Referesi 1
Referesi 1
Referesi 1
Arab World English Journal (AWEJ) Volume 9. Number 1. March 2018 Pp. 406-418
DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24093/awej/vol9no1.28
Muhammad Shahbaz
Department of English, GC Women University Sialkot, Sialkot, Pakistan
Ghulam Mustafa
Deanship of Preparatory Year, Al-Majma’ah University, Zulfi, Saudi Arabia.
Abstract
Speaking in a foreign language is considered to be a challenging aspect of language learning that
demands competence and mastery in learning any foreign language. Vocabulary learning verifies
to play an important role in oral communication. However, literature lacks the studies where both
English as a foreign language (EFL) students and teachers’ views are obtained to provide
analyses of the situations in which learners are not showing desired results in speaking. The
current study explores the problems of lack of vocabulary that Saudi EFL students face with
special focus on their speaking proficiency. It aims to seek EFL teachers’ opinions on the extent
to which lack of vocabulary has influence on EFL students’ performance in listening, and
conversation classes, and in expressing their ideas and feelings, and especially in speaking skill.
A questionnaire was used for the responses from the students and an interview for the teachers to
reveal their perceptions about the vocabulary hindrance in speaking skills. 20 EFL instructors
and teachers of Preparatory Year Program (PYP) section of a public university and 110 EFL
students participated in this study. The analysis of the data showed that both teachers and
learners indicated that lack of vocabulary is one of the major factors in students’ inability to
speaking English. In the current study, among many other suggestions it is proposed that the
inclusion of mobile assisted language learning could be useful way of developing vocabulary for
spoken proficiency of Saudi EFL learners.
Cite as: Khan, R. M.I., Radzuan, N. R. M., Shahbaz,M., Ibrahim, A.H.,& Ghulam Mustafa
(2018). The Role of Vocabulary Knowledge in Speaking Development of Saudi EFL Learners.
Arab World English Journal, 9 (1). DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.24093/awej/vol9no1.28
Introduction
Vocabulary learning is an imperative part of learning foreign language (Schmitt & Carter, 2000).
Many researchers indicate that real communication is a result of suitable and adequate
vocabulary learning as compared with learning grammar rules only (Cook, 2013). However,
most EFL students claim that they understand the new vocabulary items during the lecture but
they tend to forget newly learned words after a short period of time. Perhaps this is because of
the lack of the opportunity to use these words in their conversation. (Coady & Huckin, 1997;
McCarthy & O'dell, 2002) recommend the realization for the training of strategy in EFL classes
to boost the process of vocabulary learning for the development of oral communication in and
out of the language classrooms. August, Carlo, Dressler, and Snow (2005) express that foreign
language learners who have limited vocabulary take more time to learn new vocabulary items
and are less able to involve in comprehending text and lack involvement in oral communication
with their peers. As a result, such learners are more likely to get lower achievement in language
learning assessments and are at the edge of the risk of being indicated as disabled in learning.
More recently, (Chee, Yahaya, Ibrahim, & Hasan, 2017; Ishtiaq, Ali, & Salem, 2017; Shahbaz &
Khan, 2017; Taj, Ali, Sipra, & Ahmad, 2017) has revealed that EFL vocabulary instruction is
attracting the attention of many researchers nowadays.
Along with many other problems, Hamad (2013) agree that vocabulary is a key issue in
speaking performance of Saudi EFL learners. The importance of vocabulary in oral development
for EFL learners is the main focus of the current study at PYP section of a public university of
Saudi Arabia. It pursues to identify the main causes of EFL learners’ incompetence in spoken
aspects of the language learning. The in-depth focus of study reveals that insufficient vocabulary
is a main hindrance and affects greatly in lower performance in speaking skill which is one of the
most important parts of language proficiency and development.
The present study tries to find the answers of the following research questions:
1. What are the teachers’ perceptions about their students’ speaking performance?
2. What are the main causes behind Saudi EFL learners’ lower achievement in speaking
performance?
3. How can Saudi EFL learners develop their spoken performance through vocabulary
strategies?
The present research illustrates an exploratory investigation carried out at a public university
of Saudi Arabia in the year 2016 with similar level of Saudi undergraduate learners, who learn
English language skills for a period of one year before joining their majors and their teachers.
The major focus of the study is to identify and examine the main causes of the poor performance
in speaking skill. In the first part of the article, some findings of the previous related theories and
studies on oral or spoken skill development are presented. Then, vocabulary learning strategy
instruction model will be discussed to improve the current situation of spoken vocabulary
development.
Literature Review
Vocabulary Learning
Learning vocabulary is one of the major most important concerns of the foreign language
407
learning. Researching vocabulary was neglected by the researchers up to 1960s; however,
recently
408
it has gained the attention of a lot of researchers (Muliawati & Ismail, 2017). Vocabulary
learning demands the learners’ competence in both theory and practice. Schmitt (2008) has
explained that vocabulary learning is essential as it is a vital indication of language proficiency.
Similarly, learning any foreign language is fundamentally associated with vocabulary
knowledge, the shortage of vocabulary items obstructs the process of second language learning.
In EFL learning without having adequate vocabulary knowledge, learners may not show the
desired results in language learning process and its competence (Macis & Schmitt, 2017). In the
view of Adam (2016) lack of vocabulary knowledge hinders the real communication of EFL
learners to a great extent. Hence, it is predictable that undergraduate EFL learners should have
the appropriate vocabulary knowledge.
For the betterment of developing vocabulary learning, researchers have been making
enormous efforts to locate the different aspects of learning vocabulary to aid EFL learners.
Nunan (2017), a leading researcher in the field of L2 vocabulary, asserts that learners have to use
certain techniques and strategies for achieving certain proficiency of vocabulary knowledge. In
the field of Second Language Acquisition (SLA) vocabulary development has fascinated the
scholars to play their part where vocabulary growth is comparatively low (Hughes & Reed,
2016). Saudi Arabia is a country where English is taught as a Foreign Language and where
learners’ language competence is not at the desired standard of the language learning, therefore,
vocabulary development requires a particular attention in Saudi Arabian context. Cohen, (2014)
argues that learners, language instructors and teachers, curriculum developers and language
researchers all agree on the view that vocabulary learning is indispensable part of L2 learning.
However, learners and teachers are uncertain about the best practices of learning vocabulary
(Schmitt, 2008).
Speaking Skill
The intervening aspect and the illustration of speaking itself (perpetual and moderate flux
of production) makes the process of learning and teaching a complicated understanding of
speaking skills. In the view of psycholinguist Pawlak (2011), speaking involves the collaboration
of many different processing mechanisms, components and exertion to arrange the words in
motion to speak language fluently. In language pedagogy, the management of the handling
mutuality aspect (the role and relation among the speakers and the listeners in speaking element)
is another key factor that affects the quality of speech production with extra constraints of time
limit while trying to produce words (Hulstijn, 2000). Therefore, learners preferably need to be
involved in the production of lengthy and structured part of language without inclusion of
extended hesitation and excessive pauses. So, it is vital that learners need to get awareness of all
four components of speaking skill of which vocabulary learning has key role in this process
(Bailey, 2006; Cazden, 2001). Likewise, learners have to display a good command on; the
pragmatic awareness of the word (the capability of the usage of language in social background
keeping in view the cultural restraints), the competence of strategy, grammatical aspect and
competence of interconnecting the words to make a conversation meaningful. Learners feel
problems in creating balance in this regard that further creates complexity in lexicon and
cognition (Bulté, Housen, Pierrard, & Van Daele, 2008).
Saudi EFL learners like many other L2 learners face problems of fluency and undue
409
pauses in the course of speaking as stated by the EFL instructors during the interviews. This
assertion is
410
also confirmed by Hamad (2013) who has disclosed that Saudi female EFL learners are not
proficient in oral skill as compared to their performance in other skills of the language learning.
The purpose of his study has been to determine the hurdles of EFL university learners’ reluctance
in classroom participations and poor performance in speaking skill. Results of survey study
highlight that learners come across with difficulties to speak even in formal class and they regard
it a challenging skills.
Anthony et al. (2009) also carried out a study with a purpose to identify the prospective
involvements of early childhood learners’ vocabulary and its progression about creating
phonological understandings of Spanish 92 pre-school learners. The results of vocabulary test
and oral awareness test indicate a moderate correlation between vocabulary and oral skills
developments (rs = .42–.75, ps < .001). Similarly, Lee (2009) has carried out a research study to
highlight the influences that affect the spoken contribution of 6 Korean EFL learners enrolled at
graduate institution in America with their implication on developing speaking skills in classroom
participation over a period of four months. Data analysis from classroom observations and
informal interviews indicate that English language competency is influenced by vocabulary
insufficiency along with other factors of language learning. Saudi EFL learners also face such
problems in developing their spoken proficiency. Some central grounds have been cited in table
(3) in the section of methodology. Based on the exploration conducted to locate the main reasons
in developing speaking skill participants exhibit many factors however, both instructors and
learners indicated that inadequate vocabulary is one of the most repeated hurdles hindering to
learners’ spoken fluency. Consequently, it is required to establish an appeal to highlight
vocabulary learning strategies that may play a great role here. The following segment will
rationalize the importance of this issue.
Vocabulary Learning Strategies (VLS)
VLS is a loom which expedites and facilitates the vocabulary development and it has
fascinated a substantial attention of researchers. VLS comprises a subfield of language learning,
which as a result are underpinned in learning strategies (Nation, 2001; Takač, 2008). A learning
strategy is considered as the progression of activities a learner adapts to ease in accomplishing
the given task in learning. In the same way language learning strategies develop the self-directed
learning for EFL learners. In the light of the interpretation of Brewer (2016), self-directed
learners are self-regulating learners who have the ability to accept the responsibility of learning
outcomes on their selves and, hereafter develop the participation, motivation, proficiency and
above all the self-reliance. So, VLSs create the atmosphere of self-learning and practicing the
target words on their own, which in turn learners appear as “better learners”. Keeping this into
attention, foreign and second language learners have made numerous efforts to identify the
different kinds of VLS and most of them agree there is no best strategy that can be viewed as
model. Consequently, enormous vocabulary learning classifications and taxonomies have been
appeared comprising; (Ahmed, 1989; Catalan, 2003; Cohen, 2007; Gu, 2002; Gu & Johnson,
1996; Lawson & Hogben, 1996; Oxford, 1990; Schmitt, 1997) and more significantly (Nation,
2001). Even though many of the above mentioned taxonomies exhibit the somehow same
classifications and categories (Nassaji, 2003), the scheme of Oxford (1990) is mostly adopted by
many vocabulary learning/teaching researchers. Oxford and Burry-Stock (1995) have checked
the validity and reliability in many different ways. In addition, they have also tested that these six
categories gauge
411
the identical paradigm and strategies by factor analysis from many different backgrounds
including; USA, Canada, Taiwan, Egypt and many others.
Methodology
Study Design
Design is required to facilitate the persuasive completion of many research processes,
thereby presenting research as proficient to its potential by the means of producing utmost results
with minimum effort (Kothari, 2004). For this study, mixed method approach is applied. It
includes both qualitative and quantitative data collection and interpretation for the present study.
The selection of mixed method setting is governed by the characteristic of the study itself which
is directly relying on the data gathered from the instructors and learners’ interviews and
questionnaires to pinpoint the major problems or obstructions to speak fluently in and out of the
formal classroom setting. Likert-scale, structured questionnaires for both instructors and learners
and interviews were administered to lead study to achieve the goals of current study. Hence,
several hypotheses are progressed to predict the reasons of learners’ fluency in spoken skills. In
addition to data gathered from interviews, researcher has included his own experience of
412
teaching language skills to Saudi EFL learners in the due course of time.
Participants of the study
413
A total number of 100 EFL elementary level students of a public university of Saudi
Arabia from PYP section were selected to investigate and locate the problems in speaking skills.
All the participants were chosen from the same campus of the university having similar level of
English which is determined by the placement of university at the time of enrollment. All of
them were enrolled in second semester of the academic year 2016. In addition, 20 EFL
instructors from same campus were selected who have presented a comprehensive view of their
EFL teaching experience about the learners’ state of vocabulary. Moreover, 10 EFL instructors
were chosen randomly for interview to get more perceptive and insightful data concerning the
vocabulary learning and teaching at undergraduate level in Saudi Arabia.
Instruments
Three instruments were used to collect the data for the current study. A questionnaire was
adopted from previous studies for learners’ perceptions to indicate speaking hindrances in
learning English, and a questionnaire for teachers to locate the problems in spoken skills. Finally,
teachers’ interview questionnaire containing “Yes”/ “No” and open ended questions on the
problems of speaking insufficiency was also administered
Data Analysis
For statistical measures, SPSS 21 was used to analyze the data gathered from
questionnaires and interviews. As chi-square is a most frequently used statistical test to associate
the expected and observed data, chi-square tests have been employed here to check the manifold
hypotheses suggested by the questionnaire to the null hypothesis ( as of having no significant
differences). The chi-square will also present the likelihood ratio, the extent of freedom and the p
(probability) in addition to Chi-squared (q) values of the data. Moreover, cross-tabulation has
been complied and comprises of the frequencies ratios registered, to sort out the p and q values.
Following section will present the description of collected data.
Results
Tables below have been categorized to analyze the data gathered from EFL learners.
Table 1 and 2 chi-squared findings have been presented for the reason of their “high
significance” in this model. Conveying meaning and vocabulary insufficiency aspect of spoken
skills are highlighted. Additionally, crosstab of speaking hindrances were analyzed in table 3
which includes different hitches in speaking skills.
Table 1. Leaners’ Problem of conveying meaning.
a. 6 cells (27%) have expected countless than 4.5. The minimum expected count is 1,62.
414
Table 2. Learners’ vocabulary insufficiency.
a. 9 cells (39%) have expected countless that 4.5. Minimum expected count is 1.33
*p<.05(in all case); *q= 789; 587; 794; 783; 778 respectively.
From the table 3 an obvious high significant p value is identified. However, in all
recognized values the focus of attention and consideration should be on difficulty in finding
suitable words where p value is .000 and q value is 794, and (don’t know how to express words)
where p value is. 000 and q value is 783. It is vital form these corresponding values have
significant relationship as both of these entities are measuring and surrounding around the almost
same problem. Likewise, the ultimate corresponding q value has been recognized to have a direct
relation and link to vocabulary where q value is 794. The following section of the result will
explain the instructors’ perception to check whether above mentioned entities are persistent with
the responses of learners.
Teachers’ Perception
To know the current situation of learners’ speaking inability, the responses and
experiences of teachers are analyzed first with help of following chart. Table below summarizes
the responses of EFL teachers to the interview questions. It is obvious from the analysis that
415
most of teachers indicated that their EFL learners do not speak English fluently as 53% EFL
instructors feel that their learner are not proficient in speaking English, which is considerably
high proposition as
416
indicated in the table. This explicatory valuation of EFL instructors’ relating to speaking fluency
shows that more than half of EFL learners don’t speak as it is expected and their level demands.
Moreover, a good percentage of teachers have also stated that very are few (31%) which suggests
that 84 % of instructors agree that on the general inclination of not being capable of the
production of fluent conversation.
Table 4. Interview Chart on instructors’ perceptions of EFL Learners’ spoken proficiency.
Responses of teachers also highlighted the fact that vocabulary knowledge, fear of
classmates laughter, speech anxiety, role of environmental interference (mother tongue mainly)
and meaning conveying are main hurdles in speaking English fluently. Vocabulary is a key
element that hinders the proficiency of EFL learners, as reported by the instructors.
a.19 cells (91%) have expected countless than 5. The minimum expected count is.05.
a.12 cells (79%) have expected countless than 5. The minimum expected count is.03.
417
Table 7. Crosstab: difficulties in speaking identified by EFL instructors.
Vocabulary development is key indicator of the progression of all four skills of language
learning. The role of vocabulary as in current study develops a strong correlation between
speaking proficiency and vocabulary knowledge. It is viable from the table that EFL instructors
also highlighted the vocabulary problem and it is clear from their assertions mentioned in tables.
So, their responses develop an understanding of the persistent identification of vocabulary as a
great obstruction to EFL learners’ fluency in speaking skill in Saudi undergraduate classes. Both
learners and instructors acknowledge vocabulary insufficiency as main hurdle in speaking
inability. In both cases high p value is identified with correspondence value of q with vocabulary
problems in speaking inability.
Discussion
Vocabulary knowledge is reelected as an essential consideration in teaching and learning
any foreign language. The importance of vocabulary in learning foreign language has been
identified by many EFL teachers and researchers. The fundamental purpose of a majority of EFL
learners is to develop proficiency in communication in learning foreign language. Grasping
vocabulary knowledge is not merely essential but it’s a central area in learning and developing
foreign language. Nation (1990) explains the importance of vocabulary language as “Vocabulary
is not an end itself. A rich vocabulary makes the skills of listening, speaking, reading and writing
easier to perform.” So, by mastering vocabulary learning one can concentrate fully on other
advanced levels and features of developing foreign language learning more efficiently. The
present study intended to examine the EFL instructors and learners perception about their
speaking proficiency. In connection with the result of the data gathered, it was identified that
most of EFL learners indicated speech anxiety, in particular, vocabulary insufficiency in
speaking English language.
In the line of above mentioned data analysis and discussion, the role of vocabulary in
speaking particularly attains the considerable attention of EFL learners and instructors. It is
viable from the results that deficiency of vocabulary knowledge and the problems in enunciating
or communicating what EFL learners already have learnt or in their mind are the main indicated
reason for speaking inability. The results establish a high significant value of p where P is less
than
418
.05 and q (794; 783) as in the view of EFL learners. Correspondingly, most of EFL instructors
also classified vocabulary insufficiency as a fundamental issue in speaking English as of their
current level. The statistical interpretation of data displays a high significant value of p(p<.05)
from their
419
responses. Likewise the state of having null hypotheses with significant differences in
comparison presents that besides vocabulary learning, speech anxiety is another factor which
creates delaying effect in speaking in the view of many EFL learners.
Moreover, the analyses of instructors’ interview data also indicate that a large number of
Saudi EFL learners are unable to speak English proficiently with their classmates as well as in
classroom interaction. However, some instructors also admitted that some EFL learners take part
in speaking activities but this proportion is marginal as compared to others who are influent in
speaking English language. The main causes of speaking hindrance as of EFL instructors have
categorized are vocabulary deficiency, speech anxiety; meaning conveying problem and
environmental interference. These listed problems are interconnected with the findings of
leaners’ identification of locating their speaking problems.
The findings of present study are with line of Anthony et al. (2009) who have examined
the role of vocabulary in early childhood oral skills with significant correlation between oral
skills and vocabulary knowledge. Moreover, current findings also match the result of the study
Hamad (2013) on female Saudi EFL learners by indicating lack of vocabulary knowledge
resulting in poor performance of speaking. These outcomes originate only to verify and
authenticate the crucial demand and need on concentrating on vocabulary learning strategy
instruction pedagogy and framework and importance of vocabulary instruction to improve
current state of Saudi EFL learners’ oral skills development.
The present study has some constraints related to number of participants participated to
make the understanding of vocabulary knowledge for spoken proficiency. Extensive
420
investigations
421
with large scale of population from different campuses may indicate better outcomes and
implications of this important aspect language learning and teaching. Similarly, more
experimental studies are required to support, develop and affirm the vocabulary knowledge
development with spoken proficiency of EFL learners.
About Authors:
Raja Muhammad Ishtiaq Khan is an English language Lecturer at Al-Majma’ah University,
Saudi Arabia. He has a Cambridge CELTA certificate in teaching and has 10 years of experience
in ESL and EFL. He is a PhD scholar and his research interests include Applied Linguistics,
MALL, L2 Vocabulary learning and Teaching, Second Language Acquisition and EFL teaching.
Noor Raha Mohd Radzuan is a senior Lecturer at Univeristi Malaysia Pahang, Malaysia. She
earned her PhD in Applied Linguistics from Universiti Sains Malysia, Malysia. She has more
than 12 years working experience. Her research interest includes workplace communication,
second language learning strategies, L2 vocabulary learning and students’ motivation.
Ainol Haryati is an Associate Professor at the Center for Modern Languages and Human
Sciences, Universiti Malaysia Pahang. She holds PhD in English Language Studies (ELS) from
International Islamic University Malaysia. Her research interests include technology-enhanced
language learning, ESP, teaching and learning of English, and speaking
References:
Adam, M. A. A. (2016). Role of Vocabulary Learning Strategies in Promoting EFL Learners Performance. Sudan
University of Science and Technology.
Anthony, J. L., Solari, E. J., Williams, J. M., Schoger, K. D., Zhang, Z., Branum-Martin, L., & Francis, D. J. (2009).
Development of bilingual phonological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: The
422
roles of vocabulary, letter knowledge, and prior phonological awareness. Scientific Studies of Reading,
13(6), 535-564.
423
August, D., Carlo, M., Dressler, C., & Snow, C. (2005). The critical role of vocabulary development for English language
learners. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 20(1), 50-57.
Bailey, K. M. (2006). Issues in teaching speaking skills to adult ESOL learners. Review of adult learning and
literacy, 6, 113-164.
Brewer, S. S. (2016). Developing Self-Directed Learning in the Language Classroom: The Work of Leni Dam.
Bulté, B., Housen, A., Pierrard, M., & Van Daele, S. (2008). Investigating lexical proficiency development over time–
the case of Dutch-speaking learners of French in Brussels. Journal of French language studies, 18(3), 277-
298.
Catalan, R. M. J. (2003). Sex differences in L2 vocabulary learning strategies. International Journal of Applied
Linguistics, 13(1), 54-77.
Cazden, C. B. (2001). The language of teaching and learning. The language of teaching and learning, 348-369.
Chamot, A. U. (2004). Issues in language learning strategy research and teaching. Electronic journal of foreign
language teaching, 1(1), 14-26.
Chee, K. N., Yahaya, N., Ibrahim, N. H., & Hasan, M. N. (2017). Review of mobile learning trends 2010-2015: a
meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 20(2), 113-126.
Coady, J., & Huckin, T. (1997). Second language vocabulary acquisition: A rationale for pedagogy: Cambridge University
Press.
Cohen, A. D. M. (2007). Language learner strategies: Thirty years of research and practice.
Cook, V. (2013). Second language learning and language teaching: Routledge.
Gu, Y. (2002). Gender, academic major, and vocabulary learning strategies of Chinese EFL learners. RELC journal,
33(1), 35-54.
Gu, Y., & Johnson, R. K. (1996). Vocabulary learning strategies and language learning outcomes. Language
learning, 46(4), 643-679.
Hamad, M. M. (2013). Factors negatively affect speaking skills at Saudi colleges for girls in the south. English
Language Teaching, 6(12), 87-97.
Hughes, R., & Reed, B. S. (2016). Teaching and researching speaking: Taylor & Francis.
Hulstijn, J. (2000). Dialogue models for inquiry and transaction: University of Twente.
Ishtiaq, M. I., Ali, Z., & Salem, M. S. (2017). An Experimental Study of the Effect of Student Teams Achievement
Divisions (STAD) on Vocabulary Learning of EFL Adult Learners.
Kothari, C. R. (2004). Research methodology: Methods and techniques: New Age International.
Lawson, M. J., & Hogben, D. (1996). The vocabulary‐learning strategies of foreign‐language students. Language
learning, 46(1), 101-135.
