Skydiving and The Metaphorical Edge.: J.p.hardie-Bick@appsoc - Keele.ac - Uk
Skydiving and The Metaphorical Edge.: J.p.hardie-Bick@appsoc - Keele.ac - Uk
Skydiving and The Metaphorical Edge.: J.p.hardie-Bick@appsoc - Keele.ac - Uk
Dr James Hardie-Bick
Lecturer in Sociology
School of Sociology and Criminology
Keele University
Tel: 01782 587038
Email: [email protected]
Abstract – Much of the academic work on risk in the social sciences maintains that risk
anxiety has become a pervasive feature of everyday life. Yet despite the focus on risk and
risk aversion there is increasing evidence that high-risk activities, such as skydiving and
other extreme sports, are becoming increasingly popular. This paper presents findings from
ethnographic research carried out at a parachute centre in the United Kingdom and is
primarily concerned with participants’ perceptions of fear and risk. By reflecting on a
number of dangerous situations that both novice and experienced informants had
personally experienced or directly observed whilst at the drop zone, this paper will
investigate the meanings that are imputed to risk and how these meanings relate to
skydiving related activities.
Introduction
William James (1897) argued that it is only by taking risks from one hour to the next that
it is possible to experience life. However, in recent decades this positive view of risk has
been replaced by the notion that any risk is by definition a problem (Furedi 1999),
requiring avoidance where possible or control where unavoidable (Fox 1999). Although
risk awareness has become central to the way in which human agents and modern
institutions organise the social world, the way risk has been conceptualised in the social
sciences may have over emphasised the relevance that risk has in people‟s everyday lives
(Elliott 2002). The theoretical considerations given to the current focus on risk avoidance
fails to account for the growing numbers of individuals who actively seek experiences
that could result in personal injury and the ultimate sanction of death (Vanreusel and
Renson 1983). This paper presents findings from recent research conducted at a
parachute centre in the United Kingdom and is primarily concerned with investigating
1
participants‟ perceptions of fear and risk. By describing the practical contingencies that
can and do occur this paper considers the extent to which skydivers recognise their
activities to be particularly dangerous or extreme. This paper will not attempt to draw a
distinction between rational and irrational assessments of risk; instead, adopting a similar
approach to Tulloch and Lupton (2003), I will be concerned with investigating the
meanings that are imputed to risk and how these meanings relate to skydiving related
activities.
Much of the academic work on risk maintains that risk anxiety has become a pervasive
feature of everyday life and that individuals are increasingly attacked by feelings of
anxiety, vulnerability and uncertainty in relation to the risks to which they are exposed
(Beck 1998; Giddens 1990). Risk has become a term that is used to describe negative or
undesirable consequences and it is precisely this new position that explains why
previously commonplace activities are now treated as a safety issue. As Furedi states
„hardly a week now passes‟ without the introduction of „some new risk to the individual
being reported, and another safety measure proposed‟ (1999: 1). Experts are constantly
drawing attention to what people should be fearful of and suggesting various strategies to
ensure that people are as safe as possible. There are concerns over whether mobile
phones can cause brain tumours, whether people can trust their own General Practitioners
and environmental pressure groups are continuously warning of the potential disastrous
2
More general health issues also comprise an area dominated by debates about risks.
Health promoters continuously warn against smoking, drinking alcohol, eating fatty foods
and encourage taking regular exercise. People are urged by health promotion authorities
to evaluate their risk of succumbing to disease and to change their lifestyle accordingly.
Children‟s safety is also at the centre of risk anxiety (see Scott, Jackson and Backett-
Milburn 1998) and an increasing number of schools have now adopted a „comprehensive
range of cameras, swipe cards and other security measures‟ that has unnecessarily
resulted in schools looking „more like minimum security prisons‟ than educational
institutions (Furedi 1999: 2). Concerns relating to the exposure of risk range from
banning conkers and snow ball fights in schools to „a wave of near hysteria‟ (Jackson and
Scott 1999: 88) concerning paedophiles and the resulting vigilante behaviour. Anxiety
about food, health, crime, and the environment, together with an endless variety of new
risks have now become associated with nearly every mundane human experience. As
Beck states, even the threat of terrorism should only be considered as the latest risk „in
Voluntary Risk-Taking
An examination of the general field of risk analysis concentrates on how people are seen
to be increasingly worried about risks and highly critical of the institutions that produce
them. In many of these accounts human actors are portrayed as vulnerable and anxious,
desperate to acquire the latest knowledge to avoid becoming another victim. Yet despite
the focus on risk and risk aversion in the social sciences there is increasing evidence that
high-risk activities, such as those involved in extreme sports, are becoming more popular
3
(Palmer 2002; Stranger 1999). Since high-risk activities have the potential to increase
our understanding of how individuals view and understand risk (Ferrell et al. 2002),
voluntary risk-taking deserves far „more serious attention than it has hitherto been
accorded‟ (Boyne 2003: 118). Indeed, for societies that are imagined to be obsessed by
risk, it seems surprising that „we find an almost total absence of rigorous inquiry into
Taking into account the strong emphasis placed on avoiding and detecting risk it is hardly
surprising that those who actively seek out unnecessary risks on a voluntary basis are
generally viewed as careless and irresponsible individuals who are „dangerously out of
control‟ (Ferrell 2005: 77). Although an individual will usually take effective measures to
reduce „the eventfulness – the fatefulness – of his [sic] moments‟ and manage their
personal time so that „courses of action can be managed reliably and goals progressively
and predictably realised‟ (Goffman 1969: 128), there are many activities „that are
consequential, problematic and undertaken for their own sake‟ (Goffman 1969: 136).
