Stutter Treatment
Stutter Treatment
Stutter Treatment
D
rs. Curlee and Yairi’s (1997) recent listed by Bloodstein (1995, his Tables 3 and 6)
paper provided some interesting and that have been conducted since 1950.1 Esti-
provocative comments regarding mates of the lifetime incidence of stuttering
treatment for children who have been stuttering range from 0.70% to 15.40% across these stud-
for less than approximately 2 years. Their paper ies, whereas estimates of prevalence range from
raised several issues that were important, 0.30% to 2.12%. These differences clearly
complex, and fully deserving of critical assess- reflect substantial and significant variability
ment. It was accompanied by two “Second across studies, and they also argue against
Opinion” commentaries (Bernstein Ratner, 1997; Curlee and Yairi’s (1997, p. 9) claim that it is
Zebrowski, 1997), each of which addressed some “mathematically transparent” that “75% to 90%
Ingham additional pieces of this complex area, and each of all who begin” to stutter will recover: At the
of which essentially agreed with Curlee and Yairi extreme, these numbers actually suggest that
on two general points: that some children who the lifetime incidence of stuttering is only 0.7%
stutter may not need clinical services, and that we (Culton, 1986) at the same time that its preva-
have reason to be dissatisfied with the available lence is somehow as high as 2.12% in junior-
evidence about treatment effectiveness for high and high-school students (Gillespie &
children who stutter. We do not entirely disagree, Cooper, 1973). It is certainly not mathematically
but the purpose of this response is to present an simple to translate such data into a 75–90% re-
opinion about Curlee and Yairi’s paper that covery rate.
challenges their premises and their conclusions in
several areas. These areas include the incidence
and prevalence of stuttering, and whether those Recommending Treatments
data provide evidence of high recovery rates; for Stuttering
several issues related to determining whether The next section of Curlee and Yairi’s paper
Cordes treatments are effective and whether they should argued that it may be appropriate to withhold
be recommended; and several issues related to treatment for young children not only because
the experimental evaluation of treatment of the supposedly high rate of natural recovery
effectiveness. but also because “the efficacy of early interven-
tions with children soon after stuttering onset is
Incidence, Prevalence, and
Mathematically Necessary 1
An additional nine studies in Bloodstein’s Table 3, and
Recovery three studies in his Table 6, were conducted between 1893
and 1942. Inclusion of these studies would only lengthen
Curlee and Yairi (1997) began with the our Table 1 without changing the conclusions drawn, as
premise that there is “considerable disagreement readers may verify.
10 American American
Journal ofJournal
Speech-Language Pathology
of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol.
• Vol. 7 •7 No. 3
• 1058-0360/98/0703-0010 August 1998
© American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a Universite Laval User on 09/10/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
TABLE 1. Summary of incidence and prevalence studies of stuttering reported since 1950, selected
from those summarized by Bloodstein (1995, Tables 3 and 6).
Study N Population %
Prevalence studies
Schindler (1955) 22,976 Grades 1–12 0.55
Hull (1969) 6,287 Grades 1–12 0.30
Gillespie & Cooper (1973) 5,054 Grades 7–12 2.12
Leavitt (1974) 10,445 Grades 1–6 0.84
Leavitt (1974) 10,449 Grades 1–6 1.50
Brady & Hall (1976) 187,420 Grades K–12 0.35
Hull et al. (1976) 38,802 Grades 1–12 0.80
Leske (1981) 7,119 Grades 1–6 2.00
Incidence studies
Glasner & Rosenthal (1957) 996 First grade 15.4
Andrews & Harris (1964) 1,000 Birth–16 4.9
Andrews & Harris (1964) 206 Adults 4.8
Sheehan & Martyn (1970) 5,138 Freshmen/graduates 2.9
Dickson (1971) 3,923 K–12 9.4
Cooper (1972) 5,054 High school 3.7
Porfert & Rosenfield (1978) 2,107 University students 5.5
Seider, Gladstien, & Kidd (1983) 1,857 Stutterers’ relatives 13.9
Culton (1986) 30,586 University freshmen 0.7
essentially unknown” (p. 10). In expanding on the chances that children will recover.2
this idea, Curlee and Yairi drew on a confer- Curlee and Yairi (1997), on the other hand,
ence presentation by one of us (Ingham, 1996) concluded that treatment may not need to be
that has since become a book chapter by both offered to young children and that delaying
of us (Ingham & Cordes, in press). In this chap- treatment will have no deleterious effects:
ter, we criticized current stuttering treatment “Our contention is that active monitoring of
research, asserting that our discipline and our young preschoolers during the first 2 years of
clients are poorly served when researchers stuttering…rather than intervening actively,
promote treatments that are supported by inad- permits the unaided remission of stuttering for
equate treatment outcome data. We provided most of them and does not adversely affect later
specific examples of articles that recommended treatment of stuttering for those who do not
treatment procedures that were undocumented, stop” (Curlee & Yairi, 1997, p. 12). Clearly,
at best, and shown to be ineffective, at worst. Ingham and Cordes (in press) and Curlee and
We also pointed out that there is, in fact, an Yairi (1997) have drawn diametrically opposed
established and growing body of treatment conclusions from the same research and clinical
research studies that do satisfy a relatively literature, making opposite recommendations
standard evaluation framework, providing for young children who stutter. There would
repeated speech measures from before, during, appear to be several factors underlying this dis-
and after treatment and from both within- and agreement, several of which we explore below.
