TRẦM CẢM LO LẮNG VÀ FOMO
TRẦM CẢM LO LẮNG VÀ FOMO
TRẦM CẢM LO LẮNG VÀ FOMO
Entitled
by
Abigail E. Dempsey
Submitted to the Graduate Faculty as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the
Doctor of Philosophy Degree in Clinical Psychology
by
Abigail E. Dempsey
Submitted to the Graduate Faculty as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the
Doctor of Philosophy Degree in Clinical Psychology
Problematic social networking site (SNS) use has demonstrated associations with
depression and anxiety symptoms throughout the literature; however, less is known about
social factors that may contribute to the development of problematic SNS use, such as
(FoMO). There is an especially prominent gap in the literature regarding problematic use
of various SNSs independently of each other. The present study builds on recent research
findings that FoMO mediates the relationship between problematic SNS use with both
depression/anxiety severity and social belongingness, and that these relationships may be
different depending on the SNS used. We explored how FoMO served as a mediator
between the problematic use of three different SNSs and depression/anxiety and social
belongingness. Data were collected online from an undergraduate university sample and
analyzed using linear regression and mediation analyses. Offline belongingness and
anxiety were related to problematic Twitter use severity, while online belongingness and
anxiety were related to problematic Snapchat use severity. Depression severity was only
associated with problematic Instagram use severity. FoMO accounted for the relations
between anxiety/online belongingness and problematic use severity of all three SNSs.
iii
This dissertation is dedicated to the memory of my stepfather, who always encouraged
me to dream big, and to my nieces and nephews who give me reason to.
Acknowledgments
This endeavor would not have been possible without the support and patience of
knowledge and expertise. Words cannot describe my gratitude to Matthew Tull and Jason
Levine for their encouragement and willingness to listen to all my ideas, sensical and
otherwise.
Special thanks to my family and friends, especially my parents and partner. Their
unwavering belief in me has kept my spirits and motivation high during this process.
None of this would have been possible without their support and sacrifice throughout the
years. I would also like to thank my dear friend and local barista for keeping me inspired
and sufficiently caffeinated and my cats for their entertainment and emotional support.
v
Table of Contents
Abstract iii
Contents vi
List of Figures ix
List of Abbreviations x
1 Literature Review 1
2 Hypotheses 15
3 Methods 18
vi
3.2.3 Smartphone Addiction Scale. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21
4 Analyses 24
5 Results 27
5.3 Mediation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
6 Discussion 31
References 38
A Informed Consent 48
B Proposed Analyses 50
vii
List of Tables
viii
List of Figures
ix
List of Abbreviations
x
Chapter 1
Literature Review
For more than two decades, researchers and lay people alike have become
concerned about problematic social networking site (SNS) use, as use prevalence has
grown, and age of onset has become younger. In the United States, approximately 70% of
adults use some form of SNS; this is a dramatic increase from only 5% in 2005 and 50%
in 2011 (Pew Research Center, 2019). Among users, the majority incorporate SNS use
into their daily routines, with approximately 75% of Facebook users and 63% of
Instagram users visiting the sites at least once per day (Pew Research Center, 2019). With
the ubiquity of smartphones and their continual demands on time and attention
throughout the day, it is unsurprising that SNS sites that enable quick information sharing
literature examining antecedents and consequences of excessive use, the present study
concerns itself with SNS use behaviors. As such, the present study adapts the accepted
use. This inquiry includes adapting measures, such as the Smartphone Addiction Scale
1
(SAS; Kwon, Lee, et al., 2013), to measure the specific SNSs of interest to the present
study.
SNSs and devoting excessive time to SNS use to the extent that it causes impairments in
and/or psychological wellbeing (Andreassen & Pallesen, 2014). Problematic SNS use
and relapse, among other symptoms (Griffiths, 1996, 2005). A review of the literature
reveals evidence that mental health difficulties, such as increased depression and anxiety
symptoms, are associated with problematic SNS use (Andreassen et al., 2016; Shensa et
al., 2017; van Rooij, Mheen, & Schoenmakers, 2017). While some may experience
theory suggest that mental health difficulties lead some people to engage in problematic
SNS use, rather than the other way around. Therefore, new mental health stressors likely
perpetuates problematic SNS use (Brand et al., 2019; Brand, Young, Laier, Wölfling, &
Potenza, 2016).
however, the associations between specific SNS behaviors and mental health remains
largely unexplored. Because of the inherent social nature of humans (Baumeister &
Leary, 1995), it is important to investigate how social factors—both online and offline—
relate to psychopathology and the development and maintenance of problematic SNS use.
While there is evidence to suggest that seeking and obtaining online support is one of
2
many motives that drive individuals to use SNSs (Masur, Reinecke, Ziegele, & Quiring,
2014), some studies have found that problematic SNS use emerged when users spent
excessive time online or became socially isolated (Ceyhan & Ceyhan, 2008; Morrison &
Gore, 2010). Additionally, individuals who use SNSs in a manner that enables perceived
social support and connectedness report less severe symptoms of depression and anxiety
overall, which may act as a protective factor against problematic SNS use (Frison &
These findings in the context of SNSs are more recent, but ample research exists
suggesting that these patterns exist offline as well. In offline contexts, individuals who
feelings of sadness or anger (Williams, 2007), and individuals who lack feelings of social
belongingness in general tend to experience more negative affect than those whose social
belongingness needs are met (Beekman, Stock, & Marcus, 2016; Erzen & Çikrikci, 2018;
Steger & Kashdan, 2009). Overall, these findings suggest that many of the needs and
motives pursued in online interactions imitate their offline analogues. As such, one aim
of the present study was to investigate both online and offline social belongingness as
correlates of mental health difficulties (i.e., depression and anxiety symptoms), which
Increased depression and anxiety symptoms in young adults have been associated
with problematic SNS use levels (Andreassen et al., 2016; Shensa et al., 2017), with
some evidence that individuals may experience greater anxiety and depression severity
when engaging with several SNSs compared to engaging in only one or two platforms
3
(Primack et al., 2017). Additionally, within the context of broader problematic internet
use, one Dutch study found that depression symptoms were associated with social use of
the internet, including use of SNSs (van Rooij et al., 2017). These findings were
supported in another country, with one study from Taiwan finding additional evidence
that adolescents who experienced greater depression severity were more likely to use the
internet for social purposes (Hwang, Cheong, & Feeley, 2009). Taken together, these
results suggest that individuals who experience depression and anxiety symptoms may
Baumeister and Leary (1995) described the need to belong as the need to have
frequent, positive interactions with others in which they perceive that they are liked and
that there is stability and concern for wellbeing in the relationship. Because the need to
belong is innate, individuals who are not satisfied with their social relationships will seek
out and form additional support (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). In addition to seeking out
relationships with others to fulfill a need to belong, individuals who experience ostracism
affection for others, or conforming to group norms (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) to fortify
their need to belong (Williams & Sommer, 1997; Williams, Wheeler, & Harvey, 2001;
and motives and needs of users (Bargh & McKenna, 2004). As previously discussed,
4
these needs mimic the needs and motives for offline social interaction: seeking
information or belongingness (Ridings & Gefen, 2004). In the context of Baumeister and
Leary’s theory (1995), individuals may be especially motivated to use SNSs to pursue
feelings of belongingness if offline social relationships are lacking. This pursuit of online
social interactions may be beneficial to the user’s mental health if such a pursuit does not
interfere with offline social support (Ceyhan & Ceyhan, 2008; Morrison & Gore, 2010).
Passive SNS users also tend to report lower wellbeing than their active counterparts
(Verduyn et al., 2015), which may lead to problematic SNS use and further social
functional impairment.
