1 s2.0 S105381192100046X Main
1 s2.0 S105381192100046X Main
1 s2.0 S105381192100046X Main
NeuroImage
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neuroimage
a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t
Keywords: Adolescence is a developmental period that dramatically impacts body and behavior, with pubertal hormones
puberty playing an important role not only in the morphological changes in the body but also in brain structure and
adolescence function. Understanding brain development during adolescence has become a priority in neuroscience because
functional connectivity
it coincides with the onset of many psychiatric and behavioral disorders. However, little is known about how
resting state fMRI
puberty influences the brain functional connectome. In this study, taking a longitudinal human sample of typically
cognitive flexibility
brain network developing children and adolescents (of both sexes), we demonstrate that the development of the brain functional
connectome better fits pubertal status than chronological age. In particular, centrality, segregation, efficiency,
and integration of the brain functional connectome increase after the onset of the pubertal markers. We found
that these effects are stronger in attention and task control networks. Lastly, after controlling for this effect,
we showed that functional connectivity between these networks is related to better performance in cognitive
flexibility. This study points out the importance of considering longitudinal nonlinear trends when exploring
developmental trajectories, and emphasizes the impact of puberty on the functional organization of the brain in
adolescence.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117769
Received 26 October 2020; Received in revised form 18 December 2020; Accepted 5 January 2021
Available online 20 January 2021
1053-8119/© 2021 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
2
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
age: 6.71 – 18.06 years old), of which 37 and 11 returned for a second density; significance was corrected with a false discovery rate (FDR q <
and third session, respectively. 0.05; Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) for the thirteen functional net-
works defined in the atlas (Power et al., 2011).
2.5. Network features
2.7. Brain-cognition inference
Brain network construction was based on the 264 regions of inter-
est (ROIs) described by Power et al. (2011), which consist of 5-mm ra- A cross-sectional subset of the sample (N = 59; 24 males; age range:
dius spheres, encompassed in 13 different functional networks. This at- 9.67 - 17.98 years old) fulfilled a neuropsychological assessment that in-
las has been widely applied in pediatric studies (Satterthwaite et al., cluded a Card Sorting Task (CST; Berg, 1948; Lázaro et al., 2012). The
2013a, Satterthwaite et al., 2014; Gu et al., 2015; Marek et al., 2015; CST measures executive control, reasoning, and learning, but is particu-
Ciric et al., 2017; Chai et al., 2017; Gracia-Tabuenca et al., 2020). Edges larly sensitive to task shifting or cognitive flexibility. Briefly, a set of 64
were calculated as the functional connectivity between every pair of cards are given one at a time to the participant; each card can be catego-
ROIs, via the Pearson’s cross-correlation from their average time se- rized based on color, shape, or the number of figures. The participant has
ries. Based on these matrices, weighted graph theory metrics were com- to guess the correct category by putting the card behind four templates,
puted to characterize network organization features. The sum of edges and after that is told if the selection was correct or incorrect. After ten
or weighted degree (d), clustering coefficient (C), efficiency (E), and consecutive correct attempts, the category is shifted without informing
characteristic path length (L) were estimated to account for centrality, the participant, thus s/he has to guess again. Several scores are recorded:
segregation, and integration in terms of functional network organiza- the score of corrected attempts, perseverations (consecutive errors in
tion (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). C was calculated based on the Barrat the same category), and recurrent perseverations (non-consecutive per-
formula (Barrat et al., 2004). L was calculated as the average shortest- severations in a three attempt span). The total score can be interpreted
paths based on the Dijkstra algorithm (Dijkstra, 1959), where pairwise as a measure of cognitive flexibility; meanwhile, perseverations reflect
distances were set as the inverse of the corresponding functional connec- impulsivity and a lack of executive control and task-switching.