beyond-clinic conditions (e.g., Craig et al.,
1996; Kully & Boberg, 1991; Lincoln, Onslow,
Lewis, & Wilson, 1996; Martin, Kuhl, & Distinguishing Between
Haroldson, 1972). These studies consistently Disfluency and Stuttering
report clinically significant benefits relative to One of the first issues that deserves further
untreated control conditions, some for very consideration is as simple as the basic fact that
young children, and these benefits may also be 2
evaluated through comparisons with other The more important implication of these findings, actually,
is that parents should be given information about the
reports of children who have not received pro- benefits that may occur if they do try to intervene in their
fessional treatment. Finally, we presented a re- child’s stuttering. Studies reported by Onslow and
analysis of available data that we believed colleagues, and others, strongly suggest that children’s
strongly suggested two conclusions: that treat- stuttering can be reduced if parents employ relatively mild
verbal contingencies for occasions of stuttering and for
ment should be offered to young children who periods of fluent speech production. There is, on the
stutter, and that delaying treatment, even for as contrary, absolutely no evidence that such procedures will
little as 15 months after onset, may diminish cause stuttering to increase or become chronic.
Ingham • Cordes 11
Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a Universite Laval User on 09/10/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
a disfluency is not necessarily a stuttering. Starkweather et al. (1990) discussed very little
Measuring disfluencies cannot be equated with data at all (as Curlee and Yairi pointed out).
measuring stuttering, because measuring all
disfluencies would include measuring both
normal and stuttered disfluencies. Systems that Distinguishing Among General
distinguish between stuttered disfluencies and Approaches to Treatment
nonstuttered disfluencies (e.g., Conture, 1990; The determination of what constitutes treat-
Ryan, 1974; Wingate, 1964) also fail, because ment is equally critical to a meaningful discus-
it is consistently possible to find examples of sion about whether to provide treatment for
speech behaviors that cannot be appropriately children who stutter. Three related issues are
classified under such systems (the attempt to important here: whether treatment must be
define all between-word disfluencies as normal administered by a professional, whether treat-
provides one clear example; Cordes & Ingham, ment includes direct or indirect procedures, and
1995). This problem has led some researchers whether a procedure should be referred to as a
to measure “stutter-type” (Meyers, 1986) or treatment if there is little or no evidence that it
“stutter-like” (Yairi & Ambrose, 1992) dis- will be effective.
fluencies, an equivocating approach that does The question of whether treatment must be
not solve the problem of determining whether administered by a professional was raised by
the reported data are meant to represent stutter- Curlee and Yairi in the context of spontaneous
ing or not. Much other current research about recovery, and it is related to previous sugges-
stuttering is conducted simply in terms of dis- tions that parent- or self-initiated procedures
fluencies, with no attempt to provide data about might be a relevant factor in recoveries that
stutterings at all. appear to occur without professional interven-
The implications of such basic definitional tion (Finn, 1996; Ingham, 1983). Curlee and
problems are enormous, especially for very Yairi argued that “most advocates of this view
young children. They lead to diagnostic criteria do not address the apparent failure of care-
that label children as stuttering if their speech givers’ interventions” (p. 11) when stuttering
includes “10% or more total disfluency” (Gre- persists. In fact, Ingham (1983) addressed this
gory & Hill, 1993, p. 28), whether or not that very issue: “there is also no evidence that these
speech includes any atypical, abnormal, or procedures are necessarily effective in reducing
stuttered disfluencies. They also lead to important stuttering in all children. All that we have to
difficulties in evaluating the effects of treatment: look to is some evidence that they may be ef-
A report that disfluencies were reduced is not fective with some children who stutter” (p.