Lee, G. (2009). Speaking up: Six Korean students’ oral participation in class discussions in US graduate seminars.
424
Macaro, E. (2006). Strategies for language learning and for language use: Revising the theoretical framework. The
Modern Language Journal, 90(3), 320-337.
Macis, M., & Schmitt, N. (2017). Not just ‘small potatoes’: Knowledge of the idiomatic meanings of collocations.
McCarthy, M., & O'dell, F. (2002). English vocabulary in use: Advanced: Ernst Klett Sprachen.
Muliawati, I., & Ismail, N. M. (2017). Intricacies in Vocabulary Intake For EFL Adult Learners. Paper presented at the
CONFERENCE PROCEEDING.
Nassaji, H. (2003). L2 vocabulary learning from context: Strategies, knowledge sources, and their relationship with
success in L2 lexical inferencing. Tesol Quarterly, 37(4), 645-670.
Nation, P. (2001). Learning vocabulary in another language. UK: Ernst Klett Sprachen.
Nunan, D. (1995). Closing the gap between learning and instruction. Tesol Quarterly, 29(1), 133-158.
Nunan, D. (2017). Does learner strategy training make a difference? Lenguas Modernas(24), 123-142.
Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language learning strategies: na.
Oxford, R. L. (1996). Language learning strategies around the world: Cross-cultural perspectives: Natl Foreign Lg
Resource Ctr.
Oxford, R. L., & Burry-Stock, J. A. (1995). Assessing the use of language learning strategies worldwide with the
ESL/EFL version of the Strategy Inventory for Language Learning (SILL). System, 23(1), 1-23.
425
Pawlak, M. (2011). Instructed acquisition of speaking: Reconciling theory and practice. Speaking and instructed foreign
language acquisition, 3-23.
Schmitt, N. (1997). Vocabulary learning strategies. Vocabulary: Description, acquisition and pedagogy, 199227. Schmitt, N. (2008).
Instructed second language vocabulary learning. Language Teaching Research, 12(3), 329-363. Schmitt, N., & Carter, R. (2000). The
lexical advantages of narrow reading for second language learners. TESOL
Shahbaz, M., & Khan, R. M. I. (2017). Use of Mobile Immersion in Foreign Language Teaching to Enhance Target Language Vocabulary
Learning. MIER Journal of Educational Studies, Trends and Practices, 7(1).
Taj, I. H., Ali, F., Sipra, M. A., & Ahmad, W. (2017). Effect of technology enhanced language learning on vocabulary
acquisition of EFL learners. International Journal of Applied Linguistics and English Literature, 6(3), 262-272.
Takač, V. P. (2008). Vocabulary learning strategies and foreign language acquisition (Vol. 27): Multilingual Matters.
Tseng, W.-T., Dörnyei, Z., & Schmitt, N. (2006). A new approach to assessing strategic learning: The case of self- regulation in
vocabulary acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 27(1), 78-102.
426
FILE 2
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/327790679
CITATIONS
READS
8
15,214
1 author:
Ikhfi Imaniah
32 PUBLICATIONS 95 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Ikhfi Imaniah on 21 September 2018.
427
Globish (An English-Indonesian journal for English, Education and Culture
Vol. 6, No.1, January 2018.
P-ISSN: 2301-9913, E-ISSN: 2597-9132,
Ikhfi Imaniah
English Education Study ProgramTeachers Training and Education FacultyMuhammadiyah University of
TangerangJl.PerintisKemerdekaan I/ 33 Cikokol, Tangerang-IndonesiaTelp: 081311485305
E-mail: [email protected]
Abstract
The aim of the research is to observe the activities of the students in academic speaking classroom, to
analyze the student’s problem in presenting the academic speaking presentation and to identify the
student’s special needs in academic speaking presentation.The method used in this research is field
research that is qualitative, while the analysis of the data used descriptive analysis. The focus of the
research is on the student’s difficulties in presenting academic speaking presentation.In sum up, most of
the students of English Education Study Program of Teachers Training and Education Faculty of
Muhammadiyah University are lack of: 1) Goal setting on their academic speaking presentation, 2)
Prioritization on what the essential point need to be presented, 3) Self-awareness on themselves, while
they present the presentation in front of the audience, 4) Self-motivation while presenting the materials, it
connects with self-awareness; if students have improved their self-awareness so they will have a great deal
of the information that they need to motivate themselves. In short, the students need to be able to
motivate themselves to take action; 5) Planning, most of students are not planning the materials well so
they are not able to manage their time to present the academic presentation; and 6) Communication skill,
strong communication skills will enable the students to build supportive relationships with the audience
while presenting the materials, in fact not all the students have good communication skill.In short, the
student’s preparation well made their presentation is good and it also established good communication
between the presenter and the audiences.
INTRODUCTION
Speaking is a constructing meaning interactive process which is comprised producing and
receiving information (Brown, 1994; Burns & Joyce, 1997). Speaking is viewed to be at
the heart of second language learning among the four language skills (Egan, 1999). A
common problem confronted among English language learners in Indonesian context is
that they can understand what they read and write despite the probability of making
mistakes in writing and understanding the text incorrectly; yet, they are not being capable
of transferring their feelings, and ideas through oral language.
Academic speaking is similar in many ways to academic writing. It is linier, explicit, it
has one central point and it is presented in standard language. Academic spoken style is
also similar in many ways in that it is formal, explicit, hedged, and responsible. However,
it is less complex and objective than written language. In general, this means that when
someone is doing academic presentation he should avoid colloquial words and
expressions. So, it is the responsibility of the speaker in English to make it clear to the
listener how various parts of the talk is related. 428
Speaking in academic context is becoming increasingly important as teaching methods
change to involve more group work, joint project or group mark. Students in higher
education are not seen to be pulling their weight in collaborative work. It is therefore
important to try to be more of what is involved in seminar or group activity and to learn
44
429
Globish ISSN: 2301-9913
some of the interactional language that is used there. It is important to practice making
presentation, taking part in discussion on academic topics and so on.
When speaking in an academic context, facts and figure are given pricely. Thus, in any
kind of academic speaking, it is necessary to make decisions about the stance on a
particular subject, or the strength of the claims. Therefore, in academic speaking the
speaker are responsible for demonstrating an understanding of the source text by
providing the evidence and justification.
Due to those reasons, the students need to learn about the purpose of presenting the
academic speaking, making a power point presentation, controlling the discussion,
participating in the discussion, listening and note taking. In academic speaking
presentation, the same as with writing, the students must plan the talk. If the students are
going to get as many marks for speaking as writing, spend as much time on it as writing.
Writing language is different from spoken language, if the students just read the essays,
no one will understand them.
Wallace (1980) stated that, it can be very boring to listen to something read aloud.
Therefore, what students must do is follow the following point, 1) decide on a time limit for
your talk,2) write out your spoken presentation, 3) speak from the outline notes, 4)look at
your audience when you are speaking. 5) make a strong ending. In short, it is very important
when students doing presentation, they must tell to the audience what it is, and stick to the
time limit. Moreover, the students must do some of the works of writing the papers again, in
a sense. This means, the students concentrate only on the main points and try to make the
presentation lively and interesting. The students also need to write everything that they
have to say and when they know exactly what they are going to say, they must reduce it to
outline notes.
In fact, most of students in Muhammadiyah University of Tangerang of fourth semester
get lost when they are presenting the topics. Most of them are doing reading rather than
presenting something to the audience. Therefore, the students must remember that
listening is different with reading. Something that is going to be listened must be
prepared in a very different way from something that is intended to be read.
Therefore, the researcher is eager to conduct the research to observe the activities in the
academic speaking classroom, to analyze the student’sproblems in presenting the
academic speaking presentation, to identify the student’s special needs in academic
speaking presentation.
431
P-ISSN: 2301-9913 | E-ISSN: 2597-9132
difficulties in terms of the learner and his tendency for learning, education system and the
difficulty of the subject. Some difficulties refer to curricula and text books, or to the
psychological, social and emotional sides. Psychological disorder and lagging behind and
failure in education cause learning difficulty. So, it can be concluded that anxiety and
disquiet in the class, fear and alarm reflect directly on individual learning ability and
study progress of the students.
Learning Difficulties of University Students
Westberry (1994:45) clarifies that although technology provides easy access to
knowledge, it has some demerits. Today, culture imposed on individual specific lifestyle.
Such culture can be accepted in food and clothes field, but it is not acceptable in
education, training and skill acquisition. Consequently, the ready-made culture caused the
following issue: 1) weakness in the level of students’ preparation and qualification;
2) decreasing student acquisition in different kinds of basic education skills, thinking and
problem solving; 3) miss of credibility of scientific degrees obtained by all kinds of
students. And such degrees do not reflect true qualification; 4) spread of learning
difficulties in all different kinds of educational levels from preparatory school up to
university level, even in higher studies level; 5) providing adequate prequalification and
training.Due to some issues above, it assumes that the students should be aware on the
ready - made culture because it can be problems in their academic life particularly to their
education.
433
Globish ISSN: 2301-9913
Academic oral presentations involve complex and constant decision-makings for the
students from the beginning – the preparation stage, to the final stage – the presenting
stage. The presenting stage is likely the most anxiety- provoking stage because much of
the decision-making is required immediately. Moreover, it was found that a discrepancy
existed between the instructor and the students about what constitutes an academic oral
presentation and its goal. This may also contribute to students’ anxiety about oral
presentations because students were likely uncertain about the quality of their preparation
and performance.
Oral presentation is a common task in graduate seminars in which presenters lead seminar
discussion. An oral presentation may seem to be a straightforward activity, involving
understanding the assigned material, summarizing it and presenting it to the instructor
and classmates. However, it has been shown that oral presentation requires constant
negotiation and decision making for it to be successful (Wu, 2008).
In short, academic oral presentation is an activity through which the presenters
communicate with the audience. It is called academic because these presentations deal
with college or university life. They also deal with courses that are taught in the
universities sections and academics.
RESEARCH METHOD
Research method that is used in this research is qualitative. As Fraenkel&Wallen (2007)
state that qualitative researchers go directly to the particular setting of interest to observe
and collect their data. Here, the researcher as lecturer who observes the students in
academic speaking class. So, this research has been conducted at Muhammadiyah
University of Tangerang which is located at Jl. PerintisKemerdekaan I/33 Cikokol Kota
Tangerang. The researcher selected this location because the students at Muhammadiyah
University of Tangerang still lack awareness of speaking English, particularly in
presenting academic speaking. In this research the data was focused on the student’s
difficulties in presenting academic speaking presentation.
434
Therefore, this method will be applied in investigating the analysis of student’s
47
435
P-ISSN: 2301-9913 | E-ISSN: 2597-9132
This study has reported on well-designed oral presentations which enable students to
function successfully in the future professional surrounding. It presents a challenge to
both students and the teacher.
The challenge for the teacher is to provide a relevant framework for students upon which
they construct knowledge and become active participants in the learning process. The
teacher involves preparing detailed guidelines, organizing groups, helping students to
select topics, guiding their research and helping them learn the use of various visual aids,
providing feedback on the sequencing of ideas, and evaluating their performance (King,
2002). Importantly, the teacher is no longer perceived as the knowledge dispenser and
decision maker. Instead, the teacher has become the facilitator of learning whose main
task is to set goals and organize the learning process accordingly. The teacher creates
social and intellectual climates, where collaborative and cooperative learning methods are
supported. The teacher creates opportunities for students to interact with other learners
and with the teacher as well. On the whole, the teacher is responsible for leading and
coordinating the work and make learning progress easier.
Besides, the teacher is the supporter, one who supports the learner by means of
suggestions that arise out of ordinary activities, by challenges that inspire creativity, and
with projects that allow for independent thinking and new ways of learning information.
In Vygotsky’s (1978) theory of constructivism, the teacher serves as the mediator who
coaches and encourages students to formulate their own level of understanding.
In the Bruner’s (1979) classroom, the teacher is the instructor, (direct and guide the
learning process) who should try and encourage students to discover principles by
themselves. The teacher and students should engage in an active dialog (Socratic
learning).
Moreover, there is a demand for the teacher in the classroom not to be only a guide,
organizer, instructor, mediator and supporter, but, as King (2002) comments, to hold
questions and answers sessions, provide feedback, and evaluation of students’
performance.
We have to agree with Crystal (2003) who claims that the actor and the teacher inhibit the
same stable. They both have to put on a show (the actor on a stage and the teacher in
the classroom).
On the other hand, students function as designers using the technology as tools for
analyzing the world, accessing information, interpreting and organizing their personal
knowledge, and representing what they know to others (Jonassen, 1994). Students must
be given opportunities to be active in ways that will promote self-direction, creativity and
critical analysis of problems requiring a solution (Jonassen, 1994). “Learning becomes a
continuous, life-long process which results from acting in situations” (Brown et al.,
1989).
It has been observed by Kim et al. (1999) that students have more positive attitude
towards learning as they share their experiences with their peers and the teacher, as well
as they experience increasing discussions in the classroom. Brown (1996) has proved that
oral presentations help students to work collaboratively and make it easier to focus on a
specific area useful for future work. Students are encouraged to search for solutions to
real-world problems, and thus, they are engaged in transformative learning, leading to
critical and analytical thinking which is essential for success in the 21st century.
CONCLUSION
Students need a lot of opportunity to develop and practice communication skills.
Communication skills are required by students (future specialists) whether they are 437
49
P-ISSN: 2301-9913 | E-ISSN: 2597-9132
REFERENCES
Alderson, C. J., Wall, D. & C. Claphaim. (1996). Language Test Construction and Evaluation.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bruner, Jerome. Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 1986.
Brown, D. L. (1996). Kids, computers and constructivism. Journal of Instructional
Psychology, 23 (3), 189-196.
Brown, H. D. (1994). Teaching by principles: An interactive approach to language pedagogy.
Englewood Cliffs,NJ: Prentice Hall Regents.
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., &Duguid, P. (1989). “Situated cognition and the culture of
learning”. Educational Researcher, 18 (1), 32-41. Bippus, A. M. & Daly, J. A.
(1999). What do people think causes stage fright? Naïve attributions about the
reasons for public speaking. Communication Education, 48, 63-72.
Bruskin Associates. What are Americans afraid of? The Bruskin Report, 1973, 53.
Burns, A., & Joyce, H. (1997). Focus on speaking. Sydney: National Center for English
Language Teaching and Research.
Celce-Murcia, M., Brinton, D. & J. Goodwin. (1996). Teaching Pronunciation: a Reference for
Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Carroll, C. (2005). "Assessing Project-Based Learning: A case study of an undergraduate
selling and sales management module at the University of Limerick" in
Barrett, T., Mac Labhrainn, I., Fallon, H. (Eds). Handbook of Enquiry and
Problem-based Learning
Clement, J. (1993) Using Bridges Analogies and Anchoring Institutions to Deal with Students
Preconceptions in Physics. Journal of research in science teaching, vol 30.
Crystal, D. (2003). English as a global language (2nd ed). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Daly, J. A., &McCroskey, J. C. (Eds). (1984). Avoiding communication: Shyness, reticence, and
communication apprehension. Newbury Park: Sage.
Dauwalder, D. P. (2000). Formulating sound conclusions and recommendations.The 438
Delta Pi Epsilon Journal 42, 6-13.
Globish, Vol. 6, No. 1,January2018, 1-8.
439
Globish ISSN: 2301-9913
Egan, K. (1999). Speaking. A critical skill and a challenge. Calico Journal. 16 No: 3,
277- 293.
Gallo, C (2006). The 10 worst presentation habits.BusinessWeek.com. Retrieved October
20, 2006 from:
http://images.businessweek.com/ss/06/02/mistakes/index_01.htm
Hoff, E. & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language.Journal
of communication Disorders, 38, 271-278.
Jonassen D. H., (1994). Thinking technology: Towards a Constructivist Design Model,
Educational Technology, April, 3437
Kim, H. B., Fisher, L. D., & Fraser, J. B. (1999). Assessment and investigation of
constructivist science learning environment in Korea.
King, J. (2002). Preparing EFL learners for oral presentations preparing. Journal of
Humanistic Studies, 4, 401-418.
Krannich, C. R. (2004). 101 Secrets of highly effective speakers: controlling fear, commanding
attention [Recorded by B. McDonald]. [CD]. New York: Listen &Live Audio, Inc.
Krizan, A. C., Merrier, P., & Jones, C. L. (2005). Business Communication, (6th ed.).
United States: South-Western.
McCannon, M. & Crews, T. B. (1999). Most common grammatical and punctuation
errors made by undergraduates as perceived by business communication
professors.The Delta Pi Epsilon Journal 41, 179-186.
McCroskey, J. C. (1977). Oral communication apprehension: A summary of recent theory and
research. Human Communication Research, 4, 78-96.
McCroskey, J. C. Oral communication apprehension: Reconceptualization and a new look at
measurement. Paper presented at the Central Sate Speech Association, Chicago,
1981. Portions also presented at the SCA, Louisville, 1982.
Parvis, L. F. (2001). The importance of communication and public-speaking skills.
Journal of Environmental Health, 63, (pp. 44, 35).7
Phillips, G. M. (1968). Reticence: Pathology of the normal speaker. Speech Monographs, 35,
39-49.
Reece, P. (1999). The number one fear: Public speaking and the university student. In K.
Martin, N. Stanley and N. Davison (Eds), Teaching in the Disciplines/
Learningin Context, 341-347. Proceedings of the 8th Annual Teaching
Learning Forum, The University of Western Australia, February 1999. Perth:
UWA. http://lsn.curtin.edu.au/tlf/tlf1999/reece.html
Seligman, M. E. Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco: W. H.
Scott, J. C. (2001). Using the process approach to improve scholarly writing. The DeltaPi
Epsilon Journal 43, 57-66. Sheets, B. H. (2004). Effects of English grammar, usage,
mechanics, and spelling instruction in college business communication classes.
Journal of Business andPublic Affairs, (Fall), 31, 48-54. Strunk, W. & White, E.B.
(1959). The Elements of Style (Macmillan paperback ed.). New York: The
Macmillan Company.
Underhill, N. (1987). Testing Spoken Language. A handbook of oral testing techniques.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
440
Winsor, J., Curtis, D., & Stephens, R. (1997). National preferences in business and
communication education; A survey update. Journal of the Association
forCommunication Administration, 3, 170-179.
51
441
P-ISSN: 2301-9913 | E-ISSN: 2597-
9132
FILE 3
DOI: 10.22144/ctu.jen.2018.022
Cited as: Quyen, V.P., Nga, P.T.M. and Nguyen, H.T., 2018. Challenges to speaking skills
encountered by English-majored students: A story of one Vietnamese university
in the Mekong Delta. Can Tho University Journal of Science. 54(5): 38-44.
1 INTRODUCTION majored graduates thanks to its indispensable
In the context of Vietnam, the achievement role in international communication
of English speaking proficiency has been relationships arisen from the recent trend of
more and more concerned by most English- global integration. Such the impact has
brought about the fluency and proficiency of skills more effectively, the current research
English speaking skills as one main is aimed to explore the current situation of
objective in the tertiary curriculum of teaching and learning English speaking of
English language program at most English-majored freshmen at CTU in terms
Vietnamese universities. It is, however, still of difficulties encountered by these EFL
challenging for numerous English language freshmen and possible measures to tackle
undergraduates to fully achieve speaking such challenges. Explicitly, two following
skills, especially for first-year students in researched questions guided the study:
the tertiary context of Can Tho University
(CTU) in Vietnam. Additionally, there have 1. What difficulties do English-majored freshmen at
CTU encounter when they learn speaking English?
been few studies of this concern in this
context. Consequently, with regard to 2. What are feasible solutions to such difficulties?
helping English-majored freshmen learn 2 LITERATURE REVIEW
speaking
2.1 Speaking competence of English as a
foreign language
Different views of speaking competence in
English as a foreign language (EFL) have
been raised. As mentioned by Bachman and
Palmer (1990), one’s
speaking competence of a language is Several factors hindering EFL learners’
expressed by his linguistic competence, speaking performance have been discussed
knowledge of encountering linguistic with respect to both internal and external
communication difficulties, as well as factors. Concerning internal factors from
linguistically emotional reactions. Sharing language learners themselves, Littlewood
the same concern, Nunan (1999) also (1984) concerns this with lack of motivation
discusses language speaking competence as in speaking English. Sharing this view, Ur
one’s ability to have adequate vocabulary (1996) indicates other three possible
and syntax mastery to speak that language. challenges to learners’ speaking skills,
Nunan further emphasises the speaker’s namely learners’ worries of making mistakes,
communicative competence in relation to which lead to their fearful of criticism,
the combination of linguistic competence, learners’ low or uneven participation, or
sociolinguistic and conversational skills in learners’ fear of other good learners’
speaking process. Regarding speaking domination. These internal challenges have
competence from another aspect, Johnson been found in the studies by Rabab’ah
(1995) particularly indicates second (2005), Dil (2009), Le (2011), Ngo (2011),
language learners need to obtain Al-Jamal & Al-
communicative abilities to involve their Jamal (2014), and Izadi (2015) as the most
class activities and learn from these significant limitations of EFL learners’
activities. speaking performance. Additionally, Le
(2011), Ngo (2011) and Izadi (2015) further
2.2 Characteristi
emphasise other related
cs of English
speaking
competence
The competence of English speaking skills
has variously characterised. Canale and
Swain (1980) summarise it in three parts of
competence, namely grammatical,
sociolinguistic and strategic. As Jones
(1996), Burns (1998), and Richards (2006)
categorise, second language’s speaking
activities include talking as interaction,
talking as transaction, and talking as
performance. In particular, talking as
interaction is implied to meet social
functions, and talking as transaction is for
purposive situations while talking as
performance refers to transmitting
information to audiences with well-
structured language. However, the current
study is mainly focused on talking as
interaction in English classrooms.
2.3 Challenge
s to English
speaking
performance
internal factors which are limitations of researcher then proposes possible
vocabulary, grammatical structures, supplementary activities for English
sentence formation which cause their use speaking skills enhancement such as songs,
of mother tongue instead of speaking the poems, short stories, or conversation talks
target language. instead of focusing much on textbook
Besides the challenges related to internal activities.
factors, some difficulties for EFL learners’ In addition to the aforementioned external
speaking skills also arise from various challenges, the limitation of English
external factors. One of the factor that learning environment also contributes to
inhibits EFL speakers is their teachers’ use EFL learners’ speaking performance. As
of mother tongue in English speaking class justified by Harmer (1991) and Ur (1996),
(Littlewood, 2007) which partly influences those who learn English as a second
learners’ speaking motivation. language and have the same mother tongue
Additionally, Harmer (1991) states that tend to use their native language to
teachers’ mistake correction while their communicative even in their English
learners are speaking can be considered to classrooms since it seems easier to express
limit the learners’ confidence in speaking what they really want to exchange,
performance. As a result, it is necessary to especially when they are asked to discuss
have positive ways to correct learners’ unfamiliar speaking topics (Harmer, 1991).
speaking mistakes combined with Sharing the same concern, Kabir (2014)
appropriate stimulation to learners (Baker further indicates large classes as an
& Westrup, 2003). In the same vein, Folley inhibiting factor for EFL learners’ speaking
(2005) and Rabab’ah (2005) discover from
their studies that teaching methods can be
challenging to learners in terms of
teachers’ limitation of how to teach
speaking skills.
