Consequenceof Greenwashing
Consequenceof Greenwashing
Consequenceof Greenwashing
net/publication/296700585
CITATIONS READS
117 35,248
2 authors:
All content following this page was uploaded by Hendy Mustiko Aji on 04 March 2016.
ABSTRACT
In a review of more than 1,000 self-described “green” or eco-friendly products,
one organization [TerraChoice, 2010] found that all but one of the products
exhibited some form of greenwashing. “Greenwashing” is a type of spin in
which public relations or marketing is used deceptively to promote the perception
that a company and its products or services are environmentally safe or
“friendly.” This study examined the construct of perceived consumer skepticism
as the extended consequence of greenwashing, thus extending the study by
Chang and Chen [2013], which examined the link between greenwashing and
green trust, with a view to the extended and final consequences. The authors of
the current study formulated 10 hypotheses, developed a structural model with
six variables, and tested the relationships in the model using a purposive
sampling technique that involved an online and offline survey of a sample of
green consumers in Yogyakarta, Indonesia. This study found that greenwashing
has a positive association with green consumer skepticism (GCC), perceived
consumer skepticism (PCS), and green perceived risk (GPR). Furthermore, the
study found a surprising link between GCC-PCS-GPR and green trust (GT). The
study also discussed the practical implication of these findings and offers
suggestions for future research.
1. INTRODUCTION
Environmental issues have become increasingly popular among consumers
worldwide and are a popular topic for research and discussion in academic circles
and the industrial sector. Rising concerns about global warming have made
consumers even more conscious of environmental issues [Chen, 2008]. With
increased consumer interest in environmentally friendly products and services,
manufacturers have devoted considerable effort to the marketing and sales of so-
called “green” products [Bhatia and Jain, 2013]. The popularly of green products
has caused manufacturers to adopt eco-friendly practices that affect not only the
production process, but also the final product itself [Kivimaa and Kauto, 2010].
To sell these products, manufacturers create advertising that makes eco-friendly
or green claims in order to target consumers who lead a green lifestyle [Divine
and Lepisto, 2005]. Green marketing is viewed as the best concept and strategy
to respond to market needs and wants.
Many environmental claims focusing on the green (environmental) attribute,
however, are ambiguous and deceptive [Chen and Chang, 2012].
Environmentalists and some consumers are crying foul, saying that many
companies are making their products out to be greener than they really are [Hsu,
2011]. Of the more than 1,000 self-declared green products reviewed by
TerraChoice [2010], as cited by Lane [2012], it was found that all but one
exhibited some form of greenwashing. “Greenwashing” is a type of spin in which
public relations, advertising, or marketing is used deceptively to create the
perception that a product or service is “green.” This practice has given some
consumers the negative intent to purchase these products or services [McGrath,
1992; Newell et al., 1998). The practice also increases consumer confusion when
it comes to purchasing products with environmental features.
The objective of this study is to extend the study by Chen and Chang [2013]
by proposing perceived consumer skepticism (PCS) and switching intention as
the extended consequences of greenwashing. Chen and Chang [2013] examined
the direct relationship connecting greenwashing to green trust (GT), and the
indirect relationship between greenwashing and green trust (GT), with green
consumer confusion (GCC) and green perceived risk (GPR) as mediating
variables. All hypotheses in their study were supported, but there are gaps in the
study that can be filled.
Established theory of advertising and switching behavior implies that
consumers tend to be skeptical of ads, especially green ads, and that consumers
may have the intention to switch if they are ethically injured. Consumers are
particularly skeptical of advertising that proclaims the “greenness” of a product.
In academic literature, this behavior is known as “perceived consumer
skepticism” (PCS). The construct, which was introduced by Mohr, Eroglu, and
Ellen [1998], refers to the tendency among consumers to disbelieve
environmental claims made in advertising [Matthes and Wonneberger, 2014].
The problem is a serious one because consumers are indeed skeptical of green
claims [Sheehan and Atkinson, 2012] that are motivated by profit [Albayrak,
Cabeer, Moutinho, and Herstein, 2011].
