudice, both of these
explorations included controlling for participants’ prior racial outgroup contact. Using
structural equation modeling to explore these effects, results indicated that general
mindfulness does not appear to reduce racial prejudice levels directly or indirectly.
subconscious racial prejudice levels were unexpected, but yet, perhaps not too surprising.
Assuming that a person’s degree of awareness and attention to general stimuli (i.e.,
general mindfulness) would include racial stimuli (i.e., racial mindfulness) was possibly
too distal – especially for Whites who are generally not aware and attentive to race.
134
students, presents a more proximal research design to explore the direct effects of
Study A were more unexpected, as mindfulness has been found to increase variables that
can reduce racial prejudice levels. In this study, mindfulness did significantly increase
social decategorization and this latter variable did significantly decrease both conscious
(subtle) and subconscious prejudice levels. However, mindfulness did not significantly
increase social recategorization and empathy, and these latter variables did not
instruments used in the study. For example, the social recategorization subscale was
created for this study and although reliability and factor analyses were conducted and
provide some psychometric validity for scores on the measure, this subscale’s construct
validity was not investigated. Therefore, there is a possibility that this subscale was not
measuring its attended construct. In addition, the reason a relation between empathy and
conscious (subtle) prejudice was not observed is perhaps because the prejudice scale used
have found that empathy influences the affective dimension of racial prejudice rather than
distress subscale for the empathy measure in this study – discussed in chapter 5, may
135
prejudice through social recategorization, social decategorization, and empathy.
The next exploratory step, Study B, investigated if mindfulness can attenuate the
negative effects that can arise from cognitive dissonance, and therefore, influence White
students’ degree of acceptance towards racial biases, such as White privilege. Many
findings emerged from content analyzing written reactions to a White privilege article
from participants identified as holding a high and low degree of general mindfulness and
who expressed low agreement initially to the concept of White privilege. Overall, there
of awareness and denial to a profound degree of awareness and acceptance. The results
also appeared to indicate that participants with a high degree of mindfulness exhibited
greater awareness and acceptance to White privilege and less negative reactions resulting
from post-decisional cognitive dissonance. For example, 30% (n=5) of the high
the White privilege article in comparison to a little over 10% (n=2) of the low
appeared to exhibit anger or defensiveness to the article in contrary to 27% (n=4) of the
These overall findings and other results from these two studies underscore the (a)
importance of needing racial interventions and educational programs for White college
students, (b) support the need for a racial mindfulness intervention/program, and (c)
inform curriculum development and activities for such an intervention. In relation to the
136
need for racial interventions/programs, the finding that White participants in Study A, on
disheartening, but, according to research, was not unexpected. Moreover, the finding that
construct of White privilege, and only 7 out of the 30 disagreeing participants appeared to
information, was again, discouraging. These findings may emphasize that racial
The results from Study B support the need for future research in creating and
arrive with a set of beliefs and then receive information that could discredit their belief
system. The responses from Study B may exemplify some of the ways students might
initially react from this intervention. For instance, a strong majority of participants may
still exhibit denial or low agreement to the information being presented. Of these
participants, a quarter may respond in anger or defensiveness, close to half may disagree
by providing counter or reverse discrimination examples, and half of the participants may
students, it could be expected from Study B findings (as well as the literature on
conscious [subtle] prejudice and the limitations on prejudice reduction) that the
137
intervention could push some students away from further exploration of racial bias
reduction or increase their biased levels. As the main results of Study B showed (similar
to Lillis & Hayes, 2007), participants with a higher degree of mindfulness appeared to
exhibit greater acceptance to racial biases and less negative reactions. Therefore, a racial
may be needed for White college students in order to increase acceptance of racial biases,
decrease negative emotions resulting from the intervention, and hopefully improve racial
prejudice levels.
The results from Study B also could inform curriculum development and
and practices of mindfulness should probably be included at the beginning of the program
before any racial content or activities are incorporated in order to decrease, as much as
practices are in place, the subthemes from Study B indicate various themes that should be
incorporated within the racial intervention portion. For example, training efforts that
challenge the notion that differential treatment is unrelated to racial factors is needed.