Another external hindering factor to
speaking skills of EFL learners is the
content of teaching syllabus and English
teaching curriculum. In other words, as
mentioned in Gan’s (2012) study, learners’
speaking performance can be limited if the
content of English teaching curriculum
does not focus on communicative skills. In
another study, Ambu and Saidi (1997) also
share this concern by indicating
inappropriate distribution of time for
teaching speaking skills and teaching
syllabus without satisfying English
learners’ communicative needs.
Furthermore, Al-Abri (2008) considers this
concern with regard to lack of speaking
activities in the content of teaching
curriculum as another challenging factor
for learners’ speaking performance. This
performance in relation to EFL learners who In the context of Vietnam, Le (2011) also
have limited time to show their speaking examined the same research concern in the
ability (Nguyen & Tran, 2015). context of Ba Ria - Vung Tau University
Furthermore, challenges to speaking where students faced challenges in English
performance of EFL learners partly emerge speaking skills. The findings emphasised the
from the limitation of extra-curriculum challenges with regard to (1) learners’ issues
activities. As explained by Kabir (2014), such as learning styles, worries and
extra-curriculum activities can help EFL language proficiency; (2) lecturers’ issues,
learners improve their speaking abilities. In namely teaching methodology; and (3)
the study by Gan (2012), this concern is also teaching facilities. Another study related to
significantly raised as one of inhibiting difficulties for students’ speaking skills was
factors for most student participants’ Ngo (2011) at Thai Nguyen University. The
speaking performance in Hong Kong study mainly focuses on three possible
University. Gan further reveals that it seems challenging aspects: psychology, linguistic,
strange and unpopular for these students and social-culture. From then, the same
who are all Chinese to speak English outside suggestions as Kabir (2014) and Izadi
their classes, so building an out-of-class (2015) were recommended for both
English speaking environment for these lecturers’ and administrators’ assistance to
students is necessary and should be improve students’ speaking performance.
enhanced to improve EFL learners’ Additionally, although Nguyen and Tran’s
speaking skills. (2015) study was slightly different from
previous studies with respect to high school
2.4 Previous studies students’ English speaking barriers, the
A number of empirical studies have findings similarly analysed challenges
examined the same research concern related stemmed from learners’ weaknesses in
to difficulties encountered by university speaking English and proposed the necessity
EFL students while participating in oral of teachers’ ready supports by diversifying
classroom activities. For example, Gan’s their teaching activities.
(2012) study explored obstacles to speaking 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY
skills confronted by English-majored
students in Hong Kong University and 3.1 Participants
found that the students in this context The whole population of this study is 131
significantly encountered several problems EFL first- year student participants. Data
leading to their limitation of fluent speaking collected by the questionnaire to these
performance such as lacking vocabulary, student participants. Twelve of them were
focusing much on grammar structures, then asked to two focus group interview
having limited in-class speaking sessions. Additionally, three lecturers
opportunities, and out-of- class English currently teaching the four English speaking
speaking environment. The study then classes for these students were invited to
pointed to a need to incorporate sufficiently individual interviews. The details of the
intensive language improvement participants are summarised in Table 1 and
components in the current program. Another Table 2 as follows:
study by Al-Jamal & Al- Jamal (2014) Table 1: Characteristics of student
participants
investigated the difficulties that may
be encountered at six Jordanian public universities
by collecting the data from a survey Charact Number Percentages
questionnaire e
and semi-structured interviews and ristics
revealed that
most student participants faced challenges Gender 21 16.0%
limitation of oral skill, lack of speaking time, and Female 89 84.0%
with the
crowded classes as the most highlighted factors. Time of 03-06 years 28 21.4%
Izadi (2015) examined the views of lecturers and Male
English 07-10 years 92 70.2%
students of English speaking difficulties in the study Over 10 years 11 8.4%
REFERENCES
Al-Abri, K., 2008. Teachers’ evaluation of EFL text- books used in the Omani basic education Schools
(Unpublished master’s thesis). ELT Curriculum and Methodology- College Education of Sultan
Qaboos University.
Al-Jamal, D. A. & Al-Jamal, A. A., 2014. An investiga- tion of the difficulties faced by EFL
undergraduates in speaking skills. English Language Teaching. 7(1): 19-27.
Ambu, B. & Saidi, B., 1997. Issues in teaching English speaking in foreign language classroom: A
question- naire study in Oman-Unpublished master’s thesis. ELT Curriculum and Methodology:
College of Edu- cation of Sultan Qaboos University.
Bachman, L., & Palmer, A. S., 1996. Language testing in practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 371
pages.
Baker, J., & Westrup, H., 2003. Essential speaking skills: A handbook for English language teachers.
London: Continuum, 167 pages.
Burns, A., 1998. Teaching speaking. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics. 18: 102-123.
Canale, M., & Swain, M., 1980. Theoretical Bases of Communicative Approaches to Second Language
Teaching and Testing. Applied Linguistics. 1(1): 1-47.
Bryman, A., 2004. Social research methods, Second Edi- tion. Oxford; New York: Oxford University
Press, 748 pages.
Dil, Y., 2009. EFL Learners’ communication obstacles. Electronic Journal of Social Sciences. 8(29): 84-
100.
36(2): 223-234.
Gan, Z., 2012. Understanding L2 Speaking Problems: Implications for ESL Curriculum Development
in Teacher Training Institution in Hong Kong. Austral- ian Journal of Teacher Education. 37(1): 43-
59.
Gay, L. R., & Airasian, P. W., 2006. Educational re- search: competencies for analysis and
application. 6th ed. Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Mer- rill, 614 pages.
Harmer, J., 1991. The practice of English language teaching, Third Edition. Longman: London and
New York, 448 pages.
Hinds, D., 2000. Research instruments. In: D. Wilkinson & e. Inc. (Eds.). The researcher's toolkit: The
com- plete guide to practitioner research, London: Routledge, pp. 41-54.
Izadi, M., 2015. Understanding teachers’ and learners’ perceptions of English speaking difficulties:
An in- vestigation of gender effect. Modern Research Stud- ies: An International Journal of
Humanities and So- cial Sciences. 2(2): 227-247.
Kabir, U. S., 2014. Challenges of speaking in Bangla- deshi classrooms. Unpublished Master of
Arts in TESOL.
Le, T. M., 2011. An investigation into factors that hinder the participation of university students in
English speaking lessons. University of Languages and Inter- national Studies: Vietnam
Littlewood, W., 1984. Foreign and second language learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Littlewood, W., 2007. Communicative language teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
108 pages.
Merriam, S. B., 1988. Case study research in education: A qualitative approach. San Francisco:
Miller, B., 2010. The communicative approach in China. Accessed on 18 September 2017. Available
from Elt- linkup.org.
Ngo, P. T., 2011. A study on the difficulties in learning speaking English of the first year students at
the Fac- ulty of Information Technology, Thai Nguyen Uni- versity. MA Thesis.
Nguyen, T. & Tran, M., 2015. Factors affecting students’ speaking performance at Le Thanh Hien high
school. Asian Journal of Educational Research. 3(2): 8-23.
Nunan, D., 1999. Second language teaching & learning. Heinle & Heinle Publishers: Boston,
Massachusetts, USA, 330 pages.
Rabab’ah, G., 2005. Communication problems facing Arab learners of English. Journal of Language
and Learning 3(1): 180-197.
Richards, J. C., 2006. Developing classroom speaking activities: From theory to practice.
Guidelines.
FILE 4
WHAT ENGLISH LANGUAGE
TEACHERS NEED TO KNOW
VOLUME I, 2ND EDITION
Designed for pre-service teachers and teachers new to the field of ELT, What
English Teachers Need to Know Volumes I, II, and III are companion textbooks
organized around the key question, What do teachers need to know and be able to do
in order for their students to learn English?
In the Second Edition of Volume I, Murray and Christison return to this essen- tial question and
call attention to emerging trends and challenges affecting the contemporary classroom. Addressing
new skills and strategies that EFL teachers require to meet the needs of their shifting student
populations who are impacted by changing demographics, digital environments, and
globalization, this book, which is grounded in current research, offers a strong emphasis on
practical applications for classroom teaching.
This updated and expanded Second Edition features:
Understanding Learning
2nd edition
and by Routledge
Typeset in Perpetua
PART I
Identity and Context 1
1 Learner Identities 3
2 The World of English 21
3 English Language Learning Around the World 40
4 The Cultural Context 59
5 Learning About Identity and Setting 73
PART II
Language Awareness 93
v
C ONTENTS
PART III
Learning 177
PART IV
Professionalism 249
Index 290
vi
FIGURES
vii
TAB LES
viii
5.1 Exploring language in the community 84
6.1 Phonetic symbols and key word associations 99
6.2 English consonants 103
6.3 A traditional representation of English vowels in Standard
American English 108
6.4 Syllable structure 111
6.5 Onset and rime in English syllables 112
6.6 Syllable patterns in English 113
6.7 Common phonograms in English 113
7.1 Some word comparisons among selected Indo-European
languages 124
7.2 Most frequent derivational morphemes 129
7.3 English inflectional morphemes 130
8.1 Pronouns in English 147
8.2 Reflexive pronouns 148
8.3 Forms of verbs 149
8.4 Examples of tense and aspect interaction in English 150
8.5 Types of phrases 154
9.1 Structure of reports 163
9.2 Structure of recounts 164
9.3 Characteristics of spoken and written language 165
10.1 Average primetimes and downtimes in learning episodes of
varying lengths 191
13.1 Learning styles taxonomy 237
14.1Personal competence checklist 267
14.2Social competence checklist 268
15.1 Percentage of online users worldwide over two decades 278
ix
P R E FA C E
English language teaching worldwide has become a multi-billion-dollar enterprise, one that
the majority of nations in the world are embarking on to lesser or greater extents. For
many countries, English is seen as a commodity through which they will become more
competitive in the global marketplace. While English may have national and personal
advancement potential, it is also pervasive in the global media. Youth culture in particular is
influenced by English-dominant media and marketing. As a result, English is being
consumed and transformed transnationally.
The settings where English is taught vary from countries where English is
the official and dominant language, such as the United States or Australia, to
those where it is an official language, usually as a result of past colonialism, such
as in India or the Philippines, to those where it is taught in schools as a subject
of study, such as in Japan or the Czech Republic. In the first set of countries,
when English is taught to immigrants or to international students, the language
is often called English as a second language (ESL), and its teaching TESL. In the
second set of countries, where it is taught to citizens and increasingly to interna-
tional students, it is usually referred to also as ESL. In the third set of
countries, the language is often referred to as English as a foreign language (EFL),
and its teaching as TEFL. Because ESL and EFL carry ideological baggage, there
is much discussion in the field about the use of more appropriate terminology.
Some English language teaching professionals prefer to use (T)ESOL—(teaching)
English to speakers of other languages—because it acknowledges that the learners may
have more than one previous language, and the term can be used to include
both ESL and EFL contexts. Others prefer (T)EAL—(teaching) English as an
additional language for the same reason, whereas ESL implies there is only
English, plus one other. Other terms in use include English as an international
language (EIL) and English language teaching (ELT). Whatever the terminology
used, distinc- tions across the different contexts of English language teaching are
increasingly becoming blurred as people move around the globe and acquire
their English in
x
P REF A CE
xi
P REF A CE
(primary) school, secondary school, college or university, or adult education. The series also
includes the information teachers need to teach general English, workplace English, English for
academic purposes (EAP), or English for specific purposes (ESP). We realize that creating a
book that works for many differ- ent contexts is a big ask, but we have used examples from the
diversity of ELT settings. Of course, we cannot include examples from every country or grade
level, but we have tried to be inclusive and ensure that, whatever your current or future teaching
situation, you will find the material relevant to your learners and situation. At the same time, we
have been as specific as possible, rather than relying on generic characteristics of the field.
Our own experiences have covered a vast array of different age groups, con-
texts, and content areas—between us, we have taught in English-dominant
countries, EFL contexts in every continent, young people, adults, university
students, general English, English for business, English for the workplace,
English for science and technology, and EAP.
xii
P REF A CE
Reference
Leung, C., Harris, R., & Rampton, B. (1997). The idealised native speaker, reified
ethnicities, and classroom realities. TESOL Quarterly, 31, 543–560.
xiii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are grateful to our many students and colleagues over our careers, whose
wisdom and experiences have enhanced our own understandings of the field.
We are especially grateful to Karen Adler for her enthusiastic support for the
volumes in the series and for encouraging us to move forward with second
edi- tions. A very generous thank you also goes to Eli Hinkel for her willingness
to read and respond to, yet again, another manuscript from us. And we are, as
always, tremendously grateful to our families, especially Bill Murray, Adrian
Palmer, and Cameron Christison, who have supported our work, even when
it has meant less time together and sometimes distracted dinnertime conversa-
tions! You continue to inspire us to be creative and professional.
xiv
Part I
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
In Part I, we offer five chapters that explore the contexts for language learn- ing and teaching.
In order to develop the most appropriate language programs to facilitate learning, educators
need to know about the characteristics of their learners and the aspects of their environments that
affect student learning. We therefore begin with a chapter that examines what the research tells about
learner identity and its impact on learning. The next two chapters explore broadly the three
major contexts for English language teaching: (1) countries where English is the dominant
language (e.g., New Zealand), (2) countries where English is one of several functional
languages (e.g., Singapore), and (3) countries where English is taught in schools as a foreign
language (e.g., Spain). The fourth chap- ter discusses the relationships between language, culture,
language learning, and the roles that teachers and learners adopt in the language classroom. The
final chapter in this section provides research methods and classroom activities that teachers can
use to investigate their own contexts of teaching.
We begin with an exploration of the context because the learner and the
learning environment are essential starting points for understanding language
teaching and learning. Learning is more than the accumulation of knowledge
and skills. Learning is a sociocultural engagement that transforms the learner.
Further, language learning, whether it is a first or second language, is a
process of socialization through which individuals in a society construct and
reproduce dominant beliefs, values, behaviors, and role assignments. This
socialization is also a site of struggle among competing beliefs, values,
behaviors, and role assignments. In the classroom, therefore, the socialization
in the home lan- guage and culture can come up against those of the new
language being learned (English).
1
LEARNER IDENTITIES
VIGNETTE
3
freedom is differ- ent. Some people took it the wrong way rather than
the right way.1
4
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
Task: Reflect
1 How does Paul position himself in relation to the Sudanese commu-
nity in Australia and the wider Australian community?
2 Why do you think some of these Sudanese young people want to
be “American”?
3 Why do you think some of these Sudanese young people have the
“wrong idea” about freedom in Australia?
4 What types of educational programs would help these young
people who are in a “middle culture”?
Introduction
The young Sudanese man in the vignette above talks about his own identity—
that of someone who identifies with his own Sudanese community in
Australia. He also talks about the identity conflict experienced by other young
people who want to assimilate and become Australian but who misinterpret some of
the cul- tural values they see among their Australian-born peers. In fact, their
identifying with these peers and copying some of their behaviors leads to conflicts
with their parents, with school officials, and sometimes with the police. In this
chapter, you will read about the various experiences that affect how language
learners shape their identities.
While the development of identity among language users and learners has
long been studied, earlier approaches saw the effects of context on language and
language learning as essentially static and residing in the individual. For example,
early work on dialect speakers by Ryan and Giles (1982) found that the patterns
of language of the dominant group in a society are the model for social advance-
ment, whereas varieties used by minority groups are considered less prestigious
and result in their users being less successful. In a similar vein, work on attitudes
and motivation by Gardner and Lambert (1972) identified two dichotomous
types of motivation: integrative and instrumental. The former refers to learn-
ing a language in order to become a member of that community, that is, to
identify with that language and its community. Instrumental motivation, on the
other hand, refers to the need to learn a language for another purpose, such
as study, with no desire to identify with the community. Schumann’s (1978)
acculturation model of language acquisition also took this position. However,
his model hypothesized that the distance in values between two cultures affected
5
L EARNER I DENTITIES
language acquisition, such that learners coming from a culture close to that of
the language they were trying to learn would be more successful in acquiring
that language. What all these approaches have in common is the belief that the
choice is either/or: people either choose to assimilate to a group or nation, or
they choose to keep their variety or language in order to identify with their
native group. However, such views ignore the multiple group memberships that
individuals have, such as gender, race, language, language variety, social institu-
tions. Furthermore, research on language and learning that focuses on the social
and interactive nature of both has shown that identity is dynamic, formed,
and transformed through language and learning (Cadman & O’Regan, 2006;
Mohan, Leung, & Davison, 2001; Norton, 2013).
Task: Reflect
Think of your own identity. List all the communities to which you
belong. How do you position yourself differently in each of these
communities? What different language do you use to express your
identity? Do you express solidarity with the values of all these
communities? Which values, beliefs, and behaviors do you ascribe to and
which do you resist? Why?
What Is Identity?
What, then, do we mean by identity? It is generally agreed that identity is the view
that individuals have of themselves and of their place(s) in the world in the past, now,
and in the future. Teachers as well as learners hold views of themselves. However, in
this chapter we will only discuss learner identities; teacher identities will be discussed
in Chapter 2. We have called this chapter learner identities because learners’ places in
the world are multiple, changing, sometimes conflicting, and influenced by the power
relations in individual interactions and in society more widely. This influence may
result in the desire to assimilate, adapt, or reject. As individuals construct their
identities, they position themselves through their language (and non-verbal behavior);
that is, they use language to let others know who they are and what their sociocultural
allegiances are. At the same time, identities, both welcome and unwelcome, are
imposed on individuals. Changing identities has been posited to explain why some
learners may communicate effectively in some language situations,
6
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
yet apparently fail in others. Both the power relations between the people
interacting and the wider structural inequalities can lead to such differences
(see, for example, Norton, 2000, 2013). Norton developed the notion of
investment in the target language to explain the dynamic relationship between
the learner and the social worlds in which they interact. When learners invest
in acquiring a new language, they expect some return on their investment,
whether it be education, jobs, friendship, or other advantages. How and what
variety of English they invest in depends on what future identities they imagine
for themselves. So, for example, learners may stay silent or appear less profi-
cient, not because they are not motivated, but because they are resisting the
identity being imposed on them. Learner investment, therefore, affects which
of the “imagined communities” (Norton, 1995) they choose to participate in.
One such community is the language classroom, where learners may not invest
in the language practices of the classroom because they perceive them as ineq-
uitable, of not acknowledging their own lived experiences. Inherent in discus-
sions of identity, then, is the concept of agency, that is, that people shape their
identities as a result of what is important to them; they are not merely victims
on whom identities are imposed. Furthermore, these identities are shaped
through interactions.
One of the difficulties for language learners and those who teach them and
interact with them is how to interpret learners’ language in any specific instance.
We may not know whether they make their linguistic choices deliberately, or
because they do not have the linguistic tools to express the position they wish to
take. For example, a learner in class may choose not to use a modal to
mitigate a request, using Open the window in preference to Could you open the
window? in order to express displeasure with a classmate who has been
deliberately baiting her by opening the window near her desk on very cold days.
However, the cause may be an imperfect acquisition of modal questions.
Learners’ past and present experiences can influence how they understand
their relationship to the society and culture of the language they are learning
and, therefore, how they utilize, resist, or even create opportunities to use the
language. In TESOL, then, it is crucial to investigate and analyze the experi-
ences and social structures that influence learner identity, their acquisition of
English, and so the enterprise of English language education. These past and
present experiences include nationality, race, and ethnicity; gender; family role, and
bi- or multilingualism. These experiences are inevitably intertwined; how-
ever, for clarity of discussion, we will discuss each of these facets of learners’
lives separately.
7
L EARNER I DENTITIES
Nationality. Defining oneself within one’s larger social context is funda- mental to human life
and one such social context is one’s national identification. However, nationality is often not
examined in the work on learner identity because it is assumed that race and ethnicity subsume
nationality. A further rea- son why nationality is often ignored is because it is considered to have
little role in the construction of identity in an era of intense globalization. For English language
learners around the world, different national identifications are avail- able, depending on whether
learners are learning within their own country, are immigrants or refugees to a new country, or are
sojourning in a second country for study or work purposes.
People who move to another nation must (re)define themselves in terms
of their new national context. However, research shows that cultural identity
is essential for self-identity (Fantino & Colak, 2001; McKay & Wong, 1996).
A move from one’s country of origin results in abandonment of previous relation-
ships, along with loss of daily contact with interactions that are characterized by
the cultural values, beliefs, and behaviors that define one’s identity.
Therefore, loss of one’s country can be a dehumanizing experience. However,
many immi- grants experience a conflict between the cultural identity of their
home country and that of their new country of residence. This conflict can
result in trying to deny their heritage and assimilate, or in rejecting
identification with their new country. Thus, many immigrants refer to
themselves by the nationality of their country of origin (Nieto, 1992).
In addition, for most immigrants and refugees their identity as immigrant or refugee is
externally imposed. Further, the attributes of these identifiers are imposed by the dominant
group. Even their identification based on nationality
8
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
may be an external construct. For example, refugees from Sudan may be identi-
fied in an English-speaking receiving country as Sudanese, when they themselves
identify by ethnicity, such as Dinka, Nuer, Nuba, or Achole. Often, refugees
reject identification with the nation-state where they were born, because in fact
their identity as a minority in that country and the resulting persecution is what
led to their becoming refugees. Such is the case for many Assyrians from Iran or
Hazaras from Afghanistan.
For those learning English in their country of origin, nationality is important
for their self-identity and their perceptions (or the perceptions of their nation)
as to what they may be losing (or not) in acquiring English. We recall a telling
incident from the early 1970s. One of us (Denise) was teaching in Australia at
the time when China was just opening to the West and was looking to develop
English language programs. Several Chinese officials, in the gray Mao suits,
vis- ited our program to get ideas and stated emphatically, “We want English; we
do not want culture.” In countries that were former British colonies, English
has played different roles. It has been used as the national language in order to
pre- vent the dominance of any one indigenous linguistic group. In other countries
a local language has become the national language to reinforce a singular nation-
ality, such as in Bangladesh, which adopted Bangla as the national language,
maintaining English as a language of the privileged (Imam, 2005). Thus, many
poor, rural children in Bangladesh may have no investment in learning English
because they do not perceive that it will provide them with any advantages.
In other settings, national boundaries may be the result of war or imposed by
former colonial powers. Thus, within the boundaries are people who identify
with their ethnicity, rather than their nation. For example, on a visit to China,
one of us (Denise) met a student from a university near the Korean border. The
medium of instruction at her university was Korean. She herself identified as
Korean, not Chinese. In Spain, many Basques and Catalans do not identify them-
selves as Spaniards. For our purposes here, it is sufficient to note that nationality
and the extent to which learners’ identity is tied to their national beliefs, values,
and even myths, is as relevant in EFL as in ESL situations.
However, with English being consumed transnationally, the sociolinguistic
environment is liquid (Schneider, 2014), with people from all linguistic tradi-
tions interacting via English. These interactions have been identified as English as
a lingua franca, or ELF. Young people (and other adults) may choose to
identify with the consumerism promoted by global media through choices of
clothing, music, and even behaviors. Or they may choose to identify through
English as part of the informal virtual global world created through social
media (Murray, 2018). In fact, many virtual communities are forming,
evolving, and dissolving
9
L EARNER I DENTITIES
through online interaction. People have agency, and, therefore, some appro- priate or resist
identification with any one vision, especially because they have access to a variety of
alternative views.
