Behavsci 10 00038 v2

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

behavioral

sciences
Article
Life and Family Values Similarity in Inter-Ethnic and
Inter-Faith Couples
Elena Yu. Chebotareva 1, * and Marina I. Volk 2
1 Moscow State University of Psychology and Education, National Research University “Higher School of
Economics”, Ulitsa Sretenka, 29, 127051; Ulitsa Myasnitskaya, 20, 101000 Moscow, Russia
2 Peoples’ Friendship University (RUDN University), Ulitsa Miklukho-Maklaya, 6, 117198 Moscow, Russia;
[email protected]
* Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +7-985-998-23-30

Received: 20 November 2019; Accepted: 14 January 2020; Published: 19 January 2020 

Abstract: Inter-cultural families are an integral part of modern society, the institution of mutual
influence of different cultures, of a person’s identity transformation. The studies of marital adjustment,
values, and attitudes consistency in inter-cultural couples provide contradictory results. To resolve
contradictions in this area, comparative studies of inter-cultural families of different types are
important. The aim of the study is the comparative analysis of life and family values in inter-cultural
couples, differing by the ethnic and religious affiliation of spouses. The participants: 69 couples:
20 mono-ethnic Russian, 30—inter-ethnic, inter-faith (Russians/Arabs); 19—inter-ethnic, with a
common religion (Russian/Transcaucasian, Christians). The methods: Value Survey (Schwartz),
Marital Role Expectations and Aspirations (Volkova), Marital Satisfaction Test (Stolin et al.),
Mann–Whitney U-test, paired T-test. In inter-cultural couples, the spouses’ life-values coincidence is
significantly less strong than in mono-cultural ones. However, in couples with common religious
differences, their life values reflect not so much the contradictions, but the complementarity of
traditional gender values. In general, the different cultures of spouses (both ethnic and religious)
optimizes the process of comparing values and family attitudes. Despite a number of difficulties,
spouses from inter-cultural couples generally have more consistent ideas about their family life.

Keywords: life values; family values; inter-ethnic marriage; inter-confessional marriage; marital
satisfaction; marital adjustment

1. Introduction
Inter-cultural families have become an integral part of society, their number is steadily growing
all over the world [1]. The wave of migration in recent years has drawn increased attention to the
study of the processes of integration of migrants into the host community; including the psychological
problems of inter-cultural families and their influence on the identities of spouses. Many researchers of
the problem point out that religious identity is the main barrier for inter-ethnic marriage, although
inter-faith marriage is becoming more common [2–5]. In general, studies of marital adjustment and
satisfaction with relationships in inter-cultural couples provide contradictory results. Some studies
show that, in cross-cultural couples, different cultural traditions and values of spouses cause great
conflicts and, accordingly, these couples are less satisfied with their marriages [6,7]. Many studies
show that homogeneous marriages are more prosperous and stable [8,9]. Other studies have confirmed
an equal level of satisfaction and sustainability of inter-cultural and mono-cultural marriages [10].
But there is also some scientific evidence that satisfaction with marriage in inter-cultural families may
be greater than in mono-cultural ones [11]. Among the factors that help couples to cope with the

Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38; doi:10.3390/bs10010038 www.mdpi.com/journal/behavsci


Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 2 of 14

difficulties of inter-cultural marriages identified in the studies are the positive ethnic identity of the
spouses, a positive attitude towards the culture of the partner [7,12].
Many researchers of inter-cultural marriages agree that one of the most acute problems is the
comparison of values and attitudes of the spouses. There is a wide range of points of view on this
issue. On the one hand, it is assumed that inter-cultural families are mainly created by people who
either have already culturally assimilated or have certain values, such as universalism, tolerance, and
multiculturalism [13]. On the other hand, it is assumed that the similarity of the spouses’ worldviews
is the main problem of such marriages [13,14]. Somewhere in the middle of this continuum, there
are ideas that cultural differences can be reduced due to the similarity in education and personal
development [15] or that two people create their own “relational culture” that does not contradict
the cultural values of each spouse [10,16]. Thus, it is important to study the life values of spouses in
inter-cultural marriages, taking into account their family values and attitudes towards family life [17].
In many studies of inter-cultural marriages, the variable of religious affiliation of spouses is not
controlled, which may explain the inconsistency of data from different studies in this area.
We have conducted an empirical study with the aim of a comparative analysis of life and family
values in inter-cultural families, which differ only in in ethnic or in ethnic and religious affiliation of
spouses. We also pursued the aim of assessing differences in the degree of general marital satisfaction
of the spouses in different types of inter-cultural couples. That allows us to understand better
the psychological mechanisms for the implementation of cultural and personal values in family
relationships; about the processes of mutual adaptation (and acculturation) in inter-cultural families.
In this study of life values, we focus on the S. Schwartz approach, who defines basic values as the
desired cross-situational goals, varying in importance and serving as guiding principles in human
life [18]. S. Schwartz regards culture as a latent, hypothetical variable that can be measured only
through its manifestations, in particular, through life values. He also argues that ethnic, professional,
religious, and other groups within society can be exposed to different cultural influences and can
develop different value preferences. These differences lead to social tension, conflict, and change.
According to S. Schwartz’s theory, the person’s values exist at two levels: at the level of normative
ideals and at the level of individual priorities. The first level is more stable and reflects the person’s
ideas about how to act, thereby determining his life principles of behavior. The second level is more
dependent on the external environment, for example, on group pressure and correlates with specific
actions of a person [19,20]. Family values, accordingly, we consider as cultural values related to the
sphere of family life, in particular, to the structure, functions, roles, beliefs and ideals of the family.
In accordance with these ideas, we find it important to take into account both ethnic and religious
factors when considering the values of spouses from inter-cultural families. As P. Tillich stated, religion
and culture have an intimate reciprocal relationship and a long history of interaction, he said that
culture may be considered as the form of religion and religion as the substance of culture [21]. Religion
and ethnos are a specific type of social associations of people, not always closely interlinked with each
other. In the ethnic processes taking place in the world, the role of religion is great. However, ethnic
groups differ in the degree of their conjugation with a faith. Many people are grounded in mainstream
religious traditions, belonging to one of the world’s main faith. However, some other people belong to
small, non-mainstream groups that “may be insular and extreme–and sometimes intolerant, legalistic,
and fanatic. . . . These groups may be considered fundamentalist or radical” [22] p. 1092. Accordingly,
to identify the general patterns of inter-ethnic and inter-faith marriages, we should focus on world’s
main religions and on the ethnic groups conjugated with a faith. In the study of the role of religion in
family relationships, it is also important to take into account the parameter of the spouses’ religiosity
degree. Most often, a self-reporting scale is used to assess the degree of religiosity with atheism at
one end and extreme religiosity at the other end of the scale [23]. The assessment is based on such
parameters as faith in the immortality of the soul, in the existence of a higher power, a supernatural
world, as well as the importance of these issues for the respondent [24].
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 3 of 14

2. Materials and Methods


The study involved 138 people (69 couples). Twenty couples were with both spouses ethnic
Russians, belonging to Orthodox Christianity; 30 couples consisted of Russians, Orthodox Christian
women and men from various Arab countries (Yemen, Lebanon, Syria, Jordan, Palestine, etc.), belonging
to Islam; 19 couples consisted of Russian, Orthodox Christian women and their husbands from the
Transcaucasian region (mostly Armenians and Ossetians and some Georgians, all men were Christians;).
Armenian people belong to Armenian Apostolic Church, which have a number of features in dogma
and rituals that distinguish it from Byzantine Orthodoxy and, accordingly, from the Russian Orthodox
Church. But the well-known Italian orientalist—Armenologist Giovanni Guita—believes that, despite
a number of theological differences, if “faith is stated purely theoretically, the differences between
Catholicism, Byzantine-Slavic Orthodoxy and the Armenian Church are minimal, the commonality is,
conditionally speaking, 98 or 99 percent” [25]. What is important for this research aims, is that both
churches preach the same life values. North Ossetia is often mentioned as an Orthodox republic, due to
the very strong orientation of the population to the Orthodox religion and culture. The Constitution of
South Ossetia enshrines the provision according to which “Orthodoxy is the basis of the worldview of
the Ossetian people.” [26]. About 90% of the Georgian population profess Orthodox Christianity—most
of them are adherents of the Georgian Orthodox Church, which have prayer and eucharistic communion
with the Russian Orthodox Church [27].
So, two types of inter-cultural couples were analyzed: inter-ethnic–mono-faith (which we will call
shortly “inter-ethnic”) and inter-ethnic, complicated by different religiosity (next we will call it shortly
“inter-faith”). The control grope for both these research groups were the group of mono-ethnic couples
with common religion.
These subsamples were formed from a much larger number of respondents 225 couples
(450 persons), including 85 Russian—Arab; 80 Russian—Russian and 60 Russian—Caucasian.
This helped us to equalize subsamples that were consistent with each other by most basic
socio-demographic characteristics: age, education level, type of work, children, previous divorces,
children from previous marriages, self-rating level of material well-being and so on.
The age of the respondents was from 23 to 47 years (M = 36: women—32; men—40), they have
been married from 3 to 24 years (M = 6.3); for 30% of respondents this marriage was the second; the
majority (87%) of the surveyed families had 1–2 children, aged from 1 to 13 years old (M = 5.2); 10% of
couples had children from previous marriages, everyone has a job (full-time), mainly hired employees,
ordinary employees or mid-level managers, foreign partners lived in their spouse’s country from 5 to
30 years (M = 9.2), everyone were moderately religious.
In the groups of intercultural marriages, unlike monocultural, there were no couples
with unregistered marriages (in the monocultural group, 30% cohabitation); in the group of
Russian–Caucasian marriages, unlike the other 2 groups, women on average occupy lower-level
positions at work. We believe that the absence of cohabitation is related to cultural traditions and
migration issues. A lower level of the job position of women married to a Caucasian partner is due to
the factor of their living in a more traditional culture. Furthermore, in the Caucasian–Russian group
the age of respondents was on average lower (M = 31) than in the other two groups. Russian–Arab
(RA) and mono-cultural Russian (RR) families lived in different cities of Russia, Russian–Caucasian
(RC) families lived in their native regions of their husbands—in cities of the Transcaucasian regions.
Methods of the research. To study the life values of the respondent, the method of S. Schwartz
“Value Questionnaire” [28], adapted by V. Karandashev [29], was used; for identifying the attitudes
to family life and compatibility of family values we used the method of “Role Expectations and
Claims in Marriage” by A.Volkova et al. [30]. Additionally to the main methods the study included an
express method for assessing marital—the “Marital satisfaction Questionnaire” by V.Stolin et al. [31].
Although the study of the spouses’ satisfaction with their marriage was not the aim of the study, this
measurement was added to provide an overall assessment of the effects of consistency or discrepancies
in the spouses’ values. The general level of religiosity was also controlled. One item was included in
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 4 of 14

the socio-demographic questionnaire, in which 5 levels of religiosity were briefly described (according
to D. Chumakova [24]), and the respondents were asked to choose the description most suitable for
them. The sample did not include people who showed radical levels in terms of both involvement in
religion and denial of religion. The question of religious affiliation was formulated: “What religion do
you belong to?” The respondents indicated their religion themselves. The study included only those
respondents who indicated Orthodox Christianity or Islam, according to their ethnic group. Statistical
analysis was carried out using descriptive statistics, Mann–Whitney U-test. To assess the degree of
statistical significance of the differences between spouses, a paired T-test was used. Calculations were
performed using STATIATICA—12.
Since this is a psychological non-clinical study, it was evaluated in terms of APA “Ethical Principles
of Psychologists and Code of Conduct”, date 2003 with latest amendments 2017. The protocol of the
study was approved by the Committee on publication ethics (COPE) of Peoples’ Friendship University
of Russia (RUDN) (Project identification code—050422-0-010).

3. Results

3.1. Lfe Values in Inter-cultural Families


A comparative analysis of the life values of spouses from the three considered groups (Table 1) first
of all shows that at the normative, cultural level, there are more statistically significant differences than at
the level of personal values. In general, this once again confirms S. Schwartz’s theory position about the
differences between the normative values of a person and his/her everyday reaction [28,32]. Statistically
significant differences at the normative level of values were revealed for all values between Russian and
Russian–Arab couples; between Russian–Caucasian and homogeneous Russian couples—for all values
except conformance. For spouses from mono-cultural and Russian–Caucasian couples, conformity is
less important than for the spouses from Russian–Arab ones. As inter-ethnic–inter-faith couples have a
large social distance both between spouses and between one of the spouses and the couple’s social
environment, for better social adaptation, they need conformity. In general, most normative values are
much more important for Russian–Arab families than for Russian ones, which supports the differences
in the value of conformity and indicates a greater commitment of this group to normative life values
in general.
Comparison of the normative values of Russian–Caucasian couples with Russian–Arab ones
revealed that for Russian–Caucasian couples, many normative values are less significant than for
Russian–Arab couples. This supports the idea that a larger cultural distance between the spouses in a
couple is associated with a greater commitment of the spouses to normative, general human, universal
values. There were no significant differences between these groups in the degree of importance of the
values of independence, and achievements, which in both groups are recognized as quite important,
and in the value of stimulation, which is of low importance for both groups. The structure of the values
does not differ radically.
S. Schwartz’ value theories postulate, that each of the 10 values correlates significantly with a set
of everyday behaviors that is motivated with the value. The sets of behaviors ate largely consistent
with the motivational continuum. That is, in most cases, values correlate most positively with their
expected behaviors, and most negatively with the behaviors motivated by values motivationally
opposed. The gender role expectations may moderate correlations in some domains [32]. Thus, the first
part of the method “Normative values” reflects a person’s firm beliefs about the degree of importance
of various universal values, the second part “Personal values” reflects a person’s individual priorities
or typical behavioral attitudes in certain life situations. This allows us to see the degree of coordination
of beliefs and real motives of a person’s behavior, as well as how much a person manages to realize
his/her life values in real life (which is especially actual in situations of inter-cultural interaction).
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 5 of 14

Table 1. Comparison of the importance of life values for spouses of mono-cultural, inter-ethnic and
inter-faith couples.

Statistical Significance of
Types of Couples
Differences
Life Values Russian– Russian– Russian– R–C/ R–A/ R–C/
Caucasian Arab Russian R–R R–R R–A
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD p p p
Normative Values
Conformity 3.23 1.09 4.53 0.64 2.86 0.87 0.094 0.000 0.000
Tradition 3.69 0.69 4.62 0.79 2.44 0.91 0.000 0.000 0.000
Benevolence 4.30 0.67 4.91 0.75 3.76 0.72 0.001 0.000 0.000
Universalism 3.96 1.17 4.43 0.70 3.03 0.63 0.000 0.000 0.013
Self-Direction 4.91 0.75 4.91 0.72 3.87 0.71 0.000 0.000 0.986
Stimulation 3.17 1.24 3.15 0.93 2.44 0.94 0.004 0.000 0.943
Hedonism 3.63 0.86 4.38 1.20 3.07 0.95 0.009 0.000 0.001
Achievement 5.03 0.83 4.77 0.76 3.29 0.75 0.000 0.000 0.146
Power 3.73 0.93 4.28 1.01 2.58 1.15 0.000 0.000 0.015
Security 4.41 0.91 5.49 0.49 3.46 0.79 0.000 0.000 0.000
Personal Values
Conformity 1.10 0.69 2.27 0.74 1.74 0.76 0.0002 0.001 0.000
Tradition 1.11 0.61 2.10 0.79 0.90 0.66 0.156 0.000 0.000
Benevolence 1.52 0.69 1.94 0.82 1.83 0.62 0.042 0.441 0.009
Universalism 1.38 0.69 2.17 0.59 1.36 0.48 0.872 0.000 0.000
Self-Direction 2.41 0.59 2.31 0.60 2.11 0.59 0.037 0.131 0.461
Stimulation 1.30 0.68 1.37 0.66 1.56 1.04 0.198 0.259 0.629
Hedonism 0.96 0.49 1.88 1.08 1.88 1.04 0.0000 0.980 0.000
Achievement 1.72 0.65 2.14 0.81 1.91 1.04 0.356 0.233 0.013
Power 1.61 0.56 1.58 0.99 1.12 0.87 0.008 0.040 0.890
Security 1.71 0.45 2.82 0.62 2.18 0.88 0.006 0.000 0.000

At the level of individual priorities (everyday behavior) between three groups of couples less
significant differences were found. There were no significant differences in the importance of
individual values of stimulation for all three groups. The value of self-direction is less important for
mono-cultural couples than for both types of cross-cultural couples, but especially so in comparison
with Russian–Caucasian.
In comparison with mono-cultural Russian couples, the everyday behavior of spouses from
Russian–Caucasian couples is less motivated by the values of benevolence, hedonism and security, but
is more strongly motivated by the values of self-direction and power. This is consistent with the ethnic
stereotypes of the groups.
The differences between individual values in Russian–Arabian and mono-cultural Russian couples
shows the stronger orientation of inter-faith couples for the values of traditions, universalism, power
and security. That is also consistent with the ethnic stereotypes.
The differences between individual values in Russian–Caucasian and Russian–Arabian couples
are, in general, the same as at the normative level, with one exception. If there was no significant
difference in the importance of achievement, then at the normative level for Russian–Arab couples
achievement is more important than for Russian–Caucasian, difference in the values of power which is
significant at the normative level (with its greater importance for the Russian–Arab couples, at the
individual values are leveled.
In the hierarchy of individual values for all three groups, independence (self-direction) is the
most important value, followed by security and achievements. On the whole, the life values of couples
of Russian women that are representatives of different cultures at the normative level differ more
significantly than at the individual level.
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 6 of 14

In general, for inter-cultural families, normative (general cultural) values are more important
than for mono-cultural ones. Presumably, the greater cultural distance of the spouses in a couple
is associated with the greater commitment of the spouses to normative values. Furthermore, for
intercultural families in general, the values of self-reliance, stimulation (new impressions) and power
are more important than for mono-cultural ones.
The value of achievements, recognized by Russian–Caucasian families as one of the most important
at the normative level, does not find its realization in everyday behavior, is considered less important
than in other families. Furthermore, for this group, conformity, kindness, hedonism, and security are
important in everyday life, and power is more important than others. Apparently, at the individual
level, the value of power in some way compensates for the rejection of the value of achievements.

3.2. Family Values in Inter-cultural Families


The comparative analysis of the degree of importance of family values for the spouses (Table 2)
showed that for the Russian–Caucasian couples, the household sphere is important to a greater extent
than for the other two groups, and the parental sphere is more important than for mono-cultural
couples. Perhaps this is due to the cultural traditions of the regions where the respondents live. In the
Transcaucasian region, a good home improvement, comfort and hospitality are very much appreciated.
The Russian–Caucasian group also demonstrated a statistically significant greater willingness of
spouses, regardless of gender, to assume obligations in these two areas. It is interesting to note that the
strongest differences among Russian–Caucasian families were revealed in claims in the household
sphere with Russian families, and in the parental sphere—with Russian–Arab families.

Table 2. Comparison of the importance of family values for spouses of mono-cultural, inter-ethnic and
inter-faith couples.

Statistical Significance of
Types of Couples
Differences
Life Values
Russian– Russian– Russian– R–C/ R–A/ R–C/
Caucasian Arab Russian R–R R–R R–A
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD p p p
The importance of the family spheres
Sexual 5.84 1.26 5.55 2.12 5.60 1.52 0.462 0.904 0.475
Personal
identification with 6.53 1.15 6.47 1.89 6.65 1.54 0.691 0.614 0.864
the spouse
Householding 6.46 1.12 5.77 1.33 5.50 1.43 0.002 0.350 0.012
Parental 6.91 0.83 6.49 1.57 6.38 1.16 0.028 0.696 0.144
Social activity 6.66 0.69 6.48 1.48 6.41 1.62 0.385 0.825 0.513
Psychotherapeutic 6.61 0.95 6.99 1.46 6.86 1.37 0.338 0.663 0.161
Appearance 6.16 0.96 6.37 1.95 6.26 1.44 0.710 0.783 0.565
Role claims in the family spheres
Householding 6.42 1.36 5.60 2.00 5.60 1.88 0.048 0.128 0.033
Parental 6.95 1.27 6.08 2.24 6.28 2.10 0.034 0.225 0.091
Social activity 6.92 0.67 7.03 1.71 6.78 1.91 0.715 0.797 0.669
Psychotherapeutic 6.32 1.14 6.20 2.31 6.53 1.39 0.785 0.059 0.472
Appearance 5.95 1.23 6.28 2.24 5.48 1.99 0.452 0.118 0.211
Role expectations from the partners in the family spheres
Householding 6.50 1.38 5.93 1.71 5.40 1.88 0.106 0.272 0.002
Parental 7.05 0.92 6.90 1.78 6.48 2.10 0.627 0.487 0.050
Social activity 6.39 0.99 5.93 1.98 6.05 1.91 0.268 0.359 0.335
Psychotherapeutic 6.87 1.09 7.78 1.34 7.20 1.39 0.001 0.092 0.314
Appearance 6.39 0.93 6.45 2.30 7.10 1.99 0.890 0.011 0.023
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 7 of 14

In the expectations from the partners in these two spheres, the Russian–Caucasian families also
show significantly higher indicators than Russian—Arab in the two spheres, but significant differences
with mono-cultural Russian families on this parameter were not revealed. That is, compared to the
Russian–Arab and Russian–Caucasian, homogeneous Russian families consider the householding
and parental functions of the family to be less significant, they are less willing to fulfill these roles
and to a lesser extent expect it from their partners. For Russian–Arab families, in comparison with
Russian–Caucasian, these areas are also less significant, they are less willing to fulfill these roles
themselves, but not less than Russian–Caucasian ones expect their performance from their partners.
In addition, Russian–Arab couples showed a higher level of expectation for the partner’s
appearance, compared to mono-cultural Russian and Russian–Caucasian couples. And spouses from
Russian–Caucasian couples expected less from their partner’s therapeutic role performance, compared
to both other groups, especially mono-cultural Russian. Significant differences between the studied
groups were also revealed in the role adequacy of the couples (i.e., the degree of conformity of the
expectations of one partner from another to the role claims of the second partner, i.e., the extent to
which one is willing to take responsibility for performing a particular role). A low degree of role
adequacy implies an explicit or latent role conflict. In Russian–Caucasian families, the role adequacy
in matters of the partners’ appearance is significantly higher than in Russian (p = 0.002), and in the
therapeutic sphere—though significantly higher than in Russian–Arab (p = 0.03). Comparing these
results with the data of our previous study of Russian–Arab families’ values [33], we believe that
these data reflect a greater psychological well-being of inter-ethnic couples with a common religion,
compared with inter-faith couples; as well as a lesser significance for family life of external, not directly
related to the relationship, factors (in particular, the appearance of a partner) in inter-cultural couples,
compared with mono-cultural Russian couples.

3.3. Consistency of Spouses’ Values


Of particular interest to our study was the degree of consistency of values (value unity) between
spouses in families of different types. With the help of paired T-test, we compared the degree of
divergence in the ideas of the spouses about the importance of life and family values and attitudes
towards family life in the types of families under consideration. When assessing the consistency of
values in the couples, the difference between the indicators of husbands and wives was calculated.
Accordingly, positive values of the difference variable mean higher levels of importance of a certain
value for husbands, and negative values mean higher levels of importance of the value for wives.
The analysis of the degree of coincidence of the life values of spouses showed that statistically
significant differences in the value concepts between Russian spouses in homogeneous couples are
minimal, and the maximum number of discrepancies was noted in Russian–Caucasian couples.
In Russian families, significant differences at the normative level are noted only in the values of
universalism (p = 0.02) and power (p = 0.04). Women are more focused on the universal values, than
their husbands, and men - on the value of power, which reflects gender stereotypes. At the individual
level, a significant discrepancy exists only in the value of independence (p = 0.07), also in accordance
with gender stereotypes, for men this value is more important.
At the normative level, Russian–Arab couples significantly differ in their ideas about the importance
of conformity values (p = 0.009, prevails in women), achievements (p = 0.02) and power (p = 0.003,
prevail in men). At the individual level, differences in the values of power (p = 0.00000 *), independence
(p = 0.0005), safety (p = 0.004) and achievements (p = 0.02), which prevail in men, are also significant.
That is, in this group of families, gender stereotypes are even more pronounced in the value orientations
of the spouses, both at the normative and at the individual levels.
In Russian–Caucasian families, significant differences were found in the values of power prevailing
in men (p = 0.0000 *), achievements (p = 0.0001), independence (p = 0.01), traditions (p = 0.03) and
security (p = 0.05); in women, compared with their men, the value of kindness prevails (p = 0.02).
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 8 of 14

Differences in individual values largely coincide with the normative level, but significant predominance
of hedonism value (p = 0.05) in women was also found.
Such results suggest that the existence of a common religion in inter-ethnic families increases
the importance of traditional values (as was proved above), including in relation to the traditional
distribution of social roles between the genders. This, in turn, suggests that the differences in values
reflected not so much the contradictions in the values of the spouses as the complementarity of the
values of men and women.
Next, let us consider the ratio of family values of spouses from different types of couples. Table 3
provides data on the statistical significance of differences in the views of spouses on the importance of
different areas of family life.

Table 3. The consistency of the spouses’ attitudes about the significance of the family spheres in the
couples of different types.

Types of Couples
Family Life Russian–Caucasian Russian–Arab Russian–Russian
Spheres
Diff. * t p Diff. * t p Diff. * t p
Sexual 0.74 3.68 0.0017 1.70 3.58 0.0012 0.80 2.56 0.0193
Personal
identification with 0.32 1.03 0.3163 −0.60 −1.61 0.1188 −0.30 −1.19 0.2492
the spouse
Householding 0.82 3.55 0.0023 −0.80 −2.23 0.0339 0.00 −0.01 0.9901
Parental 0.34 1.95 0.0669 −0.52 −1.25 0.2215 0.65 2.20 0.0401
Social activity 0.11 0.70 0.4944 −0.90 −2.79 0.0093 −0.13 −0.44 0.6663
Psychotherapeutic 0.26 1.17 0.2557 −0.75 −2.27 0.0311 −0.13 −0.42 0.6804
Appearance 0.58 3.54 0.0023 −1.23 −2.52 0.0174 −0.23 −0.55 0.5891
* positive values of the difference variable mean higher levels of importance of a certain value for husbands, and
negative values mean higher levels of importance of the value for wives.

As can be seen from Table 3, the minimum number of differences is observed in Russian families,
and the maximum in Russian–Arab families. For men in Russian mono-cultural families, as in
inter-cultural families, the sexual sphere is more significant than for their wives, which is quite
traditional. Besides, for men in Russian families, more than for their wives, the sphere of parenthood is
more important. In Russian–Caucasian families, the same difference is noted at the level of significance.
It speaks, rather, of the declining significance of the traditionally female parental role in modern Russian
women, who are both in mono-cultural and inter-cultural marriages, apparently due to the assumption
of new roles, as well as to the distribution of parental responsibilities with their husbands. However,
in marriages with Muslims—in comparison with marriages with Christians—Russian women retain a
greater importance of the parent sphere compared with their husbands.
Interestingly, in Russian–Caucasian families, the household sector and the appearance of their
partners is more important for men than for their wives; whereas in Russian–Arab families, on the
contrary, these areas are much more important for women. There are also significant differences in the
views of spouses from Russian–Arab families on the importance of the psychotherapeutic sphere of
the family, which is more significant for women. Social activity is also more important for women in
these families. Perhaps these differences are explained by the fact that the families participating in this
study lived in the motherland of women, the men were in a foreign cultural environment, which could
reduce the importance of social activity for them.
In general, these results show that a large cultural distance between spouses is associated with
large differences in the ideas of spouses about the importance of areas of family life. Moreover, the
factor of common religion reduces the number of contradictions.
As can be seen from Table 4, in mono-cultural Russian families there are no significant differences
in the role claims of the spouses. Both men and women are equally willing to take responsibility for
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 9 of 14

all major areas of family life. There are significant differences in both types of inter-cultural families.
As described above, features in the distribution of roles between spouses were discovered. Thus, in
Russian–Caucasian families, the overall level of role-playing claims is higher for men than for their
wives, and in Russian–Arab ones it is higher for women. The same differences are noted in relation
to the psychotherapeutic role. Furthermore, in Russian–Caucasian families, men are much more
willing to implement an external social activity, while in Russian–Arab families there are no significant
differences in this role—in Russian–Arab families, women are more willing to fulfill household roles
and to take care of their appearance. Partially, these data are explained by gender stereotypes, but it is
also obvious that a lot determines the place of residence, i.e., which of the spouses lives in a foreign
environment [1].

Table 4. The consistency of role claims (willingness to assume responsibility for the areas of family life)
of spouses in different types of couples.

Types of Couples
Family Life Spheres Russian–Caucasian Russian–Arab Russian–Russian
Diff. * t p Diff. * t p Diff. * t p
Householding 0.42 0.88 0.3915 −2.47 −4.20 0.0002 −0.50 −0.85 0.4085
Parental 0.32 0.78 0.4457 −0.57 −0.90 0.3759 1.20 1.95 0.0655
Social activity 1.21 5.11 0.0001 0.73 1.65 0.1097 0.45 1.34 0.1965
Psychotherapeutic 0.53 2.14 0.0465 −1.60 −3.72 0.0009 −0.15 −0.38 0.7112
Appearance 0.11 0.62 0.5416 −2.17 −3.57 0.0013 −0.25 −0.40 0.6949
General level of claims 0.52 3.15 0.0055 −1.21 −3.17 0.0036 0.22 0.82 0.4204
* positive values of the difference variable mean higher levels of importance of a certain value for husbands, and
negative values mean higher levels of importance of the value for wives.