Several researchers have highlighted the relevance of fear of missing out (FoMO)
in studying SNS use behaviors (Elhai, Yang, & Montag, 2021). FoMO is a recently
investigated phenomenon that refers to the feeling that others may be having rewarding
experiences from which one is absent and is characterized by the desire to stay connected
with what others are doing (Przybylski, Murayama, DeHaan, & Gladwell, 2013). There is
some overlap between FoMO and problematic SNS use in that both constructs involve
social factors. However, there is an important distinction between the two constructs in
that problematic SNS use emphasizes functional impairment in several life domains,
while FoMO involves discomfort and anxiety related to social exclusion, and an urge to
connect with one’s social network. Furthermore, a recent review (Elhai, Yang, et al.,
2021) demonstrated that the two constructs are moderately-to-highly correlated but
distinct.
5
FoMO has been shown to mediate the relationship between online social support
and problematic SNS use, where the higher an individual’s level of online social support
was, the more severe their FoMO, which in turn led to more problematic use (Liu & Ma,
2018). Further, FoMO was found to mediate the relationship between emotional support
via SNS and “phubbing” behaviors, in which an individual opts to engage socially online
during offline interpersonal interactions (Fang, Wang, Wen, & Zhou, 2020). In addition
to predicting problematic SNS use levels, both generally and within the context of
specific SNS platforms (Rozgonjuk, Sindermann, Elhai, & Montag, 2020), FoMO also
functions as a mediator between basic psychological (Przybylski et al., 2013) and social
needs (Beyens, Frison, & Eggermont, 2016) and psychopathology (Oberst, Wegmann,
Stodt, Brand, & Chamarro, 2017). Taken together, evidence suggests FoMO should
mediate the relations between both social belongingness and psychopathology severity
with specific problematic SNS use (i.e., Instagram, Snapchat, and Twitter) severity.
studies to date have focused almost exclusively on Facebook use (Stoycheff, Liu,
Wibowo, & Nanni, 2017). With Facebook being the oldest and most widely used of the
available SNSs, it makes sense to focus on the effects of Facebook use. However, relying
so heavily on a single SNS limits the research base in a number of ways (Rains &
Brunner, 2015). First, focusing only on one platform limits generalizability of the
findings. For example, there is evidence to suggest that demographics (Hampton, Goulet,
Rainie, & al., 2011; Wilkinson & Thelwall, 2010) and culture (Boyd, 2011) influence
selection and use of SNSs. More recently, studies have shown that personality and
6
demographic differences are related to the choice of SNSs, where multi-site users tended
to be younger, more often female, and more extraverted than individuals who did not
endorse SNS use (Marengo, Sindermann, Elhai, & Montag, 2020). Further research has
found that individual and demographic differences exist among users who have different
patterns of SNS use, possibly choosing sites that provide different content and functions
relative to social belongingness. Differences in the type of online socializing may account
for differences in outcomes. For example, passively browsing one’s SNS feed may not be
found negative associations between wellbeing and online socializing (Kross et al., 2013;
O'Dea & Campbell, 2011; Pantic et al., 2012). Studies that investigated other forms of
online socializing found positive associations between wellbeing and online socializing
(Lelkes, 2013; Pendry & Salvatore, 2015; Pénard, Poussing, & Suire, 2013; Selfhout,
Branje, Delsing, ter Bogt, & Meeus, 2009). These findings suggest that the type of SNS
Moreover, SNSs undergo frequent updates at the behest of their creators. The
indirect influence of platform creators and attractiveness of specific site features may
influence which sites users are drawn to and how they engage with the platform over
time, if that site does not first become obsolete (Rains & Brunner, 2015). Indeed, there is
some evidence that platform-specific relatedness, which differs by the features offered,
7
can be used to predict wellbeing among those who socialize online (Jurgens, 2020).
Additionally, Internet users value online communities more when those communities
offer ways to fulfill users’ motives (Seraj, 2012; Tausczik & Huang, 2020), meaning that
users may value SNSs that offer a sense of belonging and provide options for acquiring
or exchanging information.
Furthermore, some evidence suggests that individual SNSs operate as their own
with differing levels of severity (Rozgonjuk, Sindermann, Elhai, & Montag, 2021). It is
therefore necessary to expand the literature to include multiple SNSs to better understand
differences driving SNS choice and use/overuse. The second aim of the present study was
to expand the research in this way by examining problematic SNS use for individual
sites.
Because there is no widely used standard for measuring SNS overuse, the present
study used modified versions of the Smartphone Addiction Scale – Short Version (SAS-
SV), adapted for SNS use as done in previous studies (Rozgonjuk et al., 2021a;
Rozgonjuk et al., 2021b). The Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS) and SAS-SV were
developed by Kwon and colleagues using samples from two universities and two
companies in South Korea (Kwon, Kim, Cho, & Yang, 2013; Kwon, Lee, et al., 2013),
and are two of the most commonly used measures of problematic smartphone use in the
literature (Harris, Regan, Schueler, & Fields, 2020). The SAS is a 33-item measure with
higher scores indicating higher levels of problematic use; however, there are no
8
suggested cut off points on the original measure (Kwon, Lee, et al., 2013). Items on the
SAS are divided into six subscales: daily life disturbance, positive anticipation,
found on the SAS include, “Using a smartphone is the most fun thing to do,” and “My
fully charged battery does not last for one full day.” The SAS demonstrates high internal
consistency and concurrent validity (Cronbach's alpha = 0.93; Harris, McCredie, &
Fields, 2020).
Because of brevity of the scale and frequency of use throughout the literature, the
present study used the SAS-SV, which condenses the original 33-item scale to a 10-item
scale that measures social and health impairment resulting from smartphone use (Kwon,
Kim, et al., 2013). The 10 items were selected based on their validity and review by
experts in the field. As with the SAS, response options on the SAS-SV range from “1 =
Strongly Disagree” to “6 = Strongly Agree.” Items include statements such as, “I have a
hard time concentrating in class, while doing assignments, or while working due to
smartphone use,” and “I use my smartphone longer than I had intended.” The SAS-SV
demonstrates reliability and validity comparable to the SAS (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.84),
suggesting that the SAS-SV is a reasonable alternative to the SAS (Harris, McCredie, et
al., 2020). We adapted the scale to ask about the individual SNS platform of interest
instead of the smartphone, as done recently (Rozgonjuk et al., 2021a; Rozgonjuk et al.,
2021b).
9
frequency of feature use may help identify which features are being overused and help
1.6 Theory
The present study was conceptualized in part using Brand and colleagues’ (2019)
Internet use disorders (Brand et al., 2019). The I-PACE model (Figure 1-1) is a
Internet use. Personal factors that may influence Internet use behaviors include such
anxiety), temperamental features, and general coping style. The model also identifies
importance to the present study are affect and cognition factors of the model. In their
model, Brand and colleagues (2019) proposed that those who perceive internal triggers,
such as negative mood, in specific situations may respond with urges to behave in certain
ways (Starcke, Antons, Trotzke, & Brand, 2018). More specifically, behaviors such as
engaging in SNS use may lead to feelings of gratification or relief from negative mood
(Laier & Brand, 2017), which subsequently changes the individual’s reward expectancies
associated with SNS use and may also modify coping style.