10
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
Asian and Pacific Islander (also used by the U.S. Census) is problematic
because it includes a variety of countries whose citizens would differentiate among
them- selves, such as Japanese, Lao, Hmong, or Samoan.
Task: Reflect
Now that you have read about the inaccuracies in trying to identify people
in terms of nationality, race, or ethnicity, think again about your own
identity. In the earlier reflection, did you identify with any nationality,
race, or ethnicity? To what extent is that identification one you have cho-
sen for yourself or one that has been imposed? What values, beliefs,
and behaviors do you share with others in this category?
Our views of racial and ethnic groups, other than the one(s) we identify with,
are often created through the media and through the way our community is per-
ceived by other groups. As language teachers, we need to understand the wider
communities’ stereotyping and attitudes towards race and ethnicity, as well as
the positioning our students adopt, in order to develop inclusive classrooms that
give all learners the opportunity to succeed.
Gender
Early work on the relationship between gender and language found differences
in speech patterns between men and women, such as women using more hedges
(Lakoff, 1975), men interrupting more in mixed-gender conservations (Thorne
& Henley, 1975), and men and women being socialized differently, with women
focusing on maintaining social relationships (Tannen, 1990). In more recent
work on second language learning and gender, Pavlenko, Blackledge, Piller,
and Teutsch-Dwyer (2001) criticize the earlier research because it sees gender
as a characteristic of individuals, rather than being socially constructed. Further,
they demonstrate that gender in and of itself does not necessarily enable (or not)
second language learning. However, they note that, as a result of social norms,
access to language skills may vary across gender, whether in immigrant coun-
tries or where English is being learned as a school subject. However, women
are not without agency and can (and do) learn English to improve their condi-
tions and to reject patriarchy in many different settings where English is taught.
11
L EARNER I DENTITIES
They and others have found that English language teaching textbooks often por- tray
stereotypical roles. Similarly, we have seen classrooms where stereotypical roles have been
assigned even by teachers who would not consider themselves sexist. For example, they
may call on boys more than girls or may accept a hus- band responding for a wife in
an adult education setting.
The concern, then, for English language teachers is to examine their contexts
to determine whether males are privileged in ways that deny females opportuni-
ties for learning the full range of English, the role portrayals mirror those of
the wider society, and the women and girls in their classrooms want to accept
or reject those roles.
Family Roles
For many immigrants, moving to a new country can entail a change in family roles. Parent and
child roles are often reversed, as young children act as inter- preters for their less linguistically
proficient parents. Children are often asked to interpret at the doctor’s or in service encounters or
parent-teacher meetings because their English is more proficient than that of their parents. Such
situa- tions, however, place children in an awkward position because they have to take on the role
of expert and caretaker.
Adolescent refugees and immigrants, as well as those facing the identification
as refugee or immigrant, have to pass through the developmental process from
child to adult, a process that is often deemed a crisis or a difficult identity change
in English receiving countries, such as Britain, Australia, Canada, New Zealand,
and the United States. Yet, these young people may have come from societies
where there is no such transitional phase and where young people move from
childhood immediately to adulthood. Additionally, their experiences as refugees
or immigrants may have forced adult responsibilities onto them while they were
still the age of childhood. Some, for example, may have been child soldiers,
been raped, or had to support their younger siblings.
Peer pressure on immigrant and refugee students is even greater than on the native-born.
Immigrant and refugee adolescents quickly adopt many of the behaviors of their peers, often
rejecting the norms of their home community, as we saw in the vignette at the beginning of this
chapter. They may reject the language of the home, refusing to use anything but the language of
their new country. This extreme acculturation often leads to conflict within families and
communities: parents often want their children to retain the values and behav- iors of their
community. Further, parents may be at a loss as to how to discipline their children if the methods
used in their home country are not considered acceptable in their new country.
12
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
In countries where English is being learned as a school subject, family roles
are usually determined by local sociocultural norms. However, a number of
factors can impact language learners in these settings. There may be tensions
between ascribed roles and those that children and young people want to adopt
because of exposure to different social rules they encounter, especially through
international media. There may also be tensions between the expectations of
an English teacher from another country with different norms. For example,
in some cultures, parents leave education to the schools but claim complete
control over moral education. A teacher coming from Australia, for example,
may see that her role is to introduce her students to social issues that are salient
in Australia. Textbooks, similarly, often choose topics aligned to middle-class
Western ideas because they are mostly written by middle-class Westerners.
Topics such as women’s roles in the home or at work may cause conflicts or, at
best, discomfort or confusion. For example, in immigrant and refugee families,
children may have an important role in the workforce after school so that the
family has sufficient income. Such workforce participation may fall especially
heavily on teenagers in a family with a single mother parent and many
younger siblings. Yet, teachers expect those young people to do extensive
homework and engage in after-school activities, the roles that a typical teenager
takes in the local culture. Language teachers need to understand what roles
learners take outside the classroom because these impact on their motivation
and investment in learning English.
Bi- or multilingualism
The majority of countries in the world are multilingual and the majority of
peo- ple use more than one language. Many countries encourage the learning of
one or more foreign languages—sometimes with utilitarian goals, as we saw
ear- lier. Yet, the use of different languages in multilingual societies usually
carries societal values and, therefore, positions users according to their
language use. For example, while English may be the language for global
interaction, learners from countries that were former colonies of English-
speaking nations may be ambivalent about, or even hostile to, learning the
language of their oppressors (Canagarajah, 2001; Chick, 2001). In many of
these former colonies, a local, indigenized variety of English has developed as
a lingua franca. (See Chapter 3 for more on this subject.) However, this variety
may not be considered pres- tigious by other speakers of English or even by
the government of the country. Singapore, for example, has conducted a series
of English language campaigns to encourage Singaporeans to use a “standard”
variety, rather than Singapore
13
L EARNER I DENTITIES
English. Liberians, who are immigrating to Australia and the United States, con- sider
themselves to be native speakers of English; yet the wider community in those countries has
difficulty understanding them, and they are placed in ESL classes, which they resent and
resist.
Having store names and signs in English is perceived as a sign of moderni-
zation in many countries, but having Spanish store names and signs in the
United States is viewed by many as a sign of rejecting English and, therefore,
the United States. Interestingly, the Council of Europe’s goal of plurilingualism
for all European citizens is that they should learn two languages in addition to
their mother tongue—a goal designed to promote a European identity, through
greater movement across countries for work, study, and tourism. While multi-
lingualism and plurilingualism are often used interchangeably, recent scholarship
has differentiated between the two terms. Some scholars use “plurilingual(ism)
to refer to the unique aspects of individual repertoires and agency and
multilingual(ism) to refer to broader social language context/contact(s) and the
coexistence of several languages in a particular situation” (Marshall & Moore,
2013, p. 474). One aspect of bi- and multilingualism that expresses identity is
the phenomenon of multiple language use in a single context, which is known as
code-switching (and more recently as translanguaging).
14
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
mark identities. Furthermore, teachers and learners can decide how to use their
multiple languages in the classroom, an issue we discuss in detail in Chapter 3.
Classroom Discourse
Here, we are referring only to the talk that takes place in classrooms, even
though postmodern and critical approaches to discourse would consider the
15
L EARNER I DENTITIES
activities and roles discussed below as part of discourse. Student-teacher inter- actions
mostly reflect the dominant societal values, such as those related to gen- der, class,
ethnicity, or race.
Classroom discourse, then, can either perpetuate the power imbalances of
society or challenge those power relations. In classes with language minority
learners in the United States, Canada, and elsewhere, minority learners are
more successful when their own cultural and linguistic experiences are legiti-
mized (Toohey, Day, & Manyak, 2007; Wong & Grant, 2007). When teachers
position learners as capable and motivated, they are more successful learners.
Included here is what talk, behaviors, and interactions teachers permit in the
classroom. By talk, we embrace both the type of language permitted (such as
terms of respect or derogatory expressions) and which language (English or the
home language of learners, or translanguaging). In many classroom settings,
the home language is highly valued; in others, learners are forbidden to use it,
based on the belief that it will prevent the acquisition of English. To deny learn-
ers the use of their own language in the classroom is to deny them an
essential marker of their own identity and to reduce them (especially if they
are adult beginners) to cognitively deficient persons who cannot convey their
wants and needs. Not only can the home (and other) language(s) be bridges to
learning English (see Chapter 12), but can also actually facilitate the learning
of English (Wei, 2018). Teachers need to ensure that the discourse of the
classroom is respectful of difference and that no single group of learners
dominates interac- tions or is privileged.
The language course book represents a “can do” society in which inter-
action is generally smooth and trouble free, the speakers cooperate
with each other politely, the conversation is neat, tidy and predictable,
utterances are always as complete as sentences and no-one else can
interrupt anyone else or speak at the same time as anyone else.
16
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
Not only does such textbook English provide unrealistic models for learners,
but it also implies that English as a language operates differently from what
learners are used to in their own language. They may view their language
as untidy and interaction in their language as troublesome, compared with
the idealized model of English presented to them. A further consideration
is the variety of English provided (a point we will return to in Chapters 2
and 3). One of us (Denise) recalls teaching in the 1970s in Australia, where
the only textbooks available were U.S. or British models that were not going
to help immigrants to Australia in their encounters with Australian English
or aid international students to understand their Australian professors. In
addition, using such models gave Australian English a second-class status
and, therefore, its users second-class status compared to British or American
speakers. Similarly, to what extent do textbooks reflect multilingual/pluri-
lingual practices?
Task: Explore
Select an ESL/EFL textbook that you are using or that is readily available
to you. Choose one chapter from the textbook and answer the following
questions:
1 How is gender portrayed? What are the roles assigned to males and
females? How do these roles reflect the cultural values where you are
using the textbook?
2 How is ethnicity portrayed? Is it stereotypical? In what way?
3 How is English portrayed? To what extent is it a language of power
and advancement? Is it shown as a “can do” society as described by
Carter (1996)? How are other languages portrayed (or not)?
17
L EARNER I DENTITIES
Conclusion
Learners bring their identities into the classroom, identities that have been shaped and
re-shaped by their experiences, by how they have been evaluated and perceived by
others. The classroom and wider school community continue to shape learners’
identity. Learners add English to their repertoire of language use, which positions
them differently than before they could use English. The values associated with this
addition to their linguistic repertoire depend on the context in which they learn and use
English. Additionally, the experiences learn- ers encounter in the learning environment
itself shape their identity—either reinforcing already held values and beliefs or re-
shaping them. Education in gen- eral and language learning in particular are not neutral
enterprises. Language teachers therefore have responsibility for creating language
learning experiences that result in student learning, while valuing learners’ self-
identification choices.
Task: Expand
Cadman, K., & O’Regan, K. (Eds.). (2006). Tales out of school: Identity and
English language teaching. Series “S”: Special edition of TESOL in Context.
(continued)
international students, and refugees. Most chapters focus on what class-
room teachers can do.
Norton, B. (Ed.). (1997). Language, identity, and the ownership of English.
Note
1 Language has been changed only for purposes of clarification.
References
Blom, J. P., & Gumperz, J. J. (1972). Social meaning in linguistic structures: Code-
switching in Norway. In J. J. Gumperz & D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions in sociolinguistics
(pp. 407–434). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Cadman, K., & O’Regan, K. (Eds.). (2006). Tales out of school: Identity and English
language teaching. Series “S”: Special edition of TESOL in Context.
19
L EARNER I DENTITIES
Fantino, A. M., & Colak, A. (2001). Refugee children in Canada: Searching for identity.
Child Welfare, 53(5), 587–597.
Ibrahim, A. E. K. (1999). Becoming Black: Rap and Hip-Hop, race, gender, and
iden- tity and the politics of ESL learning. TESOL Quarterly, 33(3), 349–369.
Imam, S. R. (2005). English as a global language and the question of nation-building
education in Bangladesh. Comparative Education, 41, 471–486.
Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and woman’s place. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Marshall, S. & Moore, D. (2013). 2B or not 2B plurilingual: Navigating languages, lit-
eracies, and plurilingual competence in postsecondary education in Canada. TESOL
Quarterly, 47(3), 472–499.
McKay, S. L., & Wong, S.-L. C. (1996). Multiple discourses, multiple identities:
Investment and agency in second-language learning among Chinese adolescent immi-
grant students. Harvard Educational Review, 66(3), 577–608.
Mohan, B., Leung, C., & Davison, C. (2001). English as a second language in the main-
stream: Teaching, learning and identity. Harlow, England: Pearson.
20
Murray, D. E. (2018). The world of English language teaching: Creating equity or ineq-
uity? Language Teaching Research, 1–11. doi.org/10.1177/1362168818777529
Myers-Scotton, C. (1993). Social motivation for codeswitching. Oxford, England: Clarendon.
Nieto, S. (1992). Affirming diversity: The sociopolitical context of multicultural education.
21
I D E N T I T Y AND C O N T E X T
Ryan, E. B., & Giles, H. (Eds.). (1982). Attitudes towards language variation. London,
England: Edward Arnold.
Schumann, J. (1978). The pidginization process: A model for second language acquisition.
Toohey, K., Day, E., & Manyak, P. (2007). ESL learners in the early school years. In
J. Cummins & C. Davison (Eds.), International handbook of English language teaching
Wong, S.-L. C., & Grant, R. (2007). Academic achievement and social identity among
bilingual students in the US. In J. Cummins & C. Davison (Eds.), International hand-
book of English language teaching (Vol. 2, pp. 681–691). New York, NY: Springer.
22
Learner Identities
Blom, J. P. , & Gumperz, J. J. (1972). Social meaning in linguistic structures:
Code-switching in Norway. In J. J. Gumperz & D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions
in sociolinguistics (pp. 407–434). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Cadman, K. , & O’Regan, K. (Eds.). (2006). Tales out of school: Identity
and English language teaching. Series “S”: Special edition of TESOL in
Context.
Canagarajah, A. S. (2001). Critical ethnography of a Sri Lankan classroom:
Ambiguities in student opposition to reproduction through ESOL. In C. N.
Candlin & N. Mercer (Eds.), English language teaching in its social context
(pp. 208–226). London, England: Routledge. Canagarajah, A. S. (2013).
Translingual practice: Global Englishes and cosmopolitan relations. New
York, NY: Routledge.
Carter, R. 1996. Speaking Englishes, speaking cultures. Plenary presented at
the 30th IATEFL Conference, April 1996, Keele, England. [A version of the
paper was published as Carter, R. (2003) Text 13: Orders of reality:
CANCODE, communication, & culture in B. Seidlhofer (Ed.) Controversies
in applied linguistics (pp. 90–104). Oxford, England: Oxford University
Press.]
Chick, J. K. (2001). Safe-talk: Collusion in apartheid education. In C. N.
Candlin & N. Mercer (Eds.), English language teaching in its social context
(pp. 227–240). London, United Kingdom: Routledge.
Fantino, A. M. , & Colak, A. (2001). Refugee children in Canada: Searching
for identity. Child Welfare, 53(5), 587–597.
García, O. & Li Wei . (2014). Translanguaging: Language, education, and
bilingualism. Basingtoke, England: Palgrave Macmillan.
Gardner, R. C. , & Lambert, W. E. (1972). Attitudes and motivation in
second language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Gollnick, D. M. , & Chinn, P. C. (2006). Multicultural education in a
pluralistic society, 7th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
Ibrahim, A. E. K. (1999). Becoming Black: Rap and Hip-Hop, race, gender,
and identity and the politics of ESL learning. TESOL Quarterly, 33(3), 349–
369.
Imam, S. R. (2005). English as a global language and the question of
nation-building education in Bangladesh. Comparative Education, 41,
471–486.
Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and woman’s place. New York, NY: Harper &
Row.
Marshall, S. & Moore, D. (2013). 2B or not 2B plurilingual: Navigating
languages, literacies, and plurilingual competence in postsecondary education
in Canada. TESOL Quarterly, 47(3), 472–499.
McKay, S. L. (2005). Sociolinguistics and language learning. In E. Hinkel
(Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (pp.
281–299). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
McKay, S. L. , & Wong, S.-L. C. (1996). Multiple discourses, multiple
identities: Investment and agency in second-language learning among
Chinese adolescent immigrant students. Harvard Educational Review, 66(3),
577–608.
Mohan, B. , Leung, C. , & Davison, C. (2001). English as a second
language in the mainstream: Teaching, learning and identity. Harlow,
England: Pearson.
Murray, D. E. (2018). The world of English language teaching: Creating equity
or inequity? Language Teaching Research, 1–11.
doi.org/10.1177/1362168818777529
Myers-Scotton, C. (1993). Social motivation for codeswitching. Oxford,
England: Clarendon. Nieto, S. (1992). Affirming diversity: The sociopolitical
context of multicultural education. New York, NY: Longman.
Norton, B. (1995). Social identity, investment, and language learning.
TESOL Quarterly, 29(1), 9–31.
Norton, B. (2000). Identity and language learning: Gender, ethnicity, and
educational change. Essex, England: Longman.
Norton, B. (2013). Identity and language learning: Extending the
conversation, 2nd ed. Bristol, England: Multilingual Matters.
Pavlenko, A. , Blackledge, A. , Piller, I. , & Teutsch-Dwyer, M. (Eds.). (2001).
Multilingualism, second language learning, and gender. Berlin, Germany:
Mouton de Gruyter.
Ryan, E. B. , & Giles, H. (Eds.). (1982). Attitudes towards language
variation. London, England: Edward Arnold.
Schneider, E. W. (2014). New reflections on the evolutionary dynamics of
world Englishes. World Englishes, 33(1), 9–32. Retrieved from
https://doi.org/10.1111/weng.12069 Schumann, J. (1978). The pidginization
process: A model for second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury
House.
Tannen, D. (1990). You just don’t understand me. New York, NY:
Morrow. Thorne, B. , & Henley, N. (Eds.). (1975). Sex roles,
interruptions, and silences in conversations. Rowley, MA:
Newbury House.
Toohey, K. , Day, E. , & Manyak, P. (2007). ESL learners in the early school
years. In J. Cummins & C. Davison (Eds.), International handbook of English
language teaching (Vol. 2, pp. 626–638). New York, NY: Springer.
Wei, L. (2018). Translanguaging and the goal of TESOL.
Retrieved from www.tesol.org/docs/default-source/ppt/li-
wei.pdf?sfvrsn=0
Wong, S.-L. C. , & Grant, R. (2007). Academic achievement and social
identity among bilingual students in the US. In J. Cummins & C. Davison
(Eds.), International handbook of English language teaching (Vol. 2, pp. 681–
691). New York, NY: Springer.
Sustaining Professionalism
Adoniou, M. (2017). Professionalism and the profession as change agent.
Retrieved from www.tesol.org/docs/default-source/advocacy/misty-
adoniou.pdf?sfvrsn=0
Allwright, D. (2003). Exploratory practice: Rethinking practitioner
research in language teaching. Language Teaching Research, 7(2), 113–
141.
Bailey, K. M. (1990). The use of diary studies in teacher education programs.
In J. C. Richards & D. Nunan (Eds.), Second language teacher education (pp.
202–214). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Bailey, K. M. , & Nunan, D. (Eds.). (1996). Voices from the classroom:
Qualitative research in second language education. Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Barduhn, S. (2002). Why develop? It’s easier not to. In J. Edge (Ed.),
Continuing professional development: Some of our perspectives (pp. 10–13).
Whitstable, England: IATEFL.
Bartels, N. (2005). Applied linguistics and language teacher education: What
we know. In N. Bartels (Ed.), Applied linguistics and language teacher
education (pp. 405–424). New York, NY: Springer.
Bartlett, L. (1990). Teacher development through reflective teaching. In J. C.
Richards & D. Nunan (Eds.), Second language teacher education (pp. 202–
214). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Borg, S. (2006). Teacher cognition and language education: Research and
practice. London, England: Continuum.
Burns, A. (1999). Collaborative action research for English language teachers.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Burton, J. (1998). Professionalism in language teaching. Prospect, 13(3),
24–34. Casanave, C. P. , & Schecter, S. R. (Eds.). (1997). On becoming
a language educator: Personal essays on professional development.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Christison, M. A. , & Murray, D. E. (Eds.). (2009). Leadership in English
language education: Theoretical foundations and practical skills for changing
times. New York, NY: Routledge.
Clarke, D. , & Hollingsworth, H. (2002). Elaborating a model of teacher
professional growth. Teaching and Teacher Education, 18(8), 947–967.
Crandall, J. (1993). Professionalism and professionalization of adult ESL
literacy. TESOL Quarterly, 27(3), 497–515.
Crandall, J. , & Christison M. A. (2016). An overview of research in English
language teacher education and professional development. In J. Crandall & M.
A. Christison (Eds.), Teacher education and professional development in
TESOL (pp. 4–34). New York, NY: Routledge.
Davison, C. (2005). The standards solution: The role of standards in
developing IT professional competencies and communities of practice in
English language teaching. In C. Davison (Ed.), Information teachnology and
innovation in language education (pp. 255–283). Hong Kong, China:
University of Hong Kong Press.
Edge, J. (1992). Co-operative development. ELT Journal, 46(1), 62–70.
Edge, J. (1996). Cross-cultural paradoxes in a profession of values. TESOL
Quarterly, 30(1), 9–30.
Edge, J. (2002a). Continuing cooperative development: A discourse framework
for individuals as colleagues. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Edge, J. (Ed.). (2002b). Continuing professional development: Some of our
perspectives. Whitstable, England: IATEFL.
European Commission . (2009). European strategy and co-operation in
education and training. Retrieved from http://ec.europa.eu/education/lifelong-
learning-policy/doc28_en.htm Frase, L. , & Sorenson, L. (1992). Teacher
motivation and satisfaction: Impact on participatory management. NASSP
Bulletin (January), 37–43.
Fullan, M. G. (1991). The new meaning of educational change, 2nd ed.
London, England: Cassell.
Ginsburg, M. , & Tidwell, M. (1990). Political socialization of prospective
educators in Mexico. New Education, 12(2), 70–82.
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence. London, England:
Bloomsbury. Grossman, J. B. , & Furano, K. (2002). Making the most of
volunteers [Electronic version]. P/PV Briefs. Retrieved from
www.ppv.org/ppv/publications/assets/152_publication.pdf Johnson, S.
(1990). Teachers at work: Achieving success in our schools. New York, NY:
Basic Books.
Kemmis, S. , & McTaggart, R. (Eds.). (1986). The action research
planner. Geelong, Australia: Deakin University Press.
Kennedy, C. (1987). Innovating for a change: Teacher development and
innovation. ELT Journal, 41(3), 163–170.
Lave, J. , & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate and peripheral
participation. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Liu, J. , & Berger, C. M. (2015). TESOL: A guide. London, England:
Bloomsbury.
Murk, P. J. , & Stephan, J. F. (1991). Volunteers: How to get them, train them
and keep them. Economic Development Review, 9(3), 73–76.
Myers, M. , & Clark, S. (2002). CPD, lifelong learning and going meta. In J.
Edge (Ed.), Continuing professional development: Some of our perspectives
(pp. 50–62). Whitstable, England: IATEFL.
New Teacher Center . (2011). State policy review: Teacher induction.