Results from Study B also seem to indicate that the intervention/program model should
often try to incorporate mindfulness into racial bias activities to continue awareness and
acceptance levels throughout the program. For example, there were many participants
within Theme 2 and 3 of Study B that seemed to waver back and forth between
138
Conclusion
The desired outcome of many racial interventions and programs is for Whites to
awaken to and challenge their own and others’ racial biases. As discussed throughout this
dissertation, an obstacle for many Whites is not only becoming aware and attentive to
race, but also moving past negative emotions, such as shame and guilt, when becoming
conscious of racial biases. The results from Study B appear to indicate that mindfulness
may be a way Whites could experience less negative reactions and become more
accepting when becoming conscious of racial biases, such as White privilege. The next
step in this program of research is to explore a racial mindfulness intervention and its
effects on White college students’ acceptance towards racial biases, racial prejudice
manner with the help of mindfulness, it is the hope of this program of research that the
results will be substantial, especially for people who racially identify as White.
139
APPENDIX A: PREVIOUS RACIAL OUTGROUP CONTACT SCALE
Directions:
For the following statements, please indicate the percentage of people who are (or were) “White.” As
noted on the consent form, your results will be kept confidential.
140
APPENDIX B: FIVE FACET MINDFULNESS QUESTIONNAIRE (FFMQ)
Directions:
For the following statements, please rate each of the following statements using the scale provided. Your
answers should indicate what best describes your own opinion of what is generally true for you.
Very
Never or
Rarely Sometimes Often often or
very rarely
true true true always
true
true
1. When I’m walking, I deliberately notice the
O O O O O
sensations of my body moving.
2. I’m good at finding words to describe my
O O O O O
feelings.
3. I criticize myself for having irrational or
O O O O O
inappropriate emotions.
4. I perceive my feelings and emotions
O O O O O
without having to react to them.
5. When I do things, my mind wanders off
O O O O O
and I’m easily distracted.
6. When I take a shower or bath, I stay alert to
O O O O O
the sensations of water on my body.
7. I can easily put my beliefs, opinions, and
O O O O O
expectations into words.
8. I don’t pay attention to what I’m doing
because I’m daydreaming, worrying, or O O O O O
otherwise distracted.
9. I watch my feelings without getting lost in
O O O O O
them.
10. I tell myself I shouldn’t be feeling the way
O O O O O
I’m feeling.
11. I notice how foods and drinks affect my
O O O O O
thoughts, bodily sensations, and emotions.
12. It’s hard for me to find the words to
O O O O O
describe what I’m thinking.
13. I am easily distracted.
O O O O O
14. I believe some of my thoughts are abnormal
O O O O O
or bad and I shouldn’t think that way.
15. I pay attention to sensations, such as the
O O O O O
wind in my hair or sun on my face.
16. I have trouble thinking of the right words to
O O O O O
express how I feel about things
17. I make judgments about whether my
O O O O O
thoughts are good or bad.
18. I find it difficult to stay focused on what’s
O O O O O
happening in the present.
19. When I have distressing thoughts or
images, I “step back” and am aware of the
O O O O O
thought or image without getting taken over
by it.
141
20. I pay attention to sounds, such as clocks
O O O O O
ticking, birds chirping, or cars passing.
21. In difficult situations, I can pause without
O O O O O
immediately reacting.
22. When I have a sensation in my body, it’s
difficult for me to describe it because I O O O O O
can’t find the right words.
23. It seems I am “running on automatic”
O O O O O
without much awareness of what I’m doing.
24. When I have distressing thoughts or
O O O O O
images, I feel calm soon after.
25. I tell myself that I shouldn’t be thinking the
O O O O O
way I’m thinking.
26. I notice the smells and aromas of things.
O O O O O
27. Even when I’m feeling terribly upset, I can
O O O O O
find a way to put it into words.
28. I rush through activities without being
O O O O O
really attentive to them.
29. When I have distressing thoughts or images
I am able just to notice them without O O O O O
reacting.
30. I think some of my emotions are bad or
O O O O O
inappropriate and I shouldn’t feel them.
31. I notice visual elements in art or nature,
such as colors, shapes, textures, or patterns O O O O O
of light and shadow.
32. My natural tendency is to put my
O O O O O
experiences into words.