Goffman prefers the term „action‟ to describe activities where individuals take a practical
gamble. For Goffman, „serious action is a serious ride‟ (1969: 199) and „is to be found
On the one side are the safe and silent places, the home, the well regulated role in
business, industry and the professions; on the other are all those activities that
4
generate expression, requiring the individual to lay himself on the line and place
their moments, there are others who are clearly prepared to wager their „future estate on
Such a practical gamble becomes a routine and mundane aspect of everyday life for
environments which are dangerous to their physical well being (Hunt 1995). Criminals,
sex workers, miners, fire fighters, high steel ironworkers, body guards, social workers,
police officers, soldiers, war correspondents and prison officers are all typical examples
important point here is that these individuals cannot voluntarily decide to withdraw from
chance taking without serious consequences for their occupational status. Nevertheless
The individual is under no obligation to continue to pursue once he [sic] has started
to do so. No extraneous factors compel him to face his fate in the first place; no
extraneous ends provide expediential reasons for his continued participation. His
activity is defined as an end itself, sought out, embraced, and utterly his own. His
record during performance can be claimed as the reason for participation, hence an
5
unqualified direct expression of his true makeup and a just basis for reputation
There are a variety of „extreme‟, „adrenalin‟ or „high-risk‟ sports (Celsi 1992; Shoham,
Rose and Kahle 2000) that are purposely „sought out‟ and „embraced‟ for the experience
they provide. Activities that seek to test the limits of human endurance include a diversity
of pursuits such as skydiving, white water rafting, B.A.S.E. jumping (an acronym for
Buildings, Antennae, Spans and Earth), swimming with sharks, para-gliding and rock and
ice climbing. As Le Breton has pointed out, part of the satisfaction of engaging in such
demanding activities comes from resisting „the temptation to give up‟ (2000: 1) and
participants claim that the risk of death is an exciting challenge to be managed and
Methods
This empirical investigation explored the routine activities that take place at a parachute
centre in the United Kingdom. Three main methods were used during the research.
Participant and non-participant observation was conducted for a period of fifteen months
(March 2003 – June 2004). This strategy allowed the researcher the opportunity to be a
direct witness to relevant events, appreciate the skills and routines skydivers need to
master and to generally build rapport with skydivers at the parachute centre. In addition,
fourteen in-depth semi-structured interviews were conducted varying in length from one
to three hours. The participants ranged from 20 to 53 years of age and five of the
6
interviewees were women. This sample reflects the high proportion of male skydivers at
the drop zone. Data from my fieldwork was therefore supported by a series of interviews
and the data from both my fieldwork and interviews were each used to „illuminate the
other‟ (Hammersley and Atkinson 1995: 131). This strategy complimented my active
membership role (Alder and Alder 1987) and enabled me to „tease out participants
(Monaghan 2002: 697). The interviewees were specifically chosen to represent the
fieldwork.
films and skydiving videos and DVDs. These secondary sources provided „checks‟
(Neuman 1994) on the data provided by interviewees together with my own observations
that I am next. I can‟t seem to look out of the window as the plane makes a sharp
right turn. I‟m feeling faint. I look out of the plane and contemplate climbing out
I have turned very pale. The instructor looks at me and genuinely asks if I am
7
OK. I tell him I am, although I know that my response is unconvincing. „How
long have I got to prepare myself‟ I ask. „About twenty seconds‟ he shouts in my
ear. All I can do is stare at the horizon, and try not to look down. I concentrate
tell myself to be confident, and that it will be no problem, but I‟m not sure my
body will move when I need it to. I try to distract myself by looking at the
aircraft controls in front of me. I can feel the instructor checking my parachute,
pulling and pushing me back and forth. The instructor leans over me to talk to
the pilot before shouting „CUT‟. He then shouts in my ear „I want you to arch
your back for Britain OK‟. I nod. Then I hear the instructor shout those dreaded
I experienced high levels of fear during my first jump. As I informed the instructor,
jumping out of a plane was the most terrifying act that I have ever voluntarily
nervous anticipation whilst climbing to altitude. I remember feeling confident about the
safety procedures and enjoyed the views from the aircraft. Nevertheless, as soon as the
first student jumped I felt my heart rate dramatically increase.2 When I was instructed to
„get into position‟ I did not know if I was going to faint or if my body would move. I
feared that I might suffer from fear paralysis (Fenz 1974) and be forced to come back
down in the plane (see Haward, 1969). During my research I learned that this happened
8
more than I imagined. One instructor commented on how he „felt sorry‟ for one student
who had refused to jump three times over three consecutive weekends. Even though this
student was determined to overcome his fear of jumping, after his third failed attempt he
decided to call it a day. Despite high levels of fear, the majority of novice students do
intention of repeating the experience. I talked to one student who I trained with after he
jumped. I asked him how it went: „It was alright, but I wouldn‟t do it again. It‟s so
dangerous, I didn‟t feel in control at all.‟ He clearly didn‟t want to talk about it and left
Although Tim also felt „extremely nervous‟ before his first jump, he had a positive
Being in the plane was rather like being in an old beaten up transit van, it really
was, there was dodgy bits of carpet on the floor, you know, and it was
reach the height at which we jump. I was up like a pole cat you know, on my
knees, and the pilot, who you know, was just a young scruffy guy who doesn‟t
look like a pilot at all, it‟s like he‟s driving a tractor you know, he turned around
and he gave me this kind of friendly wink and smile, and you know, I can read
these kind of things, they know that you‟re in a situation, and you probably look
9
very fearful, you know you‟re beaming delirious and your eyes are like pins or
whatever, you know, they can probably register that you‟re shitting yourself.