Moreover, Keaveney [1995] has shown that the ethical problems caused by
greenwashing may lead to switching intention and later to switching behavior.
Chen and Chang’s [2013] study did not include the problem of perceived
consumer skepticism (PCS) nor the problem of switching intention. The current
study takes these two problems into account, thus distinguishing the current
research from the work by Chen and Chang [2013]. It is hoped that this empirical
study will provide a theoretical contribution to academic literature on the subject
of greenwashing.
2. LITERATURE REVIEW
In their book, Kottler and Keller [2012] said that the starting point of a
business is not the company but the market. In order to survive competition,
therefore, a company has to adjust its vision and mission to match what the
market needs and wants. Since today’s consumers are more conscious of the
environment, they make a significant effort to buy eco-friendly products and
services [Roberts, 1996; Kalafatis et al., 1999]. Green marketing targets “green”
consumers by purportedly making “eco-friendly,” “environmentally friendly”
products. Before specifically explaining the variables observed, we offer the
following definition and brief explanation of green marketing concepts. Once one
grasps the definition and concept of green marketing, it is easy to understand
greenwashing.
2.1. Green Marketing
It all began on Earth Day 1990 when millions of people around globe
gathered in their communities to protest the rapidly declining health of the planet
[Gallicano, 2011]. Since then, public concern about environmental issues has
increased steadily [Choi, 2005, cited from Kaufman et al., 2012]. As a result,
2.2. Greenwashing
“Greenwashing” is defined as the act of misleading consumers regarding
the environmental practices of a company or the environmental benefits of a
product or service [Greenpeace, n.d]. Some scholars define the term as the
intentional misrepresentation of a firm’s environmental efforts (or the lack
thereof) [Alves, 2009, 2011; and Furlow, 2010]. A few years ago, consumers
began using social media to campaign against greenwashing practices. In
response, TerraChoice [2009] identified seven company “sins” with regard to
misleading advertisements for green products:
6. Sin of the lesser of two evils. This is a claim that may be true within the
product category, but may distract the consumer from the greater
environmental impact of the category as a whole.
7. Sin of fibbing. This includes environmental claims that are simply false.
perception is the notion that the environmental campaign is just a part of the
company’s marketing strategy. Another perception is that, in advertising green
products, the company is not motivated purely by environmental concerns, but
rather by profit orientation. Whatever the case, marketers must strive to create
consumer perceptions that are consistently positive because perceptions of
greenwashing can damage the consumer’s attitude toward a company [Peattie et
al., 2009]. Ultimately, the perceptions generated by misleading ads may destroy
the market by causing consumers to be suspicious of green products [Polonsky et
al., 2010; Chen and Chang, 2012].
There are three types of green consumer confusion, as categorized by
Mitchell, Walsh, and Yamin [2005]: (1) unclarity confusion, (2) similarity
confusion, and (3) overload confusion. Unclarity confusion is defined as a lack
of understanding, in which consumers are forced to re-evaluate their current
beliefs about a product. This type of confusion may be caused by technological
complexity, ambiguous information or dubious product claims, conflicting
information, or incorrect interpretation [Mitchell, Walsh, and Yamin, 2005].
Similarity confusion is the potential alteration of a consumer’s choice or an
incorrect brand evaluation caused by the perceived physical similarity of
products or services [Mitchell, Walsh, and Yamin, 2005]. Overload confusion is
caused by a too much decision-relevant information regarding the choice of
brands. With such a vast quantity of information available, it may be difficult for
consumers to focus on the vital points, thus causing confusion [Mitchell, Walsh,
and Yamin, 2005].
Connecting the three types of confusion to the topic, greenwashing
would overload consumers with information and could make it more difficult for
them to evaluate the product [Walsh et al., 2007]. Ambiguous and deceptive
green claims may create confusion among consumers with regard to its
environmental features. We hypothesized, therefore, that there is a positive
relationship between greenwashing and green consumer confusion.
All of these risks are associated with, and are relevant to, the greenwashing issue.
Assael [2004] also identified several factors associated with perceived risk:
• Consumers are highly involved with the product.
• There is little information about the product category.
• The product is new.
• The product is technologically complex.