One main idea of the I-PACE model is that the development of problematic use
aspects that specific situations deliver, such as changes in affect and cognitions (Brand et
10
al., 2019). I–PACE thus conceptualizes affective and cognitive response variables as
excessive internet use. Previous studies conceptualized using the I-PACE model (Elhai,
Levine, Dvorak, & Hall, 2016; Elhai, Levine, O’Brien, & Armour, 2018; Elhai, Tiamiyu,
Weeks, et al., 2018) tested relations between psychopathology factors (e.g., depression
belongingness both online and offline as stable, predisposing variables (i.e., specific
(SDT), which names three basic psychological needs: autonomy, competence, and
relatedness (Ryan & Deci, 2017). The Basic Psychological Needs Theory (BPNT), a mini
theory nested within the broader SDT, argues that psychological well-being and optimal
functioning are predicated on the aforementioned needs, and if any of the needs are
thwarted, there will be negative outcomes, including mental health difficulties (Ryan &
Deci, 2017; Vansteenkiste, Niemiec, & Soenens, 2010). Important to the present study is
others; when this need is thwarted, an individual may experience feelings of exclusion or
(RMT), which posits that some amount of close, personal relationships is essential for
wellbeing, as it satisfies the need for social relatedness (Ryan & Deci, 2017). When these
needs are not met, and need frustration occurs, individuals may experience stress
11
(Campbell et al., 2017; Weinstein & Ryan, 2011), depression symptoms (Cordeiro,
Paixão, Lens, Lacante, & Luyckx, 2016), and/or anxiety symptoms (Inguglia, Liga, Lo
Coco, Musso, & Ingoglia, 2018). Because of the strong correlations between
psychopathology and relatedness (i.e., belongingness), and consistent with I-PACE, for
the purposes of this study, depression, anxiety, and belongingness all functioned as
predictor variables.
In the context of both the I-PACE model and SDT, FoMO can be conceptualized
FoMO may be considered a negative result of unmet social relatedness needs (Przybylski
et al., 2013), which may motivate individuals to engage in online social behaviors to
compensate for the frustrated need (Vansteenkiste & Ryan, 2013). Conceptualized using
anxiety. This relation may, in turn, drive problematic SNS use as conceptualized
previously (Elhai, Yang, & Montag, 2019; Wegmann, Oberst, Stodt, & Brand, 2017).
This conceptualization of FoMO is consistent with previous findings that FoMO mediates
O'Brien, Tiamiyu, & Elhai, 2019). A recent review by Elhai, Yang, and Montag (2021)
found that, in addition to being correlated with both anxiety and depression severity,
FoMO has also mediated the relationship between such constructs with various forms of
problematic technology use, including SNS use (Elhai, Yang, et al., 2021). Thus, FoMO
may be a mechanism that explains how some individuals with depression and anxiety
12
symptoms, combined with a thwarted sense of belonging, may develop problematic SNS
use.
13
Figure 1-1: Revised Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model
of specific internet use disorders. Figure A shows early stages of development of
addictive behaviors, and Figure B shows later stages of the process and factors that
contribute to the maintenance of addictive behaviors.
14
Chapter 2
Hypotheses
Baumeister and Leary (1995) asserted that much of human behavior is motivated
by a need to belong. Research has shown that online social support motivates SNS use.
However, relying too heavily on online social support to the detriment of one’s offline
Furthermore, these findings may vary depending on the SNS platform used and features
offered. Despite these findings, little work has been conducted to examine the role that
both online and offline feelings of belongingness play in the selection and use of SNSs. It
is possible that individuals seek online feelings of belongingness when such feelings are
selecting sites that promote such feelings, online belongingness-seeking behaviors are
then reinforced and persist, which may negatively affect offline relationships over time.
The aims of the present study were three-fold: 1) to investigate how offline and
online social belongingness relate to problematic SNS use severity; 2) to investigate what
role depression and anxiety severity play, along with both online and offline
investigate these relations in the context of specific SNSs. Specific hypotheses were:
15
H1) Offline social belonging will be inversely related to problematic SNS use
severity, in that individuals who experience lower levels of offline social belongingness
will have higher problematic SNS use severity. This hypothesis is consistent with prior
research, which has found that individuals are motivated by social belongingness to use
SNSs, which may be particularly true for those who are lacking social support offline.
SNS use severity, in that individuals who experience lower levels of online social
belongingness will have lower problematic SNS use severity. Again, if individuals are
motivated by social belongingness to engage with SNSs, individuals who do not perceive
problematic use.
H3) Depression severity will be positively correlated with problematic SNS use
H4) Anxiety severity will be positively correlated with problematic SNS use
severity. This hypothesis is also consistent with previous findings and theory.
H5) FoMO will be positively related with problematic SNS use severity. In
conjunction with I-PACE and SDT, prior research has found moderate to large
associations for FoMO with problematic SNS use severity in college students.
H6) FoMO will account for relations between anxiety, depression, and
relationship between psychopathology and problematic SNS use severity as well as social
16
H6a) FoMO will account for the relationship between anxiety severity and
H6b) FoMO will account for the relationship between depression severity and
H6c) FoMO will account for the relationship between offline social
H6d) FoMO will account for the relationship between online social belongingness
H7) While the breakdown of above hypotheses into specific problematic SNS use
severity is largely exploratory, higher online levels of social belongingness are expected
to be positively associated with problematic use of sites that my foster more active social
use (e.g., Instagram, Snapchat), and higher offline social belongingness will be positively
associated with sites that may facilitate more passive, less social use (e.g., Twitter).
17
Chapter 3
Methods
3.1 Participants
courses at the University of Toledo. See Appendix A for a copy of the informed consent
form approved by the Institutional Review Board. The initial sample size for this study
consisted of 327 participants. Eleven (n = 11) participants were dropped from the sample
for failure to complete the survey, answering no more than the initial demographic items.
An additional 41 (n = 41) participants were dropped for responding to the survey twice.
appeared twice in the closing screen dataset, making note of the date and time the closing
screens were started, and matching these with the dates and times the surveys were
ended. Each of the participants who had duplicate responses had unique names, so it was
easy to identify these as duplicates rather than a different participant with the same name.
Second response sets occurred at times ranging from immediately after the first response
to several days after the first response. None of the second response sets occurred after
failure to complete the survey all the way through the first time. Possible explanations for
18
awarded, mistakenly thinking they would be awarded automatically upon completion of
the survey, or realizing they committed errors the first time. It is also possible that
participants forgot they already completed the survey or completed it again because they
Because of the aforementioned reasons, the first responses were excluded from
the study under the assumption that answers may be more accurate the second time taking
the survey because participants would have had a second chance to consider the questions
and reflect on their experiences. Another 140 (n = 140) participants were excluded for not
1.6; 43.0% male). The sample predominantly identified as Caucasian (n = 114; 84.4%),
with some participants identifying as African American (n = 22; 16.2%), Asian (n = 10;
0.7%). Four participants endorsed not knowing their racial background (n = 4; 3.0%).
Ten participants endorsed being of Hispanic of Latinx descent (n = 10; 7.4%). Race and
ethnic endorsements were not mutually exclusive. Most participants were freshmen (n =
82; 60.7%), employed part-time (n = 67; 49.6%) or unemployed (n = 59; 41.5%), and
Review Board. Participants were recruited using the department’s Sona Systems website,
which lists available departmental research studies throughout the semester. In exchange
for participation, students were awarded course research points. Those who chose to
19
participate were routed to an online consent statement. After consenting, participants
Depression and anxiety severity were measured using the 21-item short version of
the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale DASS-21 (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond,
1995). Items on the DASS-21 inquire about symptoms over the past week and are rated
on a Likert-type scale with responses ranging from “0 = Did not apply to me at all” to “3
= Applied to me very much or most of the time.” Items for anxiety include “I felt scared
without any good reason,” and “I was aware of the action of my heart in the absence of
physical exertion.” Items for depression include “I found it difficult to work up the
initiative to do things,” and “I felt down-hearted and blue” (Lovibond & Lovibond,
1995). For purposes of the present study, only depression and anxiety items were used in
analyses; stress items were not analyzed. There is convergent validity with other
measures of depression and anxiety (Antony, Bieling, Cox, Enns, & Swinson, 1998;
Brown, Chorpita, Korotitsch, & Barlow, 1997). Coefficient alpha for the present study
was 0.92 for the depression subscale and 0.84 for the anxiety subscale.