I got really nervous just before the first person was about to jump. The instructor
shouted cut and I can remember my stomach just going weeeeerah, I just hated
it, my stomach just went bluurgh. I mean the fear is big at that point. And then
you hear the instructor say go, and then there‟s this big flapping of their chute
opening up. When it was my turn, and the instructor said cut, my fear levels
went even higher, much higher, I mean they just shoot right up and that‟s
probably the worst point and that‟s probably when all my levels of fear go as
high as they can. When I hear the word cut, that‟s my ultimate fear which I have
All respondents experienced a considerable amount of fear on their first few jumps. Some
interviewees claimed that their second jump was even worse as they were „more aware of
what they were doing‟. Experienced skydivers also talked about their first few jumps
being „horrendous‟. Karl admitted to praying for the weather to dramatically change so
that he didn‟t have to jump. Anna declared that she was extremely fearful on her first
jump and remembered thinking that she „could be nothing‟ and Adam stated that on his
10
It is hardly surprising that novice parachutists experience high levels of fear on their first
few jumps. During the RAPS training course the instructors focused on various factors
that can realistically go wrong.3 Students learned how to deal successfully with a
malfunctioned canopy, what to do in an aircraft emergency and how to avoid hazards that
houses, factories and even planes and micro-lights. Before my first jump I remember
being particularly concerned about falling off the step and being suspended underneath
the plane. During the training the instructors had provided dramatic descriptions of a
„static line hang up‟4 and their descriptions had left a vivid image in my mind. The
prospect of injury and death has not been „stripped away‟ from this activity (see Palmer
2002). Emily was extremely aware of the various factors that could go wrong when a
When you actually put it all together I think a lot of things can actually go
wrong. Like your lines might not untangle and when you actually realise all the
things that can go wrong it‟s actually pretty amazing that things go right because
you could end up with a line that doesn‟t really hold your canopy right and so on
and your static line might get tangled up before you even go out or it hasn‟t been
packed properly, or even more basic things, you might not even have the harness
on properly maybe something gets caught up in what you‟re wearing, your arm
or leg gets caught up in the lines or you hit the plane as you jump, I don‟t know
if that is possible but it certainly seems like it should be possible. And where
11
you land, all kinds of things can go wrong there, gusts of wind. I mean the plane
might not even work, and it might crash [Laughs] so all sorts of things can
actually go wrong.
Although the instructors always emphasised that accidents were extremely rare, accidents
do occur.5 During my field work a student was taken to hospital with a broken leg after
flaring his parachute too early and I saw another student after her first jump with blood
over her face and jump suit. I learned that she had hit her face on the step when exiting
the aircraft. The most serious accident occurred to a student on her first jump who was
second jump I narrowly avoided landing on a barbwire fence, and on my sixth jump I
nearly landed on the roof of the reception. I also developed colourful bruises after being
hit by the static line on two occasions. Steve described his first „near miss‟:
I think it was on my third jump that I nearly crashed into a factory [laughs]. I
was coming into land, and I‟d just got the whole trying to control where I was
landing completely wrong. So I ended up heading over the cabin where the
reception is on the left hand side towards the car park and realised I was going to
land on a car, so I sort of hooked it left and then found myself facing the factory
with a blue roof…And I thought shit I‟m going to hit it. I landed about a metre
short of it. And I just thought damn I‟m going to break my leg because there
were a couple of big rocks in the grass in front of it, and I was more concerned
about hitting them. But actually I just landed short. The canopy went over the
12
roof of the factory, but I was completely uninjured [laughs]. I think at the time I
Another dangerous incident occurred to Karl‟s wife (Becky) on her first jump. Karl
admitted that he had being trying to convince her to „give it a go‟ for several months prior
to jumping. After several unsuccessful attempts, Karl decided to book her a place on the
RAPS course as a surprise birthday present. Becky did not return to make a second jump.
It was really bad. She was about 2ft from the floor and the wind took her up and
brought her back over on to her head and shoulder and she damaged the muscles
and everything in her back so badly the whole of her back was just a swollen
lump. Not a little lump in her back, you couldn‟t see her spine or her ribs or
anything and it really put her out for about 2 months. It wasn‟t even that windy.
It was a freak gust that lifted her up and brought her back down on to her head.
It lifted her about 10ft into the air. It was nasty to watch as well.
Like many interviewees, Andrew believed that one of the main factors for students not
returning to the centre after their first few jumps concerns fear:
As for the people that do a few jumps and then leave it, probably the major
factor is fear. Particularly after the first few jumps you start to know more and
more and it starts to seep in about how many things could potentially go wrong.
13
People do get very scared. But at that point you do know enough to know a lot
about what can go wrong, but you don‟t know enough about the training and the
Interviewees explained that fear was something that they initially had to overcome. Some
of the experienced skydivers were not ashamed to reminisce about the early stages of
their skydiving career when they also refused to jump. Steve was pleased to describe his
own experience of when he turned „white as a sheet‟ and „froze‟ on one of his student
jumps:
On my fourth jump I froze, I just told the jump master that I‟m not going. There
was no way I was jumping out of that plane. I had an experienced jumper behind
me. He just climbed over me and jumped out which scared me even more.