necessarily a report that stuttering was reduced 123). The importance of this point is high-
and should not be interpreted as such. lighted by the fact that Curlee and Yairi used it
Given these complexities, any meaningful to support another suggestion that early inter-
discussion about whether children are stutter- vention for stuttering may be unnecessary:
ing, or about whether children need treatment “If…intervention or treatment is restricted to
for stuttering, should begin by affirming that activities that are undertaken by or under the
normal disfluency will not be confused with supervision of certified clinicians trying to
stuttering and by documenting that the children remediate stuttering, current evidence indicates
in question were, in fact, stuttering. Curlee and that 60% to 70% of preschool age children who
Yairi (1997) did not address this seemingly begin to stutter stop within the first 2 years of
basic issue of defining their terms, a problem onset without having received any kind of such
that may have led them to some questionable professionally directed intervention” (p. 11).
conclusions. They wrote, for example, that “a The premises underlying this statement de-
variety of different treatment procedures may serve some thoughtful scrutiny. First, we see no
be capable of eliminating or significantly reduc- reason to reserve the label “treatment” for cases
ing almost all young children’s stuttering” (p. where a professional was involved. Adults often
10), basing this statement on the evidence that “a provide treatments for themselves or for their
number of different intervention procedures…are children, for everything from headaches to
reported to have high rates (e.g., >85%) of behavioral, emotional, or learning problems.
success or ‘recovery’ (Fosnot, 1993; Martin, Second, we know of no data to suggest that
Kuhl, & Haroldson, 1972;…Starkweather, “60% to 70%” of all children who begin to
Gottwald, & Halfond, 1990)” (pp. 9–10). The stutter receive no professional intervention at
definitional problem here is that some of these all. Third, there is evidence that some children
papers did not report data in terms of children’s who recover without professional intervention
stuttering: Fosnot (1993) discussed disfluencies are exposed to procedures that are known to
and stutter-type disfluencies, so her data may or reduce stuttering (Finn, 1996; Ingham, 1976,
may not be related to changes in stuttering, and 1983; Martin & Lindamood, 1986). In addition,
Ingham • Cordes 13
Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a Universite Laval User on 09/10/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
(1996). Kelly offered less critical reviews of study have varied from publication to publica-
this literature than Nippold and Rudzinski tion (Onslow & Packman, in press). Yairi and
(1995), who could find little supporting evi- Ambrose (1992) required, among other criteria,
dence for these procedures. Zebrowski et al. that a child display not more than 2.99 SLDs
(1996) did offer some data on the effects of per 100 words to be classified as “recovered.”
slowing the mother’s speech rate, from a study Later studies, however, have allowed as many
of 5 children who stuttered (aged 2;10–7;5) as 4 SLDs (see Paden & Yairi, 1996, p. 983;
and their mothers, but those data showed only Watkins & Yairi, 1997, pp. 387–388; Ambrose,
variable and nonsignificant effects on the Cox, & Yairi, 1997, p. 569). At the same time,
children’s disfluencies in a clinic setting. In the opposite problem is also present: In Yairi
other words, as Nippold and Rudzinski (1995) and Ambrose’s (1992, 1996) data, 4 of the 8
concluded previously, not one of the indirect continuing stutterers displayed less than 2.99
treatment strategies recommended by either SLDs at Visit 5 in the clinic but were still re-
Bernstein Ratner (1997) or Zebrowski (1997) garded as nonrecovered (presumably because
can be justified on the grounds of data that of parental reports that these children still stut-
should be interpreted as showing clinically tered outside the clinic). These and other com-
significant reductions in children’s stuttering. plexities in the data from Yairi and colleagues
would be unremarkable were it not for the fact
that these are the numbers used by Curlee and
The Desirable Outcomes for Yairi (1997, p. 10) to claim that the rate of recov-
Children Who Stutter ery in untreated stutterers within the longitudinal
Arguments about whether treatment is desir- study is as high as 89%. In a more general sense,
able or necessary for young children who stutter these conflicting reports simply demonstrate the
are further complicated by arguments about the importance of establishing and following clear
desired treatment outcome. Whether adminis- criteria for determining when a child will be said
tered by a professional or not, whether direct or to have recovered from stuttering, as well as the
indirect, whether administered immediately or difficulties inherent in doing so.3
delayed, we assume that the goals of treatment
for a child who stutters are twofold: to eliminate
the stuttering and to allow the child to grow up The Effects of Delaying Treatment
without any of the social or emotional conse- As Curlee and Yairi and others have ob-
quences of living with a communication disor- served, there is unquestionably some number of
der. We also believe that accomplishing the children who stutter for several months and
former is among the most straightforward ways then stop stuttering without formal treatment.
