Wyklad Monograficzny PDF

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 35

Origins of language,

properties of language and


the development of writing
Origins of language
• In Charles Darwin’s vision of the origins of language, early humans
had already developed musical ability prior to language and were
using it “to charm each other.” It remains, however, a speculation.
• We simply don’t know how language originated. We do know that the
ability to produce sound and simple vocal patterning (a hum versus a
grunt, for example) appears to be in an ancient part of the brain that
we share with all vertebrates, including fish, frogs, birds and other
mammals.
Origins of language
• But that isn’t human language. We suspect that some type of spoken
language must have developed between 100,000 and 50,000 years
ago, well before written language (about 5,000 years ago).
• Yet, among the traces of earlier periods of life on earth, we never find
any direct evidence or artifacts relating to the speech of our distant
ancestors that might tell us how language was back in the early
stages.
• Perhaps because of this absence of direct physical evidence, there has
been no shortage of speculation about the origins of human speech.
The divine source
• In the biblical tradition, as described in the book of Genesis, God
created Adam and “whatsoever Adam called every living creature,
that was the name thereof.”
• Alternatively, following a Hindu tradition, language came from
Sarasvati, wife of Brahma, creator of the universe.
• In most religions, there appears to be a divine source who provides
humans with language.
The divine source
• In an attempt to rediscover this original divine language, a few
experiments have been carried out, with rather conflicting results.
The basic hypothesis seems to have been that, if human infants were
allowed to grow up without hearing any language around them, then
they would spontaneously begin using the original God-given
language.
• The Greek writer Herodotus reported the story of an Egyptian
pharaoh named Psammetichus (or Psamtik) who tried the experiment
with two newborn babies more than 2,500 years ago.
The divine source
• After two years of isolation except for the company of goats and a
mute shepherd, the children were reported to have spontaneously
uttered, not an Egyptian word, but something that was identified as
the Phrygian word bekos, meaning “bread.” The pharaoh concluded
that Phrygian, an older language spoken in part of what is modern
Turkey, must be the original language. That seems very unlikely. The
children may not have picked up this “word” from any human source,
but as several commentators have pointed out, they must have heard
what the goats were saying. (First remove the -kos ending, which was
added in the Greek version of the story, then pronounce be as you
would the English word bed without -d at the end. Can you hear a
goat?)
The divine source
• King James the Fourth of Scotland carried out a similar experiment
around the year 1500 and the children were reported to have
spontaneously started speaking Hebrew, confirming the King’s belief
that Hebrew had indeed been the language of the Garden of Eden.
• It is unfortunate that all other cases of children who have been
discovered living in isolation, without coming into contact with
human speech, tend not to confirm the results of these types of
divine-source experiments.
The divine source
• Very young children living without access to human language in their
early years grow up with no language at all.
• If human language did emanate from a divine source, we have no way
of reconstructing that original language, especially given the events in
a place called Babel, “because the Lord did there confound the
language of all the earth,” as described in the book of Genesis in the
Bible (11: 9).
The natural sound source
• The basic idea is that primitive words could have been imitations of
the natural sounds which early men and women heard around them.
• When an object flew by, making a CAW-CAW sound, the early human
tried to imitate the sound and used it to refer to the thing associated
with the sound. And when another flying creature made a COO-COO
sound, that natural sound was adopted to refer to that kind of object.
The natural sound source
• The fact that all modern languages have some words with
pronunciations that seem to echo naturally occurring sounds could be
used to support this theory. In English, in addition to cuckoo, we have
splash (pluskać), bang (stukać), boom (grzmieć), rattle (grzechotać),
buzz (brzęczeć), hiss (syczeć), screech (piszczeć), and forms such as
bow-wow (hau hau). In fact, this type of view has been called the
“bow-wow theory” of language origin.
• Words that sound similar to the noises they describe are examples of
onomatopeia.
The natural sound source
• It has also been suggested that the original sounds of language may
have come from natural cries of emotion such as pain, anger and joy.
By this route, presumably, Ouch! came to have its painful
connotations. But Ouch! and other interjections such as Ah!, Ooh!,
Wow! or Yuck!, are usually produced with sudden intakes of breath,
which is the opposite of ordinary talk. We normally produce spoken
language on exhaled breath.
The social interaction source
• Another proposal involving natural sounds has been called the “yo-
he-ho” theory. The idea is that the sounds of a person involved in
physical effort could be the source of our language, especially when
that physical effort involved several people and the interaction had to
be coordinated. So, a group of early humans might develop a set of
hums, grunts, groans and curses that were used when they were
lifting and carrying large bits of trees or lifeless hairy mammoths.
The social interaction source
• The appeal of this proposal is that it places the development of
human language in a social context. Early people must have lived in
groups, if only because larger groups offered better protection from
attack. Groups are necessarily social organizations and, to maintain
those organizations, some form of communication is required, even if
it is just grunts and curses.
The physical adaptation source
• Instead of looking at types of sounds as the source of human speech,
we can look at the types of physical features humans possess,
especially those that are distinct from other creatures, which may
have been able to support speech production.
• We can start with the observation that, at some early stage, our
ancestors made a very significant transition to an upright posture,
with bipedal (on two feet) locomotion, and a revised role for the front
limbs.
The physical adaptation source
• Some effects of this type of change can be seen in physical differences
between the skull of a gorilla and that of a Neanderthal man from
around 60,000 years ago. The reconstructed vocal tract of a
Neanderthal suggests that some consonant-like sound distinctions
would have been possible.
• In the study of evolutionary development, there are certain physical
features, best thought of as partial adaptations, which appear to be
relevant for speech.
The physical adaptation source
• Human teeth are upright, not slanting outwards like those of apes,
and they are roughly even in height. Such characteristics are not very
useful for ripping or tearing food and seem better adapted for
grinding and chewing. They are also very helpful in making sounds
such as /f/ or /v/.
• Human lips have much more intricate muscle interlacing than is found
in other primates and their resulting flexibility certainly helps in
making sounds like /p/ or /b/.
The physical adaptation source
• The human mouth is relatively small compared to other primates, can be
opened and closed rapidly, and contains a smaller, thicker and more
muscular tongue which can be used to shape a wide variety of sounds
inside the oral cavity.
• The human larynx or “voice box” (containing the vocal folds or vocal cords)
differs significantly in position from the larynx of other primates such as
monkeys. In the course of human physical development, the assumption of
an upright posture moved the head more directly above the spinal column
and the larynx dropped to a lower position. This created a longer cavity
called the pharynx, above the vocal folds, which acts as a resonator for
increased range and clarity of the sounds produced via the larynx and the
vocal tract.
The physical adaptation source
• One unfortunate consequence of this development is that the lower
position of the human larynx makes it much more possible for the
human to choke on pieces of food. Monkeys may not be able to use
their larynx to produce speech sounds, but they do not suffer from
the problem of getting food stuck in their windpipe.
The tool-making source
• Manual gestures may have been a precursor of language.
• By about two million years ago, there is evidence that humans had
developed preferential right-handedness and had become capable of
making stone tools. Tool-making, or the outcome of manipulating
objects and changing them using both hands, is evidence of a brain at
work.
• The human brain is not only large relative to human body size, it is
also lateralized, that is, it has specialized functions in each of the two
hemispheres.
The tool-making source
• Those functions that control the motor movements involved in
complex vocalization (speaking) and object manipulation (making or
using tools) are very close to each other in the left hemisphere of the
brain. It may be that there was an evolutionary connection between
the language-using and tool-using abilities of humans and that both
were involved in the development of the speaking brain.
The genetic source
• At birth, the baby’s brain is only a quarter of its eventual weight and
the larynx is much higher in the throat, allowing babies, like
chimpanzees, to breathe and drink at the same time. In a relatively
short period of time, the larynx descends, the brain develops, the
child assumes an upright posture and starts walking and talking.
• Even children who are born deaf (and do not develop speech)
become fluent sign language users, given appropriate circumstances,
very early in life. This seems to indicate that human offspring are born
with a special capacity for language. It is innate, no other creature
seems to have it, and it isn’t tied to a specific variety of language.
The genetic source
• As a solution to the puzzle of the origins of language, this innateness
hypothesis would seem to point to something in human genetics,
possibly a crucial mutation, as the source. This would not have been a
gradual change, but something that happened rather quickly.
Properties of human language
• All creatures communicate in some way. However, we suspect that other
creatures are not reflecting on the way they create their communicative
messages or reviewing how they work (or not). That is, one barking dog is
probably not offering advice to another barking dog along the lines of “Hey,
you should lower your bark to make it sound more menacing.” They’re not
barking about barking.
• This is reflexivity. The property of reflexivity (or“reflexiveness”) accounts
for the fact that we can use language to think and talk about language
itself, making it one of the distinguishing features of human language.
• We’ll look in detail at another five of them: displacement, arbitrariness,
productivity, cultural transmission and duality.
Displacement
• When your pet cat comes home and stands at your feet calling meow,
you are likely to understand this message as relating to that
immediate time and place. If you ask your cat where it has been and
what it was up to, you’ll probably get the same meow response.
• Animal communication seems to be designed exclusively for this
moment, here and now. It cannot effectively be used to relate events
that are far removed in time and place.
Displacement
• Humans can refer to past and future time. This property of human
language is called displacement.
• It allows language users to talk about things and events not present in
the immediate environment. Indeed, displacement allows us to talk
about things and places (e.g. angels, fairies, Santa Claus, Superman,
heaven, hell) whose existence we cannot even be sure of. Animal
communication is generally considered to lack this property.
Arbitrariness
• It is generally the case that there is no “natural” connection between
a linguistic form and its meaning. The connection is quite arbitrary.
We can’t just look at the Arabic word‫ ک لب‬and, from its shape, for
example, determine that it has a natural and obvious meaning any
more than we can with its English translation form dog.
• This aspect of the relationship between linguistic signs and objects in
the world is described as arbitrariness.
Arbitrariness
• Of course, you can play a game with words to make them appear to
“fit” the idea or activity they indicate, as shown in these words from a
child’s game. However, this type of game only emphasizes the
arbitrariness of the connection that normally exists between a word
and its meaning.
Arbitrariness
• There are some words in language with sounds that seem to “echo”
the sounds of objects or activities and hence seem to have a less
arbitrary connection. English examples are cuckoo, crash, slurp
(siorbać), squelch (mlaskać) or whirr (warkotać). However, these
onomatopoeic words are relatively rare in human language.
Productivity
• Humans are continually creating new expressions and novel
utterances by manipulating their linguistic resources to describe new
objects and situations. This property is described as productivity (or
“creativity” or “open-endedness”) and essentially means that the
potential number of utterances in any human language is infinite.
• The communication systems of other creatures are not like that.
Cicadas (cykady, tropikalne pluskwiaki) have four signals to choose
from and vervet monkeys (koczkodany) have thirty-six vocal calls.
Productivity
• The honeybee, normally able to communicate the location of a nectar
source to other bees, will fail to do so if the location is really “new.” In
one experiment, a hive of bees was placed at the foot of a radio
tower and a food source placed at the top. Ten bees were taken to
the top, given a taste of the delicious food, and sent off to tell the rest
of the hive about their find. The message was conveyed via a bee
dance and the whole gang buzzed off to get the free food. They flew
around in all directions, but couldn’t locate the food. (It’s probably
one way to make bees really mad.) The problem seems to be that bee
communication has a fixed set of signals for communicating location
and they all relate to horizontal distance.
Productivity
• This limiting feature of animal communication is described in terms of fixed
reference. Each signal in the system is fixed as relating to a particular
object or occasion. Among the vervet monkey’s repertoire, there is one
danger signal CHUTTER, which is used when a snake is around, and another
RRAUP, used when an eagle is spotted nearby. These signals are fixed in
terms of their reference and cannot be manipulated. What might count as
evidence of productivity in the monkey’s communication system would be
an utterance of something like CHUTT-RRAUP when a flying creature that
looked like a snake came by. Despite a lot of research involving snakes
suddenly appearing in the air above them (among other unusual and
terrifying experiences), the vervet monkeys didn’t produce a new danger
signal.
Cultural transmission
• While we may inherit physical features such as brown eyes and dark
hair from our parents, we do not inherit their language. We acquire a
language in a culture with other speakers and not from parental
genes. An infant born to Korean parents in Korea, but adopted and
brought up from birth by English speakers in the United States, will
have physical characteristics inherited from his or her natural parents,
but will inevitably speak English. A kitten, given comparable early
experiences, will produce meow regardless.
Cultural transmission
• This process whereby a language is passed on from one generation to
the next is described as cultural transmission. It is clear that humans
are born with some kind of predisposition to acquire language in a
general sense. However, we are not born with the ability to produce
utterances in a specific language such as English. We acquire our first
language as children in a culture.
Duality
• Human language is organized at two levels or layers simultaneously.
This property is called duality (or “double articulation”). In speech
production, we have a physical level at which we can produce
individual sounds, like /n/, /b/ and /i:/. As individual sounds, none of
these discrete forms has any intrinsic meaning. In a particular
combination such as bin, we have another level producing a meaning
that is different from the meaning of the combination in nib. So, at
one level, we have distinct sounds, and, at another level, we have
distinct meanings.
Duality
• This duality of levels is, in fact, one of the most economical features
of human language because, with a limited set of discrete sounds, we
are capable of producing a very large number of sound combinations
(e.g. words) which are distinct in meaning.
• Among other creatures, each communicative signal appears to be a
single fixed form that cannot be broken down into separate parts.
Although your dog may be able to produce woof (“I’m happy to see
you”), it does not seem to do so on the basis of a distinct level of
production combining the separate elements of w+oo+f.

You might also like