Speed of Information Processing: Developmental Change and Links To Intelligence
Speed of Information Processing: Developmental Change and Links To Intelligence
Speed of Information Processing: Developmental Change and Links To Intelligence
51–61, 2000
Copyright 2000 Society for the Study of School Psychology
Pergamon Printed in the USA
0022-4405/00 $–see front matter
PII S0022-4405(99)00036-9
For most of the history of psychology, the study of intelligence has been
closely associated with the psychometric tradition. In this approach, pat-
terns of performance on mental tests are used to identify the structure of
human intelligence. In 1904, for example, Charles Spearman reported
findings supporting the idea that a general factor for intelligence is respon-
sible for performance on all mental tests. Other psychometricians, how-
ever, argued that intelligence consists of distinct abilities. For example,
Thurstone and Thurstone (1941) used adults’ performance on a wide
range of mental tests to identify seven distinct elements of intelligence: per-
ceptual speed, word comprehension, word fluency, space, number, mem-
ory, and induction. Thurstone and Thurstone acknowledged the presence
of a general factor in intelligence, but claimed that the specific factors were
more useful in assessing and understanding intellectual ability.
These conflicting findings led many psychometric theorists to propose
hierarchical theories of intelligence that included both general and spe-
cific components. Carroll (1993), for example, proposed a hierarchical the-
ory with three levels. At the top of the hierarchy is general intelligence. The
next level includes eight broad categories of intellectual skill, ranging from
fluid intelligence to processing speed. Each of the abilities in the second
Received April 19, 1999; accepted October 6, 1999.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Robert Kail, Department of Psychology,
University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742-4411. Phone: (301) 405-8481; E-mail: rkail@
psyc.umd.edu
51
52 Journal of School Psychology
level is further divided into more specific skills in the third level. Fluid intel-
ligence, for example, includes sequential reasoning, induction, and quanti-
tative reasoning.
In the past 25 years, critics have argued that theories of intelligence de-
rived from the psychometric perspective are too narrow; theorists have pro-
posed a host of other perspectives that purport to provide a broader view
of intelligence (Gardner, 1983, 1993; Kail & Pellegrino, 1985; Sternberg,
1985). Although these new perspectives differ in many of their details, they
share a common emphasis on drawing upon other literatures in psychol-
ogy—cognitive, developmental, and neuropsychological—in their propos-
als for new perspectives on intelligence.
The research described in this article is rooted in these new perspectives
on intelligence. Specifically, research in cognitive psychology, develop-
mental psychology, and neuropsychology has revealed that speed of infor-
mation processing is a key element in people’s ability to think, reason, and
remember. In this article, I first provide an overview of research showing
how processing speed develops. This work is consistent with the hypothesis
that age differences in processing speed reflect developmental change in
a global mechanism that limits processing speed on most tasks. Next, I ex-
amine research that links speed of information processing to intelligence.
Figure 1. Each panel depicts children’s RTs as a function of adults’ RTs in corresponding conditions. Also shown is the linear equation corresponding
to Equation 3. From Kail (1991). Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association. Reprinted with permission.
55
56 Journal of School Psychology
In the next part of this article, I describe research that links this develop-
mental change in processing speed to intelligence.
CONCLUSIONS
Processing speed is an element of intelligence. Why is this conclusion pro-
vocative or newsworthy? After all, Thurstone and Thurstone (1941) made
the same claim nearly 60 years ago, and processing speed has typically been
an element of multifactor theories of intelligence ever since. The key differ-
ence is that, in contrast to psychometric research, modern research por-
trays a dynamic assessment of processing speed. That is, processing speed
is not simply one of many different independent factors that contribute to
intelligence; instead, processing speed is thought to be linked causally to
other elements of intelligence. As children develop, they process informa-
tion more rapidly, apparently reflecting age-related change in the central
nervous system. This more rapid processing results in more effective use of
working memory, which enhances performance on many reasoning tasks
like those used to assess intelligence. Thus, in contrast to a prototypic
multifactor theory of intelligence (e.g., Carroll, 1993) that might include
processing speed, memory, and inductive reasoning as separate and inde-
pendent factors, the current view links these constructs. More rapid pro-
cessing enhances memory, which, in turn, enhances reasoning.