to achieve the latter (Lincoln & Onslow, 1997). For example, of the 43 children who ever stut-
One of the many complications in this area tered in Andrew and Harris’s 1964 longitudinal
involves determining whether stuttering has study, approximately 18 children stopped stut-
been eliminated. Clearly, this determination tering within 6 months.4 They labeled most of
cannot be made from brief speech samples these children “Transient Nonfluent,” but such
recorded within a single treatment environ-
ment; there are sufficient reports of stuttering
3
varying across time and place, and of “clinic- The issue is complicated by the fact that Yairi and
Ambrose (1996) published a correction to the Yairi and
bound fluency,” that this point should be self- Ambrose (1992) data that, itself, may require correction. In
evident (Andrews & Ingham, 1972; J. Ingham, a personal communication to one of us (R. I.) regarding the
1989; Ingham & Packman, 1977; Lincoln & original correction, Ambrose (personal communication,
Onslow, 1997; Lincoln et al., 1996; Martin et April 22, 1996) reported that by Visit 5 there were 13
Recovered and 8 Continuing stutterers. The mean SLD
al., 1972; Onslow, Costa, & Rue, 1990; Onslow scores for both groups of subjects, as shown in the 1996
et al., 1994; Reed & Godden, 1977; Ryan, Erratum, correspond precisely to that number of subjects
1971, 1974; Ryan & Van Kirk Ryan, 1983). within each category. Yairi and Ambrose (1996) also
Equally, this determination cannot be made if reported, however, that at Visit 5 there were 14 Recovered
data are reported only in terms of disfluencies, and 7 Continuing stutterers.
4
rather than in terms that make it clear whether Readers should be aware that the Andrews and Harris study
the child is stuttering or not, as discussed above. was not without its problems (Ingham, 1976). The data from
this study were first displayed in a figure (Andrews & Harris,
The longitudinal study of Yairi and col- 1964, p. 31) that has been reproduced in various editions of
leagues, referred to by Curlee and Yairi (1997), Bloodstein’s A Handbook on Stuttering. In 1984, however,
exemplifies some of the difficulties in this area Andrews (1984, p. 4) made unexplained changes to the years
and is directly relevant to Curlee and Yairi’s of stuttering onset and recovery for 10 of the 43 children. A
comparison between the original table (Bloodstein, 1981, p.
recommendations about delaying treatment. 84) and the revised table (Bloodstein, 1987, p. 94) shows
First, the performance criteria for defining several changes in the longitudinal data for which no
children as “recovered” in the longitudinal explanation has ever been provided.
Ingham • Cordes 15
Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a Universite Laval User on 09/10/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
ages, time since onset, sex, and family history” reasons (both of which Curlee and Yairi also
(p. 10).5 We agree, but the comparisons pre- recognized): an untreated control group may be
sented by Ingham and Cordes (in press) were, scientifically unnecessary, and an untreated con-
in fact, completed separately for boys and girls trol group may be unethical (Lewin, 1995). These
of different ages (preschoolers and school-age) issues are complex, in part because the overriding
and of different times since onset [more or less principle of treatment research should be to pro-
than 15 months, a dividing line chosen based vide the best possible treatment to all participants
on Yairi and Ambrose’s suggestion that (World Medical Assembly, 1989). If it truly is
“chances for chronicity increase approximately not known whether the treatment in question will
15 months after onset” (1996, p. 73)]. The be any better than no treatment at all, then a no-
claim that a particular treatment was solely and treatment control group can provide a scientifi-
directly responsible for the recovery of the cally relevant and entirely ethical comparison.
children who received it is different from the This is not the only option, however; investiga-
claim that the children who received a treat- tors can compare different components or levels
ment recovered, and this is an important dis- of a treatment, or compare newer and older treat-
tinction. Nevertheless, and even given the many ments, both procedures that have been used ef-
terminological difficulties that we have raised fectively in evaluating treatments for stuttering in
here, our comparisons consistently showed that adults (e.g., Ingham, Andrews, & Winkler, 1972;
children who received their treatment relatively Perkins, Rudas, Johnson, Michael, & Curlee,
early displayed more substantial treatment 1974).