• Consumers have little self-confidence in evaluating brands.
• There are variations in quality among brands.
• The price is high.
• The purchase is important to consumers.
[Valvi and West, 2012]. To some extent, customers may disconfirm their
expectation because of negative perceptions. In the context of environmental
concern, these perceptions are associated with green product advertising.
From the standpoint of negative perceptions, perceived risk is related not
only to the environment itself, but also to the consumer’s physical body. Peter
and Ryan [1976] defined perceived risk as perception that is connected with the
possible consequences of a wrong decision. “Green perceived risk” is the
perception that is connected with the possible consequences of a wrong decision
with regard to environmental performance. Chen and Chang [2013] defined the
term as the expectation of negative environmental consequences associated with
purchase behavior. Perceived risk is a combination of negative consequences and
uncertainty. Consequently, the assessment of perceived risk would affect a
consumer’s purchase decision [Peter and Ryan, 1976; Chen and Chang, 2012]. It
would also influence consumer attitudes [Mitchell, 1999]. The risk that
consumers face is, to some extent, noticed and felt more strongly than the benefit
they gain. This view is consistent with the theory that consumers are keen to
minimize the perceived risk rather than to maximize their utility [Mitchell, 1999].
The level of perceived risk would affect a consumer’s decision making
about whether to trust or distrust [Harridge-March, 2006; Chen and Chang,
2012]. If consumers feel high risk toward a product or brand, they would not trust
the product or brand [Mitchell, 1999]. This logic is supported by several
researchers who found that the higher the risk perceived by consumers, the lower
their trust in the product or brand associated with green claims [Mitchell, 1999;
Warrington et al., 2000; Corritore et al., 2003; Harridge-March, 2006; Gillespie,
2008; Eid, 2011; Chen and Chang, 2012). Therefore, we hypothesized:
[Kalafatis et al., 1999]. Chang [1998] also proved that the TPB can predict
unethical behavior. In the following paragraphs, we examine the relationship
between green consumer confusion (GCC)-perceived consumer skepticism
(GCS)-green perceived risk (GPR) and customer intention to switch from green
products to non-green products.
Table 1
Operational Definition of Variables
Table 2
List of Items for Each Variable
-continued
Table 2 (Cont’d)
List of Items for Each Variable
5. Green Trust [Chen and Chang, 2013, from Chen and Chang 2012]
1. You feel that this product’s environmental reputation is generally reliable.
2. You feel that this products environmental performance is generally dependable.
3. You feel that this product’s environmental claims are generally trustworthy.
4. This product’s environmental concern meets your expectation.
5. This product keeps promises and commitment for environmental protection.
Table 3
Results of the Independent t-Test
Means Std. Deviation Sig.
Variables Online Offline Online Offline (2-tailed)
Greenwashing 3.62 3.43 0.83 0.73 0.19NS
GCC 3.69 3.46 0.96 0.77 0.42NS
GPR 3.72 3.62 0.72 0.66 0.15NS
GT 3.59 3.44 0.78 0.62 0.25NS
PCS 2.48 2.78 0.81 0.64 0.02*
SI 3.39 3.06 0.90 1.08 0.80NS
Note: * p-value > 0.01; NS = not significant
Table 4
Results of Non-Response Bias Test
Means Std. Deviation
Sig.
Variables NR R NR R
(2-tailed)
Greenwashing 3.52 3.44 0.74 0.73 0.45NS
Offline
Table 5
Correlations of Constructs
Var. Mean Std. GW GCC PCS GPR GT SI
Dev.
GW 3.49 0.76 1
GCC 3.65 0.67 0.42** 1
PCS 3.53 0.83 0.57** 0.43** 1
GPR 3.49 0.67 0.45** 0.48** 0.51** 1
GT 2.70 0.71 -0.48** -0.34** -0.50** -0.48** 1
SI 3.16 0.99 0.30** 0.28** 0.38** 0.27** -0.40** 1
Note: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01
We ran data from the 134 questionnaires that were returned in order to
test the reliability and confirmatory factor analysis. There were 5 items for
greenwashing; 5 items for green consumer confusion; 3 items for perceived
consumer skepticism; 5 items for green trust; and 2 items for switching intention.
