Growthdevelopment
Growthdevelopment
Growthdevelopment
The purpose of this chapter is to present a review of the research pertaining to the impact of
music education on several aspects of a child’s growth and development. The topics to be
explored include: perception and cognition, motor development, emotional development, social
development, at-risk students, and special needs students. The research cited was found by
searching Dissertations Online, ERIC, Music Index, RILM, PsycINFO, Social Sciences Index,
and the two handbooks of music teaching research (Colwell, 1992; Colwell & Richardson,
2002). Due to the space limitations of this document, the research cited in each topic area is a
representative sample of the ideas found rather than a presentation of all existing research. Some
topics have been studied less extensively over the years than the others and, as noted, are in need
summarize the findings and will identify areas in need of future research.
In this section, I will approach the topic of perception and cognition from two discrete
perspectives: (a) the impact of music listening and (b) the impact of music instruction.
Schellenberg (2001, 2003) argues that each activity (listening vs. taking instruction) has a unique
impact on perceptual and cognitive function. I would add that as such, the implications for
Music Listening
Infants
The first indications of a response to musical stimuli occur in the last three months of
pregnancy. During this period, the auditory cortex and neurons of a fetus have stabilized and are
functioning (Lecanuet, 1996; Moore, Vareyar, Fulford, Tyler, Gribben, Baker, James, &
Growland, 2001). Researchers have found clear evidence of fetal responses to musical stimuli
(Abrams, Griffith, & Huang, 1998; Blum, 1998; Staley, Iragui, & Spitz, 1990). Furthermore, it is
thought that musical sounds introduced in utero, after the auditory cortex has developed, can
affect subsequent infant behavior (Olds, 1985). O’Connell (2003) found that infants who were
exposed to a music timbre prenatally were able to discriminate, as measured by heart rate,
between their prenatal timbre and other similar timbres through one week of age postnatally. In
an experiment examining infants born prematurely, Lynch, Short, and Chua (1995) found that
intensive care environments, Lorch, Lorch, Deifendor, and Early (1994) found that infants
responded with differential changes in vital functions including heart rate, blood pressure, and
The results of studies with infants at varying points within the first twelve months provide
much supportive evidence for the existence of sophisticated musical functioning. Trainor, Tsang,
and Cheung (2002) found that two- and four-month-old infants preferred consonant rather than
dissonant intervals. Further, the researchers found it difficult to recapture subjects’ interest after
a series of dissonant interval trials. They concluded that consonance perception could provide a
means of learning the pitch structure of the musical system to which an infant may be exposed.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.3
Hannon and Johnson (2005) employed a habituation strategy to examine whether seven-
month-old infants could categorize rhythmic and melodic patterns on the basis of the underlying
meter. Infants presented with metrical melodies detected reversals of pitch and meter while
concluded that infants can infer meter from rhythmic patterns and that they can use this metrical
Ilari (2004) investigated infants' preferences and long-term memory for two contrasting
complex pieces of music. Eight-and-a-half-month-olds were found to distinguish the two pieces
in orchestra timbre and could discriminate between the piano and the orchestra timbres. Ilari
concluded that contrary to the belief that infants are ill equipped to process complex music,
infants could encode and remember complex pieces of music for at least two weeks.
Saffran, Loman, and Robertson (2000) found that infant subjects retained familiarized music
in long-term memory. The infant’s listening preferences were affected by the extent to which
familiar passages were removed from the musical contexts within which they were originally
learned.
Schmidt, Trainor, & Santesso, (2003) examined EEG and heart rate responses to affective
musical stimuli development of infants' at the third, sixth, ninth, and twelfth month. The
distribution of EEG power was found to change across age. Younger subjects demonstrated no
difference in activity between two specific regions of the brain while older subjects exhibited
relatively more activation in one region than the other. This likely reflects the normal maturation
of frontal lobe function. Further, when compared with a baseline, the presentation of
emotionally-expressive music significantly increased brain activity at the third month, had little
effect at the sixth and ninth month, and significantly lessened brain activity at the twelfth month.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.4
The researchers concluded that there was a distinct developmental effect of music on brain
activity in the first year, with music having a "calming" influence on infants by the end of the
The array of musical responses demonstrated by infants in the first year of life is considered
to be more a reflection of innate capabilities made available through normal brain development
than through learning (Imberty, 2000; Trehub, 2000). Further research is needed to determine
whether or not there exists a point after the first year at which music learning begins to support
and possibly enhance natural brain development to result in enhanced musical capabilities.
Similarly, it is important to examine whether there exists critical periods during which particular
types of exposure must occur for future musical development to take place.
Music, originally associated with specific information, has been found to significantly
improve the recall of that information (Balch, Bowman, & Mohler, 1992; Boltz, Schulkind, &
Kantra, 1991; Wallace, 1994). However, this effect can be explained easily by stimulus
response learned to one stimulus to a similar stimulus. Research is needed that includes
comparisons with other (non-music) stimuli to determine whether a music-dependent effect can
be retained.
Since 1993, when Raucher, Shaw, and Ky (1993) published their initial findings linking an
profession has had to wrestle with a phenomenon labeled the “Mozart Effect.”® The notoriety
created by associating a simple passive music-listening task to such a highly coveted result as
Child’s Growth & Development 3.5
improved intelligence is something with which the music education community has not been
entirely comfortable. This is true for several reasons. First, valuing music education for its
Reimer, 1999). Second, the effect has been difficult to replicate consistently. In the body of
subtest found by Rauscher, et al. (1993) to be linked to music listening], some researchers found
significant positive results supporting the effect (Rideout, Dougherty, & Wernert, 1998; Rideout
& Laubach, 1996; Rideout & Taylor, 1997) while others did not (Carstens, Huskins, & Houshell,
1995; Chabris, 1999; Steele, Bass & Crook, 1999; Steele, Brown, & Stoecker, 1999; Steele,
Della Bella, Peretz, Dunlop, Dawe, Humphrey, Shannon, Kirby, & Olmstead, 1999). Further,
when examining alternative measures of IQ such as working memory (Steele, Ball & Runk,
1997) or abstract reasoning (Newman, Rosenbach, Burns, & Laitmer, 1995; Stough, Kerkin,
Bates, & Mangan, 1994), no support was found for the effect.
Some resolution to the questions surrounding the “Mozart Effect”® may be found in a series
of studies that were systematically conducted to uncover understanding about the underlying
mechanisms driving the effect. The effect was found not to be limited to the music of Mozart
(Jackson & Tlauka, 2004) nor even to music in general (Nantais & Schellenberg, 1999). Rather,
specifically higher arousal levels and positive moods being associated with improved spatial
ability (Husain, Thompson, & Schellenberg, 2002; Thompson, Schellenberg, & Husain, 2001).
Further, this arousal and mood effect could be generalized to non-spatial ability measures of IQ
as well as to measures of creative ability (Schellenberg, Nakata, Hunter, & Tamoto, in press). In
short, music has been found to affect IQ, creative ability, and other cognitive functions, but only
Child’s Growth & Development 3.6
to the extent that it does so by affecting arousal and mood. In essence, consistent supportive
evidence has been found for an “arousal and mood effect” rather than for a “Mozart Effect”® per
se.
Musical Training
There are a growing number of studies that have used electroencephalogram (EEG),
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and to a lesser extent, positron emission
tomography (PET) to track brain activity in children in an effort to uncover connections between
such activity and music learning experiences. Music is a widely-distributed system in the brain
with various musical tasks processed differentially in the hemispheres. In general the left
hemisphere tends to be more sensitive to pitch processing (e.g., melody) and the right to
temporal processing (e.g., rhythm); there are some indications that these hemispheric
specializations may develop with age (Overy, Norton, Cronin, Gaab, Alsop, Winner, & Schlaug,
2004). Further, exposure to different styles of music produces varying types of brain activity in
children (Flohr & Miller, 1995). Altenmüller, Gruhn, Parlitz, and Kahrs (1996) found that a five-
week period of musical training, compared to other types of instruction, produced unique cortical
brain activation patterns. Flohr, Persellin, and Miller (1996) found EEG activity differences in
children receiving music instruction compared with those receiving non-music instruction.