benefits than children who received their treat- Large-scale treatment trials may also be
ment relatively late, even when “late” treatment appropriately abandoned when treatment ef-
was defined as only 15 months postonset. Noth- fects become so obvious that it is no longer
ing in Curlee and Yairi’s comments on this defensible to deny those benefits to the control
issue alters this conclusion. group. Such decisions are often made using the
same logic that Sidman (1960) employed to
determine the number of subjects that are
Treatment Research needed before researchers will accept that a
Methodology powerful treatment has been identified. If a
Curlee and Yairi’s final section on “Re- treatment is introduced, for example, to a rela-
search Needs and Ethical Issues” begins by tively small number of similarly afflicted pa-
acknowledging that treatment for very young tients, all of whom had shown stable (or dete-
children who stutter can be effective, and that riorating) base-rate levels, if all immediately
some interventions can be “causally related to reduce their disorder when treatment is intro-
the changes reported in some children’s stutter- duced, and if this effect is replicated across
ing” (p. 14). Nevertheless, they still hesitate to clinics or laboratories, then it is not necessary
recommend treatment for young children, be- to repeat the procedure with too many other
cause of the “absence of scientifically credible patients before clinical scientists should accept
treatment outcome data” (p. 15). We do agree, that the treatment will most probably have the
as discussed above, that there is an absence of same positive effects on the next patient. The
credible data to support many of the procedures possibility certainly does exist that the next
that are recommended as treatments for chil- subject might respond differently, but similar
dren who stutter, but these issues also deserve problems of external validity are evident in the
to be more carefully addressed. assumption that averaged findings from a large
Curlee and Yairi suggested that the use of group are applicable to any one particular sub-
“randomly assigned, untreated control groups has ject. More importantly, sequential analysis
long been viewed as essential for evaluating (Wald, 1947) can establish the probability that
treatment effectiveness” (p. 15) and that it is a sequence of consistently positive (or nega-
“critically important…that such important scien- tive) treatment responses could not have oc-
tific standards not be abandoned until there is curred by chance (Bross, 1952), without the
sufficient evidence available” (p. 15). Untreated need for the sampling-theory assumptions of
control groups are by no means universally ac- large-group research.
cepted, however, for two distinct but overlapping Curlee and Yairi did suggest that the studies
needed to provide credible treatment outcome
5
They also rightly observed that one of the studies included data could begin with “single-subject experi-
in our comparisons (Ramig’s 1993 historical study of mental designs…to assess the effects of differ-
untreated children) was a study of older children, not of ent treatment procedures in an unequivocal
very young preschoolers. But Ramig’s study also included
8 children, including 2 girls, who were assessed within 15
manner” (p. 16). At least eight studies over the
months of reported onset—and of those 8, only 1 boy past two decades have done precisely that, and
recovered. these were cited by Curlee and Yairi. They then
Ingham • Cordes 17
Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a Universite Laval User on 09/10/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 3–7. Kully, D., & Boberg, E. (1991). Therapy for school-
Curlee, R. F., & Yairi, E. (1997). Early intervention age children. Seminars in Speech and Language,
with early childhood stuttering: A critical exami- 12, 291–300.
nation of the data. American Journal of Speech- Lewin, D. I. (1995, December). Are placebo-based
Language Pathology, 6(2), 8–18. trials unethical? The Journal of NIH Research, 7,
Finn, P. (1996). Establishing the validity of recover- 30–33.
ing from stuttering without formal treatment. Lincoln, M., & Onslow, M. (1997). Long-term
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, outcome of early intervention for stuttering.
1171–1181. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathol-
Fosnot, S. M. (1993). Research design for examin- ogy, 6(1), 51–58.
ing treatment efficacy in fluency disorders. Jour- Lincoln, M., Onslow, M., Lewis, C., & Wilson, L.
nal of Fluency Disorders, 18, 221–252. (1996). A clinical trial of an operant treatment for
Gillespie, S. K., & Cooper, E. B. (1973). Preva- school-age children who stutter. American Jour-
lence of speech problems in junior and senior nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 5(2), 73–85.
high schools. Journal of Speech and Hearing Martin, R. R., Kuhl, P., & Haroldson, S. K.