33. When I have distressing thoughts or
O O O O O
images, I just notice them and let them go.
34. I do jobs or tasks automatically without
O O O O O
being aware of what I’m doing.
35. When I have distressing thoughts or
images, I judge myself as good or bad, O O O O O
depending what the thought/image is about.
36. I pay attention to how my emotions affect
O O O O O
my thoughts and behavior.
37. I can usually describe how I feel at the
O O O O O
moment in considerable detail.
38. I find myself doing things without paying
O O O O O
attention.
39. I disapprove of myself when I have
O O O O O
irrational ideas.
Note: Items 3, 5, 8, 10, 12, 13, 14, 16, 17, 18, 22, 23, 25, 28, 30, 34, 35, 38, and 39 were reverse scored; Items 1, 6, 11, 15,
20, 26, 31, and 36 are observe subscale items. Items 2, 7, 12, 16, 22, 27, 32, and 37 are describe subscale items; Items 5, 8,
13, 18, 23, 28, 34, and 38 are act with awareness items; Items 3, 10, 14, 17, 25, 30, 35, and 39 are nonjudge subscale items;
Items 4, 9, 19, 21, 24, 29, and 33 are non-react subscale items.
142
APPENDIX C: INTERPERSONAL REACTIVITY INDEX (IRI)
Directions:
For the following statements, please indicate how well the item describes you.
Does not Describes
describe Neutral me very
me well well
1. I often have tender, concerned feelings for
people less fortunate than me. O O O O O
143
16. I believe that there are two sides to every
question and try to look at them both. O O O O O
Note: Items 2, 3, 10, 11, 13, and all personal distress subscale items were reverse scored; Items 2, 5, 8, 11, 16, 19, and 21 are
perspective-taking subscale items; Items 1, 3, 6, 10, 13, 15, and 17 are empathetic concern subscale item; Items 4, 7, 9, 12,
14, 18, and 20 are personal distress subscale items.
144
APPENDIX D: PILOT STUDY FOR THE SOCIAL RE(DE)CATEGORIZATION
SCALE
The purpose of this pilot study was to explore the psychometrics of the Social
Re(De)categorization scale (adapted from Gaertner et al., 1994) that will be used in Study
two factors of group membership: social recategorization and decategorization using nine
items on a 5-point Likert response scale. Four items of this scale have been used in prior
research (Gaernter et al., 1994) and five new items were constructed for this dissertation.
A total 124 graduate students from the University of Texas at Austin completed
the Social Re(De)categorization scale through a SurveyMonkey Web site. More of the
participants identified as female (68%) and one student (1%) identified as transgender.
In terms of race, most participants identified as White (63%), while others identified as
Asian (20%), Black (2%), and Latina/o (15%). Participants’ represented a wide range of
rotation to obtain a simple structure and improve the interpretability of the initial
solution. An oblique rotation was used, as it was expected social recategorization and
A scree test and a parallel analysis suggested two meaningful factors. Regarding
145
only one factor between -1 and +1), if the pattern and structure coefficients were greater
than .40 for that factor. From this criterion, three items were deleted.
The remaining six items were then re-investigated with exploratory factor analysis
for a final time and the same factors remained. Three items loaded on the first factor and
three items loaded on the second factor. All items appeared to load on its projected
factor. As a result, factor one was labeled Social Recategorization and factor two was
labeled Social Decategorization. Based on these six items the initial eigenvalues and
percentage of variance explained were 2.197 and 36.62% for Recategorization and 1.389
and 23.16% for Decategorization. The pattern and structure matrices from the direct
Table 16
Rotated Pattern and Structure Matrices for Responses to the Social Re(De)Categorization Scale
Factor 1 – SR Factor 2 - SD
Social Re(De)categorization Item
Pattern Structure Pattern Structure
SR 1. Despite the different groups around campus, there is .767 777 .038 .237
frequently the sense that we are all just one group.
SR 2. Although there are different groups of students on campus, .654 .631 -.087 .083
it feels as though we are all playing for the same team.
SR 3. I tend to feel that we are all the same even though there are .683 .697 .053 .231
different groups of students on campus.