In addition, in the dynamic view, processing speed can influence perfor-
mance on intelligence tests both directly and indirectly. An indirect effect
is illustrated by the impact of processing speed on memory. By allowing
working memory to be used more efficiently, increased processing speed
enhances performance on intelligence tests indirectly. However, process-
ing speed may also affect performance directly by, for example, speeding
retrieval of task-relevant information from long-term memory.
This dynamic view of different elements of intelligence also has implica-
tions for the measurement of intelligence and interpretation of test scores.
60 Journal of School Psychology
Obviously, tests like the Stanford-Binet that only assess general intelligence
are seen as much less useful than those that assess different elements of in-
telligence. However, in a typical test that assesses multiple skills, the results
are cast as a profile of an individual’s independent strengths and weak-
nesses. The dynamic view reminds us that many of these skills may be inter-
related. In the psychometric tradition, a child with below average scores on
processing speed, memory, and reasoning is subpar in three areas. In the
dynamic view, the child may well be limited primarily in one area (pro-
cessing speed); the other subpar performances are largely byproducts of
deficits in processing speed. Consequently, if this child’s processing speed
were improved, memory and reasoning would be expected to follow suit.
In conclusion, more rapid processing speed is an important aspect of
greater intellectual power. As additional research continues to clarify the
nature of links between intelligence and processing speed, theories of intel-
ligence will be enhanced and test information more valuable.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The research described in this article was supported by grants from the Na-
tional Institute of Child Health and Human Development and the National
Science Foundation.
REFERENCES
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley scales of infant development: Birth to two years (2nd ed.). San Anto-
nio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Bayley, N. (1970). Development of mental abilities. In P. H. Mussen, (Ed.), Carmichael’s
manual of child psychology (pp. 1136–1209). New York: Wiley.
Bornstein, M. H. (1998). Stability in mental development from early life: Methods, mea-
sures, models, meanings, and myths. In G. E. Butterworth & F. Simion (Eds.), The de-
velopment of sensory, motor, and cognitive capacities in early infancy: From sensation to cogni-
tion (pp. 301–332). Hove, England: Psychology Press.
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Dougherty, T. M., & Haith, M. M. (1997). Infant expectations and reaction time as pre-
dictors of childhood speed of processing and IQ. Developmental Psychology, 33, 146–
155.
Fagan, J. F. (1992). Intelligence: A theoretical viewpoint. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 1, 82–86.
Fry, A. F., & Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence:
Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 7, 237–241.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic
Books.
Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences: The theory in practice. New York: Basic Books.
Hale, S. (1990). A global developmental trend in cognitive processing speed in children.
Child Development, 61, 653–663.
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex—Developmental
changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195–205.
Kail 61
Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and ad-
olescence. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490–501.
Kail, R. (1992). General slowing of information processing by persons with mental retar-
dation. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 97, 333–341.
Kail, R. (1993). Processing time decreases globally at an exponential rate during child-
hood and adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 254–265.
Kail, R. (1995). Processing speed, memory, and cognition. In W. Schneider & F. E. Wein-
ert (Eds.), Memory development: State of the art and future directions (pp. 71–88). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Kail, R. (1997). The neural noise hypothesis: Evidence from processing speed in adults
with multiple sclerosis. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 4, 157–165.
Kail, R. (1998). Speed of information processing in patients with multiple sclerosis. Jour-
nal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 1–9.
Kail, R., & Park, Y. (1992). Global developmental change in processing time. Merrill–
Palmer Quarterly, 38, 525–541.
Kail, R., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1985). Human intelligence: Perspectives and prospects. New York:
Freeman.
Kail, R., & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psycholog-
ica, 86, 199–225.
Kopp, C. B., & McCall, R. B. (1982). Predicting later mental performance for normal,
at-risk, and handicapped infants. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span develop-
ment and behavior: Vol. 4 (pp. 33–61). New York: Academic Press.
McCall, R. B. (1989). Commentary. Human Development, 32, 177–186.
Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. W. (1990). The information
loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Re-
view, 97, 475–487.
Salthouse, T. A. (1988). Initiating the formalization of theories of cognitive aging. Psy-
chology and Aging, 3, 1–16.
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence” objectively determined and measured.
American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–293.
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge, En-
gland: Cambridge University Press.
Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, T. G. (1941). Factorial studies of intelligence. Psychometric
Monograph, No. 2.
Yakovlev, P. I., & Lecours, A. R. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of
the brain. In A. Minkowski (Ed.), Regional development of the brain in early life (pp. 1–70).
Oxford, UK: Blackwell.