All of the items had a Cronbach’s alpha greater than the required score of 0.60,
which indicated that the items in this study were reliable. All items were then
simultaneously tested to check loading factors in CFA. It was found that 1 item
in green consumer confusion and 1 item in green perceived risk had scores that
were too low. Those two items were deleted from the model. The analysis
results are presented in Table 6.
Table 6
Cronbach’s Alpha and Standardized Regression Weight Scores for Variables
Cronbach’s
Variables Items Loadings Alpha
Score
Greenwashing GW1 0.764 0.847
GW2 0.741
GW3 0.712
GW4 0.716
GW5 0.696
Green Consumer Confusion GCC2 0.636 0.799
GCC3 0.713
GCC4 0.700
GCC5 0.756
Perceived Consumer Skepticism PCS1 0.881 0.794
PCS2 0.708
PCS3 0.691
Green Perceived Risk GPR1 0.730 0.757
GPR2 0.708
GPR3 0.536
GPR4 0.622
Green Trust GT1 0.635 0.815
GT2 0.744
GT3 0.661
GT4 0.671
GT5 0.727
Switching Intention SI1 0.930 0.819
SI2 0.748
proposed hypotheses in this study. Because four paths were not significant,
however, one can conclude that four hypotheses were not supported.
Table 7
Summary of Results for the Structural Model
Hypothesis Proposed Path Results
Effect Coefficient
H1 (+) 0.62** H1 is supported.
H2 (+) 0.73** H2 is supported.
H3 (+) 0.67** H3 is supported.
H4 (-) -0.12 H4 is not supported.
H5 (-) -0.40* H5 is supported.
H6 (-) -0.29* H6 is supported.
H7 (+) 0.14 H7 is not supported.
H8 (+) 0.23 H8 is not supported.
H9 (+) -0.01 H9 is not supported.
H10 (-) -0.28* H10 is supported.
Note: **p<0.01, *p<0.05
skepticism, and green perceived risk). Yet, because these three constructs had a
significant relationship to green trust, it could be implied that green trust
mediated the link of those three constructs to switching intention.
To enrich the discussion about the structural model, we then conducted
several path tests to compare the model fit of the original model with that of the
new models. We also conducted the test to determine the relationship between
GCC and GPS as well as between PCS and GPR.
Path Test Model (1)
The first path test found that the new path addition connecting GCC to
PCS was not significant, with a regression weight of 0.06 (p-value >
0.05). The relationships between new path GCC and GPR and between
new path GPR and PCS, however, were significant. The remaining path
scores did not differ from those of the original model. The GFI (0.837)
for path test model (1) was slightly better than that for the original model
(0.832). The results for path test model (1) are depicted in Figure 3 and
are summarized in Table 8.
Table 8
Summary Results for Path Test Model (1)
Table 9
Summary Results for Path Test Model (2)
Table 10
Summary Results for Path Test Model (3)
Table 11
Summary Results Path Test Model (4)
Table 12
Regression Score Comparison Between Original Model and Models 1-4
Table 13
Model Fit Comparison between Original Model and Models 1-4
Model Fit
Original Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Model
CMIN/DF 1.481 1.423 1.477 1.473 1.488
GFI 0.832 0.837 0.831 0.831 0.830
CFI 0.914 0.925 0.915 0.915 0.912
RMSEA 0.060 0.056 0.060 0.060 0.061
4.5. Discussion
In our measurement model, not all items for GCC and GPR were used.
Because of a loading issue, we excluded the first item for GCC, which stated, It
is difficult to detect the products in terms of environmental features, and we
excluded the last item for GPR, which stated, Using this product would damage
your green reputation or image. Both items did not group into specified factors.
The first item for GCC converged in the GPR column, and last item for GPR
converged in the PCS column. As stated earlier, we think that personal image or
reputation with respect to green products is not a major concern in developing
countries like Indonesia. Our result differed from that of Chen and Chang [2013],
which found no problems with any of the items for GCC and GPR. It should be
noted, however, that Chen and Chang [2013] conducted their study in Taiwan,
where green marketing is perhaps practiced more formally.