SD 1. It is easy for me to see students as just people rather than as .159 .268 .417 .458
members of a particular group.
SD 2. I tend to first see another student on campus as a member -.073 .130 .780 .761
of a group rather than as an individual.
SD 3. It is difficult for me to see students on campus as members -.024 .085 .417 .411
of a particular group rather than individuals.
Note: SR indicates Social Recategorization and SD indicates Social Decategorization. Scale items were
renumbered for ease of presentation.
Reliability was assessed via estimates of the internal consistency of scores for
each factor of the Social Re(De)categorization scale. Cronbach’s coefficient alphas for
146
the 6-item scale were .74 and .55, respectively, for the Social Recategorization and Social
subscale failing to reach a desirable alpha of .70, or at least .60, as well as two of the
subscale items having low pattern and structural coefficients, the two items that were
predicted to measure social decategorization but were dropped during the factor analysis
procedure above will be modified and reinvestigated during Study A. Incorporating these
modified items will hopefully improve the factor structure and internal consistency of this
147
APPENDIX E: SOCIAL RE(DE)CATEGORIZATION SCALE
Directions:
The following statements inquire about your perception of the student body here at The University of
Texas at Austin. Below, please indicate the extent you agree with each statement.
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Neutral Agree
Disagree Agree
1. On campus, it usually feels as though
we are individuals and not members of
O O O O O
particular groups.
Note: Items 2, 4, and 7 were reverse scored; Items 2, 3, 5, and 8 are recategorization subscale items; Items 1, 4, 6, 7, and 9
are decategorization subscale items. Items 1, 2, and 7 are the modified items from the pilot study results.
148
APPENDIX F: SYMBOLIC RACISM 2000 SCALE (SR2K)
Directions:
Please indicate your response to the following questions. As noted on the consent form, your results are completely
anonymous.
1. It’s really a matter of some people not trying hard enough; if Blacks would only try harder they could be just as
well off as Whites.
O strongly agree
O somewhat agree
O somewhat disagree
O strongly disagree
2. Irish, Italian, Jewish, and many other minorities overcame prejudice and worked their way up. Blacks should
do the same.
O strongly agree
O somewhat agree
O somewhat disagree
O strongly disagree
3. Some say that Black leaders have been trying to push too fast. Others feel they haven’t pushed fast enough.
What do you think?
O trying to push too fast
O moving at about the right speed
O going too slowly
4. How much of the racial tension that exists in the United States today do you think Blacks are responsible for
creating?
O all of it
O most
O some
O not much at all
5. How much discrimination against Blacks do you feel there is in the United States today, limiting their chances
to get ahead?
O a lot
O some
O just a little
O none at all
6. Generations of slavery and discrimination have created conditions that make it difficult for Blacks to work
their way out of the lower class.
O strongly agree
O somewhat agree
O somewhat disagree
O strongly disagree
7. Over the past few years, Blacks have gotten less than they deserve.
O strongly agree
O somewhat agree
O somewhat disagree
O strongly disagree
8. Over the past few years, Blacks have gotten more economically than they deserve.
O strongly agree
O somewhat agree
O somewhat disagree
O strongly disagree
149
APPENDIX G: WHITE PRIVILEGE SCALE (WPS)
Directions:
Please indicate the extent you agree with each statement below.
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Neutral Agree
Disagree Agree
1. White people have certain advantages
that minorities do not have in this
O O O O O
society.
150
APPENDIX H: WHITE PRIVILEGE ARTICLE
1. I can if I wish arrange to be in the company of people of my race most of the time.
2. I can avoid spending time with people whom I was trained to mistrust and who have learned to
mistrust my kind or me.
3. If I should need to move, I can be pretty sure of renting or purchasing housing in an area, which I
can afford and in which I would want to live.
4. I can be pretty sure that my neighbors in such a location will be neutral or pleasant to me.
5. I can go shopping alone most of the time, pretty well assured that I will not be followed or
harassed.
6. I can turn on the television or open to the front page of the paper and see people of my race widely
represented.
7. When I am told about our national heritage or about "civilization," I am shown that people of my
color made it what it is.
8. I can be sure that my children will be given curricular materials that testify to the existence of their
race.