As can be seen from Table 5, in mono-cultural Russian families there are also no significant
differences in the expectations of the spouses in relation to each other. In Russian–Arab families, the
only difference was revealed—that women expect more from their husbands to realize external social
activity than their husbands from them. As already mentioned, we link this is due to the fact that
men in these families are in a foreign cultural environment, which apparently reduces their social
activity (while maintaining the desire to assume this role), which causes, perhaps, some tension in the
expectations of their wives in this domain. In Russian–Caucasian families there are differences in most
areas of family life. Men expect more from their wives to perform household roles and social activity,
women have a higher overall level of role expectations in relation to their spouses. That also confirms
the previously described influence of cultural background on women in these families. Expectations
for the spouses’ appearance in these families are higher for women. As mentioned earlier, this area is
more meaningful for women from these families; therefore, apparently, they expect their husbands to
meet their demands regarding appearance.

Table 5. The consistency of role expectations (expectations from the spouse of taking responsibility for
the spheres of family life) of spouses in different types of couples.

Types of Couples
Family Life Spheres
Russian–Caucasian Russian–Arab Russian–Russian
Diff. * t p Diff. * t p Diff. * t p
Householding 1.11 3.33 0.0038 0.87 1.85 0.0749 0.50 1.03 0.3145
Parental 0.42 1.80 0.0880 −0.47 −1.08 0.2905 −0.25 −0.53 0.6005
Social activity −1.00 −5.85 0.0000 −2.53 −5.07 0.0000 −0.70 −1.35 0.1929
Psychotherapeutic 0.26 0.86 0.3986 0.10 0.29 0.7773 −0.10 −0.30 0.7663
Appearance 1.21 4.65 0.0002 −0.30 −0.52 0.6075 −0.10 −0.26 0.8004
General level of
0.40 3.17 0.0053 −0.47 −1.61 0.1186 −0.13 −0.56 0.5793
expectations
* positive values of the difference variable mean higher levels of importance of a certain value for husbands, and
negative values mean higher levels of importance of the value for wives.
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 10 of 14

Thus, we see that in inter-cultural families there are more differences in the perceptions of spouses
about the importance of the spheres of family life and the distribution of roles between spouses. On the
one hand, these discrepancies reflect traditional stereotypes about genders’ social and family roles.
On the other hand, the factor of foreign cultural environment plays an important role in the attitudes
toward family life of spouses from inter-cultural families. The shared religion of spouses contributes
to greater consistency of family values of spouses but also contributes to a greater commitment of
spouses to traditional family life patterns.

3.4. Marital Satisfaction


Next, we will consider how the identified features in the values and the degree of their consistency
manifest in terms of satisfaction with marriage (see Table 6). We evaluated both the satisfaction
with the marriage of each spouse, and the average satisfaction of the couple, as well as the degree
of consistency of the spouses about the welfare of their marriage. As can be seen from Table 6, a
pair-wise comparison of the three types of couples showed differences in all parameters of marital
satisfaction at a very high level of statistical significance. In inter-ethnic families with a common
religion, marital satisfaction was significantly higher than in other families. In inter-ethnic and—at
the same time—inter-faith couples, the level of marital satisfaction was the lowest. Furthermore, in
the Russian–Caucasian families the lowest coefficient of differences was in the ideas of the spouses
about the welfare of their marriage—meaning they had the highest level of agreement. By contrast, in
homogeneous Russian families, this indicator is the highest, which suggests a low degree of openness
in marital communication, and the fact that marital satisfaction in this group is determined not so
much by the quality of relationships, as by lower expectations in relation to family life.

Table 6. Marital satisfaction in mono-cultural, inter-ethnic and inter-faith couples.

Types of Couples Statistical Significance of


Marital Satisfaction M (SD) Differences (p)
Rates Russian–Caucasian Russian–Arab Russian–Russian
RC/RA RR/RA RC/RR
(RC) (n = 40) (RA) (n = 60) (RR) (n = 38)
Individual marital
39.92 (2.8) 24.23 (5.3) 29.52 (13.0) 0.00010 0.0056 0.00000
satisfaction of the spouses
Average marital
39.95 (2.5) 24.23 (3.0) 29.32 (9.6) 0.0000 0.0002 0.00000
satisfaction in couples
Discrepancies in the
spouses’ marital 1.74 (1.7) 7.07 (4.8) 12.95 (11.1) 0.0000 0.0004 0.00000
satisfaction inside couple

4. Discussion
Summing up the general results of the study, we can draw the following conclusions. In general,
an inter-ethnic family is a complex structure in which different values and relationships not only
compete, coexist but also transform, change the national and cultural identity and cultural values of
the spouses. Different religions of spouses create additional difficulties for establishing interaction in
a couple, these difficulties are associated not so much with life values, as with family attitudes, the
distribution of roles in the family.
In general, for inter-cultural families, normative (general cultural) values are more important than
for mono-cultural ones (including spouses belonging to a common ethnos and religion). The greater the
cultural distance of the spouses in a couple, the more the spouses are committed to normative values.
The family values of inter-ethnic couples with a common religion, compared to inter-faith
couples, are more traditional in terms of the gender distribution of roles. Compared to mono-cultural
families, both types of inter-cultural ones are more focused on external factors (for example, the
appearance of partners). The degree of role adequacy (i.e., the conformity of role expectations to a
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 11 of 14

partner and his willingness to perform certain roles) do not significantly differ in inter-cultural and
mono-cultural families.
In inter-cultural families, the degree of coincidence of the life values of spouses is much less
pronounced than in mono-cultural ones. But in couples practicing a common religion, differences in
life values reflect not so much the contradictions in the values of spouses, as the complementarity of
the values of men and women.
In family values and role attitudes of spouses from inter-cultural marriages, there are also
significantly more differences than in mono-cultural couples. On the one hand, these discrepancies
are also associated with adherence to traditional values, on the other hand, in the family attitudes
of spouses from inter-cultural families, the factor of one of the spouses’ living in foreign culture
environment plays an important role.

5. Conclusions
The study was aimed at analyzing the specifics of the hierarchy and the degree of life and family
values consistency in inter-cultural couples of two different types: inter-ethnic and inter-faith. We also
pursued the aim of assessing differences in the degree of general marital satisfaction of the spouses in
different types of inter-cultural couples. The need for such a study is dictated by the widespread ideas
that religious differences of spouses are the main obstacle to inter-cultural marriage, as well as the
main risk factor for the relationships quality in such a marriage, and, at the same time, by insufficient
empirical evidence of these ideas. A comparative analysis of two groups of inter-cultural couples was
conducted. As an example of inter-faith couples, couples consisting of Russian Orthodox Christian
women and Arab Muslim men were taken. As an example of inter-ethnic couples with a common
religion, couples consisting of Russian women with the same characteristics—as in the first group—and
men from the Transcaucasian region belonging to the Christian religion were chosen. Each group was
also compared with a control of mono-cultural couples, consisting of ethnically Russian Christian men
and women. In cross-cultural studies, it is almost impossible to select a sample in one culture so that it
exactly matches the group in another culture. Matching on one variable, with almost no exceptions,
leads to a mismatch on other variables [34] p. 326. Therefore, cross-cultural research should carefully
generalize its results, transferring them to some larger populations [35]. Thus, when disseminating
the results of this study, it is important to take into account that, firstly, the data were obtained in
groups with a certain combination of certain cultures, and secondly, the purpose of the study was not
to study these specific cultures, but to study some common patterns of inter-cultural marriage on their
example. That allows us to better understand the psychological mechanisms for the implementation of
cultural and personal values in family relationships; about the impacts of single cross-ethnic factor or
combined cross-ethnic and cross-faith factors to the processes of mutual adaptation (and acculturation)
in inter-cultural families, about the differences in the value adjustment between spouses from these
types of couples.
According to the results obtained, in general, for inter-cultural couples, normative values are more
important than for mono-cultural ones. The greater the cultural distance of the spouses in a couple, the
more the spouses are committed to normative values. The study, contrary to popular belief, showed
no radical differences in the life or family values of spouses in inter-cultural couples. The identified
differences did, however, reflect gender stereotypes about the distribution of family roles.
The presence of a common religion in an inter-ethnic couple contributes to greater marital
satisfaction and greater consistency in the representations of the spouses about the welfare of their
relationship. In inter-ethnic–inter-faith couples, marital satisfaction is the lowest, but the degree of
the consistency of spouses about the quality of relationships is higher than in mono-cultural families,
indicating a more open communication in these families and the involvement of spouses in relationships.
That is, the affiliation of spouses to different cultures (both ethnic and religious) optimizes the process
of comparing values and family attitudes. Despite a number of difficulties, spouses from inter-cultural
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 12 of 14

families, in general, have more consistent ideas about their family life, that is usually connected with
more secure attachment in a couple.
The research data can be used by psychologists and social services to help cross-cultural families in
realizing their potential more fully and overcoming the limitations identified in the study. In particular,
more open communication and greater consistency in their family life representations of both types of
cross-cultural families can be viewed as a resource to support a foreign spouse in enhancing his or
her contacts with the social environment and in better implementation of his or her life values into
everyday behavior. The fact that the spouses with a greater cultural distance combine their discrepancy
in life and family values with high marriage satisfaction makes it possible to use cultural differences in
a couple as a resource for constructively distributing family roles and optimizing family and personal
functioning as a whole.
The study has a number of limitations associated with the prospects for further research on
this problem.
Firstly, the sample of the study was not so big; however, this is partially offset by the fact that
the three subgroups have been carefully selected and equalized to each other according to the main
socio-demographic characteristics due to their selection from a large number of respondents.
Due to the limited volume, the discussion of the results is limited. A more detailed comparison of
the data of this study with other studies of inter-cultural couples is required. Furthermore, since—as
mentioned above—the parameter of the religion of spouses is rarely taken into account in modern
cross-cultural studies of couple relationships; therefore, additional, similar studies are required
involving samples with other combinations of ethnic and religious characteristics.
In this study, we compared three groups in which women had similar characteristics, and men’s
ethnic and religious characteristics varied. To better understand the role of the gender factor, it
is advisable to conduct a similar study, which will compare the marriages of men with the same
characteristics and women with varying ones.
The disadvantage of this study is that the groups were not aligned according to their place of
residence (native or foreign environment). In one of the inter-cultural subsamples (Russians–Arabs),
the men were emigrants, in another (Russians–Caucasians), the women were emigrants. We were
forced to do this to strengthen the distinguishing ethnic and religious characteristics. But some results
obtained can be explained precisely by the factor of living in a foreign environment. Accordingly, more
research is needed to assess the role of this factor.
Factors of spouses’ ethnic identity, the level of their religiosity and religious involvement, the
degree of social adaptation in society also deserve special attention. Our study showed that all these
factors can moderate the process of harmonization of value structure in a couple, and the interaction
of the spouses’ value unity with their marital satisfaction. It would also be useful to investigate the
attitudes and relations of cross-cultural spouses at different stages of the family’s life cycle.

Author Contributions: Conceptualization, E.Y.C. and M.I.V.; Methodology, E.Y.C..; Investigation, E.Y.C. and
M.I.V.; Writing-Original Draft Preparation, E.Y.C. and M.I.V.; Writing-Review & Editing, E.Y.C. and M.I.V. All
authors have read and agree to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Acknowledgments: We would like to thank Jaber, H.M.A. and Nikolaeva, O. for their assistance in conducting
the surveys.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no competing interests.

References
1. Heikkilä, E.; Rauhut, D. Marriage Migration and Multicultual Relationships; Institute of Migration: Turku,
Finland, 2015.
2. Mahoney, A. Religion in families, 1999–2009: A relational spirituality framework. J. Marriage Fam. 2010, 72,
805–827. [CrossRef]
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 13 of 14

3. McClendon, D. Religion, Marriage Markets, and Assortative Mating in the United States. J. Marriage Fam.
2016, 78, 1399–1421. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4. Chebotareva, E. Psychological factors of ethnic extremism in Muslim youth. Mediterr. J. Soc. Sci. 2014, 5,
140–148. [CrossRef]
5. Chebotareva, E. Cultural Specifics of Life Values and Subjective Well-Being. Mediterr. J. Soc. Sci. 2015, 6,
301–307. [CrossRef]
6. Cooney, T.; Radina, M. Adjustment problems in adolescence: Are multiracial children at risk? Am. J.
Orthopsychiatry 2000, 70, 433–444. [CrossRef]
7. Killian, K. Dominant and marginalized discourses in inter-racial couples’ narratives: Implications for family
therapists. Fam. Process 2002, 41, 603–618. [CrossRef]
8. Heaton, T.B. Factors contributing to increasing marital stability in the United States. J. Fam. Issues 2002, 23,
392–409. [CrossRef]
9. Myers, S.M. Religious homogamy and marital quality: Historical and generational patterns, 1980–1997. J.
Marriage Fam. 2006, 68, 292–304. [CrossRef]
10. Gaines, S.; Brennan, K. Establishing and maintaining satisfaction in multicultural relationships. In Close
Romantic Relationships: Maintenance and Enhancement; Harvey, J., Wenze, A., Eds.; Psychology Press: London,
UK, 2001; pp. 237–253.
11. Troy, A.; Lewis-Smith, J.; Laurenceau, J. Interracial and intraracial romantic relationships: The search for
differences in satisfaction, conflict, and attachment style. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2006, 23, 65–80. [CrossRef]
12. Model, S.; Fisher, G. Unions between Blacks and Whites: England and the US compared. Ethn. Racial Stud.
2002, 25, 728–754. [CrossRef]
13. Ortega, S.T.; Whitt, H.P.; William, J.A. Religious homogamy and marital happiness. J. Fam. Issues 1988, 9,
224–239. [CrossRef]
14. Sokolovskii, S.V. Podkhody k izucheniyu ehtnicheskoi identifikatsii (Approaches to the study of ethnic
identification). SoTSIS 2003, 3, 23–28.
15. Zhenchao, Q.; Sampson, L.B.; Stacey, D.R. Asian American interracial and interethnic marriages: Differences
by education and nativity. Int. Migr. Rev. 2001, 35, 557–586. [CrossRef]
16. Wood, J. Relational Communication: Continuity and Change in Personal Relationships, 2nd ed.; Belmont:
Wadsworth, OH, USA, 2000.
17. Fischer, R.; Schwartz, S. Whence differences in value priorities? Individual, cultural, or artifactual sources. J.
Cross Cult. Psychol. 2011, 42, 1127–1144. [CrossRef]
18. Schwartz, S.H. Are there universal aspects in the content and structure of values? J. Soc. Issues 1994, 50, 21.
[CrossRef]
19. Schwartz, S.H. Cultural Value Orientations: Nature & Implications of National Differences. Psychol. J. High.
Sch. Econ. 2008, 5, 36–67.
20. Schwartz, S.H.; Sagie, G. Value consensus and importance: A cross-national study. J. Cross Cult. Psychol.
2000, 31, 465–497. [CrossRef]
21. Tillich, P. Theology of Culture; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1959.
22. Abi-Hashem, N. Religion and spirituality. In The Encyclopedia of Cross-Cultural Psychology; Keith, K.D., Ed.;
Willey Blackwell: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013; pp. 1091–1093.
23. Gorokhov, S.A.; Khristov, T.T. Religiya i Etnos (Religion and Ethnos) Religii Narodov Mira; “Izdatel’stvo”
Prospekt. S: Moscow, Russia, 2013; pp. 38–45.
24. Chumakova, D.M. Vzaimosvyaz’ Religioznosti Lichnosti i Sotsial’nogo Vzaimodeistviya v Sem’e (The Relationship
of the Person’s Religiosity and Social Interaction in the Family); Yaroslavskii gos. un-t: Yaroslavl, Russia, 2014.
25. Guaita, D. Zhizn’ Cheloveka: Vstrecha Neba i Zemli (Besedy s Katolikosom Vsekh Armyan Gareginom Pervym)
(Human life: A Meeting of Heaven and Earth (Conversations with the Catholicos of All Armenians Garegin I) [transl.
from Ital. M. Sveshnik]; FAM: Moscow, Russia, 1999.
26. Fedosova, E.V. Konfessional’naya identichnost’ i religioznye ustanovki molodezhi Severnoi Osetii: Vzglyad
sotsiolog. (Confessional identity and religious attitudes of the youth of North Ossetia: A view of a sociologist).
Izv. SOIGSI 2013, 9, 86–91.
27. Rtskhiladze, G. Religious factor and conflict potential in Georgia. Cent. Asia Cauc. 2005, 3, 39.
Behav. Sci. 2020, 10, 38 14 of 14

28. Schwartz, S.H. Universals in the content and structure of values: Theory and empirical tests in 20 countries.
In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Zanna, M., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1992;
Volume 25, pp. 1–65.
29. Karandashev, V.N. Metodika Shvartsa dlya izucheniya tsennostei lichnosti. (Schwartz’ Methodology for the Study of
Personality Values); Rech: St. Petersburg, Russia, 2004.
30. Volkova, A.N.; Trapeznikova, T.M. Metodicheskie priemy diagnostiki supruzheskih otnoshenij.
(Methodological ways for the diagnosis of marital relationship). Vopr. Psihol. 1985, 5, 110–116.
31. Stolin, V.V.; Romanova, T.L.; Butenko, G.P. Test-oprosnik udovletvorennosti brakom (Marital Satisfaction
Questionnaire). In Praktikum Po Psihodiagnostiku. Psihodiagnosticheskie Materialy. (Psychodiagnostic Workshop.
Psychodiagnostic Materials); MGU: Kottayam, India, 1988; pp. 114–118.
32. Schwartz, S.H.; Butenko, T. Values and behavior: Validating the refined value theory in Russia. Eur. J. Soc.
Psychol. 2014, 44, 799–813. [CrossRef]
33. Chebotareva, E.Y.; Jaber, H.M.A. Life Values of Persons in Cross-Cultural (Arab-Russian) and Monocultural
Marriages and Their Representation in Family Sphere. RUDN J. Psychol. Pedagog. 2017, 14, 311–325.
[CrossRef]
34. Lonner, W.J.; Berry, J.W. Field Methods in Cross-Cultural Research; Sage Publications, Inc.: Southend Oaks, CA,
USA, 1986.
35. Berry, P.; Poortinga, Y.; Segal, M.H.; Dasen, P.R. Cross Cultural Psychology. Research and Applications; Cambrige
University Press: Cambrige, UK, 2002.

© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

You might also like