In our structural model, which correlates all variables, we found that H4,
H7, H8, and H9 were not supported. We found it surprising that H4 (green
consumer confusion is negatively associated with green trust) was not supported.
This finding is in contrast to theory and the findings of previous studies. The
theory states that, when consumers are confused by misleading and unclear
advertising or messages, the situation will raise their suspicions and undermine
their trust. Furthermore, Mitchell and Papavassiliou [1999] stated that consumers
are reluctant to trust a product if they are confused about the product. Studies
conducted by Kalafatis and Pollard [1999] and Chen and Chang [2013] also
confirmed that GCC was negatively associated with GT. Although the
relationship was not significant, the direction was negative, providing support for
previous studies. We may argue that the relationship between GCC and GT was
not direct. There may be another variable that would help to explain it. Since
green products offer several values specifically concerning environmental safety
and health, consumers do not directly distrust them. Perhaps, green perceived
value may be the mediating variable.
Hypotheses 7-9 relate GCC, PCS, and GPR to switching intention. In the
current study, we connected those variables to switching intention. This
approach has not yet been considered in greenwashing literature, perhaps because
of the theory that consumers may switch if they are confused or skeptical or
perceive risks associated with green products. This study found that the three
variables did not have a significant relationship to switching intention. It can be
inferred that, even though consumers are confused or skeptical and perceive
certain risks associated with green products, they do not easily decide to switch
to non-green products. Figure 2, shown earlier, indicated that the relationship
between PCS and GPR was fully mediated by GT. This result may therefore be a
significant contribution to greenwashing literature with regard to considering the
switching intention variable in relationship to GT. In this context, the exception
was made for GCC, which did not have a direct effect on switching intention, as
well as an indirect effect via GT. As stated earlier, perhaps the insignificant
relationship can be explained by value perception (green perceived value).
4.6. Limitations and Future Research Direction
In this study, we conducted four pre-surveys to test items included in our
questionnaire. During the first and second pre-survey trials, many respondents
were confused about the questions, mostly because they were unfamiliar with
green product features, and also because we operationally defined greenwashing
in a very general way, when in fact greenwashing may be found in several
industries. Both factors were limitations. Respondents’ understanding improved
for the third and fourth pre-survey trials. With respect to the limitations of the
current study, we offer these comments and suggestions for future research
direction:
seems that Indonesian consumers still perceived several values that can be gained
by purchasing and consuming green products. We therefore recommend that
future researchers consider green perceived value as the variable mediating the
relationship between GCC and GT. In this way, the current study makes a
theoretical contribution to greenwashing literature and also contributes the
measurement model discussed earlier. Following are the implications for
companies in all sectors:
The last factor, in particular, makes Indonesia a good potential location for
future research on greenwashing. Regardless of the country in which future
research is conducted, researchers should clearly specify the industry in which
they plan to scrutinize green products; they should test mediating variables as
discussed in this study; and they should test causal relationship by conducting an
experimental survey.
We hope that the research results of this study will be useful to managers,
practitioners, academicians, and other researchers, and that by serving as a
reference, it will make a significant contribution to future research.
REFERENCES
Ajzen, I. 1991. The theory of planned behavior, Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Process 50, 179-211.
Albayrak, T.; M. Caber; L. Moutinho; and R. Herstein. 2011. The influence of
skepticism on green purchase behavior, International Journal of Business and Social
Science 2(13), 189-197
Alves, I.M. 2009. Green spin everywhere: How greenwashing reveals the limit of the
CSR paradigm, Journal of Global Change and Governance 2(1), 1-26.
Anuar, M.M.; K. Omar; and O. Mohammad. 2013. Does skepticism influence
consumers’ intention to purchase cause-related products? International Journal of
Business and Social Science 4(5), 94-98.
Assael, H. 2004. Consumer Behavior: A Strategic Approach, New York: Charles
Hartford.
Bhatia, M., and A. Jain. 2013. Green marketing: A study of consumer perception and
preferences in India, Electronic Green Journal 1(36).
Boush, D.M.; F. Marian; and G.M. Rose. 1994. Adolescent skepticism toward TV
advertising and knowledge of advertiser tactics, Journal of Consumer Research
21(1), 165-175.
Chen, Y.S. 2008. The driver of green innovation and green image—green core
competence, Journal of Business Ethics 81(3), 531-543.
Chen, Y.S. 2010. The drivers of green brand equity: Green brand image, green
satisfaction, and green trust, Journal of Business Ethics 93(2), 307-19.
Chen, Y.S., and C.H. Chang. 2012. Enhance green purchase intentions: The roles of
green perceived value, green perceived risk, and green trust, Management
Decision 50(3), 502-520.
Chen, Y.S., and C.H. Chang. 2013. Greenwash and green trust: The mediation effects
on green consumer confusion and perceived risk, Journal of Business Ethics 114,
489-500.
Chang, M.K. 1998. Predicting unethical behavior: A comparison of the theory of
reasoned action and the theory of planned behavior, Journal of Business Ethics 17,
1825-1834.
Chang, H.H. and S.W. Chen. 2008. The impact of online store environment cues on
purchase intention: Trust and perceived risk as a mediator, Online Information
Review 32 (6), 818-41.
Cohen, D. 1974. The concept of unfairness as it relates to advertising, Journal of
Marketing 38, pp. 8-13.
Corritore, C.L.; B. Kracher; and S. Wiedenbeck. 2003. On-line trust: concepts, evolving
themes, a model, International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 58(6), 737-58.
Cox, D.F. 1967. Risk handling in consumer behavior: An intense study of two cases. In
D.F. Cox (ed.), Risk Taking and Information Handling in Consumer Behavior,
Boston: Harvard University.
Darley, W.K., and R.E. Smith. 1993. Advertising claim objectivity: Antecedents and
effects, Journal of Marketing 57 (4), 100-13.
Divine, R.L., and L. Lepisto. 2005. Analysis of the healthy lifestyle consumer, Journal
of Consumer Marketing 22 (5), 275-283.
Eid, M.I. 2011. Determinants of e-commerce customer satisfaction, trust, and loyalty in
Saudi Arabia, Journal of Electronic Commerce Research 12(1), 78-93.
Ford, G.T.; D.B. Smith; and J.L. Swasy. 1990. Consumer skepticism of advertising
claims: Testing hypotheses from economics of information, Journal of Consumer
Research 16 (4), 433-41.
Furlow, N.E. 2010. Greenwashing in the new millennium, The Journal of Applied
Business and Economics 10(6), 22-25.
Gallicano, T.D. 2011. A critical analysis of greenwashing claims, Public Relations
Journal 5(3).
Ganesan, S. 1994. Determinants of long-term orientation in buyer-seller relationships,
Journal of Marketing 58 (2), 1-19.
Gillespie, E. 2008. Stemming the tide of greenwash, Consumer Policy Review 18(3), 79–
83.
Gul, M.C. 2013. Long-term orientation, perceived consumer effectiveness, and
environmentally conscious consumer behavior: The case of Turkey, International
Journal of Marketing Studies 5(5), 24-30.
Harridge-March, S. 2006. Can the building of trust overcome consumer perceived risk
online? Marketing Intelligence & Planning 24(7),746–761.
Hart, P., and C. Saunders. 1997. Power and trust: Critical factors in the adoption and use
of electronic data interchange, Organizational Science 8(1), 23-42.
Hsu, T. 2011. Skepticism grows over products touted as eco-friendly, media release, 21
May, Los Angeles Times, <http://articles.latimes.com/2011/may/21/business/la-fi-
greenwash-20110521>, viewed 28 November 2014.
Kalafatis, S.P.; M. Pollard; R. East; and M.H. Tsogas. 1999. Green marketing and
Ajzen’s theory of planned behavior: A cross-market examination, Journal of
Consumer Marketing 16(5), 441-460.
Kaufman, H.R.; M.F. Panni; and Y. Orphanidou. 2012. Factors affecting green
purchasing behavior: An integrated conceptual framework, Amfiteatru Economic
16(31), 50-69.
Keaveney, S M. 1995. Customer switching behavior in service industries, Journal of
Marketing 59 (2), 71-82.
Kim, Y.; and S.R. Choi. 2005. Antecedents of green purchase behavior: An examination
of collectivism, environmental concern and PCE, Advance in Consumer Research
32(1), 592-599.
Kivimaa, P., and P. Kauto. 2010. Making or breaking environmental innovation?
Technological change and innovation in markets in the pulp and paper industry,
Management Research Review 33 (4), 289-305.
Kohl, M.F. 1991. Good Earth Art. Bellingham: Bright Right Publishing.
Kotler, P.; and K.L. Keller. 2012. Marketing Management, 14th Edition, E. Pearson
Education, Inc., Prentice Hall.
Lane, E.L. 2012. Green marketing goes negative: The advent of reverse greenwashing,
European Journal of Risk and Regulation (4), pp. 582-588.
Laufer, W.S. 2003. Social accountability and corporate greenwashing, Journal of
Business Ethics 43(3), 253–261.
Mangleburg, T F., and T. Bristol. 1998. Socialization and adolescents' skepticism
toward advertising, Journal of Advertising 27(3), 11-21.
Manju. 2012. Green Marketing: New Hopes and Challenges: Spectrum.
Matthes, J., and A. Wonneberger. 2014. The skeptical green consumer revisited:
Testing the relationship between green consumerism and skepticism toward
advertising, Journal of Advertising 43(2), 115-127.
McGrath, A.J. 1992. Marketin’ of the green. Sales and Marketing Management, 31-32.
Meyers-Levy, J., and P. Malaviya. 1999. Consumers’ processing of persuasive
advertisements: An integrative framework of persuasion theories, Journal of
Marketing 63, pp. 45-60 (special issue).
Mitchell, V.W. 1999. Understanding consumers’ behavior: Can perceived risk theory
help? Management Decision 30(3), 26-31.
Mitchell, V.W., and V. Papavassiliou. 1999. Marketing causes and implications of
consumer confusion, Joumal of Product & Brand Management 8(4), 319-339.
Mitchell, V.M.; G. Walsh; and M. Yamin. 2005. Towards a conceptual model of
consumer confusion, Advances in Consumer Research 32, pp. 143-150.
Mohanasundaram. 2012. Green marketing-challenges and opportunities, International
Journal of Multidisciplinary Research 2(4), 66-73
Mohr, L.A.; D. Eroglu; and S.P. Ellen. 1998. The development and testing of a
measure of skepticism toward environment claims in the marketers’
communications, The Journal of Consumers Affairs 32(1), 30-55.
Newell, S.J.; R.E. Goldsmith; and E.J. Banzhaf. 1998. The effects of misleading
environmental claims on consumer perception of advertisements, Journal of
Marketing Theory and Practice, pp. 48-60.
Nimako, S.G.; B.A. Ntim; and A.F. Mensah. 2014. Effect of mobile number
portability adoption on consumer switching intention, International Journal of
Marketing Studies 6(2), 117-134.
Obermiller, C.; E.R. Spangenberg; and D.L. MacLachlan. 2005. Ad skepticism,
Journal of Advertising 34(3), 7-17.
Ottman, J.A.; E.R. Stafford; and C.L. Hartman. 2006. Avoiding green marketing
myopia: Ways to improve consumer appeal for environmentally preferable
products, Environment 48(5), 22-36.
Peattie, K.; S. Peattie; and C. Ponting. 2009. Climate change: A social and commercial
marketing communication challenge, EuroMed Journal of Business 4(3), 270-286.
Peter, J.P., and M.J. Ryan. 1976. An investigation of perceived risk at the brand level,
Journal of Marketing Research 13(2), 184-189.
Hendy Mustiko Aji is an M.Sc. student in the Faculty of Economics and Business,
Universitas Gadjah Mada, Indonesia. He obtained his bachelor of international
business management degree from Universiti Utara Malaysia (2011), and his
bachelor of management degree from Universitas Jenderal Soedirman-Indonesia
(2012). He has research interest in marketing, with a focus on general marketing
theory, green marketing, Islamic marketing, consumer behavior, and strategic
marketing.