Retrieved from
https://newteachercenter.org/wp-content/uploads/California.pdf
TESOL International Association (2018). Action agenda for the future of
the TESOL profession. Retrieved from www.tesol.org/summit-2017/action-
agenda-for-the-future-of-the- tesol-profession
TESOL Research Agenda Task Force . (2014). TESOL International
Association research agenda 2014. Alexandria, VA: TESOL International.
Retrieved from www.tesol.org/docs/default-source/pdf/2014_tesol-research-
agenda.pdf?sfvrsn=2 Thompson, C. , & Zeuli, J. (1999). The frame and the
tapestry: Standards-based reform and professional development. In L.
Darling-Hammond & G. Sykes (Eds.), Teaching as the learning profession:
Handbook of policy and practice (pp. 341–375). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-
Bass.
Wallace, M. (1994). Training foreign language teachers: A reflective approach.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Yates, L. , & Brindley, G. (2000). Editorial. Prospect, 15(3), 1–4.
Zeichner, K. M. (1981–2). Reflective teaching and field-based
experience in teacher education. Interchange, 12, 1–22.
Teaching and Learning Language in a Digital World
Anstey, M. , & Bull, G. (2011). Helping teachers to explore multimodal texts.
Curriculum and Leadership Journal, 8(16). Retrieved from
www.curriculum.edu.au/leader/helping_teachers_to_explore_multimodal_texts,
31522.html?is sueID=12141
Avalos, G. (2015). Survey: Digital gap persists. San José Mercury News,
June 16, B5–B6. Chun, M. M. , Golomb, J. D. , & Turk-Browne, N. B.
(2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of
Psychology, 62, 73–101.
Crystal, D. (2008). Txtng: The gr8 db8. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press. dailyedventures (2015). Teachers will not be replaced by
technology, but teachers who do not use technology will be replaced by
those who do. Retrieved from
http://dailyedventures.com/index.php/2015/03/12/hari-krishna-arya/
Davies, G. , Otto, S. E. K. , & Rüschoff, B. (2013). Historical perspectives on
CALL. In M. Thomas , H. Reinders , & M. Warschauer (Eds.), Contemporary
computer-assisted language
learning (pp. 19–38). London, England: Bloomsbury.
Dixon, D. , & Christison, M. A. (2018). The usefulness of massive multi-player online role- playing
games (MMORPGs) as tools for promoting second language acquisition. In J. Perren
, K. Kelch , J.-S. Byun , S. Cervantes , & S. Safavi (Eds.), Applications of CALL theory in ESL and
EFL environments. Hershey, PA: IGI Global Publishing.
Ferriss, T. (2007). The 4-hour work week: Escape the 9–5, live anywhere and join the new rich. New
York, NY: Crown.
Foltz, P. , Hilfer, E. , McClure, K. , & Stavinsky, D. (2018). The agency of Artificial Intelligence.
Language Magazine (June), 28–32.
Gee, J. P. & Hayes, E. R. (2011). Language and learning in the digital age. New York, NY: Routledge.
Gonzalez, D. , & St. Louis, R. (2013). CALL I low-tech contexts. In M. Thomas , H. Reinders , & M.
Warschauer (Eds.), Contemporary computer-assisted language learning (pp. 217–241). London,
England: Bloomsbury.
Gorlick, A. (2009). Media multitaskers pay mental price, Stanford study shows. Retrieved from
https://news.stanford.edu/2009/08/24/multitask-research-study-082409/
Hamdan, W. (2017). Tips for teaching in a low-tech classroom. Retrieved from
www.britishcouncil.org/voices-magazine/tips-teaching-low-tech-classroom Internet
World Stats . (2018). Internet users in the world 2017. Retrieved from
www.internetworldstats.com/stats.htm
Junco, R. (2012). In-class multitasking and academic performance. Computers in Human Behavior,
28(6), 2236–2243. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.06.031 Kim, D. , Rueckert, D.
, Kim, D.-J. , & Seo, S. (2013). Students’ perceptions and experiences of mobile learning. Language
Learning & Technology, 17(3), 52–73.
Murray, D. E. (2013). Technology for literacies. In C. A. Chappelle (Ed.), The Encyclopedia of Applied
Linguistics (pp. 186–199). Oxford, England: Wiley-Blackwell.
Murray, D. E. , & Christison, M. A. (2017a). Going online: Affordances and limitations for teachers
and teacher educators. In L. L. C. Wong & K. Hyland (Eds.), Faces of English education: Students,
teachers, and pedagogy (pp. 215–230). London, England: Routledge. Murray, D. E. , & Christison, M.
A. (2017b). Online language teacher education: Participants’ perceptions and experiences. Retrieved
from www.tirfonline.org/wp- content/uploads/2017/03/TIRF_OLTE_2017_Report_Final.pdf
Ong, W. (1982). Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. London, England: Methuen.
Prensky, M. (2001). Digital natives, digital immigrants. On the Horizon, 9(5), 1–6. Retrieved from
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/10748120110424816
Sadler, R. & Dooly, M. (2013). Language learning in virtual worlds: Research and practice. In
M. Thomas , H. Reinders , & M. Warschauer (Eds.), Contemporary computer-assisted language
learning (pp. 159–182). London, England: Bloomsbury.
Schneider, E. W. (2016). Grassroot Englishes in tourism interactions. English Today, 32(3), 2–10.
Schulze, M. & Heift, T. (2013). Intelligent CALL. In M. Thomas , H. Reinders , & M.
Warschauer (Eds.), Contemporary computer-assisted language learning (pp. 249–265). London,
England: Bloomsbury.
TESOL (2018). Action agenda for the future of the TESOL profession. Retrieved from
www.tesol.org/summit-2017/action-agenda-for-the-future-of-the-tesol-profession
Vittachi, N. (2010). A short course in Globalese. In D. Nunan & J. Choi (Eds.), Language and culture:
Reflective narratives and the emergence of identity (pp. 215–222). New York, NY: Routledge.
Washington Post . (2018). For digital nomads, work is where the laptop is. Retrieved from
www.washingtonpost.com/business/economy/for-digital-nomads-work-is-where-the-laptop-
is/2018/07/06/3e146a4c-7e34-11e8-bb6b- c1cb691f1402_story.html?
noredirect=on&utm_term=.ba58e543bd12
Watson, C. E. , & Plymale, W. O. (2011). The pedagogy of things: Ubiquitous learning, student
culture, and constructivist pedagogical practice. In T. T. Kidd & I. Chen (Eds.), Ubiquitous learning:
Strategies for pedagogy, course design, and technology (pp. 3–16). Charlotte, NC: Information Age
Publishing, Inc.
FILE 5
FILE 6
FILE 7
Journal of Management and Training for Industries Vol.8, No.1 DOI:
10.12792/JMTI.8.1.1
Research Note
Relationships Between Foreign Language Anxiety and Willingness to Communicate Among Japanese
EFL Learners
SATOMI Fujii
Hokkaido
University
Hokkaido, Japan
Corresponding author: [email protected]
*This paper is based on the presentation given at the JACET 58th International
Convention held in August, 2019.
Satomi Fujii received her Ph. D. From Graduate School of International Media,
Communication, and Tourism Studies, Hokkaido University, Japan. She has years of
experience in teaching English at high school, voca- tional school and university. She is
currently a researcher at Hokkaido University. Her research interests include language
anxiety, willingness to communicate (WTC), individual factors, and EFL teaching.
Abstract
Language learning can be stressful for learners when they are required to communicate with
classmates using the target language. In such situations, high-anxious learners often feel
frustrated compared to low-anxious learners. Feelings of language anxiety may obstruct
learner’s willingness to communicate (WTC). However, not many studies have
elaborated on the straight-forward relationships between these two variables. Thus, this
study seeks to ascertain: 1) the correlations between language anxiety and WTC, 2)
the differences of WTC between high-anxious and low-anxious learners, and 3) learner
willingness to use the four skills in English. A total of 145 university students
participated in this study. The results indicate a significant negative correlation between
language anxiety and WTC. The result of the t-test indicates significant dif- ferences in
WTC scores between high-anxious and low-anxious learners. Thus, the negative
relationships between language anxiety and WTC as well as a clear contrast in high- and
low-anxious learners’ WTC are established.
Keywords: Foreign language anxiety, Willingness to Communicate (WTC),
Individual factors, Japanese EFL learners
Introduction
Literature Review
Methodology
Participants
Participants in this study were 145 undergraduate students from two different
universities in Japan, with ages ranging from 18 to 22. Two teachers, including the
author, offered instructions to these students in two independent EFL classes in two
universities. Students were required to conduct a large number of communicative tasks
in both classes, and English was the only language used all the time in class. All the
students speak Japanese as their native language and their English proficiency levels
were between 500 and 785 points in TOEIC approximately.
Instruments
In this study, three different scales are chosen as instruments, e.g. Foreign Language
Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS) from Horwitz et al. (1986), Willingness to
Communicate (WTC) Scale from Peng and Woodrow (2010), and the scales which
evaluate the learners’ willingness to make practical use of the four skills in English:
speaking, writing, reading, and listening. The third scale is the original scale proposed
by the author, and is labeled as the Willingness to Use the Four Skill (WUFS) Scale,
including questions such as “I like practicing speaking with my classmates and my
teacher, as well as making presentations or discussing in English”. The FLCAS consists
of 33 items on a 5-point Likert scale. WTC scale consists of 10 items, and WUFS
consists of 4 items, respectively. Both scales are established on a 6-point Likert scale.
All the questionnaires were translated from English to Japanese, with the help of a
native speaker of English who has lived in Japan for over 10 years. The whole survey
was carried out during the academic year of 2018.
The data from University A were gathered in July 2018, and the Data from University B
were gathered in October 2018. These data from the two universities were combined in
order to conduct the analysis and draw a more general conclusion.
Data Analysis
All the data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics 22. Data obtained from this study
were computed anonymously and were analyzed statistically. As explained above, all
the questionnaire items in FLCAS were answered based on a five-point Likert scale and
all the items in WTC and WUFS were answered on a six-point Likert scale. Firstly,
Pearson’s Correlation Analysis is carried out to see the correlations between learners’
language anxiety and WTC, since the whole data followed a normal distribution. To
figure out the differences between high-anxious and low-anxious learners, the total
scores of the FLCAS were used. Statistical analysis is conducted for the data of FLCAS
to calculate the mean, standard deviation, and median. Due to the median score,
participants were divided into two groups: high-anxious and low-anxious groups. Based
on the data in these two groups of learners, independent t-tests are performed to assess
learners’ WTC and willingness to make practical use of the four skills in English.
Results
The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient is computed to see the reliability of all the scale items.
There are three subcategories in the language anxiety scale (FLCAS; Horwitz et al., 1986),
and two subcategories in the WTC scale (Peng & Woodrow, 2010). As for the language
anxiety scale (FLCAS; Horwitz et al., 1986), the overall result shows a reliability of
0.92. Among the three subscales of language anxiety, the reliability is discovered as
follows: communication apprehension:α = 0.82; test anxiety:α = 0.85; fear of negative
evaluation:α= 0.59. The second questionnaire, the WTC scale (Peng and Woodrow, 2010)
indicates a good reliability estimate of 0.89. The results of the subscales are the follows:
WTC in Meaning-focused activities: α= 0.81; WTC in Form-focused activities:α = 0.93.
The third questionnaire, WUFS, i.e., the items related to learners’ willingness to use the four
skills of English, is found to have good reliability of 0.85. As a whole, the overall
questionnaire items are proved to be most reliable.
Correlations Between Language Anxiety and WTC
Table 1 shows the overall results of the correlations between language anxiety and
WTC. Pearson’s Corre- lation Analysis is carried out based on the learners’ answers of
these two scales. The three subcategories of language anxiety, communication
apprehension, test anxiety, and fear of negative evaluations, are found to have strong
positive correlations between each other. As for the two subcategories of WTC,
WTC in meaning-focused activities and WTC in form-focused activities, moderate
positive correlations are dis-
covered. As shown in Table 1, the result suggests moderate negative correlations
between willingness to communicate in meaning-focused activities and three subcategories
of language anxiety. Besides, willingness to communicate in form-focused activities and
the three sources of language anxiety are perceived to have weak but negative
correlations. Although there are differences in the strength of correlations for each item,
language anxiety and WTC are proven to be negatively correlated with each other.
Thus, a conclusion is drawn that a negative relationship between language anxiety and
WTC is observed from the correlational analysis.
Table 2 shows the statistical analysis of the FLCAS, the language anxiety scale. This is
a 5-point Likert scale which consists of 33 items, so the total scores range from a
minimum of 33 to a maximum of 165. In this study, the mean score is 101.68, with the
minimum score of 47 and the maximum score of 154. Since the median score is 102, this
score is used as a cut-off point. According to the anxiety levels, the participants were
divided into two groups, low-anxious and high-anxious groups. Among the total of 145
learners, 84 of them are low-anxious and 61 are high-anxious (see Table 3). These two
groups of learners, low-anxious and high-anxious learners, are compared individually
according to the answers of the WTC scale and scales about their willingness to use the
four skills of English.
WTC1 WTC2
Fear of
Communication Meaning- Form-
Test Anxiety Negative
Apprehension focused focused
Evaluation
activities activities
Communication
- .784** .758** -.352** -.177*
Apprehension
Test Anxiety - .739** -.424** -.205*
Fear of Negative
- -.323** -.183*
Evaluation
WTC1 Meaning-
- .538**
focused activities
WTC2 Form-
-
focused activities
* p < .05, ** p < .01
Table 2 Statistical Analysis of the FLCAS (Language Anxiety)
Low-anxious High-anxious
t-value
M SD M SD
WTC1 3.04 0.98 2.36 0.80 4.41***
WTC2 3.99 1.30 3.73 1.40 1.18
WTC (Overall) 3.42 0.97 2.91 0.91 3.20**
** p < .01, *** p <.001
Table 4 shows the results of the independent t-tests which explain the high-anxious and low-
anxious learners’ willingness to communicate. As in this table, the mean score of WTC
in meaning-focused activities is 3.04 (SD= 0.98) among low-anxious learners, but the
mean score among high-anxious learners is 2.36 (SD= 0.80). Thus, conclusions can be
made that there is a significant difference between these two groups of learners (t= 4.41,
p < .001). On the other hand, the mean score of WTC in form-focused activities is 3.99
(SD= 1.30) among low-anxious learners and, the mean score among high-anxious
learners is 3.73 (SD= 1.40). Though there is a slight difference between the two groups
of learners, the t-value isn’t significant. When we take a look at the results of overall
WTC, the mean score is 3.42 (SD= 0.97) among low-anxious learners, but the mean
score of the high-anxious learners is 2.91 (SD= 0.91). Accordingly, the low-anxious
learners’ overall WTC scores are significantly higher than those of the high-anxious
learners (t= 3.20, p <.01). This result indicates that the low-anxious learners tend to have
higher willingness in communicative activities compared to the high-anxious learners.
Table 5 demonstrates the results of the independent t-tests which clarified the difference
in the willingness of the learners in the high-anxious and low-anxious groups to use the
four skills. According to Table 5,
Table 5 Willingness to Use the Four Skills Among High-anxious and Low-anxious Learners
Low-anxious High-anxious
t-value
M SD M SD
Speaking 3.18 1.42 2.44 1.26 3.24**
Writing 3.24 1.43 2.67 1.22 2.50*
Reading 3.35 1.48 2.93 1.45 1.67
Listening 3.50 1.43 2.97 1.33 2.31*
Four Skills (Overall) 3.32 1.21 2.75 1.04 2.93**
* p <.05 , ** p < .01
the mean scores of the low-anxious learners are significantly higher than those of the
high-anxious learners in their willingness to engage in speaking (t= 3.24, p <.01),
writing (t= 2.50, p <.05), and listening (t= 2.31, p <.05) activities. Significant
differences are perceived in all these three skills except reading between the two groups
of learners. As a whole, there are significant differences between the low-anxious and
the high-anxious learners in their willingness to use the overall four skills (t= 2.93, p
<.01). Due to this result, low-anxious learners are found to be more willing to get
involved in activities to enhance their four skills in English than the high-anxious
learners. As a reason for this, it can be assumed that low-anxious learners are more
accustomed to the practical use of the English language compared to high-anxious
learners. Anyhow, language anxiety may disrupt learners’ willingness to communicate
in English.
Discussion
The current study attempts to elaborate on the direct relationship between Japanese EFL
learners’ language anxiety and their willingness to communicate (WTC). A clear
tendency is observed by looking at the results of correlational analysis and independent
t-tests.
RQ1 of the hypothesis asks: “Is there a correlation between language anxiety and
WTC?” According to the results of Pearson’s Correlation Analysis, there are significant
moderate or weak negative correlations between language anxiety and WTC. Especially,
WTC in meaning-focused activities shows a moderate negative correlation with
language anxiety. WTC had a weak but negative correlation with language anxiety in
form-focused activities. Meaning-focused activities are, for example, role-plays and
spontaneous speaking activities, where the focus is on getting someone else to
understand what we mean. What can be seen in these results is the lower the learner
anxiety, the higher the learner WTC. This inclination is especially true with WTC in
meaning-focused activities. On the other hand, form-focused activities are effective in
training of speaking and understanding the new language forms by using grammatical
structures to make correct sentences. Among these two types of activities, the former
suggests more possibility of free communication in class and therefore might evoke
more anxiety in learners. In other words, meaning-focused activities may induce more
anxiety compared to form-focused activities in the English classroom. This might be the
reason for the stronger negative correlation of WTC with language anxiety in meaning-
focused activities than form-focused activities.
RQ2 of the hypothesis is, “What is the differences in WTC between high-anxious and
low-anxious learn- ers?” The independent t-test shows that low-anxious learners had
significantly higher WTC than high- anxious learners in this study, which suggests that
low-anxious learners are more motivated to communicate than high-anxious learners.
This major finding is consistent with Algahali’s (2016) study that confirms the negative
relationship between learners’ anxiety in a foreign language classroom and their WTC.
In addi- tion, there are significant differences in meaning-focused activities between the
two groups of learners in WTC, but no significant differences in form-focused activities.
According to these findings, a conclusion can be drawn that high anxious learners are not
willing to engage in meaning-focused activities compared to low-anxious learners. There
is a notable difference between high-anxious and low-anxious learners’ desire to
communicate.
RQ3 of the hypothesis asks, “Is there any difference in motivation between the high-
anxious and low- anxious learners to make practical use of the four-skills (speaking, writing,
reading, and listening) when learning a target language?” According to the result of the
independent t-test, there are significant differences in the degree of anxiety and willingness
to use the four skills in English between the two groups. That is, low-anxious learners score
significantly higher in their willingness to use the overall English skills than high- anxious
learners, especially in the items of speaking, writing and listening. However, the item of
reading skill does not show a significant difference. This implies that reading seems to be
less-stressful to learners compared to the other three skills. As Saito, Horwitz and Garza
(1999) contend, foreign language reading anxiety is a construct that is distinct from general
foreign language anxiety. Pae (2013) addresses that teachers should give balanced
attention to each of the four skill-based anxieties (speaking, writing, listening and reading
anxieties) since his research findings evidence an independent relationship among the four-
skill- based anxieties. Accordingly, learners’ anxious feelings can have a steady influence on
their willingness to make practical use of the English language, thus teachers should keep
this in mind when making plans of communicative activities in class.
Conclusion
This exploratory study examines the straight-forward relationship between language
anxiety and willingness to communicate (WTC) among Japanese EFL learners and leads
to interesting and profound results. Find- ings reveal that negative correlation exists
between language anxiety and WTC. Since low-anxious learners are more willing to
communicate, we can see the importance of alleviating learners’ language anxiety in order
to raise their desire to communicate. Moreover, low-anxious learners are more willing to
speak, write and listen in English learning compared to high-anxious learners. This
result suggests the fact that communicat- ing in English would be a lot easier if it were
not for anxious feelings. As Matsuoka (2008) mentions, it may be possible to reduce
learner anxiety and enhance the level of WTC if learners could use the language in a
manner appropriate to their attitudes and aptitudes. That is, language anxiety should be
lessened as much
as possible in order to promote learner WTC in the English classroom. Strategies for
increasing WTC and reducing anxiety are essential in language learning and
communication, and teachers should help learners become more active and confident in the
classroom (Liu and Jackson, 2008). As communicative English teaching is called for in
Japan in recent years, it is of cruel significance to think about ways to raise learners’ WTC
in a less-stressful classroom atmosphere.
References
Alemi, M., Dartarifard, P. and Pashmforoosh, R., 2011. “The Impact of Language
Anxiety and Language Proficiency on WTC in EFL Context”, Cross-Cultural
Communication, Vol. 7, No. 3, pp. 150-166.
Algahali, N., 2016. “Foreign Language Anxiety and Learner’s Willingness to Communicate
in the L2 Class- room”, The IAFOR International Conference on Language Learning-
Dubai 2016, Official Conference Proceedings. Accessed on Dec. 10, 2020.
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/330385186_
Foreign_Language_Anxiety_and_Learner’s_Willingness_to_Communicate_in_the_L
2_Classroom
Fujii, S., 2018. “Towards the Alleviation of Language Anxiety: A Qualitative Study”, The Proceedings of
2018 International Conference on Applied Linguistics, pp. 48-62.
Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B. and Cope, J. A., 1986. “Foreign Language Classroom
Anxiety”, The Mod- ern Language Journal, Vol. 70, No. 2, pp. 125-132.
Liu, M., 2009. Reticence and Anxiety in Oral English Lessons. Bern, Switzerland: Peter Lang.
Liu, M. and Jackson, J., 2008. “An Exploration of Chinese EFL Learners’ Unwillingness
to Communicate and Foreign Language Anxiety”, The Modern Language Journal, Vol.
92, No. 1, pp. 71- 86.
MacIntyre, P. D., Clement, R., Dörnyei, Z. and Noels, K. A., 1998. “Conceptualizing
Willingness to Com- municate in a L2: A Situational Model of L2 Confidence and
Affiliation”, The Modern Language Journal, Vol. 82, No. 4, pp. 545-562.
MacIntyre, P. D. and Gardner, R. C., 1994. “The Subtle Effects of Language Anxiety on
Cognitive Process- ing in the Second Language”, Language Learning, Vol. 44, No. 2,
pp. 283-305.
Matsuoka, R., 2008. “Communication Apprehension among Japanese College Students”, Pan-Pacific Asso-
ciation of Applied Linguistics, Vol. 12, No. 2, pp. 37-48.
Matsuoka, R. and Evans, D. R., 2005. “Willingness to Communicate in the Second Language”, The Journal
of Nursing Studies, Vol. 4, No. 1, pp. 3-12.
Woodrow, L., 2006. “Anxiety and Speaking English as a Second Language”, RELC
Journal, Vol. 37, pp.
308-328.
Wu, C. P. and Lin, H. J., 2014. “Anxiety about Speaking a Foreign Language as a
Mediator of the Relation between Motivation and Willingness to Communicate”,
Perceptual & Motor Skills: Learning & Memory, Vol. 119, No. 3, pp. 785-798.
Appendix A- Questionnaire Items of the WTC Scale (Peng & Woodrow, 2010)
I am willing to do a role-play in English at my desk with my peer (e.g.,
1
ordering food in a restaurant).
I am willing to ask the teacher in English to repeat what he/she just said in
2
English because I didn’t understand.
I am willing to give a short speech in English to the class about my
3
hometown with notes.
I am willing to do a role-play standing in front of the class in English (e.g.,
4
ordering food in a restaurant).
I am willing to ask my group mates in English how to pronounce a word in
5
English.
I am willing to ask my peer sitting next to me in English how to say an
6
English phrase to express the thoughts in my mind.
I am willing to ask my group mates in English the meaning of word I do not
7
know.
I am willing to ask my peer sitting next to me in English the meaning of an
8
English word.
I am willing to give a short self-introduction without notes in English to the
9
class.
I am willing to translate a spoken utterance from Japanese into English in
10
my group.
Notes:
ISSN 1798-4769
Journal of Language Teaching and Research, Vol. 3, No. 1, pp. 112-117, January 2012
Li Fu
School of Foreign Languages, Harbin Institute of Technology,
China
Xuesong Wang
School of Foreign Languages, Harbin Institute of
Technology, China Email: [email protected]
Yang Wang
School of Foreign Languages, Harbin Institute of Technology,
China
Abstract—Willingness to communicate (WTC) has been a hot topic and focus all through these recent years in
linguistics and EFL education in China. This paper makes a research on various factors that influence Chinese
students’ willingness to communicate in their EFL study. It also carries on an investigation on how WTC factors
affect learners’ class participation. The characteristics of Chinese students’ WTC are also discussed. Lastly, the
authors point out that EFL teachers should provide more opportunities for their students to experience success,
create a good and safe learning environment and help learners to improve their WTC ability.
I. INTRODUCTION
Since 1950s, foreign language educators have gradually come to realize that the process of
foreign language learning is not a simple learning process of language skills. Many WTC factors
such as learning motivation, learning attitude, social support, and language environment affect this
process. These views are held by many scholars like Wen & Clement (2003), Yashima T. (2002)
and Young, (1991). WTC, the new member in the family of the emotional factors, has gained a
widespread attention in EFL field. Many people believe students’ communicative ability is closely
related to their willingness to communicate, and factors like conversational objects, communication
tasks and communication forms also influence the intensity of willingness to communicate. The
study of how WTC affect EFL learning has become a popular research in modern language teaching
practices.
II. WTC AND COMMUNICATION
A. Communicative Object
Familiar communication object can enhance the students’ WTC. Teachers should try to create a
relaxing learning atmosphere for students. In EFL classes, students’ self-introduction, or a wide
range of questions may get students acquainted with each other and help demonstrate their language
standard. As Hinkel found out that the WTC will be significantly higher when talking with friends
and acquaintances than talking with teachers or strangers (1989). Conversational objects and the
degree of familiarity, as McCroskey & Richmond pointed out, also have a very profound impact on
students’ WTC (1990). Tomoko Yashima showed in his research that in terms of the number of
people in a conversation, WTC is significantly higher when two persons are talking than that when
several persons are talking (2002).Therefore, students should be allowed to select fixed
communication objects in their classroom activities. In this way, students have higher initiative for
classroom activities. Being familiar with the communicative objects, students may find they are
willing to communicate with their fellow students in English, thus their practice opportunities
increases. Meanwhile, Jiang Xin emphasized the support from teachers and other students also
contribute to students’ familiarity with their communication objects and help create a safe learning
© 2012 ACADEMY PUBLISHER
environment (2007).
B. Communicative Task
The authenticity and practicality of communication tasks are the successful keys to WTC. In other
words, the selection of reading materials should be related to students’ life. As Clement & Mac
Intyre point out, without the willingness to speak out, communication loses its foundation (2003).
The appropriate selection of communicative topics is a prerequisite and guarantee for an effective
communication. If students understand and have some relevant knowledge on a communicative
task, their desire to express themselves will be inspired. Designs of some useful and
interesting topics for students will stimulate students’ talks. For students who have an intention to
go abroad, the communicative tasks like exoticism, cultural conventions and application for
admission are more welcome and more likely to motivate the students’ class participation.
Therefore, we get a conclusion that the familiarity with the discussion topics will promote the
smooth progress in communication activities and increase students’ WTC abilities.
C. Communicative Form
Foreign language learning should be carried out in authentic situations. MacIntyre & Clement
claim that students should learn in the real communication, make use of class activities, such as
scenario simulations, games, role plays, to help them express creatively and freely (1999). In EFL
teaching practices, teachers should design a variety activities centered on students’ life and create a
relaxing learning atmosphere to help students to express themselves. By pointing out the students’
language mistakes, teachers can make students to express themselves in a comparatively coherent
language.
Teaching software and a variety of audio and video equipments provide extra means in foreign
language teaching. Vivid courseware can stimulate students’ learning interest and enable teachers to
grasp students’ attention and get the timely feedback from them. McCroskey & Baer suggested that
the variety of communication forms and teaching modes can enable students’ initiative and exploit
their enthusiasm, increase their sense of class participation and enhance their willingness to
communicate. As a consequence, the effect of learning will be improved (1985).
TABLE 1
Number MEAN Std. Deviation Variables
1 3.3 1.252 1.568
2 2.3 1.252 1.568
3 3 0 0
4 3.5 0.707 0.5
5 2.9 0.994 0.988
6 3 1.764 3.112
7 2.3 1.252 1.568
8 1.8 1.317 1.734
9 3.6 1.35 1.823
10 2.7 1.494 2.232
11 2.5 0.972 0.945
12 4.5 0.527 0.278
13 4.3 1.059 1.121
14 2.9 1.37 1.877
15 3.5 1.269 1.61
16 2.7 1.059 1.121
17 2.5 1.509 2.277
18 3.6 1.506 2.268
19 3.9 1.101 1.212
20 2.4 1.506 2.268
21 4 1.054 1.111
22 2.4 1.35 1.823
23 2.5 1.179 1.39
24 2.3 1.494 2.232
25 3.7 1.16 1.346
26 2.8 1.135 1.288
27 3.5 1.581 2.5
28 2.6 1.43 2.045
29 2.8 1.687 2.846
30 1.6 0.843 0.711
TABLE 2
TABLE 2 GIVES A VISUAL IMPRESSION OF WTC FACTORS THAT INFLUENCE CLASS PARTICIPATION.
C. Descriptive Statistics
The means, standard deviations and variables for the scales of motivation, personality factors,
self-confidence, interest and traditional culture are presented in Table1.
It is clear from the table that intensity in motivation (M=3.3, 3.5 and 3) affects the students’
participation level in classroom. The students with strong internal motivation have stronger sense of
participation and more positive attitude. They are more eager to use the opportunities to
communicate in class and can take the initiative to communicate. have better cooperation with their
teachers and their English is better. Not much difference was found in Std. Deviations and
variables.
Personality factors (M=3, 2.3, 1.8, 3.6). As is displayed, the participants with extroverted
character hold positive attitude in their study. They demonstrate strong self-confidence, and their
performance in class is active and sociable;
while introverts indicate lack confidence in their study, they take less initiative in class participation.
Confidence (M= 4.5, 4.3, 2.9, 3.5). We can see from Table 1 that the students with strong self-
confidence perform actively in class discussions. They have good control over their learning.
Interest (M=3.6, 3, 9 and 4) indicates that when the students’ needs are satisfied, their desire to
participate in class activities are more intense.
Last, traditional culture (M=2.7, 2.4, 2.3). Statistics suggests that the traditional Chinese
culture has inhibited unconsciously the Chinese students’ WTC in class participation.
D. Analysis and Discussion
1. Motivation
Motivation consists of two factors: need and stimulation. Some motivation is caused by the
body's own needs. The other motivation is the stimulation by external things. Based on our survey,
we divide our subjects into three categories: The first type is students with external motivation; the
second is students with mixed motives, that is, students not only have internal motivation, but
also have external motivation. The third type is students with no motivation. The investigation
find out that students with internal motivation have stronger sense of participation than those
without. Their attitude is more positive and they are more eager for the communicative
opportunities in class. Students with strong internal motivation are more willing to take the initiative
to communicate and their English is better. A well-organized class setting is likely to help
promote students’ motivation. Students with mixed motives have more enthusiasm in class
participation than those with external motivation, and they focus their attention on class
participation better. When coming to cooperate with teachers, students with mixed motives have a
better performance than those only with external motivation. Therefore, teachers should try to
create an favorable learning environment to
help and promote motivations of various learners’ and increase learners’ class participation.
The study also found that female students have stronger motivation to speak English than their
male peers in class. Female learners have higher self-evaluation and hold more positive attitudes
towards speaking English both in and after class. We also find out that freshmen have better
motivation to speak English in class compared with the sophomores. Freshman’ anxiety is lower.
They hold an positive attitude towards speaking English.
2. Character
Different cultures pass different judgments on introversion and extroversion. In western countries,
extroverted personality is more popular. While in the eastern countries, especially in China, less talk
and more thought is considered a good character. In class participation, the extroverted students
have stronger sense of participation, and are willing to talk, so they get more opportunities to
practice what they learn. During the learning process, students with extroverted character will hold
positive attitude, strong self-confidence. They get more language input and communication
opportunities in class owing to their characteristic traits. They perform positively in class, like they
love to ask questions, speak out without too much concern about “losing their faces” in public. They
can absorb what they’ve learned through extensive try and practice. Extroverted learners are good at
communication. The more they feel relaxed, the better they learn. Introverted learners, on the other
hand, are quiet and shy and lack of confidence in their study and class participation. In class
participation, introverted students are more often silent and stiff, and sometimes are nervous with
blush. They are easier to get anxious when coming to answering questions. They will concern more
about the impact they make on others in class participation. They are rarely speak even if they have
some questions. They are not willing to raise their questions for fear of kidding. The fear of making
mistakes and feeling awkward in class participation lead to their anxieties. As a result they have less
chances to try and practise what they learn in classes and their language skills are relatively weak.
“Silence is golden” and “Least said” and “the fear of losing face”,--these traditional Chinese mode
of thinking which are deeply rooted in many Chinese people hinder students’ class participation,
especially students with introverted characteristics.
REFERENCES
[1] Clement, R., Baker, S. C., & Mac Intyre, P.D. (2003).Willingness to communicate in a second language: The effects of context,
norms and vitality. Journal of Language and Social Psychology. 22 (2) : 190 - 209.
[2] Eli Hinkel. (l989). Culture in Second Language Teaching and Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[3] McCroskey J. C. & Baer J. E. (1985). Willingness to communicate: The construct and its measurement. Paper presented at the
APACLSP.
[4] McCroskey J. C & Richmond V. P. (1990). Willingness to communicate: Differing cultural perspectives. Southern
Communication Journal. 56, 72-77.
[5] MacIntyre P. D., Clement. R. (1998). Conceptualizing willingness to communicate in a L2: A situational model of L2 confidence and
affiliation. The Modern Language Journal. 82.545-562.
[6] MacIntyre P. D., Babin P. A. & Clement, R. (1999). Willingness to communicate: Antecedents and consequences.
Communication Quarterly. 47. 215-229.
[7] Rod Eillis. (1985).Understanding Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[8] Tomoko Yashima. (2002).Willingness to Communicate in a Second Language: The Japanese EFL Context. The Modern
Language Journal. Vol. 86. No. 1, 45.
[9] Wen W. P. & ClementR. (2003). A Chinese conceptualization of willingness to communicate in ESL. Language, Culture and
Curriculum. 16. 18-38.
[10] Yashima T. (2002). Willingness to communicate in a second language: The Japanese EFL context. The Modern Language
Journal.86. 54-66.
[11] Young, Dolly J. (1991). Creating a Low Anxiety Classroom Environment: What Does Language Anxiety Research Suggest?.
The Modern Language Journal . Vol. 76. 426- 437
[12] Jiang Xin. (2007). Psychological Exploration on foreign language teaching. Beijing: Educational Science Publishing House.
[13] Shi Yunzhang. (2008). Gender Differences in EFL reading anxiety and their relationship with Scores in Band 4 examination.
Foreign Languages in China, (2), 96-97.
[14] Wang Chuming. (1990). Applied Psycholinguistics. Changsha, Hunan Education Publishing House.
[15] Yu Weihua, Lin Minghong. (2004).A Comparative Study on Willingness to Communicate between Guangzhou and Hong Kong
University Students. Foreign Language Teaching and Research.(3),85.
Li Fu is a professor in Harbin Institute of Technology, China. Her main interest of research is applied linguistics and EFL teaching.
She has been engaged in teaching English for more than twenty years.
Xuesong Wang is currently an associate professor in Harbin Institute of Technology, China. Her main interest of research is applied
linguistics and EFL teaching. She has been engaged in teaching English for more than twenty years.
Yang Wang is a lecturer in Harbin Institute of Technology, China. Her main interest of research is applied linguistics and EFL teaching.
She has been engaged in teaching English for ten years.
FILE 9
ARTIC L E
KE Y W O R DS
J Comput Assist Learn. 2020;1–10. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jcal © 2020 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1
and Poliquin (2007), who showed that studying a in order to construct a coherent mental model
text accompanied with a readymade GO (Mayer, 1989; see also Fiorella & Mayer, 2015).
significantly improved learners' comprehen- sion, These three cognitive processes are more precisely
compared with studying a text on its own. By described in Fiorella and Mayer's select-organize-
acting as visual aids, GOs have therefore proved integrate model of generative learning (2016), which
to benefit not only students' learn- ing, but by emphasizes that learners need to actively engage in
highlighting the main ideas contained in the text information processing if they are to achieve
(macro- structure information) and the hierarchical meaningful learning. One way of promoting this
relations between them (e.g., Guri-Rozenblit, 1989; active engagement in information processing is to
Kiewra, Kauffman, Robinson, Dubois, & Staley, promote learning strate- gies that elicit these three
1999; Robinson & Kiewra, 1995), but also their critical cognitive processes. For instance, instead of
comprehen- sion outcomes, as evaluated by providing learners with a readymade GO that
transfer problems (Colliot & Jamet, 2018a, 2019). contains all the key items of information of a
pedagogical document (macro- structure
1.2 | Selection, organization and integration: information) and showing their interrelations, they
Three critical cognitive processes for deep can be instructed to create the GO themselves.
understanding Creating a GO can be reg- arded as an
information from a text is essential for achieving the learning material into a more meaningful form, by
document (e.g., Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978). These and organizing it into a coherent mental model in
two processes cor- respond to the selection and working memory (Weinstein, Acee, & Jung, 2011;
organization processes described in Mayer's Weinstein & Mayer, 1986; Wittrock, 1989, 1991).
cognitive theory of multimedia learning (CTML; However, Mayer's CTML model (2014) stresses
Mayer, 2005, 2009, 2014). The CTML model that humans possess limited cognitive capacity
(a) learners process information through two informa- tion processing. Therefore, although the
separate channels (auditory/verbal and creation of a GO can pro- mote generative learning
pictorial/visual); (b) they possess limited cogni- tive and enhance learners' performances, it can also
resources for processing information; and (c) they overwhelm the latter's limited cognitive capacity
have to engage in generative processes during by requiring too much mental effort, thereby
learning. According to this model, if learners are decreasing their overall learning performances.
According to Fiorella and Mayer (2015, p. 38), to fill in and reducing the total number of GOs
‘learning by mapping occurs when learners are they had to self-generate (from 18 in Study 2 to
asked to convert a text lesson into a spatial 10 in Study 3). These two studies showed that stu-
arrangement of words such as concept map, dents provided with readymade GOs performed
knowledge map, or matrix graphic organizer’. better on transfer problems than those provided
Although adding a readymade GO to a mul- with blank GOs to fill in. Once again, students
timedia document has been found to improve who had to engage in a generative activity (self-
learning, there are mixed reports in the literature generated group) spent more time learning the
on learning when it comes to GO self- generation. document than those in the readymade group did.
For example, an interesting set of experiments These results therefore support the cognitive load
conducted by Stull and Mayer (2007) compared hypothesis whereby involving students in a
the effects on students' learning performances of generative activity decreases the amount of
either providing them with readymade GOs or cognitive resources they can allocate to deep
asking them to self-generate GOs while learning processing of the information in the document,
from an expository text. The authors tested two thus decreasing their overall learning.
different hypotheses. The generative hypothe- sis In another study, conducted by Ponce and
predicted that students in the self-generated Mayer (2014), under- graduates learned from an
group would be encouraged to engage in expository text about steamboats in one out of five
generative processes, whereas the cognitive load conditions: text-only (control group), highlighted
hypothesis predicted that this generative activity text (criti- cal information shown in red), interactive
would take up students' cognitive resources and text (students had an editor to highlight the text),
thus hinder their generative processing (Stull & readymade GO, or interactive GO (students had an
Mayer, 2007). In the first of three studies, under- editing tool to self-generate the GO). The results
graduates had to learn from an expository text in of this study showed that, compared with the
biology. They were divided into a control group control condition, all the aids improved students'
(text-only), a readymade group (text plus 27 GOs memorization, as measured with a cloze test.
located in the margin of the text), and a self- Moreover, students who were provided with a
generated group (text plus blank spaces in the readymade GO or asked to self-generate one
margin where students had to generate the GOs). achieved higher memorization and com-
No difference emerged between the readymade prehension scores than those in the control group.
group and the self-generated group in the Furthermore, in contrast to the results reported by
learning test, even though the self- generated Stull and Mayer, students in the interactive GO
group spent more time on the document than the group significantly outperformed those who received
ready- made group did. In the second and third a readymade GO on the memorization test.
2.1 | Participants
answer a learning test. Those in the nonguided or a but- ton to go to the previous page and review
active learning group were told that they would also the information it con- tained. It also informed
have to fill in the empty boxes of a GO while students that they would not be able to review the
learning. However, those in the guided active document when answering the learning test.
learning group were simply instructed to complete The multimedia document was displayed in the
the GO. They were only informed that they would centre of the screen for the control group, and on
have to learn the document in order to answer a the left-hand side, with the readymade GO on the
learning test once they had completed the GO. right-hand side, for the readymade group. The
The nine subsequent slides featured the pedagogical multimedia document was also on the left-hand side
content that had to be learned (human memory of the screen for the nonguided and guided active
systems). This was the same for all four groups. learning groups, with an editing tool on the right-
Each slide comprised an explanatory text (150– hand side for the students to use when filling in the
200 words) and a schematic pic- ture illustrating GO's empty boxes (see Figure 1). For all the
the location of the different memory systems in groups, the learning material was learner-paced,
the brain. The overall content came to 1,500 with each slide featuring a Previous and a Next
words and nine schematic pictures. The 12th and button.
final slide offered students a choice of two buttons
to click on: either a button to go to the learning test
The GO represented the three-level
hierarchical structure of the multimedia
document. It was composed of 21 labels for the
macro- structure information (three labels at Level
1, nine at Level 2, and nine at Level 3) and 21
links (hierarchical relations). Students in the
non- guided and guided active learning groups
were provided with a blank GO showing the
hierarchical structure. Students simply had to
click on the empty boxes and type in the
macrostructural information. Based on the 12
content-slides, students self-generated a single
GO gathering the overall information of the
document.
2.3 | Measures
TA BL E 1 Mean scores (SDs) on the learning test and time spent on task for each of the four groups
Control group Readymade group Nonguided active learning group Guided active learning group
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Retention test
Macrostructure/12 4.13 (3.49) 6.92 (3.13) 6.74 (2.49) 8.80 (2.31)
Hierarchical relations /7 4.55 (1.32) 5.41 (1.50) 5.50 (1.61) 6.47 (1.07)
Microstructure/12 5.68 (2.54) 6.86 (2.05) 6.08 (1.84) 6.68 (3.04)
Pictorial information /8 4.60 (2.30) 5.09 (1.74) 3.95 (2.09) 3.63 (2.24)
Transfer problems /6 0.80 (1.06) 1.77 (1.68) 1.80 (1.67) 2.89 (1.49)
Time on task (min) 15.49 (7.19) 20.74 (9.50) 26.66 (8.99) 29.18 (8.45)
on the multimedia document, compared with four conditions. Moreover, they shed an interesting
viewing the readymade condition, t(77) = 2.202, p light on the effects of self-generation and guidance
= .031, Cohen's d = .64. Furthermore, like the on undergraduates' learning performances.
nonguided active learning group, the guided active
learning group spent more time on the multimedia 4.1 | Does adding a GO to a multimedia
document than the readymade group did, t(77) = document benefit learning?
3.139, p = .002, Cohen's d = .94, thus con-
Consistent with the results of previous studies
firming H3d.
showing the benefits of displaying GOs alongside
Separating the generative and learning tasks for
texts instead of showing the texts on their own
the guided active learning group did not
(e.g., Colliot & Jamet, 2018a, 2019; Dee-Lucas &
significantly increase the amount of time stu-
Larkin, 1995; Kiewra et al., 1999; McCrudden,
dents spent on the multimedia document,
Schraw, & Lehman, 2009; Robinson & Kiewra,
compared with the group where these tasks were
1995; Robinson & Schraw, 1994), the present
simultaneous (nonguided active learning group),
study demonstrated that adding a readymade GO
t(77) = .944, p = .348.
to a multimedia document significantly enhances
Also, the time student spent on the generation
students' learning performances. Students in the
phase and on the learning phase was calculated in
readymade group significantly outperformed students
the guided active learning group. On average,
in the control group on the retention of both
students spent 11.28 min on the generation
macrostructure informa- tion and hierarchical
phase (SD = 2.64 min) and 17.90 min on the
relations. They also scored higher on the transfer test
learning phase (SD = 8.90). In other terms,
measuring their ability to apply what they had just
students spent approximately 43% of their time on
learned from the multimedia document to novel
the generation phase.
situations, indicating that they achieved a deeper
understanding of the information in the document.
4 | DISCUSSION
These results therefore confirm that GOs have
The present investigation was intended to answer positive effects on learning. By acting as a visual
two research ques- tions concerning the effects aid to structuring the text according to its main
of GOs on students' learning in a computer- ideas and their interrelations, GOs can scaffold
based learning environment. We aimed to students'
precisely assess students' learning with two learning
measures: a retention test featur- ing questions
about the macrostructure information and
hierarchical relations, and transfer problems.
Combined with the amount of time spent on the
document, these outcomes allowed us to assess
both the efficacy and efficiency of each of the
elaboration of a high-quality mental model and, in one group of stu- dents in the active learning
turn, improve their overall learning (e.g., Hegarty, condition with guidance, asking them first to fill in
2011; Luo et al., 2017). the blank GO, then learn from the multimedia
document. Con- sistent with our hypotheses, when
4.2 | How do self-generation and the generative task was separated from the
guidance of students' information learning task, students achieved the highest scores
processing influence learning? on both the retention test (macrostructure
In the present study, students in the nonguided information and hierarchical rela- tions) and the
active learning group achieved the same level of transfer test. Moreover, their performances were
performance as those in the readymade group. signif- icantly better than those of the other three
This result replicated Colliot and Jamet (2019)'s groups (i.e., readymade, nonguided active learning
finding that facilitating the generative task simply and control). Our study therefore demon- strated
by providing a blank GO to fill in did not allow the positive effects of self-generating a GO when
students to outperform those who viewed the guidance is provided. More specifically, guiding
ready- made GO. It is important to note that students' information processing improved both
students in this nonguided active learning group aspects of their learning (i.e., memorization and
correctly filled in all the empty boxes of the blank com- prehension). These results support Fiorella and
GO. Thus, although the task may not have been Zhang's recommenda- tion that guidance should be
difficult for the students, it was quite time- provided when generative activities are performed
consuming, as indicated by the time spent on the (Fiorella & Zhang, 2018). Although these students
multimedia document. Therefore, students exhibited spent more time learning from the document than
the same efficacy as those in the readymade group, those in the readymade group, this improved their
efficient in terms of time spent on the docu- One possible explanation for higher
ment. We can hypothesize that students in the performances for the guided active learning group
nonguided active learning group failed to over the nonguided active learning group is that the
outperform those in the readymade group because essential processes elicited by the generative and
they were placed in a dual-task situation. These learning tasks were better distributed. When these
students had to simultaneously fill in the GO's tasks were performed simulta- neously, they
empty boxes and learn the informa- tion appeared to decrease the amount of generative
displayed in the multimedia document. The resources that could be allocated to selecting,
absence of positive effects in the present study, organizing and integrat- ing the information in the
as well as in previous studies, may stem from the text. When the learning task was separated from the
dual-task situation induced by the instructions, generative one, the cognitive load was presumably
generative activity.
