RRL
RRL
RRL
“as feelings of tension or fear that appear when a person is required to undertake a
Zakaira, Zain, Ahmad, and Erlina (2012) expressed pupils with high mathematics anxiety
levels exhibit negative thinking towards their abilities and capabilities. These students will
display less certainty working with numbers and numerical concepts and ideas through a critical
or problem solving process. A pupil who has a profound interest can be urged to work and train
without being asked by the teacher. The interest and confidence in this facet are vital in learning
to lessen the anxiety in mathematics and yet to acquire great outcomes on the assessment:
feelings in specific situations, such as examinations, as well as overall learning, and even
lifelong academic and vocational development. Along with more overarching anxiety disorders,
individuals may suffer from specific forms of test and performance anxiety that are connected to
a knowledge domain. Clearly, the most prominent of these disorders is math anxiety
Baloglu and Kocak (2006) outlined three major factors that cause Mathematics anxiety,
namely, Dispositional, situational and environmental factors. The dispositional factors are
concerned with psychological and emotional features such as; attitudes towards Mathematics,
self-concept and learning styles. The self-concept refers to students’ perception of their own
ability to perform well in Mathematics and to learn new topics. The situational factors are direct
features that result from their particular Mathematics courses, the nature of the course, and how
it is designed and carried out, pace of instruction, etc. The environmental factors are
characteristics that affect the students prior to their Mathematics course; for instance, age,
mathematics. For many children, negative attitudes toward mathematics begin early in life,
sometimes even before they enter kindergarten. The child’s educational context at home and at
The first years of elementary school are critical for learning basic mathematical skills.
Yet until recently the dominant view among educators and researchers alike was that math
anxiety only arose in the context of complex mathematics (e.g. algebra) and thus was not present
in young children. Math anxiety was thought to develop in junior high school, coinciding with
the increasing difficulty of the math curriculum towards the end of elementary school. Recent
research challenges this assumption. Not only do children as young as first grade report varying
levels of anxiety about math, which is inversely related to their math achievement, but this
anxiety is also associated with a distinct pattern of neural activity in brain regions associated
Mathematics anxiety in elementary school students indicates that its onset coincides with
early years of schooling. This could in part be due to social learning from parents and teachers
with mathematics anxiety or negative perceptions of math. Parents or teachers might give
children mixed messages (Williams, 1988) about mathematics. They might emphasize how
highly difficult mathematics is and at the same time tell them how mathematics skills are of
Vann (1993) observed that mathematics anxiety in mothers was significantly predictive
of mathematics anxiety in children. This could be so for excessive expectations as well. Children
of parents with excessive expectations whose love and acceptance is conditional to how well
children live up to these expectations have high degree of anxiety. These circumstances might
lead to self-consciousness about one’s performance and to anxiety arising from not living up
high standards of parents. Children’s excessive self-critical attitude might cause anxiety
disproportionate to their failure in living up to these expectations. In fact, studies do show that
low self-esteem, confidence and efficacy are closely related to mathematics anxiety (Woodard et
al., 2004).
Children begin to construct the foundations for future mathematical concepts during the
first few months of life (Geist, 2003). Before a child can add or even count, they must construct
ideas about mathematics that cannot be directly taught. Many of these basic ideas are constructed
through interaction with the surrounding environment and the adults in that environment. Ideas
that will support formal mathematics later in life such as order and sequence, seriation,
comparisons, classifying, addition and other more advanced mathematical skills have their
genesis before the age of five. The seemingly simple understanding that numbers have a quantity
Approximately 93% of adult US-Americans indicate that they experience some level of math
anxiety.4 Estimations are that approximately 17% of the US-American population suffers from
high levels of math anxiety.5 In a sample of adolescent apprentices in the United Kingdom,
approximately 30% of the study participants reported high math anxiety, and a further 18% were
at least somewhat affected by it.6 The most extensive set of data is provided by the Programme
for International Student Assessment (PISA) studies. In its 2012 assessments, across the 34
participating Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) countries, 59%
of the 15- to 16-year-old students reported that they often worry math classes will be difficult for
them; 33% reported that they get very tense when they have to complete math homework; and
another 31% stated they get very nervous doing math problems (Luttenberger et al., 2018).
the students. According to Karimi and Venkatesan, (2009), there is a relationship between
Mathematics anxiety, mathematics performance and academic hardness in high school students.
Their findings revealed that Mathematics anxiety has significant negative correlation with
anxiety. They also reported no significant difference between boys and girls in Mathematics
performance. They also suggest that the performance of students in Mathematics can be
al. (2013), a significant correlation was observed between high level anxiety and low academic
performance. Significant difference between males and females in Mathematics anxiety was also
reported.
Mathematics one of the core academic disciplines since it provides crucial foundational
skills and an important component of early learning for students. Mathematics aids children with
problem solving, measuring, developing their own spatial awareness, using and understanding
shapes.
potent set of tools with which to understand and alter the world. These qualities include critical
of various aspects of our lives. It is a creative discipline that is skillfully applied to communicate
When a boxer’s corner man throws a white towel into the ring, the defeated boxer
surrenders. The beating must end and the boxer will be able to fight another day without major
injury. Most people understand the reference of tossing a white towel onto the floor as a sign of
giving up. Whether out of frustration or lack of self-confidence, the white towel is a symbol of
giving up and defeat. Frustration in mathematics often results in a similar outcome. “Seventy-
five percent of Americans stop studying math before they have completed the educational
The important role of mathematics recognized Cockcroft (1986), for example, he writes:
“It would be very difficult – perhaps impossible – to live a normal life in very many parts of the
world in the twentieth century without making use of mathematics of some kind.” It would be
very difficult or not possible for a person to live on the earth in the 20th century without the
Mathematics Anxiety
From the study of Zakaria et al., (2012) reveals that there still exists mathematics anxiety
among secondary school students. However, gender-related factors do not influence mathematics
anxiety. This study also shows that there is a difference in mathematics achievement of students
based on their level of mathematics anxiety. Therefore, teachers should strive to understand
mathematics anxiety and implement teaching and learning strategies so that students can
also employ teaching methods that empower students to develop healthy attitudes toward
mathematics. In addition, Smith (2004) suggests that teachers demonstrate their own interest in
reduce their math anxiety. Thus, this study has implications for all parties, including teachers,
schools and parents, encouraging those with a vested interest in the success of their students to
take into account math anxiety levels before determining effective and appropriate strategies
when teaching and learning is carried out. In this way, it is hoped that the level of mathematics
Mathematics anxiety can have multiple origins and, as noted by Shields (2005), can be
The home. In the home, parents who themselves suffer mathematics anxiety can
unintentionally transfer such anxiety to their children. In the context of doing mathematics, the
emotions expressed by one will inevitably and reciprocally shape the other within parent-child
interactions (Else-Quest, Hyde, & Hejmadi, 2008). For example, children who are reproached for
their errors may develop a fear of taking risks and exploring new possibilities, and may start
hating mathematics. Parental disappointment and despair are especially demoralizing due to the
value placed on the high positive regard of parents by children (Dossel, 1993), while parents
giving mathematics low status or applying pressure to children may also contribute to the
development of mathematics anxiety (Fraser & Honeyford, 2000). Stolpa (2004) also identifies
how parents may unintentionally raise mathematics anxiety in their children by providing them
with an excuse to stop trying when they are frustrated or upset due to difficulties with a
mathematical task. A response from parents, such as ‘Don’t worry, I’ve never understood
fractions’ or ‘Never mind, mathematics was always tricky for me at school too’, plants a seed
that may grow into a strong belief for children that they are incapable of learning mathematics.
High achievers are not immune to pressures from parents. Over-bearing parental pressure for
success or concern about the difficulty of mathematics for their children (Bernstein, Coté-
Bonanno, Reilly, Carver, & Doremus, 1995) may contribute to mathematics anxiety in high
achievers.
Society. Social factors such as mathematical myths may also induce or reinforce
mathematics anxiety for some students. For example, the myth that boys are better than girls in
mathematics and that only some people have a ‘mathematics mind’ can undermine positive self-
efficacy beliefs. Too often, situations are encountered in which it is ‘cool’ to hate mathematics,
with people readily stating, with some pride, ‘I’m no good at mathematics’, as though displaying
subject mathematics is unique as embarrassment often does not result from failure. A study
involving over 1000 undergraduate students in the United States affirms the view that failure at
mathematics is socially acceptable – the participants were less embarrassed in relation to lack of
The classroom. Despite young children starting school having, for the most part, a well-
developed, informal competence in mathematics, it is apparent that the classroom is also a place
where mathematics anxiety can develop and flourish. Research studies (e.g., Vinson, 2001)
suggest that, in combination with the parental and societal factors, mathematics anxiety may
have its roots in teaching and teachers, with mathematics anxious teachers resulting in
and work sheets; assigning the same work for everyone; teaching to the textbook; insisting on
only one correct way to complete a problem; concentrating more on basic skills rather than
concepts; and, whole class instruction (Gurganus, 2007). Despite New Zealand’s systemic
attempts to reform primary mathematics programmes (Higgins & Parson, 2009), we have ‘a long
tail of underachievement in mathematics’ (Neill, Fisher, & Dingle, 2010, p. 1) and traditional
Student performance is a concern for all educators and is an object of study that stands-
out in many research papers. Due the importance of mathematics, its achievement and
performance gained educators attention and has been an increasing field of study. There have
been several researchers who have discussed the issue of mathematics learning and the factors
influencing it. The factors identified as influencing the learning of mathematics can be divided
into two distinct groups: the demographic and psychographic factors (Alves et.al, 2012)
The findings of Zakaria et al., (2012) also showed significant differences between
students’ mathematics achievements based on their math anxiety levels. Students who are high
achievers have lower levels of anxiety, while low achieving math students have high levels of
anxiety. This is because high achievers have a strong understanding of mathematics and have
more confidence than low achievers. These findings support the findings of Woodard (2004) and
Karimi and Venkatesan (2009), who determined that students who have high anxiety levels tend
to earn lower mathematics scores. Conversely, students who have low levels of anxiety tend to
In the study of Siebers (2015), A 12-item Math Questionnaire (MQ) was developed and
distributed to 381 middle school students in a northern Colorado middle school during the 2013-
2014 school year. Data from the Transitional Colorado Assessment Program (TCAP) during the
2012-2013 school year were used to compare mathematics achievement to mathematic anxiety.
Middle school grades consist of sixth, seventh, and eighth grade students who range in ages of 11
to 14 years old. Results from the quantitative study showed there were statistically significant
differences between mathematics anxiety and achievement on TCAP. Students who have high
mathematics anxiety tend to have low mathematics achievement. Other results showed that sixth
grade students had less mathematics anxiety than seventh grade students. Sixth grade students
had less mathematics anxiety compared to eighth grade students. Seventh grade students had a
higher level of mathematics anxiety compared to eighth grade students. Lastly, results showed
sixth grade students had the highest mathematics achievement across the middle school grades.
Eighth grade students showed the lowest mathematics achievement compared to sixth and
seventh grade.
Mathematics anxiety and mathematics achievement was studied by Ma and Jiangming Xub
(2004). Tocci & Engelhard (1991) found that students with a higher level of mathematics anxiety
analysis reported an average correlation of -.34 between mathematics anxiety and mathematics
for this relationship is -.27. This magnitude was associated with a prediction that “measures (or
treatments) that resulted in the movement of a typical student in the group of high mathematics
anxiety in the group of low mathematics anxiety would be associated with improvement of the
typical student’s level of mathematics achievement from the 50th to the 71st percentile”.
Performance in Mathematics
Grades and other aspects of classroom assessment influence student motivation to learn and
these provide students with information that they use in their learning (Brookhart, 2008). Many
aspects of college education are included in the totality of this performance whether sports and
cultural exercises of the university were also part of academic growth of all students. The
valuable perception of students on how they perceived their grades at the end of semester is a
partial overview of how they thought of their capability to perform and surpass all the challenges
grading where effort should be weighted comparably to actual performance in importance to the
composition of a grade, with the expectation that grade allocation should reflect this perception
(Tippin, 2012).
The Math Anxiety Level of the Students. The anxiety level of a person is a behavioral
quality that should be quantified using psychological concepts. Thus, the expertise of two (2)
Psychology professors and a psychometrician was asked by the researcher in analyzing the data
pertaining to the anxiety of the students. Table 1.1 presents the level of math anxiety of the
respondents. Of the 88 respondents, 55 of them or 63% were identified with High Anxiety in
Pre- calculus and 12 respondents or 14% were diagnosed with Very High Anxiety. This resulted
to a total percentage of 77% or 67 respondents with anxiety in Pre- calculus. On the other hand,
only 5% or 5 respondents and 18% or 16 respondents were diagnosed with Very Low Anxiety
and Low Anxiety respectively. As a whole, only 23% of the respondents have lower math
anxiety or tolerable anxiety while overwhelmingly 77% have higher anxiety. These findings
mean that majority of the senior high school students of SSC, despite the fact that they are taking
the STEM strand, were diagnosed with high anxiety in Pre- calculus. This finding is possibly the
reason why most of these respondents were hesitant in participating classroom discussions in
their Pre- calculus subject. During the interview conducted by the researcher, certain symptoms
of math anxiety were observed. Likewise, these symptoms were also been observed during class
discussions whereby the researcher was also the teacher of the respondents. The behavioral
manifestations that had been observed by the researcher were: (1) lack of confidence to solve
mathematical problems when called by the teacher; (2) habitual tardiness and absences in
Precalculus subject; (3) Non- compliance to the course requirements of the subject ; (4) non-
submission of projects in Pre- calculus; (5) sweating palms and shaking voices when asked by
the teacher to explain a concept; and (6) Intentionally prioritizing other subjects more than Pre-
In another study, the value of math increased for middle school students when they had a
teacher whom they perceived to be high in support (Eccles, 1993). Maurillo (1999) undertook a
study on the assessment of grade five pupils’ mastery of the basic mathematics skills in the
division of Tacloban city. He revealed that the extent of mastery of the pupils in the different
mathematics skills was determined by the strategies, techiques, approaches, evaluative measures,
In the demographic factors gender is remarked as a factor that could explain differences
in academic performance. Since 1970s, gender has been investigated as a factor on which student
performance and attitude toward mathematics differ. It is a general perception that boys are
better at math than girls. However, studies on gender and mathematics show that the advantage
held by boys over girls in mathematics achievement has diminished markedly over the last 40
years and gender differences in mathematics achievement are no longer a relevant issue.
According to the same studies, research on gender and mathematics is often limited to the
relationship between gender differences in attitudes toward mathematics and gender differences
in mathematics achievement. However, the gender alone may not explain significant differences
in performance when viewed in the context of multiple types of mathematical knowledge (Alves
et.al, 2012)
One variable that has, over the years, received considerable attention in many studies on
analysis of 77 studies conducted between 1980 and 1991 among middle and high school
students, DeBaz (1994) found a significant gender effect favoring males in overall science
achievement. Hedges and Newell (1999) discovered that boys outperform girls in science but in
reading and writing girls have the advantage. Researchers have indicated that gender affects
mathematics achievement. For example, Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study
(TIMSS), found significant differences between male and female students in mathematics
Several studies have been conducted over the years to determine the predictors of
discovered are: socio- economic status (Ajayi et al., 2011), students’ employment status
(Wantanabe, 2005), teaching methods (Eniayeju, 2010), gender and continuous assessment
(Owolabiet al., 2009). Other factors found to affect achievement in mathematics are: self-concept
and learning style (Rech & Stevens, 1996), reading abilities, mathematics self-efficacy and
teacher evaluation (Larwin, 2010) and students’ previous knowledge (Siegler et al., 2012).
Students who have mathematics anxiety especially females seem to enjoy less in learning
mathematics process and they will lack of confidence with their abilities in mathematics. Gender
differences in mathematics have long been explained as deficits, particularly inferior spatial
visualization among girls (Collins et al., 1997). Some presume this to be a sex-linked
characteristic of females. Justification for this point of view is often based on deficits found in
boys, such as higher levels of reading disabilities and attention deficit disorders, as well as the
superiority of males on spatial tests (Nass, 1993; Nordvik & Amponsah, 1998). As a result,
innate differences have long been used to explain the performance gap between the sexes. A
differences on differential treatment of girls in the classroom, curricula that either ignores or
and cultural aspects of mathematical development’ (Walshaw et al., 2008) require teachers to
ensure that all students have opportunities to develop mathematical proficiency that includes a
positive mathematical disposition. As math anxiety is a learned condition (Nolting, 2011), one
hopes it can be unlearned. Consequently, teachers have an important role in the reduction or
prevention of student math anxiety. The research literature points to several promising ways
teachers can assist in the unlearning, or even prevention, of math anxiety in students. The variety
of ways available to teachers discussed here include: building positive attitudes towards
pictures, bibliotherapy, and math related fiction books; promoting an appropriate classroom
culture; utilizing effective teaching practices; working to reduce one’s own math anxiety; and,
To begin, the need to attend to the affective needs of students is discussed. Because
emotions drive and intensify thinking in mathematics in profound and powerful ways,
cognitive skill (Zambo et al., 2006). There is a range of ways in which teachers might first
identify student emotions before bringing them out into the open through a classroom discussion
of math anxiety. Journal writing, while typically utilized to create opportunities for students to
express their understandings of mathematical concepts, can also be used for sharing and
reflecting on feelings about, and experiences with mathematics (Furner et al., 2003).
Autobiographies (Ellsworth et al., 2000) in which students are encouraged to explain their
also provide students with the opportunity to express their feelings about mathematics.
The most widely cited scale used to measure and explore mathematics anxiety is the
Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale (MARS) (Suinn, 1972). Researchers suspected that some
individuals who did not normally suffer from general anxiety were still affected by mathematics
anxiety, so Suinn developed the MARS to look more specifically at mathematics anxiety. The
purpose of the MARS was to help researchers explore mathematics anxiety and to evaluate
On the other hand, a questionnaire was developed with items addressing students’
Mathematics Self-Efficacy and Anxiety Questionnaire (MSEAQ) fits my study and majority of
the items came from a pilot version, which was developed to provide college mathematics
instructors and mathematics education researchers with information about students’ self-efficacy
in their ability to learn mathematics (May et al., 2008). For the pilot version, a large pool of
items was generated that addressed both the mathematics self-efficacy and mathematics anxiety
of college students.
These items were based on the research literature, and some of them were adapted from
previous questionnaires designed to measure mathematics self-efficacy (Betz & Hackett, 1983),
mathematics anxiety (Richardson & Suinn, 1972), and science self-efficacy and science anxiety
(Glynn et al., 2007). The items chosen for the final version of the MSEAQ also took into account
college mathematics instructors’ input regarding their experiences with college students’ self-
For many people, math anxiety is an issue. It can happen at any educational level, from
elementary school to college, and once it's established, it can continue throughout life,
obstructing further arithmetic study and everyday activities involving numeracy (Oxford &
Vordick, 2006).
Mathematics test anxiety, numerical anxiety, and abstraction anxiety are the three
subtypes of mathematics anxiety. Before, during, and after performing mathematical activities,
people experience test anxiety related to mathematics. When performing fundamental operations
or a mixture of these operations, numerical anxiety occurs. When working with mathematical
concepts and properties to solve equations, algebraic notations or symbols, and abstraction
Even more surprising is the possibility that there may not be a consistent relationship
between teacher anxiety and student progress for all students and teachers. Youths are more
inclined than adults of the opposite gender to model their actions and attitudes after them. One
could anticipate seeing a correlation between teacher anxiety and both boys' and girls' math
achievement given that Beilock (2010) claimed that teachers who are very concerned about
arithmetic are worse math teachers. However, research appears that math teachers who exhibit
high levels of math anxiety are directly affecting girls' math ability through influencing girls'
Smail (2017) used Bayesian study to find a strong correlation between mathematics fear
and gender, personality type, and study habits. According to him female learners are more likely
than male to undergo mathematical anxiety, feel comfortable learning mathematics, and have
regular study habits. Recognizing elements of learners' In order to meet the demands of math-
phobic students, it is important to consider their gender, personality types, and study habits.
Part II is the mathematics anxiety rating scale which will determine the level of
(MSEAQ), a 29-item questionnaire checklist developed by May (2009) which has a very good
statements on anxiety will be used to determine the level of mathematics anxiety of the
respondents. The items were based on the research literature, and some of them were adopted
from previous questionnaires designed to measure mathematics self-efficacy (Betz & Hackett,
1983), mathematics anxiety (Richardson & Suinn, 1972), and science self-efficacy and science
Carey et al., (2017) adapted the Abbreviated Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS) to be utilized with British
leraners aged 8 to 13. They evaluate the scale's reliability, factor structure, and divergent validity in their
study. The modified AMAS (mAMAS) was given to a large group of British children and adolescents (n =
1746). Factor analysis of the mAMAS confirmed that it has the same underlying factor structure as the
original AMAS, with subscales measuring anxiety about Learning and Evaluation in math. The mAMAS
provides a valid and reliable scale for measuring MA in children and adolescents, from a younger age
than is possible with the original AMAS.