After his forth jump Karl stopped jumping for three months. Nevertheless, he was
I just thought about it, you know, just getting on the step and I thought why on
Skydivers talked about a gradual transition that occurs during the early stages of their
career. This transition involves viewing fear as a positive rather than a negative aspect of
the sport. As Adam stated, „it wouldn‟t be positive if I was still as scared as on my first
14
jump, if I had to do that every jump I think I‟d probably have quit by now‟. Experienced
respondents claimed that although fear was an important safeguard against complacency,
it was also vitally important to „embrace‟ and „live‟ with the fear. Steve provided an
You know where he pours the acid on his hands and says „embrace the fear‟.
I: Yeah.
There‟s something about embracing the fear as well, you know, living through
the fear. I know that I am going to jump. As a student I bottled out of a couple
of jumps because I didn‟t feel that I was capable of what I had to do. But now
that I know that I can do what I have to do I know that I‟m never going to not
jump, so fear in a sense doesn‟t matter. You just have to live with it. I mean if
you didn‟t have any fear at all, then what‟s the point [laughs]. You might as well
go cycling instead.
Other studies (Hargreaves 1997; Stranger 1999) have also demonstrated that voluntary
risk-taking is often pursued for the sake of facing and conquering fear and displaying
courage. The challenge is to prove to themselves and to others that they have successfully
controlled their fear and feelings of susceptibility and anxiety. The participant‟s first
challenge is to see how they will respond to the „emotional intensity‟ produced by the
„dialectic of fear and pleasure‟ (Le Breton 2000: 4). Nevertheless, there is a transition
15
that takes place that involves transforming fear into pleasure. Whereas novice
parachutists constantly referred to the craziness of jumping and concentrated on the thrill,
longer relied on such stereotypic responses. For the novice parachutist the initial
challenge involved simply jumping out of a plane. But the challenge for skydivers
becomes incorporated into the various disciplines of the sport. The vocabularies of
motive for experienced skydivers focused on the different disciplines and the endless
array of new challenges that the sport is seen to provide. Experienced skydivers also
relation to the sport (also see Branningan and McDougall 1983; Celsi et al. 1993). The
accounts are subculturally acquired (Monaghan 2002). The changing accounts that claim
a skydiver.
Perceptions of Risk
Skydiving is an activity where the „stakes are high‟ (Lipscombe 1999) and individual
decisions can have serious consequences. As Steve explained „if you do cock it up,
basically, you‟re in big shit whereas if you make a mistake playing rugby or whatever it‟s
16
not such a big deal‟. Although the general public may not have enough technical
knowledge to have an informed opinion, such incidents do confirm why skydiving should
be understood as an extreme sport. Every time a skydiver jumps they have to deal with
„extreme heights‟ and „extremely hostile conditions‟ and sometimes it can and does „bite
as „institutionalised risk environments‟ that provide individuals with the choice „to risk
scarce resources, including their lives‟ (1991:124). Nevertheless, Giddens provides little
sense of the details and complexities of these risks and the various ways in which they are
managed and controlled by participants. Although all my respondents insisted that they
were not „doing something crazy‟ they did not try to deny that there are serious dangers
Everyone thinks skydivers are dangerous and crazy. But now that I have done it
I see it quite differently. There are dangers, but I‟m not doing something crazy.
(Karl)
Most people wouldn‟t consider getting out of a perfectly good aeroplane and
they would definitely see it as being far too dangerous. There‟s obviously a
danger element involved, and I suppose I would say it‟s an extreme sport
because you know there‟s no sort of denying that but I think there are more
dangerous things to do. There‟s no reason why you shouldn‟t live every time
17
(Stewart)
As these quotes suggest, skydivers do not deny that there are risks involved with
eliminated, he also claimed that the amount of research that has gone into producing the
equipment has significantly reduced the risks – „Anyone who knows the statistics knows
that it‟s really unlikely that you‟re going to end up dead.‟ Informants believed skydiving
to be a calculated risk:
I suppose I think of it as a calculated risk. You know you get all the training,
you know the training works, you see all of the equipment, you have faith and
confidence in the equipment you‟ve got. To me dangerous is, you know, I used
to do a lot of climbing. I‟ll use that as an example. Now I always climbed with
ropes because then although it is high risk, there is a safety net there. I also
know lads that don‟t climb with any ropes at all. Now I would consider that as
dangerous, if you fall, there‟s no safety net and you are basically dead. You
know the heights that I‟m talking about you know, 100‟s of feet, 1000‟s of feet.
ropes. For Karl, climbing without ropes is „dangerous‟ and „crazy‟ because unlike
skydiving there is no safety net. If one of his friends made a mistake, in all probability
they would be killed. Skydivers always jump with two (main and reserve) canopies. If
they have a malfunction, they can activate the reserve. Also, most skydivers jump with an
18
automatic activation device (AAD) which will automatically open the reserve canopy at a
pre-determined height.7 This is in case any unforeseen events that may occur such as
being knocked unconscious during freefall. Obviously the AAD might not work and the
reverse canopy may have a malfunction, and that is precisely why skydiving is
considered to be a calculated risk. Like Karl, Stewart also had faith in his equipment and
in his own ability. Stewart described his malfunction, which was the most dangerous
I was still going at a hundred and ten miles, a hundred and twenty miles an hour
towards the ground, most people usually have something above their head [their
main canopy], it might be spinning but it‟s still slowing down the rate of
descent. Mine was like one of the fastest ones to deal with but it was then and
there, that was it sort of thing…it‟s really nice to know that in that situation you
can cope.