To avoid this dual-task situation, we provided
across time thereby leaving students with more to analyze reading behaviours in these different
cognitive resources to make sense of the material learning situations and to see if learners are ignoring
they encountered. some information sources (such as pictorial
These findings also raise the question of how information). This would allow to study how learner's
the students in the nonguided active learning group attention is parceled out between the text, the
represented the task. In other words, did they pictures and the GO. More studies focusing on
perceive it more as a generative task than as a self-generation effects are needed in order to better
learning task? If this was the case, they may have understand the processes involved in these learning
focused their processing on the generative activity, situations.
leaving fewer resources for learning the informa-
tion. Future research should deal with how 4.3 | Limitations
cognitive resources are distributed across the
In the present study, students' learning was only
different tasks and aim to redirect essential pro-
assessed with an immediate learning test.
cesses toward the text and the selection-
Administering a delayed learning test could yield
organization-integration pro- cesses. Eye-tracking
useful results that help us to have a better
studies could shed light on how students study in
understanding of the effects of each condition.
nonguided and guided active learning conditions.
Moreover, delayed testing would allow researchers
They could show whether students spend more
to see how students' performances change over
time on the text or on the generative activity, or
time. It would be interesting to know whether the
whether they spend equal amounts of time on
benefits observed in the guided active learning group
both, and whether they start by studying the text
persist over time, and if some effects are not
or by generating the GO. Moreover, the obtained
immediately observable and only appear in a
results could stem from the fact that stu- dents in
delayed test. Although we failed to find a difference
the guided active learning group read the text
between the nonguided active learning and
twice while those in the other groups did not.
readymade groups on the immediate learning test,
However, this hypothesis cannot be tested in this
it might be possible to observe one on a delayed
study. Eye-tracking recordings could be used in
learning test. In accor- dance with Craik and Lockart
future studies to analyze how learners study a text
(Craik & Lockhart, 1972)'s levels of processing
whether they are guided or not, and if they read it
theory, we could hypothesize that being engaged in a
again in the same way. Finally, guided active
gen- erative task, whether or not it is guided,
learning seems to be a more ecological condition,
induces deeper information processing, and make
insofar as students first create a visual aid, then use
the knowledge students learn less likely to be
it to learn the pedagogical document. Moreover,
eye-tracking methodology could be used in future
studies in order to see how students process the
GOs on-screen when they are provided with one
or self-generating one. It would also be interesting
forgotten and affected by time. By the same increases nor decreases learning when students are
token, although the readymade group performed placed in a dual-task situation (generation plus
well on the immediate test, their learning may only learning), but enhances students' learning when
have been superficial, and may have decreased guidance is provided (generation then learning).
over time. Therefore, future studies should Finally, more research is needed to study the
endeavour to measure students' learning with effects of generative activities in natural settings via
both an immediate test and a delayed test, in immediate and delayed learning tests. Recent
order to gain a more precise understanding of self- studies on GOs have once again confirmed their
generative effects. beneficial impact on students' learning (e.g.,
Moreover, no measure of cognitive load was Ponce, Mayer, Loyola, López, & Méndez, 2018).
realized in this study. It could be very relevant to However, to our knowledge, no research has yet
measure and compare the different kinds of been conducted in natural settings to test the effects
perceived cognitive load in the guided and on learning of asking students to self-generate a
nonguided active learning group. Indeed, one GO during a lesson. This is nonetheless critical, in
could hypothesize that germane cognitive load – order to see whether the results obtained in
indicating how deeply students process the laboratory set- tings can be reproduced in
document – would be higher in the guided active ecological conditions.
learning group compared to the non- guided
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
active learning group (Sweller, 1994, 2010; see
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
also Mutlu- Bayraktar, Cosgun, & Altan, 2019 for
a review). Moreover, extraneous cognitive load, DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
caused by a poor instructional design could also The data that support the findings of this study are
be measured (Sweller, van Merriënboer, & Paas, available from the corresponding author upon
1998). One could expect the extraneous cognitive reasonable request.
load to be higher in the nonguided active learning
group compared to the guided active learning
ORCID
group. Conse- quently, future research should Tiphaine Colliot https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9843-0014
5 | CONCLUSIONS
E Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flex- ible statistical power analysis program for the social,
behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.
R
E Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2015). Learning as a generative activity: Eight learning strategies that promote understanding.
C Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). Eight ways to promote generative learn- ing. Educational Psychology Review, 28(4), 717–741.
E Fiorella, L., & Zhang, Q. (2018). Drawing boundary conditions for learning by drawing. Educational Psychology Review, 30(3), 1115–
S 1137. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s10648-018-9444-8
Colliot, T., & Jamet, É. (2018a). Does
Guri-Rozenblit, S. (1989). Effects of a tree diagram on students' compre- hension of main ideas in an expository text with
self-generating a graphic organizer
multiple themes. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(2), 236–247. https://doi.org/10.2307/
while reading improve students'
learning? Computers & Education, 747866
126, 13–22.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compe Hegarty, M. (2011). The cognitive science of visual-spatial displays: Impli- cations for design. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(3), 446–
du.2018.06.028 474. https:// doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2011.01150.x
Colliot, T., & Jamet, É. (2018b). How Kiewra, K. A., Kauffman, D. F., Robinson, D., DuBois, N., & Staley, R. K. (1999). Supplementing floundering text with adjunct
does adding versus self-generating displays. Journal of Instructional Science, 27, 373–401.
a hierarchical outline while
Kintsch, W., & Van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehen- sion and production. Psychological Review, 85(5), 363–
learning from a multimedia
394. https://doi. org/10.1037/0033-295X.85.5.363
document influ- ence students'
performances? Computers in Leppink, J., Paas, F., van Gog, T., van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2014). Effects of pairs of problems and
Human Behavior, 80, 354–361. examples on task performance and different types of cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 30, 32–42.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.20
17.11.037 Leutner, D., & Schmeck, A. (2014). The generative drawing principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The
Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 433–448). New York, NY: Cambridge Univer- sity Press.
Colliot, T., & Jamet, É. (2019). Asking
students to be active learners: The Lin, L., Lee, C. H., Kalyuga, S., Wang, Y., Guan, S., & Wu, H. (2016). The effect of learner-generated drawing and imagination in
effects of totally or partially self- com- prehending a science text. The Journal of Experimental Education, 85(1), 142–154.
generating a graphic organizer on
stu- dents' learning performances. Luo, L., Peteranetz, M. S., Flanigan, A. E., & Kiewra, K. A. (2017). Using eye-tracking technology to understand how graphic
Instructional Science, 47, 1–18. organizers aid stu- dent learning. In C. A. Was, F. J. Sansosti, & B. J. Morris (Eds.), Eye- tracking technology applications in
https:// doi.org/10.1007/s11251- educational research (pp. 220–238). Hershey, PA: IGI Global.
019-09488-z
11.002
Reviewed by:
Singapore
Citation: Wisniewski B, Zierer K and Hattie J (2020) The Power of Feedback Revisited:
A Meta-Analysis of Educational
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03087
The Power of research (Hattie and Timperley, 2007; Hattie, 2009; Hattie and Zierer,
Feedback 2019) from meta-synthesis. Overall results based on a random-effects
Revisited: A Meta- model indicate a medium effect (d = 0.48) of feedback on student
Analysis of
Educational learning, but the significant heterogeneity in the data shows that
Feedback feedback cannot be understood as a single consistent form of treatment.
Research A moderator analysis revealed that the impact is substantially
Benedikt Wisniewski1*, influenced by the information content conveyed. Furthermore,
Klaus Zierer1 and John
Hattie2*
feedback has higher impact on cognitive and motor skills outcomes
1
than on motivational and behavioral outcomes. We discuss these
Department of School Pedagogy,
University of Augsburg, Augsburg, findings in the light of the assumptions made in The power of
Germany, 2 Melbourne Graduate
School of Education, University of feedback (Hattie and Timperley, 2007). In general, the results suggest that
Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
feedback has rightly become a focus of teaching research and practice.
However, they also point toward the necessity of interpreting different
forms of feedback as independent measures.
Keywords: feedback, student learning, student achievement, meta-analysis, teaching
INTRODUCTION
Feedback is information provided by an agent regarding aspects of one’s
performance or understanding (Hattie and Timperley, 2007). There is an
extensive body of research on this subject: Kluger and de Nisi (1996)
conducted among the most comprehensive review, based on 131 studies,
over 12,000 participants, with an average effect of 0.38, noting that about
a third of the effects were negative. More specifically, in the classroom
domain, Hattie and Timperley (2007), Hattie (2009), and Hattie and Zierer
(2019) conducted meta-syntheses relating to the effects of feedback on
student achievement (which we refer to as Visible Learning research). These
indicated a high effect (between 0.70 and 0.79) of feedback on student
achievement in general. However, the authors noted the considerable variance
of effects, identifying those forms of feedback as powerful that aid students
in building cues and checking erroneous hypotheses and ideas, resulting in
the development of more effective information processing strategies and
understanding (Hattie and Timperley, 2007).
Given the impact of the Visible Learning research (over 25,000 citations on
Google Scholar), it is important to ask whether the results presented on the
effectiveness of feedback and the variables which moderate this effectiveness
will stand up to scrutiny. A comprehensive meta-analysis on educational
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org
4 January 2020 | Volume 10 | Article 3087
feedback which
integrates the existing
primary studies is still
a desiderate.
METHOD
General Procedure
This meta-analysis is a quantitative integration of
empirical research comparing the effects of
feedback on student learning outcomes. The
typical strategy is (1) to compute a summary
effects for all primary studies, (2) to calculate the
heterogeneity of the summary effect, and (3) in
The following procedure was employed in this criteria that were used to include meta-analyses in
review (see Figure 1): First, we identified the Visible Learning research syntheses but allow
primary studies from existing meta-analyses and to exclude studies from meta-analyses that
decided whether to include these based on four encompass both an educational and a non-
inclusion criteria. Then we developed a coding educational context (Wiersma, 1992; Kluger and
scheme to compare the effects of different DeNisi, 1996; Standley, 1996).
feedback interventions. In the next step, we We included studies with controlled designs as
defined an effect size for each primary study or well as pre- post-test designs, and this became a
study part, either by extracting it from an existing moderator to investigate any differences related to
meta-analysis or (when this was not possible) design (Slavin, 2008). Whenever existing meta-
calculating it from information provided in the analyses reported the relevant statistical data from
respective primary study. the primary studies, we used this data. When no
We used the random-effects model for relevant statistical data from primary studies were
integration of the effect sizes that met our reported, we contacted the authors of the meta-
inclusion criteria to calculate an average effect analyses via e-mail and asked to provide the
size for all studies, and, in a next step, for missing information. Four authors responded and
subgroups defined by our coding scheme. We three of them provided the effect sizes and sample
checked for heterogeneity across the studies and sizes of the primary studies which they used in
conducted outlier analysis and moderator analysis their meta-analysis. When no relevant data were
to assist in reducing the heterogeneity of effect reported in a meta-analysis and authors didn’t
sizes. provide them, we reconstructed the effect sizes
directly from the primary studies whenever
Identification of Studies and Inclusion possible. Some studies were excluded, either
Criteria because we could not reconstruct their effect size,
To identify the primary studies for our meta- or because of other reasons as specified in Table 1.
analysis, we searched 32 existing meta-analyses
that were used in the context of the Visible
Learning research for information on primary
studies that included relevant data for integration
(effect sizes, sample sizes). To be included, each
study had to meet the following inclusion criteria:
It had to
contain an empirical comparison of a form of •
feedback intervention between an
experimental and a control group, or a pre-
post comparison;
report constitutive elements to calculate an •
effect size (e.g., include means, standard
•
deviations, and sample sizes) report at least
one dependent variable related to student
learning (achievement, motivation, or
behavioral change) and
have an identifiable educational context (data •
obtained with samples of students or teachers
in a kindergarten, primary school, secondary
school, college or university)
The inclusion criteria are comparable to the
FIGURE 1 | Flow diagram of study identification and selection.
TABLE 1 | Existing meta-analyses investigating the factor feedback.
Coding of Study Features article abstracts. Generally, the study features for
To be able to identify characteristics that our coding scheme are orientated toward Hattie’s
influence the impact of feedback, a coding and Timperley’s (2007) coding features.
scheme was developed. It includes the We analyzed inter-coder consistency to ensure
following categories of study features: publication reliability among coders by randomly selecting
type (i.e., journal article, dissertation), outcome 10% of the studies and
measure (i.e., cognitive, motivational, physical,
behavioral), type of feedback (i.e.,
reinforcement/punishment, corrective, high-
information), feedback channel (i.e., written, oral,
video-, audio- or computer- assisted), and direction
(i.e., teacher > learner, learner > teacher). Some
of the study features of interest had to be dropped
(i.e., perspective of feedback, way of measuring
the outcome) because there were insufficient data,
or the feature could not be defined based on the
assessed intercoder reliability of each coding studies.
variable. For the 6 moderator variables,
Krippendorff’s alpha ranged from 0.81 to 0.99,
Calculation of Effect Sizes
and therefore above the acceptable level For the computation of effect sizes, tests for
(Krippendorff, 2004). The two coders then heterogeneity, and in the analysis of moderator
discussed and resolved remaining disagreements variables, we used the Meta and Metafor packages
and established an operational rule that provided for R (R Core Team, 2017). To compare study
precise criteria for the coding of studies according results, Cohen’s (1988) d effect size measure was
to each moderator variable. The lead author then applied. This is calculated as
used these operational rules to code the rest of the
X¯ 1 −
X¯ 2
=
having them coded separately by two coders. d (1)
Based on this, we σpool
∫
with the pooled standard deviation of sampling errors and/or true effects are
(n1 − 1)σ2 + (n2 − 1)σ2 correlated. This can be the case when studies
s = (2
) report more than one effect and these effects
σ pool 1 2
stem from comparisons with a common control
group (multiple treatment studies, Gleser and
Olkin, 2009). To
n1 + n2 − 2 adequately integrate statistically dependent effect
sizes, there are
Hedges and Olkin (1985) demonstrated that the different approaches, for example selecting one
unsystematic error variance of a primary study is effect size per study, averaging all effects reported
determined by the variance of the effect size. The in one study, or conducting multivariate meta-
higher the variance, the less precise the study analysis (which requires knowledge of the
effect. Because study effects that have higher underlying covariance structure among effect
precision are to be weighted more strongly than sizes). If a study reported more than one effect size
effects that have lower precision, the inverse of the and the multiple outcomes could not be treated as
variance of the study effect in relation to the independent from each other (because they used
inverse of the sum of the variance inverse values one common sample), we accounted for this non-
of all k primary studies serves as a correction independence by robust variance estimation (RVE,
factor (Rustenbach, 2003). The inverse variance Sidik and Jonkman, 2006; Hedges et al., 2010).
weight is calculated as This method allows the integration of
1 statistically dependent effect sizes within a
wi = σ 2
meta-analysis without knowledge of the
di (3)
P
k 1
underlying covariance structure among effect
i=1 sizes.
σdi 2
The average weighted effect size d is the sum of all Bias and Heterogeneity
weighted effect sizes of the k primary studies. In Possible selection bias was tested by the means of
the fixed-effect
•i model, the variance σd 2 equals ν2, a funnel plot, a scatter diagram that plots the
which is derived h from the individual study treatment effect on the x-axis against the study size
e on the y-axis, and the means of a normal- quantile-
variances
•i (5). In t random-effects model, the
variance σd 2 consists of a first component ν2 (5) plot, in which the observed effect sizes are
and a second component, τ2 (6), which is the compared with the expected values of the effect
variance of the effect size distribution. sizes drawn from a normal distribution.
σ 2
= ν2 + τ2 (4) Additionally, Egger’s et al. (1997) regression test was
used to detect funnel plot asymmetry.
i
d•
A Q-test (Shadish and Haddock, 1994) was
performed to test the homogeneity of the observed
P
1 (5) effect sizes.
νi2 = k 1
kX (di − 2
d•)
σdi 2 σ
i=1 Q= 2 (7)
i=1 di
Q − (k − 1)
τ =P
2
w2 (6) The test variable Q is χ2-distributed with degrees
w − P
of freedom of the number k-1. Q can be used to
i check whether effect sizes of
i
wi Integration Model
The model of random effects (Hedges and Vevea, a group are homogeneous or whether at least one
1998) was used to integrate the study effects. With of the effect sizes differs significantly from the
a random-effects model we attempted to generalize others. In order to be able to provide information
findings beyond the included studies by assuming about the degree of heterogeneity, I2was computed
that the selected studies are random samples from (Deeks et al., 2008). I2 is a measure of the degree
a larger population (Cheung et al., 2012). of heterogeneity among a set of studies along a 0%
Consequently, study effects may vary within a – 100% scale and can be interpreted as moderate
single study and between individual studies, hence for values between 30 and 60%, substantial for
no common population value is assumed. values over 50%, and considerable for values over
The random-effects model takes two variance 75% (Deeks et al., 2008).
components into account. These are the sum of the Outlier Analysis
individual standard errors of the study effects By definition, no outliers exist in the random-
resulting from the sample basis of the individual effects model because the individual study effects
studies, and the variation in the random selection are not based on a constant population mean.
of the effect sizes for the meta-analysis. A Extreme values are attributed to natural variation.
meaningful interpretation of average effect sizes An outlier analysis, however, can serve to identify
from several primary studies does not necessarily unusual primary studies. We used the method of
require homogeneity (i.e., that the variation of the adjusted standardized residuals to determine
study effects is solely random, Rustenbach. 2003). whether effect sizes have inflated variance. An
The basic assumption here is that differences in adjusted residual is the deviation of an individual
effect sizes within the sample are due to sample study effect from the adjusted mean effect, i.e., the
errors as well as systematic variation. mean effect of all other study effects. Adjusted
The integration of multiple effect sizes does not standardized residuals follow the normal
only require independence of the primary studies distribution and are therefore significantly
included in the meta- analyses, but also different from 0 when they are >1.96. They are
independence of the observed effects reported in conventionally classified as extreme values when
the primary studies. The second assumption is > 2 (Hedges and Olkin, 1985).
violated when
Moderator Analysis DeShon, 2002).
For heterogeneous data sets, suitable moderator
variables must be used for a more meaningful Publication Type
interpretation. In extreme cases, this can lead to a
The type of publication (journal article or
division into k factor levels if none of the
dissertation) was used as a moderator. Published
primary studies can be integrated into a
studies may be prone to having larger effect sizes
homogeneous group. QB reflects the amount of
than unpublished studies because they are less
heterogeneity that can be attributed to the
likely to be rejected when they present significant
moderator variable, whereas QW provides
results (Light and Pillemer, 1986).
information on the amount of heterogeneity that
remains within the moderator category. The
actual suitability of a moderator variable within a Outcome Measure
fixed-effect model is demonstrated by the fact that The Visible Learning research investigated the
homogeneous effect sizes are present within the impact of the factor feedback on student
primary study group defined by it (QW empirical < achievement. However, not all primary studies that
QW critical) and at the same time the average effect were integrated in the meta-analyses contain an
sizes of the individual groups differ significantly achievement outcome measure. Consequently, for
from each other (QB empirical > QB critical). If both our meta- analysis, we differentiated between four
conditions are fulfilled, homogeneous factor types of outcome measure: cognitive (including
levels are present, which are defined by student achievement, retention, cognitive test
moderator variables, leading to a meaningful performance), motivational (including intrinsic
separation of the primary studies. However, by motivation, locus of control, self-efficacy and
this definition, homogeneity of effect sizes persistence), physical (development of motor skills)
within hypothesized moderator groups will and behavioral (student behavior in classrooms,
occur rarely in real data, which means that discipline).
fixed-effect models are rarely appropriate for
real data in meta-analyses and random- effects
models should be preferred (Hunter and Schmidt,
2000). In random-effects models, it can be tested
if moderators are suitable for reducing
heterogeneity (the random-effects model then
becoming a mixed-effects model), but without
assuming homogeneity (Viechtbauer, 2007).
Therefore, we used the article abstracts of the
primary studies to define meaningful moderator
variables and set the moderator values for each
primary study according to our coding scheme.
The following moderators were used:
Research Design
Studies with control groups were separated from
studies with a pre-post-test design. Effect sizes
from pre-post designs are generally less reliable
and less informative about the effects of the
intervention because they are likely to be
influenced by confounding variables (Morris and
Type of Feedback (see Figure 1). After the selection process, in the
A further distinction was made between final data set, 994 effect sizes from 435 studies
different types of feedback, namely (listed in the Supplementary Appendix),
reinforcement/punishment, corrective feedback, including about 61,000 subjects, were used for our
and high-information feedback. Forms of meta-analysis. Figure 2 shows the distribution of
reinforcement and punishment apply pleasant or the included effect sizes related to the years of
aversive consequences to increase or decrease the publication. The median of publication year is
frequency of a desired response or behavior. 1985. Fifteen percent of the integrated effect sizes
These forms of feedback provide a minimum are taken
amount of information on task level and no from studies published in the last 15 years.
information on the levels of process or self-
regulation. Corrective forms of feedback typically General Impact of Feedback
contain information about the task level in the The integration of all study effects with the
form of “right or wrong” and the provision of the random-effects model leads to a weighted
correct answer to the task. Feedback not only average effect size of d = 0.55. 17% of the
refers to how successfully a skill was performed effects were negative. The confidence interval
(knowledge on result), but also to how a skill is ranges from
performed (knowledge of performance). For some 0.48 to 0.62. Cohen’s U3, the percentage of those
scores in the experimental groups that exceed the
forms of feedback, i.e., modeling, additional
average score in the control groups is 70%. The
information is provided on how the skill could be
probability of homogenous effects is <0.001 with
performed more successfully. Feedback was Q = 7,339 (df = 993) and I2 = 86.47%.
classified as high-information feedback when it was
constituted by information as described for
corrective feedback and additionally contained
information on self-regulation from monitoring
attention, emotions, or motivation during the
learning process.
Feedback Channel
Some studies investigated the effects of feedback
according to the channel by which it is provided.
Hence, the distinction between three forms: oral,
written, and video-, audio- or computer- assisted
feedback.