9. If I want to, I can be pretty sure of finding a publisher for this piece on white privilege.
10. I can be pretty sure of having my voice heard in a group in which I am the only member of my
race.
11. I can be casual about whether or not to listen to another person's voice in a group in which s/he is
the only member of his/her race.
12. I can go into a music shop and count on finding the music of my race represented, into a
supermarket and find the staple foods, which fit with my cultural traditions, into a hairdresser's
shop and find someone who can cut my hair.
13. Whether I use checks, credit cards or cash, I can count on my skin color not to work against the
appearance of financial reliability.
14. I can arrange to protect my children most of the time from people who might not like them.
15. I do not have to educate my children to be aware of systemic racism for their own daily physical
protection.
16. I can be pretty sure that my children's teachers and employers will tolerate them if they fit school
and workplace norms; my chief worries about them do not concern others' attitudes toward their
race.
17. I can talk with my mouth full and not have people put this down to my color.
18. I can swear, or dress in second hand clothes, or not answer letters, without having people attribute
these choices to the bad morals, the poverty or the illiteracy of my race.
19. I can speak in public to a powerful male group without putting my race on trial.
20. I can do well in a challenging situation without being called a credit to my race.
21. I am never asked to speak for all the people of my racial group.
22. I can remain oblivious of the language and customs of persons of color who constitute the world's
majority without feeling in my culture any penalty for such oblivion.
23. I can criticize our government and talk about how much I fear its policies and behavior without
being seen as a cultural outsider.
24. I can be pretty sure that if I ask to talk to the "person in charge", I will be facing a person of my
race.
151
25. If a traffic cop pulls me over or if the IRS audits my tax return, I can be sure I haven't been singled
out because of my race.
26. I can easily buy posters, post-cards, picture books, greeting cards, dolls, toys and children's
magazines featuring people of my race.
27. I can go home from most meetings of organizations I belong to feeling somewhat tied in, rather
than isolated, out-of-place, outnumbered, unheard, held at a distance or feared.
28. I can be pretty sure that an argument with a colleague of another race is more likely to jeopardize
her/his chances for advancement than to jeopardize mine.
29. I can be pretty sure that if I argue for the promotion of a person of another race, or a program
centering on race, this is not likely to cost me heavily within my present setting, even if my
colleagues disagree with me.
30. If I declare there is a racial issue at hand, or there isn't a racial issue at hand, my race will lend me
more credibility for either position than a person of color will have.
31. I can choose to ignore developments in minority writing and minority activist programs, or
disparage them, or learn from them, but in any case, I can find ways to be more or less protected
from negative consequences of any of these choices.
32. My culture gives me little fear about ignoring the perspectives and powers of people of other
races.
33. I am not made acutely aware that my shape, bearing or body odor will be taken as a reflection on
my race.
34. I can worry about racism without being seen as self-interested or self-seeking.
35. I can take a job with an affirmative action employer without having my co-workers on the job
suspect that I got it because of my race.
36. If my day, week or year is going badly, I need not ask of each negative episode or situation
whether it had racial overtones.
37. I can be pretty sure of finding people who would be willing to talk with me and advise me about
my next steps, professionally.
38. I can think over many options, social, political, imaginative or professional, without asking
whether a person of my race would be accepted or allowed to do what I want to do.
39. I can be late to a meeting without having the lateness reflect on my race.
40. I can choose public accommodation without fearing that people of my race cannot get in or will be
mistreated in the places I have chosen.
41. I can be sure that if I need legal or medical help, my race will not work against me.
42. I can arrange my activities so that I will never have to experience feelings of rejection owing to
my race.
43. If I have low credibility as a leader I can be sure that my race is not the problem.
44. I can easily find academic courses and institutions, which give attention only to people of my race.
45. I can expect figurative language and imagery in all of the arts to testify to experiences of my race.
46. I can choose blemish cover or bandages in "flesh" color and have them more or less match my
skin.
47. I can travel alone or with my spouse without expecting embarrassment or hostility in those who
deal with us.
152
REFERENCES
Acuña, R. F. (2007). Occupied America: A history of Chicanos (6th ed.). New York:
Longman.
Website: http://www.aaanet.org/issues/policy-advocacy/AAA-Statement-on-
Race.cfm.
Amodio, D. M., & Devine, P. G. (2005). Changing prejudice: The effects of persuasion
on implicit and explicit forms of race bias. In M. C. Green & T. C. Brock (Eds.),
Ancis, J. R., & Szymanski, D. M. (2001). Awareness of White privilege among White
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its
learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89-195).
153
Bäckström, M., & Björklund, F. (2007). Structural modeling of generalized prejudice:
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-
45.
Balfanz, R., & Legters, N. (2004). Locating the dropout crisis (Technical Report #70).
Baltimore, MD: The John Hopkins University, Center for Research in the
Longman.
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American
Barth, P. (2003). A common core curriculum for the new century. Thinking K-16, 7, 2-31.
154
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more than Lakisha and
Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype
Blank, R. M., Dabady, M., & Citro, C. F. (Eds.). (2004). Measuring Racial
Bonilla-Silva, E. (2001). White supremacy and racism in the post-civil rights era.
Boyd, D., Grossman, P., Lankford, H., Loeb, S., & Wyckoff, J. (2007). Who leaves?
Research.
37, 203-215.
155
Brewer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1998). Intergroup relations. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske,
G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th edition, pp. 554-594),
Brown, K., & Ryan, R. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in
848.
foundations and evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 211-
237.
California Newsreel (2003). Race – The power of an illusion. Episode III: The house we
http://www.newsreel.org/transcripts/race3.htm.
Campbell, D. T. (1971). White attitudes toward Black people. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute
students’ racial views and attitudes. The Journal of General Education, 51, 21-42.
Academic/Plenum Publishers.
156
Sociology, 29. 167-207.
49–59.
Corcoran, M. (1995). Rags to rags: Poverty and mobility in the United States. Annual
Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C., Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police officer’s dilemma:
Studies, 7, 75-91.
Devine, P. G. (2001). Implicit prejudice and stereotyping: How automatic are they?
81, 757-759.
157
Devine, P. G., & Monteith, M. J. (1993). The role of discrepancy associated affect in
Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 339–
Devine, P. G., Plant, E. A., Amodio, D. M., Harmon-Jones, E., & Vance, S. L. (2002).
The regulation of explicit and subconscious race bias: The role of motivations to
835-848.
Devos, T., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). American = White? Journal of Personality and Social
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (1998). On the nature of contemporary prejudice: The
Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism: The problem and the response (pp. 3-32).
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Kawakami, K. (2003). Intergroup contact: The past,
present, and the future. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 6, 5-21.
158
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Reducing contemporary
prejudice: Combating explicit and implicit bias at the individual and intergroup
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A. (1997). On the
control prejudiced reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 316-
326.
The Education Trust. Retrieved August 8th, 2008 from The Education Trust Web
site: http://www2.edtrust.org/EdTrust/Product+Catalog/Fact+Sheets.
Education Trust. Retrieved August 8th, 2008 from The Education Trust Web site:
http://www2.edtrust.org/EdTrust/Product+Catalog/Fact+Sheets.
159
Esses, V. M. & Dovidio, J. F. (2002). The role of emotions in determine willingness to
1202-1214.
Esses, V. M., Jackson, L. M., & Armstrong, T. L. (1998). Intergroup competition and
Esses, V. M., Jackson, L. M., Dovidio, J. F., & Hodson, G. (2005). Instrumental relations
Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in
Fazio, R., & Olson, M. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their
Filardo, M., Vincent, J. M., Sung, P., & Stein, T. (2006). Growth and disparity: A decade
Together – BEST.
Finlay, K. A., & Stephan, W. G. (2000). Improving intergroup relations: The effects of
160
Fiske, S.E. (2005). Social cognition and the normality of prejudgment. In J. F. Dovidio,
P. Glick, & L. A. Rudman (Eds.), On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after
appreciation among Black, White, and Hispanic households. Social Forces, 82,
1523-1551.
Frankenberg, E., Lee, C., & Orfield, G. (2003). A multiracial society with segregated
schools: Are we losing the dream? Cambridge, MA: The Civil Rights Project of
Harvard University.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1977). The subtlety of White racism, arousal, and
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In J. F. Dovidio &
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup
racism: From aversive racism to the Common Ingroup Identity Model. Journal of
161
Gaertner, S. L., Rust, M. C., Dovidio, J. F., Bachman, B. A., & Anastasio, P. A. (1994).
Gawronski, B., Strack, F., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2008). Attitudes and cognitive
H. Fazio, & P, Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures
Goldstein, J. (2002). One dharma: The emerging Western Buddhism. New York:
HarperCollins Publishers.
Gould, S. J. (1996). The mismeasure of man. New York: W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.
Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2001). Health of the Implicit Association Test at age
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the
Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, S., & Walach, H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress
162
Gunaratana, B. H. (1992). Mindfulness in plain English. Somerville, MA: Wisdom
Publications.
Hanson, J., & Hanson, K. (2006). The blame frame: Justifying (racial) injustice in
America. Harvard Civil Rights - Civil Liberties Law Review, 41, 413-480.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment
Press.
individual racism, and Hurricane Katrina. Analyses of Social Issues and Public
Policy, 6, 99-124.
Henry, P. J., & Sears, D. O. (2002). The Symbolic Racism 2000 Scale. Political
Heppner, W. L., Kernis, M. H., Lakey, C. E., Campbell, W. K., Goldman, B. M., Davis,
496.
Hertz, T. (2005). Rags, riches, and race: The intergenerational economic mobility of
Black and White families in the United States. In S. Bowles, H. Gintis, and M. O.
Groves (Eds.), Unequal chances: Family background and economic success (pp.
163
American Progress.
Hodgins, H., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and conscious experience. In E.
L. Deci & R.M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self determination research (pp. 87 –
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity
453.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Isaacs, J. B. (2007). Economic mobility of Black and White families. In J.B. Isaacs, I.
Jost, J.T., & Banaji, M. (1994). The role of stereotyping in system justification and the
164
Kabat-Zinn J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain
Kaplan, J., & Valls, A. (2007). Housing discrimination as a basis for Black reparations.
Katz, I., & Hass, R. G. (1988). Racial ambivalence and American value conflict:
Katznelson, I. (2005). When affirmative action was White: An untold story of racial
Inc.
Kawakami, K., Dion, K. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1998). Racial prejudice and stereotype
Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to
Kearney, M. (2006). Intergenerational mobility for women and minorities in the United
165
Keith, T. Z. (2006). Multiple regression and beyond. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Kinder D. R., & Sanders L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic
Kinder, D., & Sears, D. O. (1981). Prejudice and politics: Symbolic racism versus racial
threats to the good life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 414-
431.
Klarman, M. J. (2004). From Jim Crow to civil rights: The Supreme Court and the
Kozol, J. (1991). Savage inequalities: Children in America’s schools. New York: Crown
Publishers, Inc.
220-223.
Langer, E., Bashner, R., & Chanowitz, B. (1985). Decreasing prejudice by increasing
Langer, E., Hatem, M., Joss, J., & Howell, M. (1989). Conditional teaching and mindful
139-150.
Langer, E., & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of mindfulness. Journal of Social
166
Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1995). Measuring belongingness: The Social
Lillis, J., & Hayes, S. C. (2007). Applying acceptance, mindfulness, and values to the
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not
Modeling, 9, 151-173.
Mace, C. (2008). Mindfulness and mental health: Therapy, theory, and science. New
York: Routledge.
Manglitz, E., Johnson-Bailey, J., & Cervero, R. M. (2005). Struggles of hope: How
White adult educators challenge racism. Teachers College Record, 107, 1245-
1274.
McBrier, D. B., & Wilson, G. (2004). Going down? Race and downward occupational
mobility for White-collar workers in the 1990s. Work and Occupations, 31, 283-
322.
167
McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale.
McConnell, A. R., & Liebold, J. M. (2001). Relations between the Implicit Association
McIntosh, P. (1989). White privilege: Unpacking the invisible knapsack. Peace and
Miller, C. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our
Miller, J. P., & Nozawa, A. (2002). Meditating teachers: A qualitative study. Journal of
Monteith, M. J., Ashburn-Nardo, L., Voils, C. I., & Czopp, A. M. (2002). Putting the
168
Nazroo, J. Y. (2003). The structuring of ethnic inequalities in health: Economic position,
racial discrimination, and racism. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 277-
284.