speed malfunction. His individual capacities were challenged and control was
maintained. Stewart explained that when you have a problem at one hundred and twenty
miles an hour, when „things don‟t go quite according to plan‟, you have to do „what
everyone is trained to do in such a situation. Diagnose the problem and deal with it.‟
For skydivers a malfunctioned main parachute „can be sorted‟ and is therefore viewed as
a controllable risk. Informants maintained that they were far more concerned about the
19
risks of something going wrong with the plane than having a malfunction. Some
skydivers literally „hated‟ being in the plane below one thousand feet, because if anything
went wrong with the plane they were too low to jump out. This was a risk that was
beyond their control. The following quote emphasises the importance of control:
You‟re in control, it‟s like you‟ve got that control, with a roller coaster ride you
do nothing, ninety-nine point nine, nine, nine percent of the time you just sit
there and enjoy it, there‟s nothing you can do to stop it, if it goes wrong there‟s
nothing you can do, it‟s basically just there, you are there for the ride whereas
parachuting you‟re not there for the ride, you‟re the pilot, if that makes sense.
(Stewart)
threatening situations that they have no control over. Whereas rollercoaster enthusiasts
constantly place their lives in the hopefully competent hands of others (Holyfield 1999),
one of the main attractions of skydiving is that the skydiver is literally the pilot as
opposed to a trusting passenger who has no control over life threatening situations.
Skydivers are not interested in „putting their life in somebody else‟s hands‟. Even at an
early stage of their skydiving career, novice students are keen to learn how to pack their
own parachutes, rather than being forced to rely on the competence of people they hardly
know. As Celsi et al. (1993) also recognised, skydivers seek controllable risk contexts
20
Dicing with Death?
According to Lyng and Snow, skydiving offers the opportunity for individuals to get as
Jumping under the influence of drugs…jumping into landing areas that are
geographically restricted, deploying one‟s canopy at a low altitude are all examples
of how the individual jumper has the opportunity to move „a little close to the
Adam informed me that the majority of skydivers do not deliberately try to increase the
risks and he knew no-one who took drugs to enhance their experience. Informants
maintained that they „want to get to the ground safely‟ so they can „go up and do it
the actual danger limits they‟re, they‟re already there and I don‟t need to push
them any further, I know they‟re there. I don‟t want to increase them.
According to Emily, skydivers are „definitely not dicing too much with death‟ because
„they‟re never doing something which is beyond their level…I mean in some ways they
could make it more extreme, but they‟re not doing that. They‟re still playing it safe‟. I
asked Karl why he wasn‟t tempted to move „a little closer to the edge‟:
21
Well frankly I want to live, I don‟t, you know, I don‟t want to die, I don‟t want
to be injured you know and I think that‟s it, you call skydiving an extreme sport
and you have to ask yourself what do you mean by extreme? I call extreme
pushing things to the limit, some people say extreme sport is something that‟s on
the edge dangerous-wise you know I don‟t necessarily see that it is dangerous, I
Taking drugs was certainly viewed as reckless and irresponsible behaviour. The majority
of my interviewees were surprised that I even had to ask. I explained that previous
research had suggested that some skydivers enjoyed the challenge of jumping „under the
influence of intoxicating substances‟ (Lyng 1998: 225) and that „It was not uncommon
for a group of skydivers to “blow a joint”‟ (Lyng 1998: 228) together as they climbed to
altitude. Rather than providing a „seductive way to “stay in the action”‟(Lyng 1998: 230),
I was informed that such behaviour would ensure no „action‟ at all. I asked Paul if he had
I‟ve not heard of anything like that, no I‟ve never heard anyone do that at all. I
mean drugs and alcohol are a big no-no in skydiving you know and if I thought
that anyone was taking drugs or drinking before skydiving then they certainly
22
According to Stewart, skydiving was far more „extreme back in like early 80‟s and 70‟s
because there were so few people so they, they did everything they could.‟ Stewart
maintained that skydiving has now become far more orientated around issues of safety.
Although he knew people at the drop zone to „smoke weed‟, jumping under the influence
It‟s basically considered like if you see anyone or if you know that anyone
smoked or had been drinking or anything it‟s like anyone, even if they like weed
smoking themselves, they will say get off the lift [plane]. They don‟t want to see
Not only would the skydiver place herself in danger, but she would also create
unnecessary risks for everyone else on the „lift‟. All experienced informants emphasised
the importance of safety and attempted to reduce rather than increase the dangers.
If you‟re careful about what you do, have a well maintained kit, there‟s no real
reason for it to be dangerous at all, in fact it can be extremely safe. It‟s when you
start taking unnecessary risks, or not looking after your kit or doing things that
are beyond your capabilities that it can be extremely dangerous. People get dead
(Andrew)
23
Although some respondents commented on how certain individuals „are just too cautious‟
by jumping with a canopy that was too large and not „pushing themselves enough‟ their
main criticism was reserved for the minority whose behaviour was, in their perception,
believed to voluntarily take „unnecessary risks‟. Although such unnecessary risks did not
involve taking mind altering chemical substances, cutting other skydivers up „in the air‟,
flying and landing in front of other skydivers, „bombing‟ formations are all extremely
There a couple of people here who I would not jump with, and if they asked me
I would have to say no thanks. It‟s the way they are in the air. If things go wrong
things go wrong but I don‟t want someone putting me into a position where
things didn‟t have to go wrong. I don‟t want anyone who can‟t control their
flight through the air and come crashing into me. I know it can happen, and
sometimes it is unavoidable, but there are a couple of people here who have got
no real control while they‟re up there and that scares me. There was a situation a
couple of weeks ago where someone decided that it would be funny to fly right
in front of somebody else, and I don‟t want anybody to do that to me. It can be
really dangerous and it‟s that kind of irresponsibility that I‟m not to keen on.
You know, Keith [Chief Central Instructor] was away and I don‟t think it was
(Karl)
24
Clearly there are both formal and informal mechanisms at work here. If the CCI directly
front of another skydiver they would be grounded and would no longer be permitted to
jump at the centre. Such behaviour was not tolerated, as the CCI informed me „we don‟t
want any unnecessary accidents here‟. But if the CCI failed to notice „irresponsible
behaviour‟, actions were generally not reported. In such situations more informal
methods were employed. Skydivers would casually inform the accused individual that
they were not prepared to jump with them. Other informants also admitted that they have
refused to jump with certain individuals. They were described as „arrogant‟ and „bloody
irresponsible‟ for „jumping with a canopy which is far too small‟, for „doing stupid things
in the air‟ and generally „being an idiot‟. Julia claimed that skydivers „didn‟t have a
problem‟ with being open and honest by saying „sorry mate, I don‟t think you‟re safe to
jump with. I think you cut people up under canopy and I don‟t trust you‟. Stewart was
convinced that such techniques had positive effects on the accused behaviour:
There‟s one guy down there, now, he‟s got a bit of a better reputation for
himself because people said, no, we‟re not jumping with you, and a lot of people
said that to him and now he‟s starting to think right, well I‟m not going to be
able to jump at this rate, I‟m going to have to start switching on. And he‟s sort
Hunt‟s research on deep sea divers also found that those individuals who took
unnecessary risks received negative sanctions and often became subject to ridicule.
25
Divers who violated the cultural constructions of normal risk were labelled as „assholes‟
and „accidents waiting to happen‟ (Hunt 1995: 452). In a similar way to Hunt‟s divers,
skydivers who were seen as irresponsible were also subjected to rumour and gossip. Both
formal and informal mechanisms served to restrict such activities at the drop zone and
Discussion
High risk sports are not for the faint hearted (Palmer 2002) and skydiving provides an
experience that goes beyond the usual everyday activities of the recreationist (Lipscombe
1999). For those outside the skydiving community the consequences of skydiving may
seem obvious: jumping out of an aeroplane at roughly thirteen thousand feet and being
overwhelmed by the experience of freefalling towards the earth at a speed of 120 miles
an hour clearly involves the possibility of serious injury and even death. Skydivers are
aware of the negative stereotypical assumptions held by those outside the skydiving
they had a death wish and how the media constantly portrays skydivers as reckless
individuals who purposely seek out unnecessary risks. Skydivers believed that the
addicts‟ or that skydivers‟ had to be „mad dare devils and completely nuts‟ were totally
26
The stereotype is that we‟re just crazy, on the edge, that there‟s no rationale to
our behaviour. Apparently we‟re all adrenalin junkies who are just a bit weird.
Some of the really negative stereotypes say that skydivers are people who are
not happy with their lives, that we‟re bored, you know, that we‟re not satisfied
with normal things and so on. And I don‟t think that‟s true at all.
(Paul)
Whilst individuals who seem to purposefully threaten their own personal safety are often
„regarded as the height of irresponsibility‟ (Reith 2005: 230) and generally thought to be
their own management of risk. Rather than risk being a problem to be avoided, skydivers
confidently embraced risk and believed it to be a positive rather than a negative aspect of
the sport. Without taking risks, life would be intolerably restricted, unable to offer
I think it [skydiving] is a way of pushing yourself and seeing what you are
capable of whereas taking up something like golf really wouldn‟t be that much
of a challenge. I think it‟s important to have challenges. Unless you have seen
how far you can go, how far you can push yourself, where your own limits are,
from a personal point of view I don‟t think you‟ve explored your life that well.
(Andrew)
27
Analysing my interview data I became increasingly aware how skydivers viewed the risk
skydivers in this research did not intentionally increase the risks of jumping. Skydivers
believed that the risks of skydiving allowed individuals the chance to challenge
themselves and to explore and negotiate their own personal boundaries, but they did not
attempt to increase the risks that were already there. Skydivers could have made their
sport more extreme. For example, skydivers could jump without an AAD, bomb
formations, fly in front of other skydivers, take legal and/or illegal drugs whilst jumping,
refuse to carry out safety checks, jump with canopies that were too small, insist on
jumping in bad weather conditions, deliberately open their canopy at a low altitude or
jump without a reserve parachute. Far from what I had expected before entering the field,
the majority of skydivers actively sought to reduce, manage and control the risks
associated with their sport. Despite their level of skills and ability, experienced skydivers
still recognised that there were some circumstances beyond their control. Only those
described as „cowboys‟ took what informants believed to be „unnecessary risks‟ and both
formal and informal sanctions served to discourage „irresponsible‟ and „extremely stupid‟
acts.