Feedback Direction
This moderator refers to who gives and who
receives feedback. We differentiated between
feedback that is given by teachers to students,
feedback that is given by students to teachers, and
feedback that is given by students to students.
RESULTS
Identification of Studies
Our search strategy yielded 732 primary studies
Bias and Heterogeneity in which
In the funnel plot (Figure 3) all feedback
effects are plotted on the x-axis against the study
sizes on the y-axis. The funnel plot is a visual aid
to identify conspicuous data characteristics,
producing a symmetric inverted funnel for data
bias and systematic heterogeneity are unlikely.
The normal- quantile-plot (Figure 4) compares
the effect sizes from the primary studies with
hypothetical values predicted for a standard
normal distribution. It indicates that the existing
data shows
unexpected distribution characteristics. The funnel
plot displays
an asymmetric distribution of effect sizes for the The most extreme values were found in the meta-
whole sample and there is an unusually large analysis by Standley (1996). This meta-analysis
range of effect sizes and an unusually large deals with a special form of feedback, namely
number of extreme effect size values. This is music as reinforcement for education/therapy
confirmed by Egger’s et al. (1997) test for funnel objectives. The author reports five effect sizes
plot asymmetry (z = 9.52, p < 0.001). However, larger than 5, two of them larger than 10 for the
the asymmetry is only produced by the effect sizes educational context (the effect sizes for the
from studies published in journals (z = 9.75, p < therapeutical context were not considered in our
0.0001), while there is no asymmetry for effect meta-analysis). These effect sizes must be classified
sizes from dissertation articles (z = 1.03, p = 0.30). as gigantic. In an educational context, effect sizes
The normal-quantile- plot hints at more extreme that large are uncommon and much higher than
values than would be expected from a normal usually expected from any treatment.
distribution of effect sizes. The plot is markedly
non-linear (i.e., the points do not approximately lie
on the regression line), making it implausible that
the data come from a normal distribution.
Outlier Analysis
Thirty-five (3.5%) of all effect sizes were
identified as extreme values (standardized
residuals > 2) and excluded. An exclusion of these
extreme values leads to a reduction of the average
weighted effect size to 0.48 (CL: 0.44–0.51, Q =
5,771.43, df = 958,
I2 = 83.40%).
Following Brand and Bradley (2012) they can be
called “voodoo” effects. For example. for one
primary study with gigantic effects (Madsen et al.,
1976), Standley calculated four effect sizes of 1.74,
3.88, 5.60, and 11.98. Madsen et al. (1976)
report a pre-post- test ANOVA with four groups
(F = 7.54, df = 3.76, p < 0.1), a Newman–Keuls
multiple range comparison of mean pre-posttest
differences scores (5.2; 3.5; 0.95; 0.90) and the
means of two experimental and two control
groups (n = 20 in all groups) regarding their
results in a math test. Because Madsen et al.
(1976) do not report standard deviations, it is
unclear how the respective effect sizes were
calculated by Standley (1996) and a re-
calculation was impossible. Most importantly,
though, it remains unclear why four effect sizes
are reported, given the fact that the primary study
uses two control and two experimental groups.
Another study for which Standley’s meta-analysis
finds gigantic effect sizes is Madsen et al. (1975),
which reports a pre-pos-test ANOVA (F = 3.18,
df = 2, p > 0.05) and a pre- post group ANOVA
(F = 8.11, df = 2.72, p < 0.1) for two dependent
variables (behavior and math test score). Again,
no standard deviations are reported in the primary ×
study and it is unclear how the effect sizes of
10.34, 3.71, 5.17, and 3.13 were calculated by
Standley (1996). The author was contacted but
did not respond.
Comparison With
Existing Meta-Analyses
The average effect sizes of the subsets of
primary studies as used in the existing meta-
analyses are shown in Figure 5. For three meta-
analyses (Rummel and Feinberg, 1988; Standley,
1996;
FIGURE 5 | Random-effects model calculation for the subsets of previous meta-analyses.
Moderator Analysis
Table 2 shows the results of the tests for
heterogeneity between and within the subgroups
defined by our six moderator variables. Table 3
shows the outputs from the mixed-effects
statistically significant, the feedback channel
proved to be a non-significant moderator.
DISCUSSION
The aim of the present study was to investigate
the effectiveness of feedback in the educational
context with a meta-analytic approach. With d =
0.48 (cleared of extreme values), the overall effect
of different forms of feedback on student learning
is
TABLE 2 | Tests of heterogeneity between and within the moderator subgroups.
Research design 29.06 (1) < 0.0001 5639.74 (955) < 0.0001 83.4%
Publication type 6.15 (1) < 0.05 5699.39 (957) < 0.0001 83.4%
Outcome measure 14.12 (3) < 0.001 4380.69 (750) < 0.0001 83.0%
Feedback type 41.52 (2) < 0.0001 1541.06 (316) < 0.0001 80.9%
Feedback channel 5.12 (2) > 0.05 2218.20 (337) < 0.0001 85.2%
Feedback direction 9.35 (2) < 0.001 4695.60 (852) < 0.0001 81.9%
QB, heterogeneity between groups; QW, heterogeneity within groups; I2, total amount of heterogeneity.
Moderator k d C.I. Q I2
Research design
Publication type
Outcome measure
Type of feedback
Feedback direction
2
k, number of study effects; d, average effect size; C.I., 95% confidence interval; Q, heterogeneity within subgroup; I , total amount of heterogeneity within subgroup.
medium-high (RQ1), although the variability of effect sizes by precision which was not applied in
the effects is most notable. the Visible Learning research. We used a random-
The average weighted effect size differs effects model and gave every single effect size a
considerably from the results of meta-synthesis (d weight based on its variance, while the meta-
= 0.79, Hattie and Timperley, 2007). This is likely synthesis approach is based on a fixed-effect
due to several factors which have been pointed out model and sums up average effect sizes.
by Wecker et al. (2017): One reason for this Comparing the average effect sizes that were
difference could be that we excluded duplets and synthesized with the calculation conducted in this
therefore avoided studies being double-counted. A meta- analysis for the subsets of meta-analyses
second reason for the difference between synthesis (Figure 5), there is basic agreement between the
and meta-analysis could be our weighting of the synthesis approach and the meta- analysis with 21
of 24 confidence intervals overlapping. but it influence (Borenstein et al., 2009). Consequently,
also becomes apparent that the synthesized if there is a large number of studies with large
average mean effect contains three overestimated samples and high effects, the average effect size
values. In the random- effects model, large studies will be higher under the fixed-effect model than
lose influence and small studies gain under the random-effects model and vice versa.
As our data includes a relatively large number of
small studies with high effects, the use of the
random-effects model leads to a higher average
effect size than the fixed-effect model (the mean
would be 0.41 with a confidence interval of 0.40 –
0.43). Consequently, the difference in effect size
between synthesis and meta-analysis cannot be
explained by the use of a random-effects model.
We assume that the different results mainly stem
from the fact that a number of studies used in the
synthesis were excluded from our meta-analysis,
either due to a lack of detailed information on the
statistical data or due to content-related
considerations (studies that did not explicitly deal
with an educational context or did not report
information on learning outcomes). The average
effect size from this meta-analysis is based on a
smaller sample of studies than the synthesis, but at
the same time, the selection of studies produces
more accurate results because it could be checked
for each single study if it actually fulfills the
inclusion criteria.
Care is needed, however, with focusing too cognitive variables (Kulik and Kulik, 1988;
much on the average effect size as the integrated Bangert-Drowns et al., 1991; Azevedo and Bernard,
studies show high heterogeneity, suggesting that, 1995; Kluger and DeNisi, 1996; Lyster and Saito,
conform to expectations, not all feedback is the 2010) and significant heterogeneity remains within
same and one (average) effect size does not fit the sub-group defined by this moderator in our
all. The funnel- and normal-quantile-plots illustrate analysis. From a motivational perspective, feedback
that the observed data does not capture the is mainly considered to influence dependent
construct of feedback in an unbiased way and that variables like intrinsic motivation, locus of control,
there is an distribution of effect sizes which is not self-efficacy, or persistence. For these outcomes,
conform to a symmetric inverted funnel the average effect is low. A possible explanation
theoretically expected for data in which bias and from motivation theory is that feedback can have
systematic heterogeneity are unlikely. These issues negative effects on motivation by reducing the
and the results of the tests for homogeneity speak experience of autonomy and self-efficacy when it is
largely to the variability in effects and the need to controlling, negative and uninformative (Ryan and
search for meaningful moderators. Deci, 2000). Twenty one percent of the effect sizes
related to motivational outcomes in our data were
Effects of Different Forms of Feedback negative, with 86% of the feedback interventions
Heterogeneity likely results from different forms
leading to these negative effects being
of feedback, ranging from the simplest forms of
uninformative (rewards or punishments). Hattie
operant conditioning to elaborate forms of error
and Timperley (2007) have stated that rewards
modeling, from feedback to kindergarten children
significantly undermine intrinsic motivation and
to feedback to university professors, from
feedback administered in a controlling way caused
feedback that people get while learning a
negative effects, taking away responsibility from
handstand to feedback that people get while
learners for motivating or regulating themselves.
learning a foreign language.
The results do not indicate that
This study investigated six moderators (RQ2) –
research design, publication type, outcome
measure, type of feedback, feedback channel, and
feedback direction. Generally, and conform to
expectations (Light and Pillemer, 1986; Morris and
DeShon, 2002), the average effect of feedback as
reported in pre-post design studies is higher than in
controlled studies and higher reported in published
journal articles than in dissertations. Feedback
effects seem to be less likely to be published when
they are low or even negative.
Feedback is more effective for cognitive and
physical outcome measures than for motivational
and behavioral criteria. These claims must be
partly interpreted with some caution because there
are few studies available related to physical and
behavioral outcomes, substantially reducing the
precision of the average effect size. From a
cognitive perspective, feedback is often considered
a source of information that is necessary to
improve on a task. Previous meta-analyses have
produced inconsistent results of feedback on
feedback effects on motivation per se are low non-significant moderator.
but that effects of uninformative forms of Only a very small percentage of the primary
feedback on motivation are low or even studies investigated feedback from students to
negative. teachers and out of these,
Feedback is more effective the more 26 effect sizes could be used to compute an
information it contains. Simple forms of average effect size. These effects were located
reinforcement and punishment have low effects, mainly in studies dealing with higher education,
while high-information feedback is most i.e., with feedback from university or college
effective. Hattie and Timperley (2007) have noted students to their professors. Consequently, the data
the importance of not just the information in the does not allow conclusions on the effectiveness of
feedback, but the appropriateness of the timing of student > teacher feedback in the K-12 context. In
the feedback relating to where the students are general, feedback from teachers to students is
in the instructional cycle, moving from focusing more effective than from students to teachers,
on the task, the strategies underlying the task, but the average effect of student > teacher
and the self-regulation of the processes. Claims feedback has a high variance and there is a rich
by Lysakowski and Walberg (1981) that the literature related to this variance (Marsh, 1987;
effects of rewards or positive reinforcement on Uttl et al., 2017). With respect to the direction of
classroom learning are strong with an average feedback, student-student-feedback is the most
effect size of 1.17 have to be placed in the effective form, although, again care is needed as
context, therefore, of being effective more at the these estimates are based on a very small sample of
task level (which rightly and more likely wrongly only eight studies.
is the focus of most teacher feedback; Hattie, General Limitations
2009). Miller (2003), Russel and Spada (2006), We tried to shed more light on the role and
Truscott (2007), Li (2010), Kang and Han (2015) variability of feedback in the educational context
and Brown (2016) have all similarly noted that the with the help of meta-analysis in comparison to
effectiveness of corrective feedback is influenced meta-synthesis. Both approaches are often
by additional moderating variables, such as confronted with the accusation of comparing
learners’ proficiency, the setting, and the genre of apples and oranges.
the task (Kang and Han, 2015). These variables
were not taken into account in our meta-analysis.
High-information feedback contains information
on task, process and (sometimes) self- regulation
level. Its effect is very large, which suggests
that students highly benefit from feedback when it
helps them not only to understand what mistakes
they made, but also why they made these
mistakes and what they can do to avoid them
the next time. These results are in line with claims
of Hattie and Timperley (2007) who assume
forms of feedback “most useful when they assist
students in rejecting erroneous hypotheses and
provide direction for searching and strategizing”
(pp. 91–92).
Findings by Biber et al. (2011) that written
feedback is more effective than oral feedback
could not be confirmed. Although there is a
tendency of our results pointing in a similar
direction, the feedback channel proved to be a
Still, it is legitimate to aggregate heterogenous data is more effective, the more information it contains
in order to make general statements, but it has and research on estimating this information would
to be kept in mind that these statements are be a valuable addition to the area. Developing
often the first step to later understanding the models, such as the Hattie and Timperley (2007)
critical moderators. The Visible Learning model, would also advance the research, as such
research aimed to develop, present and defend a models provide a more nuanced view of feedback,
set of propositions and a story about not only aims to include the moderators, and can be refuted.
the mean effects of many influences on student Estimates of the effects of feedback range
achievement but the variability of these means. between 0.48 (this meta-analysis), 0.70 (Hattie,
As Hattie and Clarke (2018) have recently stated, 2009), and 0.79 (Hattie and Timperley, 2007) but
a danger lies in over-simplifications, simply using the pursuit of seeking the optimal moderators is the
average effect sizes, and ignoring the variability core business of future research. Feedback, on
across many studies, influences, contexts, and average, is powerful, but some feedback is more
moderators. powerful.
In this study, we used a random-effects/mixed-
effects model to deal with heterogeneity of effect
sizes and accounted for non- independence of AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
study effects by RVE. Raudenbush and Bryk
(1985) have recommended that the use of BW developed the idea for this manuscript and the
hierarchical linear modeling may be more optimal methodological framework and performed the
to account for the nesting of studies within a meta- analytic calculations. KZ and JH verified the
analysis and this would improve the fidelity of the analytical methods and supervised the findings of
estimation. this work. All authors discussed the results and
contributed to the final manuscript.
CONCLUSION
Notwithstanding, there has been a long search for SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
the optimal measures of central tendency – and we
have added another approach to better understand The Supplementary Material for this article can
the power of feedback. be found online at:
Feedback must be recognized as a complex and https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.
differentiated construct that includes many 2019.03087/full#supplementary-material
different forms with, at times, quite different
effects on student learning. Most importantly,
feedback
Cheung, M. W. L., Ho, R. C., Lim, Y., and Mak, A. (2012). Conducting a meta- Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-
analysis: basics and good practices. Int. J. Rheum. Dis. 15, 129–135. doi: 10.1111/ analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br. Med. J. 315, 629–634. doi:
j.1756-185X.2012.01712.x 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, Getsie, R. L., Langer, P., and Glass, G. V. (1985). Meta-analysis of the effects of type
NJ: Erlbaum. and combination of feedback on children’s discrimination learning. Rev. Educ.
Res. 55, 9–22. doi: 10.3102/00346543055001009
Deeks, J. J., Higgins, J. P., and Altman, D. G. (2008). “Analysing data and
undertaking meta-analyses,” in Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Gleser, L. J., and Olkin, I. (2009). “Stochastically dependent effect sizes,” in The
handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis, eds H. Cooper, L. V. Hedges,
and J. C. Valentine, (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 357–376.
Graham, S., Hebert, M., and Harris, K. R. (2015). Formative assessment and
writing. Elem. Sch. J. 115, 523–547.
Hattie, J., and Clarke, S. (2018). Visible Learning: Feedback. London: Routledge,
doi: 10.4324/9780429485480
Hattie, J., and Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Rev. Educ. Res. 77,
81–112. doi: 10.3102/003465430298487
Hattie, J., and Zierer, K. (2019). Visible Learning Insights. London: Routledge, doi:
10.4324/9781351002226
Hedges, L. V., and Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis. San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., and Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in
meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Res. Synth. Methods 1,
39–65. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.5
Hunter, J. E., and Schmidt, F. L. (2000). Fixed effects vs. random effects meta-
analysis models: implications for cumulative research knowledge. Int. J. Sel.
Assess. 8, 275–292. doi: 10.1111/1468-2389.00156
Kang, E., and Han, Z. (2015). The efficacy of written corrective feedback in improving L2 written accuracy: a meta-analysis. Modern Lang. J. 99, 1–
18. doi: 10.1111/modl.12189
Kluger, A. N., and DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions on performance: a historical review, a meta-analysis, and a preliminary
feedback intervention theory. Psychol. Bull. 119, 254–284. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.
254
Krippendorff, K. (2004). Reliability in content analysis: some common misconceptions and recommendations. Hum. Commun. Res. 30, 411–433.
doi: 10.1093/hcr/30.3.411
Kulik, J. A., and Kulik, C.-L. C. (1988). Timing of feedback and verbal learning.
L’Hommedieu, R., Menges, R. J., and Brinko, K. T. (1990). Methodological explanations for the modest effects of feedback from student ratings. J.
Educ. Psychol. 82, 232–241. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.232
Light, R. J., and Pillemer, D. B. (1986). Summing Up: The Science of Reviewing Research. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lysakowski, R. S., and Walberg, H. J. (1981). Classroom reinforcement and learning: a quantitative synthesis. J. Educ. Res. 75, 69–77. doi: 10.1080/
00220671.1981.10885359
Lyster, R., and Saito, K. (2010). Oral feedback in classroom SLA: a meta-analysis.
Madsen, C. K., Dorow, L. G., Moore, R. S., and Womble, J. U. (1976). Effect of music via television as reinforcement for correct mathematics. J. Res.
Music Educ. 24, 51–59. doi: 10.2307/3344936
Madsen, C. K., Moore, R. S., Wagner, M. J., and Yarbrough, C. (1975). A comparison of music as reinforcement for correct mathematical
responses versus music as reinforcement for attentiveness. J. Music Ther. 12, 84–95. doi: 10.1093/jmt/12.2.84
Marsh, H. W. (1987). Students’ evaluations of university teaching: research findings, methodological issues, and directions for future research. Int.
J. Educ. Res. 11, 253–388. doi: 10.1016/0883-0355(87)90001-2
Menges, R. J., and Brinko, K. T. (1986). “Effects of student evaluation feedback: a meta-analysis of higher education research,” in Paper Presented
at 70th Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA,
Miller, P. C., and Pan, W. (2012). Recasts in the L2 classroom: a meta-analytic review. Int. J. Educ. Res. 56, 48–59. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2012.07.002
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA
statement. Ann. Int. Med. 151, 264–269. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed1000097
Morris, S. B., and DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta- analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups
designs. Psychol. Methods 7, 105–125. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.105
Neubert, M. J. (1998). The value of feedback and goal setting over goal setting alone and potential moderators of this effect: a meta-analysis.
Hum. Perform. 11, 321–335. doi: 10.1207/s15327043hup1104_2
Rummel, A., and Feinberg, R. (1988). Cognitive evaluation theory: a meta-analytic review of the literature. Soc. Behav. Person. 16, 147–164. doi:
10.2224/sbp.1988. 16.2.147
Russel, J. R., and Spada, N. (2006). 4. The effectiveness of corrective feedback for the acquisition of L2 grammar. Lang. Learn. Lang. Teach. 13,
133–164. doi: 10.1075/lllt.13.09val
Rustenbach, S. J. (2003). “Metaanalyse - Eine anwendungsorientierte Einführung [Metaanalysis - An application-oriented introduction],” in
Methoden der Psychologie [Methods of Psychology], Vol. 16, ed. K. Pawlik, (Bern: Huber).
Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 54–67. doi:
10.1006/ ceps.1999.1020
Sadler, D. R. (1989). Formative assessment and the design of instructional systems.
Schimmel, B. J. (1983). “A meta-analysis of feedback to learners in computerized and programmed instruction,” in Paper Presented at the Annual
Meeting of the American Educational Research Association Montreal, Canada.
Shadish, W. R., and Haddock, C. K. (1994). “Combining estimates of effect size,” in The Handbook of Research Synthesis, eds H. Cooper, and L. V.
Hedges, (New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation), 261–281.
Sidik, K., and Jonkman, J. N. (2006). Robust variance estimation for random effects meta-analysis. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 50, 3681–3701. doi:
10.1016/j.csda. 2005.07.019
Slavin, R. E. (2008). Perspectives on evidence-based research in education—what works? Issues in synthesizing educational program evaluations.
Educ. Res. 37, 5–14. doi: 10.3102/0013189x08314117
Standley, J. M. (1996). A meta-analysis on the effects of music as reinforcement for education/therapy objectives. J. Res. Music Educ. 44, 105–133.
doi: 10.2307/ 3345665
Swanson, H. L., and Lussier, C. M. (2001). A selective synthesis of the experimental literature on dynamic assessment. Rev. Educ. Res. 71, 321–363.
doi: 10.3102/ 00346543071002321
Tenenbaum, G., and Goldring, E. (1989). A meta-analysis of the effect of enhanced instruction: cues, participation, reinforcement and feedback
and correctives on motor skill learning. J. Res. Dev. Educ. 22, 53–64.
Travlos, A. K., and Pratt, J. (1995). Temporal locus of knowledge of results: a meta-analytic review. Percept. Mot. Skills 80, 3–14. doi:
10.2466/pms.1995. 80.1.3
Truscott, J. (2007). The effect of error correction on learners’ ability to write accurately. J. Sec. Lang. Writ. 16, 255–272. doi:
10.1016/j.jslw.2007. 06.003
Uttl, B., White, C. A., and Gonzalez, D. W. (2017). Meta-analysis of faculty’s teaching effectiveness: Student evaluation of teaching ratings and
student learning are not related. Stud. Educ. Eval. 54, 22–42. doi: 10.1016/j.stueduc. 2016.08.007
van der Kleij, F. M., Feskens, R. C., and Eggen, T. J. (2015). Effects of feedback in a computer-based learning environment on students’
learning outcomes: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 85, 475–511. doi: 10.3102/00346543145 64881
Viechtbauer, W. (2007). Accounting for heterogeneity via random-effects models and moderator analyses in meta-analysis. Zeitschrift für
Psychologie/J. Psychol. 215, 104–121. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.215.2.104
Walberg, H. J. (1982). What makes schooling effective? Contemp. Educ. Rev. 1, 1–34.
Wecker, C., Vogel, F., and Hetmanek, A. (2017). Visionär und imposant–aber auch belastbar?. [Visionary and impressive – but also reliable?].
Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft 20, 21–40. doi: 10.1007/s11618-016-0696-0
Wiersma, U. J. (1992). The effects of extrinsic rewards in intrinsic motivation: a meta-analysis. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 65, 101–114. doi:
10.1111/j.2044-8325. 1992.tb00488.x
Wilkinson, S. S. (1981). The relationship of teacher praise and student achievement: a meta-analysis of selected research. Diss. Abstr. Int.
41:3998A.
Witt, P. L., Wheeless, L. R., and Allen, M. (2004). A meta-analytical review of the relationship between teacher immediacy and
student learning. Commun. Monogr. 71, 184–207. doi: 10.1080/036452042000 228054
Yeany, R. H., and Miller, P. (1983). Effects of diagnostic/remedial instruction on science learning: a meta analysis. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 20, 19–26. doi:
10.1002/tea. 3660200103
Conflict of Interest: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2020 Wisniewski, Zierer and Hattie. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution
License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are
credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is
permitted which does not comply with these terms.