Similarly, when comparing three distinct types of notation used to represent music (musical,
verbal, and numerical), Schon, Anton, Roth, & Besson (2002) found that reading musical
notation produced activity in unique regions of the brain, indicating that the visuo-motor
transcoding pathways used for reading musical notation may differ from those used with reading
examine the connection between music learning and cognitive development. Music instruction is
(Hetland, 2000), visual-motor integration (Orsmond & Miller, 1999), selective attention
(Hurwitz, Wolff, Bortnick, & Kokas, 1975), memory for verbal stimuli (Chan, Ho, & Cheung,
1998; Ho, Cheung, & Chan, 2003; Jakobson, Cuddy, & Kilgour, 2003; Kilgour, Jakobson, &
Cuddy, 2000), reading ability (Butzlaff, 2000), and mathematical skills (Vaughn, 2000).
In the effort to venture beyond correlational research and explore a causal connection
between music instruction and various relevant independent variables, several researchers have
employed a random subject assignment to various music and non-music instruction treatments to
curricular activities, etc.) would not affect the results. When using a randomized sample with an
experimental design, Lu (1986) found no significant effect of music instruction on the reading
ability of first-grade students. In contrast, several researchers have found a causal influence of
music instruction on spatial ability using random subject-assignment procedures (Gromko &
Poorman, 1998; Rauscher, 2002; Rauscher & Zupan, 2000). However, their results could not
support the notion that the influence of music instruction is unique among other types of
instruction or that the influence would be not found with other, more general indicators of
In a more recent study, Schellenberg (2004) sought to examine the unique effect of music
Intelligence Scale for Children Third Edition (Weschler, 1991). The Weschler test is comprised
of four indices, verbal comprehension, perceptual organization, processing speed, and freedom
from distractibility that are combined to provide an overall IQ score. Compared to the drama
group and the no instruction control group, the music subjects produced greater increases on all
four indices. These findings support the notion that music instruction when compared to drama
by a global indicator that included dimensions other than spatial ability. Further research is
needed to examine the effects of music instruction on additional global indicators of intelligence.
There is also a need to continue comparing music instruction with other types of instruction to
When reviewing brain research using EEG and other neuroscientific tools, it is clear that
musical experiences and music instruction produce specific and predictable patterns of activity in
the brain. What is not so clear is the exact meaning of those predictable patterns of brain activity
regarding cognitive function. Conversely, there is substantial correlation evidence linking music
Further, researchers are beginning to establish causal connections between music instruction and
improved performance on some of those operational indicators. The challenge to this line of
research is that operational indicators, no matter how reliable, constitute indirect measures of a
phenomenon. Perhaps the next logical step in exploring the effect of music instruction on
cognitive function might be to combine the use of neuroscientific tools and operational indicators
to determine whether predictable and directly observable patterns of brain activity can be
associated with specific cognitive abilities. In fact, such a study is now underway (Schlaug,
Child’s Growth & Development 3.9
Norton, Overy, Cronin, Lee, & Winner, 2004). Beginning instrumentalists are undergoing brain
scans during the first three years of study along with taking a battery of cognitive assessments.
Ideally, this study will provide critical information concerning the effects of music instruction on
brain and cognitive development. More research is needed that combines the use of
neuroscientific tools and operational indicators to explore the effects of music learning effects of
MOTOR DEVELOPMENT
Many researchers have examined how motor skill development affects music learning
(Campbell, 1991; Sidnell, 1986; Synder, 1988 Turner, 1998;) and performance, especially that of
conductors (Kun, 2004; Neiman, 1989; Neidlinger, 2003) and pianists (Ragert, Schmidt,
Altenmüller, & Dinse, 2004; Waters, 1992). Phillips-Silver and Trainor (2005) found that even
healthy seven-month-old infants learned to distinguish between duple and triple meter with the
aid of accompanying bouncing movements significantly more accurately than with a passive
listening treatment.
Far fewer have examined how musical stimuli and musical learning impact motor
expressed purpose of this section to feature the latter. The small amount of existing research
concerning the impact of music education on motor development uses subjects grouped in the
following categories: (a) early childhood and preschool, and (b) school-aged children.
Gruhn, (2002) conducted a longitudinal study that was designed to examine the phases and
stages in children's early music learning. A group of 12 children (six male; six female) aged one
Child’s Growth & Development 3.10
to two years was recruited from an urban, upper middle class area in Freiburg, Germany and
observed with respect to their musical behavior in a stimulating musical setting for 15 months.
This group was compared with a control group aged one-two years (N = 9; three male, six
female) from a local nursery school. All children were videotaped and then evaluated by two
independent judges using a criterion-based observation form with ratings for the categories
both groups displayed a similar developmental level at the start of the study, differences between
the two groups became evident throughout the observation period. The control group, which was
exposed to no particular music except the songs of the daycare program, developed body
movement and vocal performance at a significantly lower level than their counterparts. The most
significant effect within the experimental group was a strong interaction between flow of
movement and motor coordination, and vocalization of tonal and rhythm patterns.
Gilbert (1980) designed the Motoric Music Skills Test (MMST) to examine how young
children utilized motor tasks with music. The MMST consisted of five subtests: (a) motor pattern
coordination, (b) eye-hand coordination, (c) speed of movement (d) range of movement, and (e)
compound factors. Gilbert administered the MMST to 808 children, ages three through six. Girls
coordination, and compound factors. This result is consistent with findings of Flohr (1991) on
tests of maintaining steady beat and those of Schleuter and Schleuter (1985, 1989) on three
specific motor skills (clapping, stepping, and chanting). Gilbert (1981) conducted a follow-up
study utilizing a stratified random sample from the population of 808 original study subjects to
determine whether or not the original findings could be confirmed and to examine whether
motoric music skills stabilized with age. The gain scores of four-year-olds were significantly
Child’s Growth & Development 3.11
greater than those of seven-year olds in every subtest of the MMST. Gilbert asserts that these
results substantiate the idea, common among motor theorists, that most fundamental motor
patterns emerge before age five and stabilize beyond that point. However, in both of Gilbert’s
investigations, it was unclear whether the improvement was due in any part to music instruction.
Brown, Sherrill, & Gench, (1981) examined two approaches to facilitating perceptual-motor
development in four-six year old kindergartners (N = 30). The experimental group received 24
sessions of integrated physical education/music instruction, while the control group received 24
indicated that significant improvement occurred only in the experimental group with changes in
the motor, auditory, and language aspects of perceptual-motor performance as well as in the total
score.
Kalmar, (1982) examined the effects of a method of singing instruction involving the
accompaniment of music with rhythmic movements and the verbal or physical representation of
songs on the development of young children. Twenty three-year-olds were pretested and
assigned to either the experimental group, that received twice-weekly special singing lessons
based on the Kodaly method over three years, or the control group, that attended only regular
nursery school programs. The experimental group showed greater improvement than the control
thinking, and play improvisation and originality. On an adaptation of the Torrance Tests of
requiring verbal responses, but not on those involving drawing. Further, no between-group
students regarding musical aptitude, rhythmic ability, and motor performance. Subjects (N = 95),
including five-year-old girls (n = 50) and boys (n = 45), were administered the (Gordon) Primary
Measures of Music Audiation, the High/Scope Rhythmic Competence Analysis Test, and the
Gross Motor Development Test. Although no significant gender differences were found in
musical aptitude and gross motor skills performance, girls outperformed boys in four of the six
Zachopoulou, Tsapakidoub, & Derric, (2004) compared the effect of a music and movement
program to a physical education program on the development of jumping and dynamic balance in
children ages four through six. Subjects (N = 90) were placed into either an experimental group
(n = 50) that followed the music and movement program or a control group (n = 40) that
followed the physical education program. All subjects received instruction two days a week for
35-40 minutes each day over a two-month period. Pretest and posttest data were analyzed using a
multivariate analysis of variance with repeated measures. The control group showed no
improvement. The experimental group improved significantly in both the jumping and dynamic
balance tasks. According to the authors, rhythmic ability is the ability to observe, control, and
differentiate the rhythm of a movement according to the environmental demands at a given time,
enabling the quick motor adjustments of the performer in an unpredictable environment and
assuring success in performance. The authors conclude that music and movement education
School-Aged Children
DeVries (2004) sought to investigate the extra-musical effects of a music education program
in one preschool classroom over a period of six weeks. The class had not previously been
Child’s Growth & Development 3.13
exposed to regular music lessons. Among increases in other variables including socialization,
Baer (1987) sought to examine the strength and nature of the relationships (a) between music
aptitude and motor development and (b) between instrumental music achievement and motor
development utilizing, as subjects, 136 middle school students in grades seven through nine who
were members of their school band or orchestra. A low-moderate positive correlation was found
between music aptitude and motor development (r = .33) and between instrumental music
achievement and motor development (r = .26). Further, higher correlations between motor
development and musical performance achievement were found for string instrument players as
compared to wind and percussion instrument players, and for girls as compared to boys. Music
aptitude was related to music gross motor skills more so than to fine motor skills; however,
musical performance achievement was related to fine motor skills more so than to gross motor
skills. Results need to be interpreted with caution due to the intact nature of the subject sample.
Although a small number of references were found for studies examining the effect of music
and music education on motor development, a few clear patterns appear to emerge. For
maintaining a steady beat, specific music motor skills, and movement tests of rhythmic ability.
They also do better on general motor tasks with music such as measures of motor pattern
coordination and eye-hand coordination. More research is needed to determine whether those
differences persist or fade as students age and/or mature in other ways. Such developmental
information would be helpful to practitioners when planning for the use of instructional
materials.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.14
Another emerging pattern is that a stimulating musical environment has a positive effect on
motor coordination and perceptual motor development, as well as on general motor skills. For
school aged children, less is apparent due simply to the paucity of research on this level.
Currently, most of the conclusive evidence is with samples drawn from younger populations,
foreshadowing the possibility that music involvement at an early age may have a fundamental
impact on a variety of different resultant skills. More research is needed at all levels of
development to determine the extent to which musical experiences affect motor development.
EMOTIONAL DEVELOPMENT
Nelson (1983) addressed basic issues concerning the dual nature of music learning by
defining the relationships between cognitive and affective growth in children and by discussing
aspects of music learning in relation to its aesthetic nature. A theoretical model of affective
development proposed by Giblin (1981) was compared to the Piagetian theories of intellectual
development. Nelson concluded that such a comparison “falls short” in accurately describing the
personal nature of aesthetic responsiveness to music. He stated the need for specific definitions
Since the early 1990’s, attention has been afforded the Continuous Response Digital
Interface (CRDI) as a tool for synchronously measuring subjects’ emotional responses to musical
stimuli (Madsen, Brittin, & Caperella-Sheldon, 1993; Madsen, Caperella-Sheldon, & Johnson,
1991; Madsen & Fredrickson, 1993). With advent of the CRDI, researchers investigating
emotional response to music have examined a variety of variables, including among others,
visual/aural conditions (Adams, 1995; Frego, 1999), preference for music of other cultures
Child’s Growth & Development 3.15
(Brittin, 1996), the effect of performer use of rubato on listener perception of tension
Of particular interest to the present review are those studies in which subjects of varying
levels of music experience or education have been compared. Brittin (1996) compared college
music majors’ (n = 75), non-music majors’ (n = 75), and junior high school musicians’ (n = 75)
preferences for music of other cultures. Further, subjects from each group were randomly
assigned one of three response modes: (a) Likert-type scales, (b) one CRDI device indicating
preference only, and (c) two CRDI devices, one indicating preference and the other indicating
complexity of the excerpts. No differences were found between subjects using one CRDI and
those using two CRDIs. Subjects using one or two CRDIs rated sections significantly higher than
did subjects using paper-and-pencil rating scales. No significant differences in preference for
music from various cultures were found among any of the three levels of music experience.
Frego (1999) compared college-level musicians (n = 81) and non-musicians (n = 81) when
examining the effects of aural, visual, and combined aural/visual conditions on the emotional
response to music and dance. Subjects were randomly assigned to the three conditions and asked
to respond to three dance and music performances by indicating the degree of perceived artistic
tension using a CRDI device. No significant differences were found among the three conditions.
Further, no significant differences were found in perceived artistic tension between musicians
and non-musicians.
Fredrickson (1999) examined whether musicians who rehearse and perform a musical
selection perceive tension in the music differently than do listeners who have not had the
performance experience. Visual inspection of CRDI graph results was used to confirm that
Child’s Growth & Development 3.16
experience of performing the music did not seem to greatly affect perception of tension as
Forty college music majors and thirty non-majors were asked to use a CRDI device to record
their perceptions of tension in two selections of jazz music (Fredrickson & Coggiola, 2003).
Each selection was a uniquely stylized version of “St. Louis Blues” by W. C. Handy. Music
majors’ responses did not seem to differ markedly in overall contour from non-music majors’
responses, which is consistent with previous research. The authors noted that subjects had no
trouble performing the task or using an existing internal definition of musical tension.
Common to all of the studies above is that level of experience or education does not seem to
be a factor when asking subjects to provide an emotional response to a variety of musical stimuli.
Those with less music experience seem to have similar emotional responses to musical stimuli as
do those with more music experience. Further, when presented two or more distinct musical
stimuli, subjects, regardless of experience level, offered distinctly unique responses to each
stimulus. Perhaps, as Fredrickson and Coggiola (2003) assert, people tend to employ an existing
internal definition of musical tension and that definition does not seems to be affected by a
Parisi (2004) assessed fourth- and fifth-grade students’ (N = 102) affective response and
ability to discriminate between melody and improvisation after receiving instruction in singing
and/or playing a piece in the blues style. To facilitate responses being differentiated across time
rather than in aggregate by use of arithmetic means, an a priori decision was made to analyze the
data graphically and descriptively. Subjects receiving specific instruction in melodic and
Child’s Growth & Development 3.17
improvisatory discrimination responded with a higher level of discriminatory skill and positive
Coggiola (2004) compared the aesthetic responses of jazz musicians (n = 64) and non-jazz
musicians (n = 64) when listening to jazz music selections that vary in level of conceptual
advancement (melodic complexity during improvised solos). Data were gathered as participants
manipulated a CRDI dial to indicate the magnitude of their aesthetic responses as they listened to
four audio selections. Of the four examples, a significant difference between the two participant
groups was found only for the most conceptually advanced selection. Jazz musicians rated this
selection significantly higher than did non-jazz musicians. Coggiola concluded that greater
instrumental jazz ensemble experience is related to greater aesthetic interest when listening to a
Misenhelter and Price (2001) asked undergraduate non-music majors (n = 32) and
undergraduate and graduate music majors (n = 42) to listen and manipulate a CRDI dial to
indicate their affective responses to excerpts from a sophisticated piece of music (Igor
between music and non-music majors. Visual analyses revealed that the variability between the
groups most often corresponded with changes in musical texture. The authors concluded that
non-musicians in particular may have insufficient musical background or exposure to “absorb the
Ruggeri (2003) sought to describe the experience of adult amateur musicians as they pursued
their passion of playing chamber music. Ruggeri also explored the importance and meaning of
Child’s Growth & Development 3.18
this activity in their lives. Musicians in the study reported the ability to express their identity in a
non-verbal conversation, producing a pleasurable sound and engaging their faculties in a state of
process involving perceptual, emotional, intuitive and kinesthetic development and pattern
comprehension as well as a sense of deep fulfillment derived from sustained attention. These
alternative modes of understanding seem to support the assumption that learners create their own
knowledge and meaning. Ruggeri concluded by arguing that aesthetic experience is central to
adult education and learning. I would add that such an experience can and should be central to
Sloboda (1985) suggests that “untrained musicians have implicit knowledge of that which
musicians can talk about explicitly” (p. 5). Perhaps such “implicit knowledge,” similar to
Fredrickson and Coggiola’s (2003) “existing internal definition of musical tension,” is self-
evident when responding to common musical stimuli. The results found by Coggiola (2004) and
by Misenhelter and Price (2001), however, support the idea that responding to musical stimuli
that exceed a particular level of musical or compositional sophistication might not be implicit to
the less experienced or less knowledgeable listener. Similarly, musical instruction in much
younger students seems to influence how they might respond emotionally to musical stimuli
(Parisi, 2004). Perhaps what may be concluded from this portion of the review is that although
most people respond emotionally to music, music instruction seems to have a positive effect on
the level of sophistication with which one is able to experience that emotional response.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.19
From a broader perspective, however, there is a need to study the impact of music education
on emotional response and development using a wider range of tools than only the CRDI dial.
Although the CRDI provides rich and vast amounts of quantitative information, it is but one way
to examine a topic that is highly complex and inherently multifaceted. Research, using additional
tools, is needed to verify the “sophistication effect” mentioned above. Nelson (1983) noted the
need for specific definitions of affective learning as it might relate to music concept
achieving what Maslow (1964) describes as the self-actualizing individual or what Rogers (1961)
describes as the fully-functioning adult is often at the heart of what many philosophically offer as
a primary benefit of music and music education. Policy makers tend to reward or support
initiatives that demonstrate some sort of measurable pragmatic benefit. What is needed is
convincing philosophical inquiry as well as other types of research into the benefits of emotional
development and an exploration into the unique contributions that music and music education
may provide.
SOCIAL DEVELOPMENT
Many researchers exploring the connection between music learning and the social
development of children have utilized special populations as subjects; however, very few social
development investigations have been conducted using normal populations. Forrai (1997)
conducted a three-year longitudinal study comparing children, aged six months to three years,
exposed to a greater number of musical influences with children of the same age exposed to little
singing or music. The group with more musical influences exceeded the other group over several
dependent variables, including initiations of social contact. Hood (1973) examined the effect of
daily music instruction on nonmusical personal and school activities and found that students
Child’s Growth & Development 3.20
receiving daily music instruction had a significantly lower rate of absenteeism than did students
music experiences. The program was implemented in the toddler playroom of a daycare center
for ten months. The music experiences provided in the program included free music play,
sociable music experiences, and music incorporated into care-giving routines. The subject’s
responses were recorded and changes in the physical, language and social development were
documented. Based on the findings the researcher concluded that much of the learning occurred
through imitation and social interaction and that participation in self-selected music experiences
McCusker (2001) qualitatively examined the graphic invented notations created by eleven
children, ages five to seven, in an effort to gain an understanding of the cognitive processes
underlying emerging musical literacy. The researcher found that the difference between children
responding only to teacher-initiated activities and those using familiar music materials for
creative self-statements was a stronger influence than age. An unexpected theme that emerged
from the data was subjects’ development as a community of learners and the influence this social
context had on children's music making. Miller (1983) found a similar phenomenon when
examining behaviors that young children demonstrate naturally and describing how these
behaviors manifested in social interactions with peers. Three-, four-, and five-year-olds served as
subjects (N = 95). Subjects were found to be capable of creating music without teacher
intervention. Further, three-year-olds tended to be involved in symbolic solitary play, while the
older children exhibited more social interactions such as imitating each other.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.21
From the scant literature that exists concerning the effect of music on normal populations’
social development, there appears to be several positive effects. Those include an increased
initiation of social contact, lowered rates of absenteeism, and an emerging propensity for self-
directed and group-directed learning, rather than teacher-directed learning. Though this
information provides some new knowledge, it is disparate and, as such, difficult to draw useful
number of studies are needed to examine particular aspects of music’s effect on social
development. For example, the existing literature provides a start to examining connections
between musical involvement and self- and/or group-directed learning. Investigations could be
conducted into how and why such connections occur as well as effects on the development of
subsequent skills such as leadership, dynamic cooperative learning, and independent and creative
a greater awareness of others around them. Research is needed to examine how music learning
may facilitate such awareness for students as well as long-term impacts on society in general.
Much has been written about the positive effects of participation in extra curricular activities,
the arts in particular, and music specifically on at-risk students, precious little of it being
research. Authors of books, book chapters, and journal articles tend to describe programs,
projects, and particular situations in which participation in the arts have positively affected
specific students and groups of students. Although a good many of these descriptions are rich
with detail and may provide a vast number of potential researchable ideas, none can be said to
Child’s Growth & Development 3.22
have undergone the rigor associated with systematic inquiry. In the following section I will
review the small amount of research that exits in this area and highlight trends, draw
There is consistent empirical support for a positive correlation between being involved in
extra-curricular activities and staying in school. Bowman and Matthews (1960) conducted a
longitudinal study and found that dropouts were involved in fewer extra curricular activities than
those who remained in school through graduation. Similarly, Brooks (1989) found that Hispanic
females who remained in school participated in extracurricular activities more often than
dropouts. Two studies commissioned by state organizations found similar results. The New York
were mailed to 2,448 graduates and 1,286 dropouts. Graduates were found to participate more in
all activities and services than dropouts and they placed more value on the activities than did the
commissioned by the West Virginia Department of Education, 93% of dropouts were found to
never participate in extra curricular activities (West Virginia School Dropout Prevention Task
Force, 1991).
Another trend is that early intervention has a strong positive effect. Gudeman (1987) studied
the Union School District of New Jersey as a model in the area of dropout control with a
population of over 6,000 students and a dropout rate of 1 percent. He concluded that dropping
out of school can be prevented with reduced effort and expense when strategies are implemented
during the preschool years and the first several grades of elementary school. Simner and Barnes
Child’s Growth & Development 3.23
(1991, Winter) found that 70% to 80% of those children who are at risk for early school failure
can be identified one to two years before they enter first grade. Similarly, Wilcynski, (1987)
sought to determine the extent to which selected characteristics that exist among elementary age
children are accurate predictors of students who will leave school prior to high school
cluster had the greatest relationship with dropouts and graduates. Adding either student
Several researchers have found that participation in the arts has positive effects on at-risk
students. Acer (1987) concluded that integrating the study of language, music, drama, and dance
for at-risk students could address many recommendations for juvenile delinquency prevention.
Teachers from 43 urban and ten suburban elementary schools, subjects in the study, noted
their at-risk students. After comparing self-concept scores of before and after participation in a
project providing experiences in dance, drama, music, and visual arts, Barry (1992) found
between achieving and underachieving students, Ciborowski (1986) found that achieving
students expressed more interest in life activities that involved music and drama, among other
variables, than did underachieving students. In a study published by the Center for Music
Research (1990), at-risk students were found to remain in school solely because of their
commitment to music programs such as band, choir, and orchestra, as well as other arts including
Child’s Growth & Development 3.24
dance, drama, painting, and sculpture. Bond, Smith, Ross, Nunnery and Goldstein (1992)
investigated the effectiveness of a reading program that incorporated singing. The program was
found to be more effective for younger children (K-first grade) and for children of low economic
status (many of them being at risk). Coronado (1999) investigated the effects of a summer
performing arts program, Summer At The Center, on at-risk students' school attendance, GPA,
and arts participation among other variables. No significant increase in school attendance rates
was found; however, the grade point averages of the students increased significantly between
before and after participating in the program. Further, arts participation was also found to have
significantly increased after Summer At The Center finished. Taetle (1999) investigated the
relationships among daily school attendance, grade point average (GPA), and enrollment in fine
arts electives. Students with lower absentee rates tended to have higher GPAs, Further, students
not enrolled in fine arts electives had significantly higher absentee rates than those students with
at least one fine arts elective. Finally, students with low GPAs who were not enrolled in fine arts
electives had significantly higher absentee rates than those students who were enrolled in at least
There are very few investigations in which the specific role of music learning is examined for
its effect on at-risk students. Jenlink (1993) conducted a qualitative study of a school's attempts
to raise the self-esteem of its at-risk students by emphasizing the school's music program. The
author concluded that the music program lessened students' feelings of alienation, promoted
individual growth, and provided a common bond between the home and the school. Further,
participation in the select musical performing group promoted goal attainment, teamwork,
Nelson (1997) investigated participation in choral music as an affective intervention for high-
risk adolescent males. The choral program was found to be an affective intervention for high-risk
adolescent males. Shifts in self-perceptions and in how the choir members grew to value their
choral experience emerged in three categories: status, co-musical benefits, and inner rewards.
Affective assertions included: (a) performing in choir was special to them, (b) the relationships
they developed in choir were different than those they had with other residents, and (c) there
were moments in choir that were wonderful, difficult to verbalize, and for many, deeply
personal.
program and attempt to account for such success. The three primary factors appearing to affect
the success of the program were (a) teacher commitment, (b) suitability of curriculum, and (c)
students who were successful in music but not in other classes. Five conclusions were drawn
personal accomplishment.
3. Participation in a music ensemble can provide students with leadership roles and give
4. Music itself seems to be a salient factor in improving the lives of at-risk students.
5. The extent to which the music teacher becomes involved in the life of the music
Shields (2001) investigated the role and importance of music education as an intervention for
at-risk urban adolescents through participation in performance groups and being mentored.
Further, students’ ranking of music as being important in their lives improved from 76% to 82 %.
Interviews provided evidence of the importance of music, music education, and the music
positively correlated with staying in school. Further, precursors to dropping out can be detected
and effective interventions implemented during the preschool and early elementary school years.
Participation in the arts, as a specific extra curricular area, is consistently found to (a) affect self-
esteem, (b) be correlated with lower absenteeism, and (c) be associated with staying in school.
Of the studies in which the effects of music instruction on at-risk students are examined, the
results are positive but difficult to use as a basis from which to draw conclusions. This is due to
the few number of investigations and the disparate variety of approaches across the
investigations. Similar to the suggestions for further research into the impact of music learning
on social development, systematic lines of research over a number of studies are needed to
examine specific aspects of music’s effect on at-risk students. Such lines of research need to be
of sufficient length and similarity so as to provide the depth of inquiry needed to draw
appropriate and valid conclusions. One example of such a line might be to examine whether
music learning is an effective early intervention. In what ways might music serve as an early
intervention that other disciplines might not? Another line might be to examine each of the
Child’s Growth & Development 3.27
affective assertions proposed by Nelson (1997) to determine the degree to which involvement in
Researchers exploring the connection between music and special needs students have
examined the topic in a variety of different ways. In a substantial number of studies, researchers
have examined attitudes toward and acceptance of those with special needs using music students
as the population from which samples were drawn (Jellison, Brooks, & Huck, 1984: Johnson &
Darrow, 1997, 2003). Others have examined the effect of inclusion on the music learning of
those with and without disabilities (Jellison, 2002; Force, 1983). Still others have tested the
effectiveness of various musical instructional methods especially modified for students with
special needs (Colwell, 1995; McCord, 2002). Each of these perspectives is worthy of study
because music class is typically where students with disabilities are initially mainstreamed.
Knowing more about attitudes toward special needs students, how their inclusion affects
learning, and how instructional strategies might be best designed is beneficial to music teachers
who tend to encounter special needs students more regularly than their non-arts peers.
Nevertheless, none of these common research tacks actually examine the impact of music and
music education on special needs students, which is the purpose of this section.
Similar to the studies associated with social development and at-risk students, there are few
investigations into the effect of music and music learning on special needs students. Most of
those researchers who have examined how special needs students are impacted by music learning
experiences have specifically investigated the effects of music therapy (as the music learning
variable) on special populations. Aldridge, Gustorff, & Neugebauer, (1995) examined the effects
Child’s Growth & Development 3.28
children, ages four to six and a half, receiving three months of music therapy were compared
with three DD children serving as waiting-list controls. The researchers noted that (a) focused
platform for improved communication, (b) musical dialogue seemed to bring about improvement
in the ability to form and maintain personal social relationships in other contexts, and (c) eye and
activities.
Conversely, Duffy and Fuller (2000) compared subjects in music (n = 16) and in non-music
interventions (n = 16) and found that significant improvement in five target skills (turn-taking,
interventions.
Frick (2000) conducted a case study designed to describe the classroom music activities and
communication patterns of four young children with disabilities in an early childhood special
education classroom, and to explore how types of music, methods of music inclusion, and
The researcher noted that music, presented in a routine manner and supportive of instruction,
resulted in more vocalization. Additionally, Frick noted that music created a social context for
child-to-child interaction.
Robb (2003) studied the behaviors of six visually impaired children between the ages of four
and six years. Subjects participated in four, 30-minute instructional sessions. Two instructional
sessions were music-based and two were play-based with the four sessions equally distributed
Child’s Growth & Development 3.29
across a two-week period. Each session was videotaped to facilitate collection of behavioral data.
Humpal (1991) examined the effects of an integrated early childhood music program on
social interaction among children with handicaps and their typical peers. Interaction among the
children increased following the music therapy intervention phase. It was also concluded that the
program had facilitated peer interaction and had fostered acceptance of differences among
individuals.
Although there is fairly consistent support for use of music as an effective therapy for special
needs students, the profession needs more research. Music therapists are in the unique position of
having to substantiate the worth of their practice to non-musicians (e.g., those in the medical
field). Similar to previous recommendations, the research in this area needs to be conducted in
lines of sufficient length to provide practical information for the therapist as well as clear,
The first indications of responses to musical stimuli occur in the last three months of
pregnancy. Once an infant is born, however, there is a flurry of brain activity and associated
innate capabilities and normal brain development. Research is needed to determine whether there
exists a point after the first year of life at which music learning begins to support and enhance
brain development. Similarly, it is important to examine whether there are critical periods during
which particular types of exposure must occur for future development to take place.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.30
Due to the notoriety of Rauscher, Shaw, and Ky’s (1993) research, measures of spatial ability
have been used as operational indictors of intelligence in a large number of studies. Additional
research is needed to examine the effects of music instruction using other global indicators of
intelligence. Further, there is also a need to continue comparing music instruction with other
types of instruction to determine the degree to which music might be unique in this regard.
Brain research using neuroscientific tools substantiates predictable patterns of brain activity,
but without providing the meaning of that activity. Operational indicators, on the other hand,
provide rich meaningful information, but employ indirect measures to gather that information.
Perhaps the next logical step in exploring the effect of music instruction on cognitive function
might be to combine the use of neuroscientific tools and operational indicators to determine
whether predictable and directly-observable patterns of brain activity can be associated with
Motor Development
Although, at the preschool level, girls tend to outperform boys in music-related psychomotor
tasks, more research is needed to determine whether such differences persist or fade as students
age and/or mature in other ways. Currently, most of the conclusive evidence is with samples
drawn from younger populations, foreshadowing the possibility that music involvement at an
early age may have a fundamental impact on a variety of different resultant skills. More research
is needed at all levels of development to determine the extent to which musical experiences
Emotional Development
Research is needed that investigates the nature of various types of music instruction and how
they may impact emotional response. Performance, as explored by Ruggeri (2003), is one type of
Child’s Growth & Development 3.31
instruction that, due to its participatory nature, evokes a particular type of emotional response.
Judging from the number and popularity of performance-oriented music learning settings,
especially at the junior high and high school levels, emotional response associated with this type
of learning is quite strong. There is a need, however, to investigate other types or hybrid types of
instructional approaches to see how they may impact the emotional development of children. It
would also be helpful to revisit Nelson’s (1983) work in which he compared Giblin’s (1981)
investigations could be conducted to see how a possible alignment of the two theories manifests
stimuli began using the CRDI as a tool for synchronously measuring responses to musical
stimuli. From a broader perspective, there is a need to study the impact of music education on
emotional response and development using a wider range of tools than only the CRDI dial.
Although the CRDI provides rich and vast amounts of quantitative information, it is but one way
to examine a topic that is highly complex and inherently multifaceted. What is needed is
convincing philosophical inquiry as well as other types of research into the benefits of emotional
development and an exploration into the unique contributions that music and music education
may provide.
Social Development
Systematic lines of research over a number of studies are needed to examine particular
aspects of music’s effect on social development. Investigations could be conducted into how and
why such connections occur as well as effects on the development of subsequent skills such as
leadership, dynamic co-operative learning, and independent and creative thinking. Additionally,
Child’s Growth & Development 3.32
others around them. Research is needed to examine how music learning may facilitate such
Of the studies in which the effects of music instruction on at-risk students are examined, the
results are positive but difficult to use as a basis from which to draw conclusions. This is due to
the few number of investigations and the disparate variety of approaches across the
investigations. Similar to the suggestions for further research into the impact of music learning
on social development, systematic lines of research over a number of studies are needed to
examine specific aspects of music’s effect on at-risk students. Such lines of research need to be
of sufficient length and similarity so as to provide the depth of inquiry needed to draw
Similar to previous recommendations, the research in this area needs to be conducted in lines
of sufficient length to provide practical information for the therapist as well as clear, objective,
CHAPTER SUMMARY
The purpose of this chapter was to present a review of the research pertaining to the impact
of music education on several aspects of a child’s growth and development, including perception
and cognition, motor development, emotional development, social development, as well as on at-
risk students, and special needs students. Although individual music education researchers have
explored each of these aspects, very few sustained lines of research exist that can be used to
Child’s Growth & Development 3.33
support the effects of music education on a child’s growth and development. One exception can
be found in the area of perception and cognition; however, most of the work in that area has been
Perhaps the paucity of investigations from our own research community stems from an effort
to avoid substantiating the worth of music and musical education on extra-musical benefits. Such
a philosophical “line in the sand” loses merit when adherence to it results in less knowledge
rather than more. It is important to continue pursuing new information about the impact of music
and music learning on all aspects of a child’s growth and development so that we are fully
equipped to provide the greatest support to our profession. We, as members of the music
education research community, must be equally vigilant that our work be consistently
REFERENCES
Abrams, R. M., Griffith, K. & Huang, X., (1998). Fetal music perception: The role of sound
transmission. Music Perception, 15, 307-317.
Acer, C. C. (1987). Crime, curriculum and the performing arts: A challenge for inner city schools
to consider integrated language, music, drama and dance experiences as compensatory
curriculum for at-risk urban minorities in elementary school. (Doctoral dissertation, State
University of New York at Buffalo, 1987). Dissertation Abstracts International, 48(12A),
3047.
Adams, B. L. (1995). The effect of visual/aural conditions on the emotional response to music.
Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 55 (8A),
UMI No. AAM9434127.
Aldridge, D., Gustorff, D., & Neugebauer, L. (1995). A preliminary study of creative music
therapy in the treatment of children with developmental delay. Arts in Psychotherapy,
22(3), 189-205.
Altenmüller, E., Gruhn, W., Parlitz, D., & Kahrs, J. (1996). Music learning produces changes in
brain activation patterns: A longitudinal DC-EEG study. International Journal of Applied
Music, 1(1), 28-33.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.34
Baer, D. E. (1987). Motor skill proficiency: Its relationship to instrumental music performance
achievement and music aptitude. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, 1987).
Dissertation Abstracts International, 48(6A), 1410.
Balch, W. R, Bowman, K., & Mohler, L. A. (1992). Music-dependent memory in immediate and
delayed word recall. Memory and Cognition, 20(1), 21-28.
Barry, N. H. (1992). Project ARISE: Meeting the needs of disadvantaged students through the
arts. The Professional Educator, 14(2), 1-7.
Boltz, M., Schulkind, M., Kantra, S. (1991). Effects of background music on the remembering of
filmed events. Memory and Cognition, 19(6), 593 606.
Bond, C. L., Smith, L. J., Ross, S. M., Nunnery J. A., & Goldstein, R. R. (1992). An alternative
for teaching “at risk” children? A look at sing, spell, read, and write. (Policy/ Practice
Brief No. 9302). Memphis, TN: Memphis State University, Center for Research in
Educational Policy.
Bowman, P. H., & Matthews, C. V. (1960). Motivations of youth for leaving school
(Cooperative Research Program #200). Quincy, Il: University of Chicago Quincy Youth
Development Program.
Brittin, R. V. (1996). Listeners' preference for music of other cultures: Comparing responses
modes. Journal of Research in Music Education, 44(4), 328 340.
Brown, J., Sherrill, C., & Gench, B. (1981). Effects of an integrated physical education/music
program in changing early childhood perceptual-motor performance. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 53(1), 151-154.
Butzlaff, R. (2000). Can music be used to teach reading? Journal of Aesthetic Education,
34(3/4), 167-178.
Campbell, P. S. (1991). Rhythmic movement and public school music education: Conservative
and progressive views of the formative years. Journal of research in Music Education,
39(1), 12-22.
Carstens, C. B., Huskins, E., & Houshell, G. W. (1995). Listening to Mozart may not enhance
performance on the revised Minnesota Paper Form Board Test. Psychological Reports,
77, 111-114.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.35
Center for Music Research (1990). The role of fine and performing arts in high school dropout
prevention. Tallahassee, FL: Center for Music Research. ED: 354168.
Chabris, C. F., (1999). Prelude or requiem for the Mozart effect. Nature, 400, 826-827.
Chan, A. S., Ho, Y. C., & Cheung, M. C. (1998). Music training improves verbal memory,
Nature, 396, 128.
Coggiola, J. C. (2004). The effect of conceptual advancement in jazz music selections and jazz
experience on musicians' aesthetic response. Journal of Research in Music Education,
52(1), 29 42.
Colwell, R. J. (Ed.) (1992). Handbook of research on music teaching and learning. New York:
Schirmer Books.
Colwell, R. J., & Richardson, C. P. (Eds.) (2002). The new handbook of research on music
teaching and learning. New York: Oxford University Press.
Developing work-study programs for potential dropouts: A manual. (1971). Albany, New York:
University of the State of New York, The State Department Bureau of Guidance.
Duffy, B., and Fuller, R. (2000). Role of music therapy in social skills development in children
with moderate intellectual disability. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual
Disabilities, 13(2), 77-90.
Duke, R. A. (2000). The other Mozart effect: An open letter to music educators. Update:
Applications of Research in Music Education, 19(1), 9-16.
Ebie, B. D. (1998). Can music help? A qualitative investigation of two music educators’ views
on the role of music in the lives of at-risk students. Contributions to Music Education,
25(2), 63-78.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.36
Flohr, J. W. (1991). A preliminary study of young children’s ability to perform a steady beat.
Paper presented at the 1991 Texas Music Educators Convention, San Antonio.
Flohr J. W., & Miller, D. C. (1993). Quantitative EEG differences between baseline and
psychomotor response to music. Texas Music Education Research, ??, 1-7.
Flohr, J. W., & Miller, D. C. (1995). Developmental quantitative EEG differences during
psychomotor response to music. Paper presented at the Texas Music educators
Convention, san Antonio, February, 1995 (ERIC Document PS025653).
Flohr, J. W., Persellin, D. C., & Miller, D. C. (1996). Children’s electrophysical responses to
music. Paper presented at the 22nd International Society for Music Education Worldd
Conference, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, July, 1996 (ERIC Document PS025654).
Fredrickson, W. E., & Coggiola, J. C. (2003). A Comparison of Music Majors' and Nonmajors'
Perceptions of Tension for Two Selections of Jazz Music. Journal of Research in Music
Education, 51(3), 259 270.
Frego, R. J. D. (1999). Effects of aural and visual conditions on response to perceived artistic
tension in music and dance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47(1), 31 43.
Frick, J. W. (2000). A qualitative study of music and communication in a musically rich early
childhood special education classroom. (Doctoral dissertation, George Mason University,
2000). Dissertation Abstracts International, 60(8A), 2868.
Forrai, K. (1997). The Influence of Music on the Development of Young Children: Music
Research with Children between 6 and 40 months. Early Childhood Connections, 3(1),
14-18.Giblin, P. T. (1981). Affective development in children: An equilibrium model.
Genetic Psychology Monographs. 103(1), 3-30.
Gromko, J. E., & Poorman, A. S. (1998). The effect of music training on preschollers’ spatial-
temporal task performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 46, 173-181.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.37
Gruhn, W. (2002). Phases and stages in early music learning: A longitudinal study on the
development of young children's musical potential. Music Education Research, 4(1), 51-
72.
Halsband, U., Binkofski, F., & Camp, M. (1994). The role of the perception of rhythmic
grouping in musical performance: Evidence from motor-skill development in piano
playing. Music Perception, 11(3), 265-288
Hannon, E. E. & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies:
Implications for musical structure learning. Cognitive Psychology, 50(4), 354-77.
Hetland, L. (2000). Learning to make music enhances spatial reasoning. Journal of Aesthetic
Education, 34(3/4), 179-238.
Ho, Y. C., Cheung, M. C., & Chan, A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual
memory: Cross sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology, 17,
439-450.
Hood, B. S. (1973). The effect of daily instruction in public school music and related experiences
upon non-musical personal and schoolattitudes of average achieving third-grade students.
(Doctoral dissertation, Mississippi State University, 1973). Dissertation Abstracts
International, 34, 3024A. (University Microfilms No. 73-31897).
Hurwitz, I., Wolff, P. H., Bortnick, B. D., & Kokas, K. 1(975). Nonmusical effects of the Kodály
music curriculum in primary grade children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 8, 167-174.
Husain, G., Thompson, W. F., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2002). Effects of musical tempo and mode
on arousal, mood, and spatial abilities. Music Perception, 20, 151-171.
Humpal, M. (1991). The effects of an integrated early childhood music program on social
interaction among children with handicaps and their typical peers. Journal of Music
Therapy, 28, 161-177.
Ilari, B. S. (2004). Music cognition in infancy: Infants' preferences and long-term memory for
complex music. Dissertation Abstracts International, A. The Humanities and Social
Sciences, 64(12A). (UMI No. AAINQ85713).
Jackson, C. S., & Tlauka, M. (2004). Route learning and the Mozart effect. Psychology of Music.
32(2), 213-220
Jakobson, L. S., Cuddy, L. L., & Kilgour, A. R. (2003). Time tagging: A key to musicians’
superior memory. Music Perception, 20, 307-313.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.38
Jenlink, C. L. (1993). The relational aspects of a school, a music program, and at-risk student
self-esteem: a qualitative study. (Doctoral dissertation, Oklahoma State University,
1993). Dissertation Abstracts International, 55(2A), 0214.
Jellison, J. A. (2002). On-task participation of typical students close to and away from classmates
with disabilities in an elementary music classroom. Journal of Research in Music
Education, 50, 343-355.
Jellison, J. A., Brooks, B. & Huck, A. (1984). Structuring small music groups and music
reinforcement to facilitate positive interactions and acceptance of severely handicapped
students in the regular music classroom. Journal of Research in Music Education, 32,
243-264.
Johnson, C. M., & Darrow, A. A. (2003). Attitudes of junior high school music students from
Italy and the USA toward individuals with a disability. Bulletin of the Council for
Research in Music Education, 155. 33-43.
Johnson, C. M., & Darrow, A. A. (1997). The effect of positive models of inclusion on band
students' attitudinal statements regarding the integration of students with disabilities.
Journal of Research in Music Education, 45(2), 173-184.
Kalmar, M. (1982). The effects of music education based on Kodaly's directives in nursery
school children: From a psychologist's point of view. Psychology of Music, Special Issue,
63-68.
Kilgour, A. R., Jakobson, L. S., & Cuddy, L. L. (2000). Music training and rate of presentation
as mediators or text and song recall. Memory and Cognition, 28, 700-710.
Lecanuet, J. (1996). Prenatal auditory experience. In I. Deliège & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Music
beginnings: Origins and development of musical competence (pp. 3-34). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Lorch, C. A., Lorch, V., Deifendor, A. O., & Early, P. W. (1994) Effect of stimulative and
sedative music on systolic blood pressure, heart rate, and respiratory rate in premature
infants. Journal of Music Therapy, 31, 105-118.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.39
Lu, D. T. (1986). The effects of teaching music skills on the development of reading skills
among first graders: an experimental study. (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Washingon, 1986). Dissertation Abstracts International, 47(12A), 4344.
Lynch, M. P., Short, L. B., & Chua, R. (1995) Contributions of experience to the development of
musical processing in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 28(7), 377-398
Madsen, C. K., Brittin, R. V., & Caperella-Sheldon, D. (1993). An empirical method for
measuring the aesthetic experience to music. Journal of Research in Music Education,
41, 57-69.
Madsen, C. K., Caperella-Sheldon, D., & Johnson, C. M. (1991). Use of the continuous response
digital interface (CRDI) in evaluating music responses of special populations. Journal of
the International Association for the Handicapped, 6(2), 3-15.
Madsen, C. K., & Frederickson, W. E. (1993). The experience of musical tension: A replication
of Nielsen's research using the continuous response digital interface. Journal of Music
Therapy, 30(1), 46-63.
McCord, K. (2002). Children with special needs compose using music technology. Journal of
Technology in Music Learning, 1(2), 3-14.
McCusker, J. (2001). Emerging musical literacy: Investigating young children's music cognition
and musical problem-solving through invented notations. (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Rochester, Eastman School of Music, 2001). Dissertation Abstracts
International, 62(02A), 504.
Moore, R., Vareyar, S., Fulford, J., Tyler, D., Gribben, C., Baker, P., James, D., & Growland, P.
(2001). Antenatal determination of fetal brain activity in response to an acoustic stimulus
using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 94-99.
Nantais, K. M., & Schellenberg, E. G. (1999). The Mozart effect: An artifact of preference.
Psychological Science, 10, 370-373.
Neiman, Z. (1989). Teaching specific motor skills for conducting to young music students.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68(3), 847-858.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.40
Orsmond, G. I., & Miller, L. K. (1999). Cognitive, musical and environmental correlates of early
music instruction. Psychology of Music, 27, 18-37.
Overy, K., Norton, A. C., Cronin, K. T., Gaab, N., Alsop, D. C., Winner, E., & Schlaug, G.
(2004). Imaging melody and rhythm processing in young children. Neuroreport: For
Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research. 15(11), 1723-1726.
Parisi, J. (2004). Fourth- and fifth-grade students' affective response and ability to discriminate
between melody and improvisation after receiving instruction in singing and/or playing a
piece in the blues style. International Journal of Music Education, 22(1), 77-86.
Phillips-Silver, J. & Trainor, L. J. (2005, June). Feeling the beat: Movement influences infant
rhythm perception, Science, 308(5727), 1430.
Pollatou, E., Karadimou, K., & Gerodimos, V. (2005). Gender differences in musical aptitude,
rhythmic ability and motor performance in preschool children. Early Child Development
& Care, 175(4), 361-370.
Ragert, P., Schmidt, A., Altenmüller, E., and Dinse, H.R. (2004). Superior tactile performance
and learning in professional pianists: Evidence for meta-plasticity in musicians.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 19(2), 473 478.
Rauscher, F. H. (2002). Mozart and the mind: Factual and fictional effects of musical
enrichment. In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement: Impact of
psychological factors on education (267-278). San Diego: Academic Press.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L. & Ky, K. N (1993). Listening to Mozart enhances spatial-temporal
reasoning: Towards a neurophysiological basis. Neuroscience Letters, 185, 44-47.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.41
Reimer, B. (1999). Facing the risks of the "Mozart Effect:" Grand masters series. Music
Educators Journal, 86(1) 37-43.
Rideout, B. E., Dougherty, S., & Wernert, L. (1998). Effect of music on spatial performance: A
test of generality. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 512-514.
Rideout, B. E., & Laubach, C. M. (1996). EEG correlates of enhanced spatial performance
following exposure to music. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 427-432.
Rideout, B. E., & Taylor, J. (1997). Enhanced spatial performance following 10 minutes
exposure to music: A replication. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 112-114.
Robb, S. L. (2003). Music interventions and group participation skills of preschoolers with visual
impairments: Raising questions about music, arousal, and attention. Journal of Music
Therapy, 40(4), 266-82.
Saffran, J. R., Loman, M. M., & Robertson, R. R. W. (2000). Infant memory for musical
experiences. Cognition, 77(1), B15-B23.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2001). Music and non-musical abilities. Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences, 930, 355-371.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2003). Does exposure to music have beneficial side effects? In Peretz & R.
J. Zatorre (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of music (pp. 430-448). Oxford, England:
Oxford University Press.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, 511-514.
Schellenberg, E. G., Nakata, T., Hunter, P. G., & Tamoto, S. (in press). Exposure to music and
cognitive performance: Tests of children and adults. Psychology of Music.
Schlaug, G., Norton, A. C., Overy, K., Cronin, K. T., Lee, D. J., & Winner, E. (2004). Effects of
music training on children’s brain and cognitive development. In S. D. Lipscomb, R.
Ashley, R. O. Gjerdingen, & P. Webster (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th International
Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (pp. 133-134). UK: Causal Publications.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.42
Schleuter, S. L., & Schleuter, L. J. (1985). The relationship of grade level and sex differences to
certain rhythmic responses of primary grade children. Journal of Research in Music
Education, 33(1), 23-30.
Schleuter, S. L., & Schleuter, L. J. (1989). The relationship of rhythm response tasks and PMMA
scores with music training, grade level, and sex among K-3 students. Bulletin of the
Council for Research in Music Education, 100. 1-13.
Schon, D., Anton, J. L., Roth, M., & Besson, M. (2002). An fMRI study of music sight-reading.
Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 13(17), 2285-2289.
Shields, C. G. (2001) Music education and mentoring as intervention for at-risk urban
adolescents: Their self-perceptions, opinions, and attitudes. Journal of Research in Music
Education, 49(3), 273-286.
Simner, M. L. & Barnes, M. J. (1991, Winter). Relationship between first-grade marks and the
high school dropout problem. Journal of School Psychology, 29, 331-335.
Sloboda, J. A. (1985) The musical mind: The cognitive psychology of music. Oxford, UK:
Clarendon Press.
Snyder, D. M. (1985). Effects of a motor development curriculum upon motor music skills of
first-grade children as compared with regular music instruction (motor skills). (Masters
thesis, University of Kentucky, 1985). Masters Abstracts International, 24(2), 0093.
Staley, K., Iragui, V., & Spitz, M. (1990). The human fetal auditory evoked potential.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neirophysiology, 77(1), 1-5.
Steele, K. M. Ball T. M. & Runk, R., (1997). Listening to Mozart does not enhance backwards
digit span performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 1179-1184.
Steele, K. M., Bass, K, E., & Crook, M. D. (1999). The mystery of the Mozart effect: Failure to
replicate. Psychological Science, 10I, 366-369.
Steele, K. M., Brown, K. M., & Stoecker, J. A. (1999). Failure to confirm the Rauscher and
Shaw description of recovery of the Mozart effect. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88, 843-
848.
Child’s Growth & Development 3.43
Steele, K. M., Della Bella, S., Peretz, I., Dunlop, T., Dawe, L. A., Humphrey, G. K., Shannon, R.
A., Kirby Jr., K. L. & Olmstead, C. G. (1999). Prelude or requiem for the “Mozart
effect”? Nature, 400, 827.
Stough, C., Kerkin, B., Bates, T., & Mangan, G. (1994). Music and spatial IQ. Personality and
Individual Differences, 17, 695.
Suthers, L. (2001). Toddler diary: A study of development and learning through music in the
second year of life. Early Child Development and Care. 171, 21-32.
Taetle, L. (1999). The relationship between fine arts participation and daily school attendance at
the secondary level. Contributions to Music Education, 26(1), 50-66.
Tervaniemi, M., Medvedev, S. V., Alho, K. Pakhomov, S. V., Roudas, M. S., van Zuijen, T. L.,
& Naatanen, R. (2000). Lateralized automatic auditory processing of phonetic versus
musical information: A PET study. Human Brain Mapping. 10(2), 74-79.
Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., & Husain, G. (2001). Arousal, mood and the Mozart
effect. Psychological Science, 12, 248-251.
Trainor, L. J. Tsang, C. D., & Cheung, V. H. W. (2002) Preference for sensory consonance in 2-
and 4-month-old infants. Music Perception, 20(2), 187-194.
Trehub, S. (2000). Foundations: Music perception in infancy. In J. Flohr (ed.) The musical lives
of young children (pp. 24-29). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Turner, M. (1998). Motor learning research: Applications and directions for elementary music
teaching and learning. Update: Applications of Research in Music Education, 16(2), 12-
16.
Vaughn, K. (2000). Music and mathematics: Modest support for the oft-claimed relationship.
Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3/4), 149-166.
Wallace, w. T. (1994). Memory for music: Effect of melody on recall of text. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(6), 1471-1485.
Waters, A. W. (1992). Factors affecting motor control in the five-finger pitch pattern
performance of beginning pianists (finger movements). (Doctoral dissertation, Kent State
University, 1992). Dissertation Abstracts International, 52(7A), 2456
Weschler, D. (1991). Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition. San Antonio,
TX: Psychological Corporation.
West Virginia School Dropout Prevention Task Force. (1991). In class, in step: A community
resource guide for school dropout prevention. Charleston, WV: State Department of
Education (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED-330941).
Child’s Growth & Development 3.44
Williams, L. R. (2005). The effect of musical training and musical complexity on focus of
attention to melody or harmony. Dissertation Abstracts International Section A:
Humanities and Social Sciences, 65 (7A), 2536. (UMI No. AAI3137508)
Zachopoulou, E., Tsapakidoub, A., & Derric, V. (2004). The effects of a developmentally
appropriate music and movement program on motor performance. Early Childhood
Research Quarterly, 19(4), 631-642.
Footnotes
1
The Mozart Effect is a registered trademark of Don Campbell, Inc.