As I have demonstrated, notions of control are vitally important for skydivers. All
believed they were capable of managing the context in which they perform, with most
28
personal capabilities. Not only do such findings shed some „critical light on the strong
present-day focus on risk avoidance‟ (Ferrell, Milovanovis and Lyng, 2002: 180), but
they also highlight how risk epistemologies are bound to specific social cultural contexts
in which they are generated (Lupton 1999b). Rather than being viewed as an objective or
static phenomena that can be determined by observation, this research has demonstrated
how risks are routinely constructed. Individuals outside the skydiving community may
perceive skydiving to be a selfish and irresponsible pursuit, whilst the moral career of
skydivers clearly involves the normalisation of such risks and redefines them as
acceptable and even pleasurable. Another turning point in the skydiver‟s moral career
involves recognising the difference between what they consider to be responsible and
irresponsible risks. The latter are subjected to both formal and informal sanctions and
Douglas (1992) notes, rather than being viewed as the products of individual knowledge,
perceptions of risk are shared and serve to maintain symbolic boundaries within a
particular community.
Conclusion
Becoming a skydiver offers the individual far more than the passive, fabricated and pre-
packaged sameness of the amusement park (Ferrell 2001; Holyfield, Jonas and Zajicek
2005). Skydiving occurs outside the realm of ordinary experience and provides the
context for a „radical one-to-one contest‟ through which participants can continuously
„test their strength of character, their courage and their personal resources‟ (Le Breton
2000: 1). So against the dominant discourses on risk that portray it as negative there also
29
exists a counter discourse in which risk-taking is represented far more positively (Lupton
technique for enhancing the overall experience that life has to offer. The following quotes
To me risk is part of living, if there was no risk in it, you can‟t wrap yourself up
in cotton wool and sit indoors or you wouldn‟t be living, you‟ve got to go out
(Julia)
I think that we are way too risk averse. You know the way that children seem to
yeah, I think it‟s a terrible thing. People live very sheltered lives. It seems to me
risk takers, it‟s something that we should do, and to try to eliminate that is
terrible I think. And I think that maybe the reaction to that which we‟re seeing is
that people are seeking out their risks elsewhere because they‟re being
eliminated from them in their everyday lives. One reason why people could be
(Steve)
So perhaps the imagined obsession with risks does not penetrate so deeply after all
(Boyne 2003). As this paper has demonstrated, rather than the notion of risk being a
problem (Furedi 1999), „acceptable‟ risks were understood to be a positive feature of the
30
activity and provided skydivers with a heightened sense of personal agency. As novice
participants gradually become competent skydivers, they learn and develop skills that are
of everyday life (O‟Malley and Mugford 1994). As Goffman (1969) recognised, „serious
action‟ can be seen as compensating individuals for the absence of direct personal control
and autonomy they experience in their everyday lives. Indeed, the experiences that have
been pushed to the fringes in modern times are precisely the experiences that skydivers
value and purposely bring back into their lives (see Simmel 1997). If, as Smith has
stated, a defining feature of the risk society may imply that voluntary risk-taking is
„happening around us in our everyday lives to a much greater extent than we realise‟
(2005: 199), then future sociological research should also cast a critical eye on activities
not previously associated with the metaphorical edge. Indeed, the increasing popularity of
such behaviours in a diversity of social contexts may „mirror the climate of late-
modernity itself‟ (Reith 2005: 241). Future research will need to determine whether the
skills and abilities required for participating in a high-risk sport such as skydiving are
Notes
1
All static line students jumped from a Cessna 182. As soon as the student parachutist exits the aircraft the
static line opens the parachute container and pulls the bag and lines out into the airflow. Before jumping,
static line students have to climb out on to a short step whilst holding on to the wing strut. Novice
informants were particularly anxious about this procedure. As one student said „I can‟t believe we‟ve got to
do that, I thought I could just shut my eyes and jump.‟
2
Research has demonstrated that parachuting significantly increases physiological activity. For research
relating to physiological changes during a parachute jump see Anfilogoff, Hale, Nattness, Hammond and
Carter, 1987.
3
The Ram Air Progression System Static Line course is a progression system that takes the complete
novice through various progressive stages until they become a licensed skydiver. The first few jumps
involve a static line to ensure that their parachute automatically opens. The static line is securely attached
31
to a „strongpoint‟ in the aircraft and is checked by both the student and instructor before take off.
Undoubtedly the most popular course among student parachutists, it was also the chosen route for most of
my interviewees. The other route available to the complete novice is the Accelerated Free Fall course. Both
the accelerated free fall and the ram air progression system allow the student to reach BPA category 8 and
therefore become a licensed skydiver. The AFF course is considered to be the „fast track‟ approach to
becoming a skydiver. It is literally an accelerated teaching method which relies on a high instructor student
radio. The only disadvantage with the AFF concerns the cost. Consequently, most parachute students
enrolled on the RAPS course.
4
Although the least common type of parachuting emergency, a static-line hang-up results in a student
hanging underneath the aircraft after exit. This can happen if the student has a fouled static line which has
passed through some of the student‟s equipment. If this situation does occur, students are taught to place
their hands on their head. This indicates to the instructor that they are conscious and aware of this particular
emergency procedure. The instructor will show a knife to the student before cutting the static line. As soon
as the static line is cut, the student must go through the emergency drills to operate the reserve parachute.
5
Research conducted in the United Kingdom claims that the likelihood of death is 1 in 55,000 jumps
(Davison 1990), although a Danish study suggests 3 in 55,000 (Ellitsgaard 1987). The most recent statistics
from the British Parachute Association cover a period of fourteen years (1990-2004). Out of a total of
3,042,114 jumps, there were 37 fatalities. 12 fatalities occurred on student jumps and 5 students were killed
on their first jump. Novice injury rates of 0.43 – 1.1% are much higher than those of experienced jumpers
which range from 0.05 – 0.13%. A staggering 84-87% of injuries occur on landing (Amamilo et al. 1987;
Davison 1990; Ellitsgaard, 1987; Straiton et al. 1986). As one of my informants suggested, once the canopy
has successfully opened the most dangerous part of the skydive has just begun.
6
According to her instructor she had actually created the malfunction herself by exiting the aircraft
incorrectly. Not only did she create a malfunction, but she also „failed to act correctly‟ by not activating the
reserve parachute. The instructor informed me that the student simply „froze in the air‟ and maintained that
the student „had a real panic attack‟. In the words of her instructor: „Had she cut away and employed her
reserve she would have been fine but she didn‟t, she didn‟t do anything to save her life. When she hit the
ground we assumed that she was dead‟.
7
The AAD is a mechanical or electronic back up system that compares ambient air pressure with the
skydivers decent rate. The AAD will initiate the reserve deployment at a pre set height and therefore serves
as a „back up‟ rather than a „substitute for a good manual reserve drill‟ (Donaldson 2000: 53). Even during
the initial RAPS training course, students are informed that the AAD should not be relied upon. Like any
other piece of machinery it can go wrong and should therefore be viewed as a life saving back up device.
References
Alder, P.A. and Alder, P. (1987) Membership Roles in Field Research. London: Sage.
Prospective Study of Parachute Injuries in Civilians‟, The Journal of Bone and Joint
32
Anfilogoff, R., Hale, P.J., Nattress, M., Hammond, V.A. and Carter, J.C. (1987)
Beck, U. (2004) „Ulrich Beck: The Cosmopolitan Turn‟, in Gane, N. The Future of Social
Celsi, R. L., Rose, R. L., and Leigh, T.W. (1993) „An Exploration of High-Risk Leisure
33
Donaldson, C. (2000) Skydive: Sport Parachuting Explained. Trowbridge: The Crowood
Press.
Douglas, M. (1992) Risk and Blame: Essays in Cultural Theory. London: Routledge.
293-315.
Ferrell, J. (2001) Tearing Down The Streets: Adventures In Urban Anarchy. New York:
Palgrave.
Ferrell, J. (2005) „The Only Possible Adventure: Edgework and Anarchy‟, in Lyng, S.
Ferrell, J., Milovanovis, D. and Lyng, S. (2002) „Edgework, Media Practices, and the
34
Fox, N. (1999) „Postmodern Reflections on “Risk”, “Hazards” and Life Choices‟, in D.
Lupton. (ed.) Risk and Social Cultural Theory: New Directions and Perspectives.
Furedi, F. (1999) Culture of Fear: Risk-Taking and the Morality of Low Expectation.
London: Cassell.
Routledge.
Hargreaves, J. (1997) „Women‟s Boxing and Related Activities: Introducing Images and
35
Hayward, K. (2002) „The Vilification and Pleasures of Youthful Transgression‟, in
Muncie, J., Hughes, G. and McLaughlin, E. (eds.) Youth Justice: Critical Readings.
Holyfield, L., Jonas, L. and Zajicek, A. (2005) „Adventure Without Risk Is Like
Routledge.
Hunt, J. C. (1995) „Divers Accounts of Normal Risk‟, Symbolic Interaction 18 (4): 439-
62.
Jackson, S. and Scott, S. (1999) „Risk Anxiety and the Social Construction of
Childhood‟, in Lupton, D. (ed.) Risk and Sociocultural Theory: New Directions and
James, W. (1897) The Will to Believe. London : Longmans, Green & Co.
Le Breton, D. (2000) „Playing Symbolically with Death in Extreme Sports‟, Body and
36
Lipscombe, N. (1998) „Trainee Skydiving: The Value of Research to Safety‟, The
267-288.
Risk and Social Cultural Theory: New Directions and Perspectives. Cambridge:
Lyng, S.H. (1998) „Dangerous Methods: Risk-Taking and the Research Process‟, in
Ferrell, J. and Hamm, M. S. (eds.) Ethnography at the Edge: Crime, Deviance and Field
Lyng, S.H. and Snow, D.A. (1986) „Vocabularies of Motive and High-Risk Behaviour:
37
Monaghan, L.F. (2002) „Vocabularies of Motive for Illicit Steroid Use among
Palmer, C. (2002) „“Shit Happens”: The Selling of Risk in Extreme Sport‟, The
Reith, G. (2005) „On the Edge: Drugs and the Consumption of Risk in Late Modernity‟,
Scott, S., Jackson, S. and Backett-Milburn, K. (1998) „Swings and Roundabouts: Risk
Simmel, G. (1997 [1950]) „The Metropolis and Mental Life‟, in Frisby, D. and
38
Smith C. W. (2005) „Financial Edgework: Trading in Market Currents‟, in Lyng, S. (ed.)
Straiton, N. and Sterland, J. (1986) „Sponsored Parachute Jumps – Can they cause
Tulloch, J. and Lupton, D. (2003) Risk and Everyday Life. London: Sage.
Vanreusel, B. and Renson, R. (1982) „The Social Stigma of High Risk Sport
Subcultures‟, in Dunleavy, A., Miracle, A., and Rees, R. (eds.) Studies in the Sociology of
39