For descriptive purposes, we assessed how much SNS use participants reported.
To measure frequency of use, participants answered questions regarding how often they
liking, posting stories, posting to timeline, reading news, and checking notifications.
Items were rated on a Likert-type scale with response options ranging from “1 = Never”
20
to “6 = Very Often.” A previous study used a similar questionnaire to measure frequency
of Facebook use and found good internal consistency ( = 0.87) as well as significant
bivariate Pearson correlations with FoMO, r(294) = -.19, p < .01 (Dempsey et al., 2019).
We also asked one question per SNS platform, inquiring how often participants
use the platform. We asked how many hours the participant uses the platform each week
with options including 0, 1-2, 3-4, 5-6, 7-8, 9-10, and 10+ hours. As mentioned above,
only participants reporting at least 1-2 weekly hours of each SNS platform were included
in analyses.
Kim, et al. (2013) was adapted. The SAS-SV is a 10-item scale with response options
measure social and health impairment related to smartphone use and include items such
as “The people around me tell me that I use my smartphone too much,” and “Won’t be
able to stand having a smartphone.” Several items were reworded into a first-person voice
for greater accessibility and consistency for participants. For example, “Missing planned
work due to smartphone use,” was reworded to “I missed planned work due to
smartphone use” (Duke & Montag, 2017). Scores on the SAS-SV are related to measures
of problematic internet use and PSU severity (Kwon, Kim, et al., 2013) and smartphone
use frequency (Elhai, Tiamiyu, Weeks, et al., 2018; Lopez-Fernandez, 2017). The SAS-
SV was modified from smartphone use to measure each SNS individually by replacing
21
“Snapchat” (PSNS-S; = 0.91), consistent with previous studies (Rozgonjuk et al.,
To assess for both online and offline social belongingness, participants completed
the Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale (BPNSFS), which is a
24-item scale that assesses both frustration and satisfaction in the domains of autonomy,
relatedness, and competence (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Gagné, 2003). For the purposes of the
present study, only the relatedness satisfaction subscale was used: an internally consistent
( = 0.76), reliable, and validated 8-item subscale that measures participants’ satisfaction
with their degree of relatedness. Items are rated on a Likert-type scale with response
options ranging from “1 = Not true at all” to “5 = Completely true.” Items include
statements such as, “I feel that the people I care about also care about me,” and “I feel
connected with people who care for me, and for whom I care” (Deci & Ryan, 2000;
Gagné, 2003). Again, participants were asked to complete this measure (8 items) with
consideration for their relationships offline ( = 0.79) and once more for their
To assess FoMO, participants completed the Fear of Missing Out Scale, which
consists of 10 items rated on a Likert-type scale with response options ranging from “1 =
Not at all true of me,” to “5 = Extremely true of me” (Przybylski et al., 2013). Examples
of items include, “I get anxious when I don’t know what my friends are up to,” and “I get
worried when I find out my friends are having fun without me.” Scores on the FoMO
Scale are related to measures of basic psychological need satisfaction, general mood, life
22
satisfaction, and high levels of engagement with SNS (Przybylski et al., 2013);
23
Chapter 4
Analyses
Small amounts of missing item-level data were observed for the measures.
(Graham, 2009) was used to estimate missing item-level data for each scale separately
among participants missing less than 50% of items on a given scale; subsequently, total
scale scores were computed and missing total scale scores were estimated for those
participants missing fewer than half of their scale scores. Initially, we planned to compute
a measurement model for the SAS-SV using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and test
while over 300 people participated in our study, a surprising number of them had to be
excluded for not using all three SNSs, rendering these analyses unusable. As such, we
Sex and age served as covariates of PSNS-I, PSNS-S, and PSNS-T in the models
separately (as three separate models), as problematic use is associated with female sex
and younger age (Jeong, Kim, Yum, & Hwang, 2016; Wang, Wang, Gaskin, & Wang,
2015). The model in Figure 4-2 was tested using linear regression. Depression, anxiety,
FoMO, online social belongingness, and offline social belongingness were specified to
24
predict problematic Twitter use severity (Figure 4-1). Similar models were tested for
problematic Instagram use (Figure 4-3) and Problematic Snapchat use (Figure 4-4)
separately.
25
Figure 4-3: Regression model for problematic Snapchat use.
4.2 Mediation
belongingness) and each of the social networking sites (separately) using indirect effect
testing by computing cross-products of direct path coefficients. The Delta method was
used for estimating the standard error of a given indirect effect. Dividing the indirect
effect’s path coefficient by its Delta-estimated standard error produces a z-test for
assessing statistical significance of the mediation effect. Because indirect effect estimates
obtained this way are normally distributed on a sampling distribution, 1,000 bootstrapped
replications were implemented for accurate estimates. Such indirect testing procedures
26
Chapter 5
Results
PSNS-I and PSNS-T having the largest values for skewness (1.28 and 1.25, respectively)
and kurtosis (1.17 and 0.87, respectively) and all other values being under an absolute
value of one. Summed scores for FoMO averaged 23.30 (SD = 8.04). Summed scores for
the DASS-21 averaged 5.21 (SD = 5.09) for depression and 4.73 (SD = 4.34) for anxiety.
Based on total scores from the SAS-SV, the average for PSNS-I was 18.80 (SD = 8.99),
for PSNS-S was 24.40 (SD = 11.40), and for PSNS-T was 17.40 (SD = 8.94). Summed
scores for online belongingness averaged 42.70 (SD = 6.66), and summed scores for
offline belongingness averaged 43.9 (SD = 7.33). A correlation matrix of these scaled
items marked “Very Often,” participants most engaged in checking notifications and
liking posts, images, or links. Additional frequency data for using smartphone features
27
Table 5.1: Correlations among primary variables. *p < .05, **p < 0.01
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Age ---
2. Sex -.26** ---
3. Depression -.09 .27** ---
4. Anxiety -.14 .23** .72** ---
5. Instagram -.14 .03 .14 .14 ---
6. Snapchat -.22** .17* .13 .20* -.47** ---
7. Twitter .04 .01 .26** .32** .43** .37** ---
8. Online -.01 -.09 -.35** -.22** -.14 -.10 -.03 ---
9. Offline -.04 -.11 -.47** -.27** -.09 -.09 -.21* .79** ---
10. FoMO -.20* .15 .22** .29** .42** .60** .33** -.05 .01 ---
Reposting Content 10 26 39 19 31 17
Reading News 8 18 28 36 32 19
Checking Notifications 2 4 18 18 40 60
28
5.2 Linear Regression
Linear regression was used to test if online and offline social belongingness,
depression, anxiety, and FoMO significantly predicted problematic social networking site
usage. Three models were tested separately, one for each SNS site. Variance inflation
factors (VIF) were used to detect multicollinearity, and none of the independent variables
were greater than five, with offline belongingness having the highest VIF of 3.22.
The results of the regression indicated that the model overall predicts problematic
Instagram use significantly, R2=.76, F(7, 134)=10.98, p<.0001. However, only depression
was found to significantly predict problematic Instagram use severity (=.28, S.E.=.10,
p=.005). Neither FoMO (=-.16, S.E.=.11, p=.14), anxiety (=.05, S.E.=.11, p=.66),
The results related to problematic Snapchat use severity indicated that the model
was statistically significant, R2=.61, F(7, 134)=9.28, p<.0001. Both anxiety (=.39,
predicted problematic Snapchat use severity. Neither FoMO (=-.03, S.E.=.08, p=.71),
depression (=.10, S.E.=.09, p=.28), nor offline belongingness (=.08, S.E.=.08, p=.37)
Finally, relevant to problematic Twitter use severity, the overall regression was
statistically significant, R2=.38, F(7, 134)=6.30, p<.0001. It was found that anxiety
29
(=.30, S.E.=.08, p<.0001) and offline belongingness (=.58, S.E.=.06, p<.0001)
Depression (=.03, S.E.=.07, p=.62), and online belongingness (=-.04, S.E.=.07, p=.56)
5.3 Mediation
Overall, FoMO was found to mediate relations between both anxiety and online
belongingness and all three measures of problematic SNS use, offering support for H6a
and H6d. However, FoMO did not mediate relations between depression or offline
belongingness with any measure of problematic SNS use severity, thus failing to support
H6b and H6c. Mediation results are displayed in Table 5.3 with p-values displayed for
30
Chapter 6
Discussion
associated with problematic SNS use severity (Andreassen et al., 2016; Shensa et al.,
2017; van Rooij et al., 2017). Despite the growing body of literature linking
psychopathology with problematic SNS use severity, associations between mental health
and specific SNS use behaviors remain largely unexplored. People are innately social
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995), and there are well-documented inverse correlations between
psychopathology and social belongingness (Beekman et al., 2016; Erzen & Çikrikci,
2018; Steger & Kashdan, 2009). Therefore, it is important to investigate online and
offline social factors as they relate to psychopathology and the development and
The aims of the present study, then, were to investigate 1) how offline and online
social belongingness relate to problematic SNS use severity; 2) what role depression and
anxiety severity play, along with both online and offline social belongingness, in
association with problematic SNS use severity; and 3) the role of the above relationships
within the context of specific SNSs. We predicted that offline belongingness would be
inversely related to problematic SNS use severity (H1) while online belongingness would
31
be positively correlated with problematic SNS use severity (H2). Additionally, we
predicted that depression severity (H3), anxiety severity (H4), and FoMO (H5) would be
positively correlated with problematic SNS use severity, and that FoMO would account
for relationships between anxiety, depression, and both online and offline belongingness
with problematic SNS use (H6). Finally, while the breakdown of the above hypotheses
into specific problematic SNS use severity was largely exploratory, we predicted that
problematic use of SNSs that may foster more active social use (e.g., Instagram,
Snapchat), and higher offline belonging would be positively associated with problematic
use of SNSs that may facilitate passive, less social use (e.g., Twitter).
We found only partial support for H1 and H7. In bivariate correlations, offline
severity. It was, however, negatively associated with problematic Twitter use severity,
suggesting that individuals who experience lower levels of offline social belongingness
have greater problematic Twitter use severity. Similarly, offline belongingness predicted
problematic Twitter use severity but not problematic Snapchat or Instagram use severity
in regression analyses. Also surprising was the limited support found for online social
significantly predicted problematic Snapchat use severity; it did not predict problematic
Given that the present study was exploratory relative to social belongingness, and
the relationship between social belongingness and SNS use remains a prominent gap in
the literature, it is unclear why these differences between SNSs exist. One possible
32
explanation is that in offline contexts, those who experience thwarted belongingness tend
decreased social belongingness in general tend to experience more negative affect than
those whose social belongingness needs are met (Beekman et al., 2016; Erzen & Çikrikci,
2018; Steger & Kashdan, 2009). In reaction to this psychological discomfort, individuals
may try to remind themselves of groups to which they belong, intensifying feelings of
affection for others (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) to fortify their need to belong (Williams
& Sommer, 1997; Williams et al., 2001; Williams & Zadro, 2005).
consequently experiences anger, and they choose to engage in Twitter use, there may be
simultaneous processes happening: expressions of and engagement with anger online are
reinforced (Brady, McLoughlin, Doan, & Crockett, 2021), and a person is not
experiencing the same social connection as they might experience on another site
associated with online belongingness, like Snapchat. Therefore, they are prolonging their
experience of anger and not obtaining the benefits of online social support, which may
lead to problematic Twitter use, where other SNSs more readily fortify needs for social
belonging that may protect against problematic use. More information is needed about
how individuals engage with specific SNSs to better understand the differences in
We found only partial support for H3 and H4. Depression severity showed
significant bivariate correlations with problematic Twitter use severity but not
33
significantly predicted Instagram use severity but not problematic Twitter or Snapchat
Snapchat and Twitter (but not Instagram) use severity in bivariate correlations and
regression analyses. These results are somewhat consistent with previous findings that
depression and anxiety symptoms are related to problematic smartphone use (Elhai,
Dvorak, Levine, & Hall, 2017) and problematic SNS use (Keles, McCrae, & Grealish,
2020). While there are some discrepancies between meta-analytic findings and results of
the present study, these differences may be explained by the more granular approach
taken here. That is, in looking at specific SNSs rather than problematic SNS use as a
whole, the present study offers a slightly more nuanced understanding of the relationship
Instagram, Twitter, and Snapchat use severity (H5). Furthermore, FoMO accounted for
relations between anxiety and online belongingness with problematic use of all three
SNSs but did not account for the relationship between depression or offline
belongingness with all three sites, demonstrating only partial support for H6. FoMO,
belongingness and relations with problematic SNS use. These findings are consistent with
the conceptualization of FoMO, which involves discomfort and anxiety related to social
exclusion and an urge to connect with one’s social network, particularly in online
contexts (Elhai et al., 2017; Przybylski et al., 2013). Similarly, mediation results are
consistent with previous findings that FoMO mediates the relationship between online
34
social support and problematic SNS use, where the higher an individual’s online support
was, the more severe their FoMO, which in turn led to more problematic SNS use (Liu &
Ma, 2018).
Taken together, the present study has two important implications: 1) depression
and anxiety differentially predict problematic use severity of different SNSs, and 2)
social belongingness is related to problematic SNS use severity. These results provide
some support for the I-PACE model, which posits that the development of problematic
variables and certain aspects that specific situations deliver, such as changes in affect and
cognitions (Brand et al., 2019). Overall, results of the present study demonstrate this link
FoMO.
However, there was little support for depression predicting problematic SNS use,
suggesting that the pathway from psychopathology to problematic use through FoMO is
nuanced and differs across SNSs. Results of the present study lend credence to the idea
that the expected and accepted relations between psychopathology severity and
problematic SNS use are not necessarily generalizable across social networks. This is
consistent with previous evidence that people’s problematic use of different SNSs vary
(Rozgonjuk et al., 2021a) and provides additional support for studying problematic use of
The limited support for depression as a predictor of problematic SNS use found in
this study is consistent with findings from a previous study, which found that depression
35
symptoms were associated with social use of the internet (van Rooij et al., 2017). It is
turn to image-based over text-based SNSs like Instagram to feel more connected to others
(Pittman & Reich, 2016). Furthermore, it is possible that depression symptoms act as a
protective factor against problematic use of sites like Twitter and Snapchat (Choi et al.,
2015) in that individuals have less energy and motivation to actively engage in SNSs.
Interestingly, within the context of SDT (Ryan & Deci, 2017), results of the
present study suggest that overall, it is high belongingness rather than thwarted offline
suggest that seeking and obtaining online support is one of many motives that drive
individuals to use SNSs (Masur et al., 2014), and thus different motives may help account
for results of the present study and problematic use of certain SNSs over others.
6.2 Limitations
Limitations of this study include use of a college student sample, which may not
represent the larger population. Additionally, the present study is limited by a relatively
small sample size given the number of analyses performed. We used a cross-sectional
than clinical diagnostic interviews to assess depression and anxiety, and we did not use
objective measures to assess Internet or SNS use (for an example, see Elhai, Tiamiyu, &
Weeks, 2018).
Similarly, this study did not evaluate use of specific features on sites, so insight
into how Twitter use might differ from Instagram and Snapchat use is limited. This study
may also be limited by the specific SNSs selected. Finally, and importantly, the above
36
findings may have been affected by the COVID-19 pandemic. With changes in form and
availability of offline socializing and increases in online learning, it is likely that relations
between people and SNSs, and subsequent impairment from excessively using SNSs,
More information is needed about how individuals engage with specific SNSs to
problematic use across SNSs. Future studies would benefit from further exploring the
driving forces behind problematic Instagram use compared to other SNSs. Additionally,
future studies may benefit from further exploring online and offline belongingness,
perhaps by assessing the number of offline close friends and acquaintances and number
superficial sense of social connectedness. Relatedly, future studies would benefit from
examining other aspects of SDT theory in relation to problematic SNS use, such as
competence and autonomy (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Finally, future studies should explore
differences in problematic use of specific features across SNSs and include more widely
used sites for this age group, such as TikTok (Montag, Yang, & Elhai, 2021).
37
References
Andreassen, C., Billieux, J., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., Demetrovics, Z., Mazzoni, E.,
& Pallesen, S. (2016). The relationship between addictive use of social media and
video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional
study. Psychol Addict Behav, 30(2), 252-262. doi:10.1037/adb0000160
Andreassen, C. S., & Pallesen, S. (2014). Social Network Site Addiction - An Overview.
Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 4053-4061.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990616
Antony, M. M., Bieling, P. J., Cox, B. J., Enns, M. W., & Swinson, R. P. (1998).
Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression
Anxiety and Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample.
Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176-181.
Bargh, J. A., & McKenna, Y. K. (2004). The Internet and Social Life. Annual Review of
Psychology, 55(1), 573-590. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141922
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3),
497-529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497
Beekman, J. B., Stock, M. L., & Marcus, T. (2016). Need to Belong, Not Rejection
Sensitivity, Moderates Cortisol Response, Self-Reported Stress, and Negative
Affect Following Social Exclusion. The Journal of Social Psychology, 156(2),
131-138. doi:10.1080/00224545.2015.1071767
Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). “I don’t want to miss a thing”:
Adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’ social needs,
Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 1-
8. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.083
Boyd, D. (2011). White flight in networked publics? How race and class shaped
American teen engagement with MySpace and Facebook. In L. Nakamura & P. A.
Chow-White (Eds.), Race after the Internet (pp. 203-222). New York: Routledge.
38
Brady, W. J., McLoughlin, K., Doan, T. N., & Crockett, M. J. (2021). How social
learning amplifies moral outrage expression in online social networks. Sci Adv,
7(33). doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abe5641
Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W., & Potenza,
M. N. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE)
model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors
beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of
addictive behaviors. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 1-10.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032
Brand, M., Young, K. S., Laier, C., Wölfling, K., & Potenza, M. N. (2016). Integrating
psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and
maintenance of specific internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-
Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,
71, 252-266. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Psychometric
properties of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical samples.
Behav Res Ther, 35(1), 79-89.
Campbell, R., Tobback, E., Delesie, L., Vogelaers, D., Mariman, A., & Vansteenkiste, M.
(2017). Basic psychological need experiences, fatigue, and sleep in individuals
with unexplained chronic fatigue. Stress and Health, 33(5), 645-655.
Ceyhan, A. A., & Ceyhan, E. (2008). Loneliness, depression, and computer self-efficacy
as predictors of problematic internet use. Cyberpsychol Behav, 11(6), 699-701.
doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.0255
Choi, S., Kim, D., Choi, J., Ahn, H., Choi, E., Song, W.,… Youn, H. (2015). Comparison
of risk and protective factors associated with smartphone addiction and Internet
addiction. Jounral of Behavioral Addictions, 4(4), 308-314.
Cordeiro, P., Paixão, P., Lens, W., Lacante, M., & Luyckx, K. (2016). The Portuguese
Validation of the Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale:
Concurrent and Longitudinal Relations to Well-being and Ill-being. Psychologica
Belgica, 56(3), 193-209. doi:10.5334/pb.252
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The" what" and" why" of goal pursuits: Human needs
and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological inquiry, 11(4), 227-268.
Dempsey, A. E., O'Brien, K. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., & Elhai, J. D. (2019). Fear of missing
out (FoMO) and rumination mediate relations between social anxiety and
problematic Facebook use. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 9, 100150.
39
DiStefano, C., & Morgan, G. B. (2014). A comparison of diagonal weighted least squares
robust estimation techniques for ordinal data. Structural Equation Modeling,
21(3), 425-438. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.915373
Duke, É., & Montag, C. (2017). Smartphone addiction, daily interruptions and self-
reported productivity. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 6, 90-95.
doi:10.1016/j.abrep.2017.07.002
Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone
use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and
depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251-259.
doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030
Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of missing out, need
for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use.
Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 509-516. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079
Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., O’Brien, K. D., & Armour, C. (2018). Distress tolerance and
mindfulness mediate relations between depression and anxiety sensitivity with
problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 84, 477-484.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.03.026
Elhai, J. D., McKay, D., Yang, H., Minaya, C., Montag, C., & Asmundson, G. J. G.
(2021). Health anxiety related to problematic smartphone use and gaming
disorder severity during COVID-19: Fear of missing out as a mediator. Hum
Behav Emerg Technol, 3(1), 137-146. doi:10.1002/hbe2.227
Elhai, J. D., Tiamiyu, M., & Weeks, J. (2018). Depression and social anxiety in relation
to problematic smartphone use: The prominent role of rumination. Internet
Research, 28(2), 315-332. doi:doi:10.1108/IntR-01-2017-0019
Elhai, J. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., Levine, J. C., Picard, K. J., & Hall, B. J.
(2018). Depression and emotion regulation predict objective smartphone use
measured over one week. Personality and Individual Differences, 133, 21-28.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.051
Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., & Montag, C. (2021). Fear of missing out (FOMO): Overview,
theoretical underpinnings, and literature review on relations with severity of
negative affectivity and problematic technology use. Brazilian Journal of
Psychiatry. doi:10.1590/1516-4446-2020-0870
Elhai, J. D., Yang, H., & Montag, C. (2019). Cognitive- and Emotion-Related
Dysfunctional Coping Processes: Transdiagnostic Mechanisms Explaining
Depression and Anxiety’s Relations with Problematic Smartphone Use. Current
Addiction Reports, 6(4), 410-417. doi:10.1007/s40429-019-00260-4
40
Erzen, E., & Çikrikci, Ö. (2018). The effect of loneliness on depression: A meta-analysis.
International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 64(5), 427-435.
doi:10.1177/0020764018776349
Escobar-Viera, C. G., Shensa, A., Bowman, N. D., Sidani, J. E., Knight, J., James, A. E.,
& Primack, B. A. (2018). Passive and Active Social Media Use and Depressive
Symptoms Among United States Adults. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 21(7),
437-443. doi:10.1089/cyber.2017.0668
Fang, J., Wang, X., Wen, Z., & Zhou, J. (2020). Fear of missing out and problematic
social media use as mediators between emotional support from social media and
phubbing behavior. Addictive Behaviors, 107, 106430.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106430
Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2015). Exploring the Relationships Between Different
Types of Facebook Use, Perceived Online Social Support, and Adolescents'
Depressed Mood. Social Science Computer Review, 34.
doi:10.1177/0894439314567449
Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annual
Review of Psychology, 60, 549-576.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530
Hampton, K. N., Goulet, L. S., Rainie, L., & al., e. (2011). Social networking sites and
our lives. Retrieved from https://www.pewinternet.org/wp-
content/uploads/sites/9/media/Files/Reports/2011/PIP-Social-networking-sites-
and-our-lives.pdf
Harris, B., McCredie, M., & Fields, S. (2020). Examining the psychometric properties of
the Smartphone Addiction Scale and its short version for use with emerging adults
in the US. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 1, 100011.
Harris, B., Regan, T., Schueler, J., & Fields, S. A. (2020). Problematic Mobile Phone and
Smartphone Use Scales: A Systematic Review. Frontiers in Psychology, 11.
41
Hu, L.T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure
analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
Modeling, 6(1), 1-55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
Hwang, J. M., Cheong, P. H., & Feeley, T. H. (2009). Being young and feeling blue in
Taiwan: examining adolescent depressive mood and online and offline activities.
New Media & Society, 11(7), 1101-1121. doi:10.1177/1461444809341699
Indian, M., & Grieve, R. (2014). When Facebook is easier than face-to-face: Social
support derived from Facebook in socially anxious individuals. Personality and
Individual Differences, 59, 102-106.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.016
Inguglia, C., Liga, F., Lo Coco, A., Musso, P., & Ingoglia, S. (2018). Satisfaction and
frustration of autonomy and relatedness needs: Associations with parenting
dimensions and psychological functioning. Motivation and Emotion, 42(5), 691-
705. doi:10.1007/s11031-018-9702-6
Jeong, S.-H., Kim, H., Yum, J.-Y., & Hwang, Y. (2016). What type of content are
smartphone users addicted to?: SNS vs. Games. Computers in Human Behavior,
54, 10-17. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.035
Jurgens, C. T. (2020). Relatedness and well-being in the digital age. (Masters of Arts).
University of Dayton,
Keles, B., McCrae, N., & Grealish, A. (2020). A systematic review: the influence of
social media on depression, anxiety and psychological distress in adolescents.
International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 25(1), 79-93.
doi:10.1080/02673843.2019.1590851
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., . . . Ybarra, O. (2013).
Facebook Use Predicts Declines in Subjective Well-Being in Young Adults.
PLOS ONE, 8(8), e69841. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069841
Kwon, M., Kim, D.-J., Cho, H., & Yang, S. (2013). The Smartphone Addiction Scale:
Development and Validation of a Short Version for Adolescents. PLOS ONE,
8(12), e83558. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083558
Kwon, M., Lee, J.-Y., Won, W.-Y., Park, J.-W., Min, J.-A., Hahn, C., . . . Kim, D.-J.
(2013). Development and validation of a Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS).
PLoS ONE, 8(2).
Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2017). Mood changes after watching pornography on the Internet
are linked to tendencies towards Internet-pornography-viewing disorder.
Addictive behaviors reports, 5, 9-13.
42
Lelkes, O. (2013). Happier and less isolated: Internet use in old age1. Journal of Poverty
and Social Justice, 21, 33-46. doi:10.1332/175982713X664047
Liu, C., & Ma, J. (2018). Social support through online social networking sites and
addiction among college students: The mediating roles of fear of missing out and
problematic smartphone use. Current Psychology. doi:10.1007/s12144-018-0075-
5
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states:
Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck
Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3),
335-343. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U
Marengo, D., Sindermann, C., Elhai, J. D., & Montag, C. (2020). One Social Media
Company to Rule Them All: Associations Between Use of Facebook-Owned
Social Media Platforms, Sociodemographic Characteristics, and the Big Five
Personality Traits. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 936.
Masur, P. K., Reinecke, L., Ziegele, M., & Quiring, O. (2014). The interplay of intrinsic
need satisfaction and Facebook specific motives in explaining addictive behavior
on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 39, 376-386.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.047
Montag, C., Yang, H., & Elhai, J. D. (2021). On the Psychology of TikTok Use: A First
Glimpse From Empirical Findings. Front Public Health, 9, 641673.
doi:10.3389/fpubh.2021.641673
Morrison, C. M., & Gore, H. (2010). The relationship between excessive Internet use and
depression: a questionnaire-based study of 1,319 young people and adults.
Psychopathology, 43(2), 121-126. doi:10.1159/000277001
O'Dea, B., & Campbell, A. (2011). Online social networking amongst teens: friend or
foe? In.
Oberst, U., Wegmann, E., Stodt, B., Brand, M., & Chamarro, A. (2017). Negative
consequences from heavy social networking in adolescents: The mediating role of
fear of missing out. Journal of Adolescence, 55, 51-60.
doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.008
43
Pantic, I., Damjanovic, A., Todorovic, J., Topalovic, D., Bojovic-Jovic, D., Ristic, S., &
Pantic, S. (2012). Association between online social networking and depression in
high school students: behavioral physiology viewpoint. Psychiatria Danubina,
24(1.), 90-93.
Pendry, L. F., & Salvatore, J. (2015). Individual and social benefits of online discussion
forums. Computers in Human Behavior, 50, 211-220.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.067
Pew Research Center. (2019). Social Media Fact Sheet. Retrieved from
https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/fact-sheet/social-media/
Pittman, M., & Reich, B. (2016). Social media and loneliness: Why an Instagram picture
may be worth more than a thousand Twitter words. Computers in Human
Behavior, 62, 155-167. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.084
Primack, B. A., Shensa, A., Escobar-Viera, C. G., Barrett, E. L., Sidani, J. E., Colditz, J.
B., & James, A. E. (2017). Use of multiple social media platforms and symptoms
of depression and anxiety: A nationally-representative study among U.S. young
adults. Computers in Human Behavior, 69, 1-9.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.013
Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational,
emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human
Behavior, 29(4), 1841-1848. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014
Pénard, T., Poussing, N., & Suire, R. (2013). Does the Internet make people happier? The
Journal of Socio-Economics, 46, 105-116.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2013.08.004
Rains, S. A., & Brunner, S. R. (2015). What can we learn about social network sites by
studying Facebook? A call and recommendations for research on social network
sites. New Media & Society, 17(1), 114-131. doi:10.1177/1461444814546481
Ridings, C. M., & Gefen, D. (2004). Virtual Community Attraction: Why People Hang
Out Online. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 10(1), 00-00.
doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2004.tb00229.x
Rozgonjuk, D., Sindermann, C., Elhai, J. D., Christensen, A. P., & Montag, C. (2021).
Associations between symptoms of problematic smartphone use, Facebook,
WhatsApp, and Instagram use: An item-level exploratory graph analysis
perspective. Journal of Bheavioral Addictions.
Rozgonjuk, D., Sindermann, C., Elhai, J. D., & Montag, C. (2020). Fear of missing out
(FoMO) and social media’s impact on daily-life and productivity at work: do
44
WhatsApp, Facebook, Instagram and Snapchat use disorders mediate that
association? Addictive Behaviors, 106487.
Rozgonjuk, D., Sindermann, C., Elhai, J. D., & Montag, C. (2021). Comparing
smartphone, WhatsApp, Facebook, Instagram, and Snapchat: Which platform
elicits the greatest use disorder symptoms? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Social Networking.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs
in motivation, development, and wellness: Guilford Publications.
Selfhout, M. H. W., Branje, S. J. T., Delsing, M., ter Bogt, T. F. M., & Meeus, W. H. J.
(2009). Different types of Internet use, depression, and social anxiety: The role of
perceived friendship quality. Journal of Adolescence, 32(4), 819-833.
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.10.011
Shensa, A., Escobar-Viera, C. G., Sidani, J. E., Bowman, N. D., Marshal, M. P., &
Primack, B. A. (2017). Problematic social media use and depressive symptoms
among U.S. young adults: A nationally-representative study. Soc Sci Med, 182,
150-157. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.03.061
Sindermann, C., Elhai, J. D., & Montag, C. (2020). Predicting tendencies towards the
disordered use of facebook's social media platforms: on the role of personality,
impulsivity, and social anxiety. Psychiatry Research, 285, 112793.
Starcke, K., Antons, S., Trotzke, P., & Brand, M. (2018). Cue-reactivity in behavioral
addictions: A meta-analysis and methodological considerations. Journal of
behavioral addictions, 7(2), 227-238.
Steger, M. F., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Depression and Everyday Social Activity,
Belonging, and Well-Being. J Couns Psychol, 56(2), 289-300.
doi:10.1037/a0015416
Stoycheff, E., Liu, J., Wibowo, K., & Nanni, D. (2017). What have we learned about
social media by studying Facebook? A decade in review. New Media & Society,
19, 146144481769574. doi:10.1177/1461444817695745
Tausczik, Y., & Huang, X. (2020). Knowledge generation and sharing in online
communities: Current trends and future directions. Current Opinion in
Psychology.
45
van Rooij, A., Mheen, D., & Schoenmakers, T. (2017). Time to abandon Internet
Addiction? Predicting problematic internet, game, and social media use from
psychosocial well-being and application use. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 14, 113–
121.
Vansteenkiste, M., Niemiec, C. P., & Soenens, B. (2010). The development of the five
mini-theories of self-determination theory: An historical overview, emerging
trends, and future directions. In The decade ahead: Theoretical perspectives on
motivation and achievement: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., . . . Kross, E. (2015).
Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and
longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2),
480.
Wang, J.-L., Wang, H.-Z., Gaskin, J., & Wang, L.-H. (2015). The role of stress and
motivation in problematic smartphone use among college students. Computers in
Human Behavior, 53, 181-188. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.005
Wegmann, E., Oberst, U., Stodt, B., & Brand, M. (2017). Online-specific fear of missing
out and Internet-use expectancies contribute to symptoms of Internet-
communication disorder. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5, 33-42.
Wilkinson, D., & Thelwall, M. (2010). Social network site changes over time: The case
of MySpace. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and
Technology, 61(11), 2311-2323. doi:10.1002/asi.21397
Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism: The Kiss of Social Death. Social and Personality
Psychology Compass, 1(1), 236-247. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00004.x
Williams, K. D., Wheeler, L., & Harvey, J. A. R. (2001). Inside the social mind of the
ostracizer. In The social mind: Cognitive and motivational aspects of
interpersonal behavior. (pp. 294-320). New York, NY, US: Cambridge
University Press.
46
Williams, K. D., & Zadro, L. (2005). Ostracism: the indiscriminate early detection
system. In K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. v. Hippel (Eds.), The social
outcast: ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 19-34). New
York: Psychology Press.
47
Appendix A
Informed Consent
Department of Psychology
2801 West Bancroft Street
Toledo, Ohio 43614
Phone: 419-530-2717
Fax: 419-530-8479
ADULT RESEARCH SUBJECT - INFORMED CONSENT FORM
Purpose: You are invited to participate in the research project entitled “Social
networking and emotion,” which is being conducted at the University of Toledo under the
direction of Dr. Jon D. Elhai and Abigail E. Dempsey. The purpose of this study is to
investigate how social media use is related to emotion.
Description of Procedures: This research study will take place online via
PsychData.com and will take approximately 30 minutes to complete. You will be asked
to fill out demographic information and various questionnaires in which you will evaluate
your mental health and social media use.
Potential Risks: A potential risk for participating in this study is loss of confidentiality.
However, to minimize risk, data will be de-identified and kept in secure, password-
protected locations.
Potential Benefits: The only direct benefit to you if you participate in this research may
be that you will learn about how psychology survey studies are run, and you may learn
more about social networking site use and mental health. The field of psychology may
benefit from this research by furthering our understanding of how mental health relates
to social networking site use. Others may benefit by learning about the results of this
research. Upon successful completion of this survey, you will be awarded research
48
credits for your undergraduate psychology course. If you decide not to participate, you
will not receive research credit. However, there are alternatives to participating in
research (e.g., participating in other research studies or writing reviews of research
articles
Voluntary Participation: The information collected from you may be de-identified and
used for future research purposes. As a reminder, your participation in this research is
voluntary. Your refusal to participate in this study will involve no penalty or loss of
benefits to which you are otherwise entitled and will not affect your relationship with The
University of Toledo or any of your classes. You may skip any questions that you may be
uncomfortable answering. In addition, you may discontinue participation at any time
without any penalty or loss of benefits.
Contact Information: If you have any questions at any time before, during or after your
participation or experience any psychological distress as a result of this research, you
should contact a member of the research team: Dr. Jon D. Elhai (419-530-2829) or
Abigail E. Dempsey (419-530-2721).
If you have questions beyond those answered by the research team or your rights as a
research subject or research-related injuries, the Chairperson of the SBE Institutional
Review Board may be contacted through the Human Research Protection Program on
the main campus at (419) 530-6167.
You are making a decision whether or not to participate in this research study. By
checking the box below, you are giving an electronic signature. Your signature indicates
that you have read the information provided above, you have had all your questions
answered, and you have decided to take part in this research. You may take as much
time as necessary to think it over.
By participating in this research, you confirm that you are at least 18 years old.
49
Appendix B
Proposed Analyses
The initial plan was to compute a measurement model for the SAS-SV—as the
primary dependent variable—using CFA. Items on the SAS-SV would have been treated
would have been estimated using weighted least squares estimation with mean-and
variance-adjusted chi-square, and factor loadings would have been estimated using probit
regression (DiStefano & Morgan, 2014). The latent factor would have been scaled by
fixing its factor variance to 1, freely estimating all factor loadings. Missing item-level
values would have been estimated by maximum likelihood procedures. Residual error
According to this initial plan, reported fit indices would have been the
comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), root mean square error of
approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) with CFI
and TLI > .95, RMSEA < .06, and SRMR < .08 typically indicating adequate fit (Hu &
Bentler, 1999). The original sample size (n = 300) could have worked for a fully latent
50
(n = 140) had to be excluded for not using all three SNSs despite all three sites being
popular among young people. Given the remaining subjects after finishing a second
there would not be adequate power for SEM. Therefore, I proceeded with the backup plan
discussed below, which includes testing regression and mediation in lieu of SEM. The
The model in Figure B-1 would have been tested using structural equation
modeling (SEM). Depression severity, anxiety severity, and belongingness would have
been specified to predict FoMO, and FoMO would have been predicted to mediate the
problematic SNS use severity on Instagram, Twitter, and Snapchat. The SAS-SV for each
of the SNSs would have been modeled as latent variables; all other psychological
constructs would have been treated as observed scaled scores to preserve statistical
power. The same estimation approach would have been used for SEM as for CFA. If sex
was bivariately associated with problematic SNS use variables, we would have used sex
51
Figure B-1: Original hypothesized model. PSNS-I, PSNS-S, and PSNS-T = problematic
Instagram use, problematic Snapchat use, and problematic Twitter use, respectively.
Circles represent latent variables; squares represent observed variables. For visual clarity,
the latent problematic SNS use variables’ items are not pictured.
52