Research, 16, 739–743. (1972). An experimental treatment with two
Gregory, H. H., & Hill, D. (1993). Differential preschool stuttering children. Journal of Speech
evaluation—Differential therapy for stuttering and Hearing Research, 15, 743–752.
children. In R. F. Curlee (Ed.), Stuttering and Martin, R. R., & Lindamood, L. R. (1986). Stutter-
related disorders of fluency (pp. 23–44). New ing and spontaneous recovery: Implications for the
York: Thieme. speech-language pathologist. Language, Speech,
Ingham, J. C. (1989). Generalization in the treat- and Hearing Services in Schools, 17, 207–218.
ment of stuttering. In L. V. McReynolds & J. E. Meyers, S. C. (1986). Qualitative and quantitative
Spradlin (Eds.), Generalization strategies in the differences and patterns of variability in dis-
treatment of communication disorders (pp. 63– fluencies emitted by preschool stutterers during
81). Toronto: B. C. Decker. dyadic conversations. Journal of Fluency Disor-
Ingham, R. J. (1976). “Onset, prevalence and recov- ders, 11, 293–306.
ery from stuttering”: A reassessment of findings Nippold, M. A., & Rudzinski, M. (1995). Parents’
from the Andrews and Harris Study. Journal of speech and children’s stuttering: A critique of the
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 41, 280–281. literature. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re-
Ingham, R. J. (1983). Spontaneous remission of search, 38, 978–989.
stuttering: When will the emperor realize he has Onslow, M., Andrews, C., & Lincoln, M. (1994).
no clothes on? In D. Prins & R. J. Ingham (Eds.), A control/experimental trial of operant treatment
Treatment of stuttering in early childhood: Meth- for early stuttering. Journal of Speech and Hear-
ods and issues (pp. 113–140). San Diego, CA: ing Research, 37, 1244–1259.
College-Hill. Onslow, M., Costa, L., & Rue, S. (1990). Direct
Ingham, R. J. (1996, May). On watching a disci- early intervention with stuttering: Some prelimi-
pline shoot itself in the foot: Some observations nary data. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-
on current trends in stuttering treatment re- ders, 55, 405–416.
search. Paper presented at the meeting of the Onslow, M., & Packman, A. (in press). Recovery
American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa- from early stuttering with and without treatment:
tion Special Interest Division 4, Monterey, CA. The need for consistent methods in collecting and
Ingham, R. J., Andrews, G., & Winkler, R. interpreting data. Journal of Speech, Language,
(1972). Stuttering: A comparative evaluation of and Hearing Research.
the short-term effectiveness of four treatment Paden, E., & Yairi, E. (1996). Phonological charac-
techniques. Journal of Communication Disor- teristics of children whose stuttering persisted or
ders, 5, 91–117. recovered. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re-
Ingham, R. J., & Cordes, A. K. (in press). On search, 39, 981–990.
watching a discipline shoot itself in the foot: Perkins, W. H., Rudas, J., Johnson, L., Michael,
Some observations on current trends in stuttering W. B., & Curlee, R. F. (1974). Replacement of
treatment research. In C. Healey & N. Bernstein stuttering with normal speech: III. Clinical effec-
Ratner (Eds.), Stuttering treatment efficacy. New tiveness. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-
York: Lawrence Erlbaum. ders, 39, 416–428.
Ingham, R. J., & Packman, A. (1977). Treatment Ramig, P. (1993). High reported spontaneous recov-
and generalization effects in an experimental ery rates: Fact or fiction? Language, Speech, and
treatment for a stutterer using contingency man- Hearing Services in Schools, 24, 156–160.
agement and speech rate control. Journal of Reed, C. G., & Godden, A. L. (1977). An experi-
Speech and Hearing Disorders, 42, 394–407. mental treatment using verbal punishment with
Kelly, E. M. (1993). Speech-rate and turn-taking two preschool stutterers. Journal of Fluency
behaviors of children who stutter and their par- Disorders, 2, 225–233.
ents. Seminars in Speech and Language, 14(3), Ryan, B. P. (1971). Operant procedures applied to
203–214. stuttering therapy for children. Journal of Speech
Kelly, E. M. (1995). Parents as partners: Including and Hearing Disorders, 36, 264–280.
mothers and fathers in the treatment of children Ryan, B. P. (1974). Programmed therapy for stutter-
who stutter. Journal of Communication Disor- ing in children and adults. Springfield, IL: Charles
ders, 28, 93–106. C. Thomas.
Ingham • Cordes 19
Downloaded From: http://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/ by a Universite Laval User on 09/10/2016
Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx