Holcomb 1993
Holcomb 1993
Holcomb 1993
To cite this article: Phillip J. Holcomb & Jane E. Anderson (1993) Cross-modal semantic priming:
A time-course analysis using event-related brain potentials, Language and Cognitive Processes,
8:4, 379-411, DOI: 10.1080/01690969308407583
INTRODUCTION
Over the past several decades, psycholinguists and cognitive psychologists
have shown substantial interest in the processes and representations under-
lying language comprehension during reading and listening. However,
comparatively little emphasis has been placed on determining the locus or
extent of common language processes across the modalities (but see
Bradley & Forster, 1987; Ellis & Young, 1988; Radeau et al., 1992;
Shallice, 1988). On the one hand, it seems obvious that written and spoken
words place unique demands on the readerllistener and that each modality
must therefore enlist a set of its own “modality specific” processes during
comprehension. However, it is equally clear that at some point after initial
‘In a semantic priming task, the “ N W effect” refers to the difference in the N400
components elicited by unprimed VJ primed words-unprimed words elicit a more negative-
going N400 compared to words that are primed.
CROSSMODAL SEMANTIC PRIMING AND ERPs 381
Wote that it is doubtful that the N400 could be w d to differentiate between the recoding
and rraodation hypotheses because of it being sensitive to a relatively late post-lexical priming
process (Brown & Hagoort, 1993; Holcomb, 1993; Rugg. 1990).
382 HOLCOMB AND ANDERSON
EXPERIMENT 1: VISUAL/AUDlTORY
The purpose of the first experiment was to determine if the same mechan-
isms supporting semantic priming within the auditory modality also medi-
ate priming between visual and auditory words. It was predicted that if the
same processes are involved, then there should be evidence of an N400
effect between modalities at the shortest SOAs. However, if between-
modality priming is mediated via a conversion process, then only the
longest prime-target interval (800 SOA) should show significant N400
effects.
Method
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
Subjects
Twelve right-..anded Tufts University undergraduates (7 females, 5
males) with a mean age of 19.42 years (SD = 1.68 years) received partial
course credit or $10.0 for their participation. All of them were native
speakers of English with normal visual and auditory acuity.
pseudoword pairs, and had an SOA of either 0,200 or 800 msec, resulting
in a total of 40 stimulus pairs in each of nine conditions (3 SOAs x 3 target
types)-
The visual stimuli were displayed as black lower-case letters on a white
background. Each word subtended from 0.5” to 1.8”of horizontal and 0.4”
of vertical visual angle. The auditory stimuli were spoken by a female
member of our research team and were digitised (16 kHz, 24 pole 7.9 kHz
Butterworth filter) by a Data Translations analogue-to-digital converter
(12 bits, model DT2821). Each stimulus was edited using software that
allowed us to listen to the stimulus while visually inspecting its waveform
in order to store it from the time of onset. This was done so that the precise
time of its onset could be time-locked with EEG digitisation. At the time
of the experiment, the stimuli were output through a digital-to-analogue
converter, then filtered (7.9IrHz) and Sent to the subject’s headphones.
The average duration of auditory targets was 568msec (range 300-862
msec) .
Each trial began with a warning stimulus (a red “X”)in the middle of
the screen. Then, 500 msec later, the prime replaced the warning stimulus
and remained on the screen for 200 msec. For the 0 msec SOA condition,
the target onset was simultaneous with the onset of the prime; for the other
two SOAs, the target onset was either 200 or 800msec after the onset of
the prime. Next, 1500msec after the onset of the target, a green “ X ’
appeared in the middle of the screen, indicating to the subject that it was
alright to blink. Finally, after a 1250msec inter-trial interval, the green
“X”changed to a red “X”and the next trial began.
The subjects were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as
possible by pressing a button labelled “YES” with one thumb if the target
was a real word, or a button labelled “NO”with their other thumb if it
was not a real word. They were told to try to pay attention to the visual
prime but not to make an overt response. The hand used for each response
was counterbalanced across subjects. The subjects were told not to blink
386 HOLCOMB AND ANDERSON
or move their eyes while the stimuli were being presented. The experiment
lasted about 35 min, including short breaks about every 60 trials. A
practice block of eight trials preceded the experiment.
EEG Procedure
Tin electrodes were held in place on the scalp with an elastic cap
(Electrode-Cap International). The scalp locations included standard
International 10-20 system locations over the left and right hemispheres at
the frontal (ITand F8) and occipital sites ( 0 1 and 0 2 ) and three locations
on the midline: frontal (Fz), central (Cz) and parietal (Pz). In addition,
six electrodes were placed at the following non-standard locations pre-
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
Data Anel p i s
The mean reaction times for correct responses between 200 and 2000
msec and the percentage of errors were calculated for each subject. EFWs
to targets were quantified by measuring the mean amplitude in three
latency windows: 300-550, 550-800 and 800-1150msec. To examine the
time-course of priming effects more closely, the mean amplitude measures
of 100msec epochs were also taken starting 100msec post-target and
CROSS-MODAL SEMANTIC PRIMING AND ERPs 387
extending to 1100 msec. The use of multiple windows carries a greater risk
of type 1error; however, we utilise these analyses only as a supplementary
measure to examine timecourse effects.
Repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed on
the above dependent measures. A 2 X 3 ANOVA was done with target
type (related vs unrelated: note that the pseudoword condition was not
included in any of the analyses to be reported here) and SOA (0 vs 200 vs
800msec) as the factors. For the ERP analyses, the midline and lateral
sites were analysed separately. In addition to target type and SOA, for the
midline analyses there was an electrode site factor [frontal (Fz) vs central
(Cz) vs parietal (Pz)], and at the lateral sites there was an electrode site
factor (frontal vs anterior-temporal vs temporal vs Wernicke’s vs occipital)
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
ResuIts
Electrophysiological Data
The grand mean target ERPs are plotted in Fig. 1. Notice that these
waveforms appear somewhat different at the three SOAs. This is due, in
part, to the differential overlap of the visual prime and auditory target
ERPs, particularly in the 0 and 200 msec SOA conditions. In the case of
the 200 msec condition, the early P2 component from the prime-elicited
388 HOLCOMB AND ANDERSON
TABLE 1
Mean ( i S D ) Reaction Times (rnmc) and Percent Errors
Exprimem I : Visdaudirory
0 RT 822f91 889floo 985f98
PE 1.9f1.9 4.0k2.2 8.6f6.7
Experiment 2: Audirorylvitual
0 RT 7Mf104 735f 115 834f127
PE 1.2f1.7 2.1f4.3 4.8fS.9
ERP occurred just as the auditory target stimulus onset. Thus the resulting
target ERP is a summation of the activity generated by the target and the
ongoing activity generated by the prime. Nevertheless, a large negativity,
which peaked at approximately 100 msec (N100 or Nl), can be seen at all
three SOAS. The N1 was present at all but the most posterior sites (01,
02) and was largest from the fronto-central electrodes. Following the N1
there was an equally large positive-going wave which peaked at approxi-
mately 200 msec (€200or €2). The €2had a similar scalp distribution to
the N1.
The P2 component was followed by a broad negative-going wave peak-
ing between 400 and 500 msec. This negativity, which overlaps the window
usually associated with the N400, was largest (i.e. was most negative) at
the more anterior sites. The broad negativity was followed at the more
posterior sites by a late peaking positivity (P3), which continued through
the end of the recording epoch (1180 msec).
or N400 effect did not interact with the SOA variable. The interaction
between priming and electrode site approached significance at the lateral
sites (F(4,44) = 3.79, P < 0.0621, indicating that target type differences
were largest at the Wernicke’s and temporal sites. Finally, there was no
evidence for a lateral asymmetry in this epoch.
Discussion
In Experiment 1, we examined semantic priming at different SOAs with
the prime presented in the visual modality and the target in the auditory
modality. Large effects, both behavioural and electrophysiological, were
found across SOA conditions. The reaction time differences tended to be
slightly larger as the SOA became longer (indicated by an interaction that
approached significance). However, the magnitude of the ERP priming
effect did not differ between the SOAs. This N400 effect began around
300 msec and continued to the end of the measuring epoch.
The fact that the visual primes were able to prime the auditory targets
at the simultaneous (0 SOA) and 200 SOA conditions is most consistent
-+ a1
-. ..
..........
..
......
?7
-Pa
+ I ......
...--..-...
n
--+$-...
WL.
............
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
+
- T
01 +
L Lxx) *I*! - . o*%
........................ 600900
+ + -
+ +
-T GT -1 ......
01
+
I . .
300 6 0 0 - p d o '
.
O2 y r ............
390
CROSS-MODAL SEMANTIC PRIMING AND ERPs 391
..... -
$A.L
*.....
.-...
-p.,x
: .
.......’.. 5..-
....
P)
...
.,A. Fa
....
Fz
* + ......-.-.....
..........._ +
.C.
ATL
.: .. ATR
._..
CZ
\ ... +J.
+
.-:
........
..
n
,(woo
I
......
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
m
...
+ +
01 02
+ +
(C) - Rebed torteis ..... U n n & d taqers
FIG. 1 Grand mean ERPs (n = 12) to related and unrelated auditory target words in
Experiment I: (a) 0 SOA condition; (b) 200 SOA condition; (c) 800 SOA condition. The
ERPs in the left-hand column arc from electrodes placed over the left hemisphere sites, the
middle column is from the midline sites and the right-hand column is from the right
hemisphere sites. T i e is measured in msec, each tic mark representing 100 msec. Stimulus
onset is the vertical calibration bar.
with the hypothesis that the two modalities share a common semantic
process. If priming had been due to a conversion process, there should
have been evidence of cross-modal priming effects only when there was
sufficient time for such a process, that is, in the 800 SOA condition.
The pattern of results obtained in Experiment 1 was similar to that of
Anderson and Holcomb (submitted). They found ERP evidence of short
interval (0 SOA) priming when both the prime and target were auditory,
although their effects were not as large or consistent as those in the present
study. The similar early timecourse of priming between these two studies
is consistent with the hypothesis that within- and between-modality seman-
tic priming (as measured here) rely on a common amodal semantic system.
The early onset of the N400 effect is also consistent with previous ERP
findings (Holcomb & Neville, 1990; 1991) that auditory words can be
recognised prior to their completion (Marslen-Wilson, 1987). The average
length of the targets was 568msec, with the shortest duration being 300
msec. The N400 effect began in the 300-400 msec window and approached
TABLE 2
The Size of the Semantic Priming Effect (Unrelated-Related) in pV at Each of the 100
msec Epoch8 for Each SOA in Experiment 1 (Visual Prime, Auditory Target)
SOA P-Vdues
Epoch 0 200 800 77- mXSOA
1oo-mmSCc
midline -1.19 1.10 -0.42 NS NS
lateral -0.32 0.35 -0.09 NS NS
2m-300 m x c
midline -1.5V 1.03 1.00 NS 0.0267
lateral -0.46 0.42 -0.48 NS NS
300-400llWC
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
EXPERIMENT 2: AUDITORYNISUAL
The purpose of this experiment was to determine if the findings from
Experiment 1 could be generalised to the case where the prime and target
modalities are reversed (i.e. auditory/visual). Given the tentative conclu-
sions outlined above for Experiment 1, it seems reasonable to predict a
similar pattern of results for visual targets primed by auditory words-
even at short prime-target SOAS. That is, when an auditory prime onsets
either simultaneously or shortly before a visual target, the processing of
the initial sounds from the prime should influence the ongoing processing
of the visual target. This prediction is based on the presence of semantic
effects which onset as early as 200msec in spoken word pairs (Holcomb
& Neville, 1990) and in writtedspoken pairs (Experiment 1). From these
findings it was reasoned that if spoken words can be primed at this latency
and if between-modality priming relies on the same mechanism as within-
modality priming, then a spoken word should also be able to prime a visual
word at short intervals. It is noteworthy that the 0 SOA conditions in the
two experiments are very similar, since in both cases a visual and an
auditory stimulus are presented simultaneously. The major difference is in
the instructions to the subject concerning which stimulus they should
respond to.
Zwitserlood (1989) used a cross-modal procedure with auditorily pre-
sented sentence contexts and a sentence final word which was or was not
semantically related to a visual target word. The visual probes were
presented at the offset of auditory final word fragments which varied in
394 HOLCOMB AND ANDERSON
Method
Subjects
Twelve right-handed Tufts University undergraduates (7 females, 5
males) with a mean age of 18.5 k 1.0 years received partial course credit
for their participation. They were all native speakers of English with
normal visual and auditory acuity. None of the subjects had participated
in the first experiment.
Data Analysis
The data from Experiment 2 were analysed using the same procedures
employed in Experiment 1.
CROSS-MODAL SEMANTlC PRIMING AND ERPs 395
Results
Behavioural Data
Across the SOA conditions, related targets were responded to more
quickly than unrelated targets [main effect of target type: F(1,ll) = 31.86,
P < 0.001; see Table 11. There was also a main effect of SOA [F(2,22) =
34.91, P < 0.0011, indicating that the RTs became shorter as the SOA
became longer. However, there was no significant interaction between
target type and SOA (P > 0.25), indicating that priming was not signifi-
cantly different in each of the SOA conditions.
The responses to the unrelated targets were only marginally less accurate
than the related targets [F(l,ll) = 3.59, P < 0.0851 and there was no
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
Electrophysiological Data
The grand mean ERPs are plotted in Fig. 2. As in Experiment 1, the
differential overlap of visual and auditory stimuli made the early compo-
nents of the ERPs appear slightly different for each SOA. In the 0 SOA
condition, where auditory and visual stimuli onset at the same time, the
early N1 and P2 components are similar to those seen in Experiment 1.
Note that from a purely physical standpoint, this condition is very similar
to the 0 SOA condition in Experiment 1 in that they both have a
simultaneous visual and auditory stimulus. However, in the 200 and 800
SOA conditions, where the onset of the visual target is not simultaneous
with the onset of the auditory prime, the distributions of the N1 and F 2
are quite different from Experiment 1. At the anterior sites the N1 is small,
and at the posterior sites (01, WL,Pt, WR and 0 2 ) it peaks later (200
msec). Following the N1, there was a positivity (P2)around 200-250 msec,
which was anteriorly distributed in the 200 SOA condition, but was more
widely distributed in the 800msec condition. From this point on, the
waveforms were more positive relative to the baseline than they were in
Experiment 1 (auditory target).
As in Experiment 1, the P2 in this experiment was followed by a
prominent negative-going component (N400), peaking between 350 and
400msec. This negativity was widely distributed and at the anterior sites
was slightly larger over the left hemisphere. Following the negativity was
a large positive wave which peaked between 500 and 600 msec (P3) at the
posterior sites (note that the late positivity at the lateral anterior sites
peaks closer to 900msec). The P3 tended to be larger over the right
hemisphere at anterior sites for all SOAs and at posterior sites for the 800
SOA.
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
396
CROSS-MODAL SEMANTIC PRIMING AND ERPs 397
- T -T
-T \A.
" w.xm
+T3\1'-,
+ .. r:
-......... - T ..... . -1
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
01
Q -R # b r . d W
FIG. 2 Grand mean ERPs (n = 12) to related and unxelatcd visual target words in
Experiment 2: (a) 0 SOA condition; (b) 200 SOA condition; (c) 800 SOA condition. For
further information, see legend to Fig. I.
loo-mmsec
midline 0.81 -0.63 -0.47 NS NS
lateral 0.49 -0.27 0.00 NS NS
u)0-300~
midline 1.2~ -0.47 -1.m NS 0.0746
lateral 0.67 -0.23 -0.27 NS NS
3ak4almxc
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
400-500mSec
midline -0.10 -2.05' -3.45' 0.0030 0.0092
lateral 0.12 -1 .03' -1 .37' 0.0509 0.0408
Mo-6oomscc
midliae -0.41 -0.63 -0.61 NS NS
lateral 0.13 -0.08 0.21 NS NS
600-700 mSCc
midline -0.67 -0.75 -0.08 NS NS
lateral 0.15 -0.41 0.63 NS NS
700-800-
d&C -1.17 -1.35 -0.80 NS NS
Iamd -0.36 -0.86 0.11 NS NS
800-900mxf
midline -0.93 -1.15 -1.38 NS NS
lateral -0.36 -l.W -0.51 NS NS
900-1ooo msec
midline -1.01 -1.16 -1.63' 0.0638 NS
lateral -0.31 -0.90 -0.70 NS NS
1am-1100mSec
midline -1.e -1.796 -1.82' 0.0077 NS
lateral -0.27 -1.256 -0.86 0.0750 NS
NOW:Supencripa indicate dgaificana of separate analyses at each SOA. IT,target type.
'P < 0.1;*P < 0.05; 'P < 0.01;'P < 0.001;'P-value for main effect of target type.
CROSS-MODAL SEMANTIC PRIMING AND ERPs 399
Discussion
When the prime was auditory and the target visual, there was a significant
RT priming effect across the SOAs. However, ERF' priming (i.e. the N400
effect) was significant in the 200 (midline only) and 800 SOA conditions,
but there was no hint of an N400 in the 0 SOA condition. Moreover, the
effect was larger and began earlier in the 800 than the 200 SOA condition.
This pattern of effects is similar, but not identical, to that obtained by
Anderson and Holcomb (submitted) for stimuli within the visual modality.
While they found evidence of EFW priming in the 200 and 800 SOA
conditions, they also obtained a significant N400 effect in the 0 SOA
condition which onset between 300 and 400msec. Therefore, the failure
of the 0 SOA auditory-visual condition to show a similar effect suggests
that cross-modality priming may not rely on the exact same processes as
within-modality priming and calls into question the veracity of the common
semantic system hypotheses offered at the end of Experiment 1. Further
discussion of these findings and their implications will be presented in the
General Discussion.
400 HOLCOMB AND ANDERSON
BETWEEN-EXPERIMENT COMPARISONS
Behavioural Data
In order to test for the existence of any effects due to the modality of
presentation, analyses were done with Experiment as a between-subjects
variable. Visual targets (Experiment 2) were responded to faster (161
msec) than auditory targets (Experiment 1) [main effect of experiment:
F(1,22) = 15.82,P < 0.001].Across experiments, targets were responded
to faster as the SOA became longer [main effect of SOA: F(2,44) = 28.88,
P < 0.0011, but when the target was visual, this decrease had a steeper
decline [experiment x SOA interaction: F(2,44) = 8.15,P < 0.0021. For
both auditory and visual targets, the unrelated targets were responded to
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
more slowly [main effect of target type: F(1,22) = 105.66,P < 0.001],but
this effect was greater when the target was auditory [experiment x target
type interaction: F(l,22) = 25.97, P < 0.001]. Across experiments, the
priming effect was greatest in the 800 SOA [target type x SOA interaction:
F(2,44)= 3.9,P < 0.0341.
More errors were made in Experiment 1 (auditory target) than in
Experiment 2 (visual target) [main effect of experiment: F(1.22) = 12.22,
P < 0.002].Across experiments, the subjects made more errors to unre-
lated targets than to related targets [main effect of target type: F(1,22) =
40.85,P < 0.001],and this effect was greater when the target was auditory
[target type x experiment interaction: F(1,22) = 17.59,P < 0.0011.
Difference Waves
In order to facilitate ERP comparisons between the experiments, differ-
ence waves are formed by subtracting the related from the unrelated
waveforms. This procedure tends to remove waveform features that the
two conditions of interest have in common (e.g. modality-specific “exoge-
nous” activity such as the N1 and F’2 component^)^ and permits an analysis
of pure ERP priming differences between the experiments. The mean
amplitude from 200 to 700 was calculated for each difference wave and the
resulting measures from both experiments were entered into a mixed-
design ANOVA with one between-subject factor (experiment) and two
(SOA and electrode site) or three (SOA, electrode site and hemisphere)
within-subject factors (note that the difference wave technique collapses
JI
+ + A
wL*
+ A
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
the target type factor). These difference wave analyses are intended to
serve as supplementary analyses to the more conventional ones reported
above.
As Fig. 3 illustrates, in Experiment 1 (visual prime and auditory target)
the difference between unrelated and related targets ( N W effect) was
large, peaked around 500-600msec and extended through much of the
measuring epoch in all three SOA conditions. However, in Experiment 2
(auditory prime and visual target), the difference was visible in only the
200 and 800 SOA conditions, peaked around 400-500msec and lasted only
until about 600msec.
A single latency window (200-700msec) was used to quantify the
difference waves. ANOVAs on this measure indicated that the N400 effect
was significantly greater in Experiment 1 than in Experiment 2 [main effect
of experiment, midline: F(1,22)= 5.61,P < 0.027; lateral: F(1,U) = 8.38,
P < 0.008]. The distribution of the effect was not significantly different
between the two experiments (midline: P > 0.90; lateral: P > 0.12),
although across experiments there was a difference in the distribution
[midline: F(2,44) = 3.95,P < 0.047; lateral: F(4,88) = 9.94,P < 0.0011,
indicating that the N400 effect was greater at the more posterior sites.
The interaction of SOA and experiment was significant at the midline
[F(2,44)= 4.01,P < 0.033) and approached significance at the lateral sites
[F(2,44)= 2.65,P < 0.0881.This interaction reflects the finding reported
earlier of statistically similar N400 effects of Experiment 1 (visual prime/
auditory target) but differential effects across SOA for Experiment 2
(auditory prime/visuaI target).
Since previous studies (e.g. Holcomb et al., 1992; Kutas, Van Petten,
& Besson, 1988)have found a difference in the laterality of the visual N400
402 HOLCOMB AND ANDERSON
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Summary of Findings
The main purpose of this study was to examine the interaction of word
processes between the visual and auditory modalities. This was done by
comparing semantic priming effects in two cross-modal experiments in
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
which the interval between the prime and target words was manipulated.
It was predicted that if a common semantic system is shared in the
processing of visual and auditory words, relatively consistent N400 effects
would be seen across SOAs. However, if cross-modal priming is due to a
word-level recoding process or to a post-recognition translation process,
priming would be delayed (due to the extra time required for conversion)
and should only occur for the 800 SOA condition.
When the prime was visual and the target auditory (Experiment l), there
were large behavioural and ERP priming effects observed at all three
SOAs, and the ERP effects onset at a relatively early point after the onset
of the target (between 200 and 400 msec in the 0 SOA condition). These
findings are most consistent with the common semantic system hypothesis
and would seem to contradict the alternative position that cross-modal
priming results from the addition of a word-level recoding or post-
recognition translation process. Further evidence in favour of the common
semantic system hypothesis comes from two sources. First, while the
auditory target effects (Experiment 1) tended to be larger than the visual
ones (Experiment 2), there were no reliable N400 differences across the
scalp between the modalities. That is, the N400 had the same posterior-
maximum, bilaterally symmetrical distribution regardless of whether the
target was visual or auditory. This suggests that for cross-modality presen-
tations, the N400 effect reflects little if any modality-specific pr~cessing.~
‘However, this finding would seem to be at odds with that of Domalski, Smith and Halgren
(1991), who reported a different scalp distribution for auditory-auditory and auditory-visual
words. In their study, auditory targets elicited a relatively larger frontal N400 effect and visual
targets a larger posterior effect. The most likely explanation for the discrepancies between
the studies is differences in methodologies. Dolmalski et al. (1991) w d repetition priming
(as opposed to semantic priming), a long primdrarget interval and an oldhew judgement
task (as opposed to a lexical decision task). Perhaps more important. their comparisons were
not symmetrical, with auditory targets being a withimmodality comparison and visual targets
a beween-modality comparison.
CROSS-MODAL SEMANTIC PRIMING AND ERPs 403
Second, the pattern of effects across SOAs in Experiment 1 was similar to
the within-modality studies of Anderson and Holcomb (submitted). This
suggests that the same mechanism was operating for between- and within-
modality semantic priming. This is inconsistent with the conversion
hypothesis, which argues that somewhat different mechanisms should
mediate priming within and between modalities.
However, it would appear that the above conclusions should be tem-
pered by the findings from Experiment 2, where the prime was auditory
and the target visual. Although reaction time differences were significant
in the 0 SOA condition, there was no semantic priming effect on the N400.
In the 200 SOA condition there were small N400 effects, while in the 800
SOA condition there were quite robust effects. These results are quite
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
BehaviouralERP Differences
There was also evidence of differences in auditory and visual processing
in the comparisons between the behavioural and ERP data. For example,
although there were no ERP priming effects at the 0 SOA in Experiment
2, there was a significant (33msec) reaction time priming effect. One
possibility for these different findings is that the RT and N400 effects might
reflect the operation of a somewhat different set of processes (Holcomb,
1993; Kounios & Holcomb, 1992). For example, it may be that RT is more
sensitive to certain post-lexical decision processes such as Neely and
Keefe’s (1989) retrospective matching strategy. According to this account,
some part of RT semantic priming in the lexical decision task is due to the
relatedness of the prime and target being used to help make the appropri-
ate wordnonword decision. In the case of related primes and targets, when
a high degree of relatedness is detected, a rapid “word” decision can be
made, but in the case of an unrelated prime and target, there is an initial
tendency to decide “nonword” which delays the correct “word” response.
Evidence that subjects use this strategy comes from the higher error rates
for unrelated targets, which presumably result from subjects occasionally
acting on their initial semantically based impulse to respond “nonword”.
All of the unrelated conditions in both experiments produced higher error
rates than the related condition, suggesting that the subjects were using a
retrospective semantic-matching strategy. Unfortunately, there has not
been a study designed to look at the effect of the retrospective matching
strategy on the N400.However, Kounios and Holcomb (1992)have shown
that certain other late decision processes that influence RT in a sentence
verification task do not seem to have much impact on the N400.
406 HOLCOMB AND ANDERSON
Conclusions
Most of the data from the current experiments seem to favour the interpre-
tation that cross-modality semantic priming, as measured by the N400, is
mediated by a common semantic system that receives input from separate
modality-specific recognition systems. The data do not fit as well with
either of the alternative .interpretations (recoding or post-recognition
translation) because neither of these can account for the presence of
equivalent priming across SOAs in the visual/auditory experiment (Experi-
ment 1). The seemingly most damaging finding for the common semantic
system hypothesis was the absence of a robust ERP priming effect for the
0 SOA auditorylvisual condition. However, neither of the most plausible
explanations for this finding-namely, that visual target processing cap-
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
tured most of the available attentional resources leaving little for auditory
prime processing andor that visual target processing was fast enough to
not benefit from the partial auditory information available at this short
interval-are inconsistent with the common semantic system position.
One line of future studies will need to extend this research to other
domains. For example, while it makes sense that language would maintain
a common semantic system for spoken and written words, it is less clear
why image-based processes important for picture recognition would neces-
sarily tap the same semantic system. By examining the time-course of
wordpicture and picture/word priming, it should be possible to determine
if there is a similar or different pattern of priming between words and
pictures.
Manuscript received December 1991
Revised manuscript received April 1993
REFERENCES
Anderson, J.E.,& Holcornb, P.J. (submitted). Auditory and visual semantic priming using
different stimulus onset asynchronies: An event-related brain potential study.
Bradley, D.C.,& Fonter. K.I.(1987). A reader's view of listening. Cognirion,25,103-134.
Brown, C.. & Hagoort, P. (1993). The processing nature of the N400: Evidence from
masked priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5. 34-44.
Colavita. F.B. (1974). Human sensory dominance. Perception and Prychophysics, 16,
409-412.
Colavita. F.B., & Weisberg, D. (1979). A further investigation of visual dominance. Percep-
rion and Psychophysiu, 25,345-347.
Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical aaxss in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.),
Strategies of inform'on processing. London: Academic Press.
Domalski, P., Smith, M.E.,& Halgren, E. (1991). Cross-modal repetition effects on the
N4. Psychological Science. 2 , 173-178.
Ellis. A.W., & Young, A.W. (1988). Human cognitive neuropsychology. Hove: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
CROSS-MODAL SEMANTIC PRIMING AND ERPs 407
Geisscr, S.,& Greenhouse, S. (1959). On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psycho-
mem'ka, 24, 95-112.
Holcomb. P.J. (1988). Automatic and attcntional procews: An event-related brain poten-
tial analysis of semantic priming. Broin and Lunguge, 35,685.
Holcomb, P.J. (1993). Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: Implications for the role
of the N400 in language p-ing. P ~ ~ h o ~ h ~ s i o 30,
l o g47-61.
y,
Holcomb, P.J., & Neville, H.J. (1990). Semantic priming in visual and auditory lexical
decision: A between modality comparison. h g u a g e and Cognitive Procases, 5,281-312.
Holcomb, P.J., & Neville, H.J. (1991). The electrophysiology of spoken sentence proces-
sing. Psychobiology, 19, 286300.
Holcomb, P.J., Coffey, S., & Neville. H. (1992). The effects of context on visual and
auditory sentena processing: A developmental analysis using event-related brain poten-
tials. Developmend Newopsychology, 8, 203-241.
Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P.J. (1992). Structure and process in semantic memory: Evidence
from event-related braio potentials and reaction times. Journal of E X p c M Psy-
Downloaded by [University of Exeter] at 16:35 05 June 2016
APPENDIX
Related Pairs
Word-Pseudoword Pain