Nosek, B.A., Banaji, M.R., & Greenwald, A.G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The Implicit Association Test
Automatic processes in social thinking and behavior (pp. 265-292). New York:
Psychology Press.
Pager, D. (2003). The mark of a criminal record. American Journal of Sociology, 108,
937-975.
Pedersen, A., Walker, I., & Wise. M. (2005). “Talk does not cook rice”: Beyond anti-
racism rhetoric to strategies for social action. Australian Psychologist, 40, 20-30.
Peske, H. G., & Haycock, K. (2006). Teaching inequality: How poor and minority
Trust. Retrieved July 16, 2008 from The Education Trust Web site:
http://www2.edtrust.org/NR/rdonlyres/010DBD9F-CED8-4D2B-9E0D-
91B446746ED3/0/TQReportJune2006.pdf.
Pettigrew, T.F., & Meertens, R.W. (1995). Subtle and blatant prejudice in Western
169
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory.
Chicago Press.
Pierce, C., Carew, J., Pierce-Gonzalez, D., & Willis, D. (1978). An experiment in racism:
Rivera, A., Cotto-Escalera, B., Anisha, D., Huezo, J., & Muhammad, D. (2008).
Foreclosed: State of the dream 2008. Boston, MA: United for a Fair Economy.
Rowe, W., Bennett, S. K., & Atkinson, D. R. (1994). White racial identity models: A
Rumelhart, D. E., & Norman, D. A. (1978). Accretion, tuning, and restructuring: Three
Salomon, G., & Globerson, T. (1987). Skill may not be enough: The role of mindfulness
170
Research, Acquisition and Transfer of Knowledge and Cognitive Skills (pp. 623-
Schuman, H., Steeh, C., Bobo, L., & Krysan, M. (1997). Racial attitudes in America:
Shapiro, T. (2005). The hidden cost of being African American: How wealth perpetuates
Shapiro, S. L., Brown, K., Biegel, G. (2007). Self-care for health care professionals:
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C. W. (1988). The
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social
Sidanious, J. H., & Veniegas, R. C. (2000). Gender and race discrimination: The
171
Solórzano, D., Ceja, M., & Yosso, T. (2000). Critical race theory, racial
Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test
Stephan, W. G., & Finlay, K. (1999). The role of empathy in improving intergroup
Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination (pp 47-69). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Sue, D. W., Capodilupo, C. M., Torino, G., Bucceri, J. M., Holder, A. M. B., Nadal, K.
Swim, J. K., & Miller, D. L. (1999). White guilt: Its antecedents and consequences for
25, 500–514.
Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups (pp. 61-98). New York:
Academic Press.
172
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G.
Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp.
Tatum, B. D. (1994). Teaching White students about racism: The search for White allies
Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination (pp 71-99). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau,
Turner, M. A., Ross, S. L., Galster, G. C., & Yinger, J. (2002). Discrimination in
Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory
173
diverse classroom. In A. Howell & F. Tuitt (Eds.), Race and higher education:
Delgado & J. Stefancic (Eds.), Critical White studies: Looking behind the mirror
Wilson, J. (1998). The Earth shall weep: A history of Native America. New York: Grove
Press.
Wilson, W. J. (1996). When work disappears: The world of the new urban poor. Chicago,
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the
Yinger, J. (1993). Access denied, access constrained: Results and implications of the
1989 housing discrimination study. In M. Fix and R. J. Struyk (Eds.), Clear and
Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Corneille, O. (2005). Cognitive process: Reality constraints and
Blackwell.
174
VITA
John Vincent Kucsera, Jr. received a degree of Bachelor of Arts from the
University of Texas at Austin in 2001. Following, he spent some time as a mental health
counselor, crisis specialist, and then found his home as a special education teacher and
diversity trainer. John then returned to the University of Texas at Austin to pursue
research and teaching interests center on teaching effectiveness and educational programs
for social equality. John considers himself racially prejudiced, and tries to mindfully
regulate and counter such attitudes daily. He hopes to continue to influence growth, on
175