tivity edge. E was computed as the average of the inverse of the shortest- The relationship between the task scores and the functional connec-
paths of the connectome. For L, the shortest-path of disconnected nodes tomes was assessed with the Network-Based Statistics approach (NBS;
was set to the maximum observed value of the network in order to avoid Zalesky et al., 2010) using the R-package Network-Based R-statistics
infinite outcomes (Fornito et al., 2010). Barrat and Dijkstra’s algorithms (NBR; Gracia-Tabuenca and Alcauter, 2020; RRID: SCR_019114). NBR
were calculated via the R package ‘igraph’ (Csardi and Nepusz, 2006; implements the NBS framework allowing to implement other models
RRID:SCR_019225). instead of the general linear model, in this case the edgewise relation-
Additionally, we tested the relationship of the head motion (i.e., FD- ship was tested via Generalized Additive Models (GAM) splines. This
RMS) and the brain network properties via linear mixed-effects models algorithm computes the likelihood of each set of connected edges that
(LME) for the two confounding regression strategies, in order to iden- surpass an initial statistical threshold (also referred to as clusters of con-
tify the best preprocessing strategy, as suggested by Ciric et al. (2017). nections, sub-networks, or components). Such likelihood estimation is
Besides, we tested if such relationships are affected by the Euclidean based on their size (sum of the weights of the edges in this case) and
distance of the network’s nodes (Satterthwaite et al., 2013b). how it compares to a null distribution generated by permutations of
the original data. Specifically, the null distribution is the density of the
2.6. Neurodevelopmental trajectories maximum size of the suprathreshold component for each permutation of
the original dataset. In this case, the three standardized cognitive scores
Six models were tested to delineate the neurodevelopmental ef- were fit for each intra- and inter-connection of a connectome of 13 × 13
fects. Two linear mixed-effect models (LME) for the age (1) and functional networks (Power et al., 2011). The model includes the GAM
the age-sex interaction (2), plus four generalized additive mixed spline of the residual degree at the 25% connectivity density. LOESS
models (GAMM) with smooth splines for the age (3), age by sex PDS, sex, head-motion, and coil terms were previously regressed out. A
(4), and with nonparametric Locally Estimated Scatterplot Smooth- null distribution was computed based on 1000 permutations setting p <
ing (LOESS) for the PDS (5) and the PDS-sex interaction (6). Every 0.05 for individual connections.
model included random-effects intercepts, head-coil, and average head-
motion as covariates. All models were implemented in R, using the 2.8. Code accessibility
libraries ‘lme4’ (Bates et al., 2014; RRID:SCR_015654) for the LME,
‘gamm4’ for the GAMM with smooth splines (Wood and Scheipl, 2014; The preprocessed data and the code/software described in the paper
https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gamm4), and ‘gamlss’ for the is freely available online at https://github.com/BrainMapINB/Pubertal_
GAMM with LOESS (Stasinopoulos and Rigby 2007; https://CRAN.R- Connectome.
project.org/package=gamlss). Basis functions for the smooth splines
were set to four (k = 4) according to van Duijvenvoorde et al. (2019), 3. Results
meanwhile LOESS terms were set as ‘gamlss’ defaults (span = 0.75; de-
gree = 2). Mixed-effects were estimated via Maximum Log-likelihood 3.1. Head-motion mitigation strategies over functional connectivity
in order to compare between models. Model selection was based on
the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). The AIC evals the goodness of We tested two confounding strategies to minimize the effect of head
fit of the model via the log-likelihood function (lnL) plus a penaliza- motion on the brain connectivity estimates; one included the global sig-
tion of the number of parameters of the model (k), being AIC=2k-2lnL nal regression (GSR) and the other did not (no-GSR). For the functional
(Akaike, 1974). Therefore, the model with lower AIC is the one with a connectivity density, both strategies showed a sharp distribution cen-
better trade-off of adjustment and parsimony. To evaluate the robustness tered on zero. On the other side, graph theory measures showed different
at different connectivity thresholds, network parameters were computed associations with head motion after implementing such strategies, but in
in a range of 1% to 48% of connectivity densities. general for all measures, using GSR resulted in lower associations with
At the 25% connectivity density, the terms of the model with lower head motion, as reflected by their distributions being centered closer to
AIC were tested via the Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT). That is, we com- 0 (Fig. 2A). Additionally, we tested if these confounding strategies were
puted the LRT of the full model and a null model dropping the term of affected by the Euclidean distance between the nodes for the head mo-
interest (Stasinopoulos and Rigby 2007). In addition, network proper- tion at the edgewise level, however, both approaches showed a pattern
ties were estimated for each functional network at the 25% connectivity with a slope near to zero (Fig. 2B).
3
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
When comparing the adjustment of the six different models for each
network measure at different connectivity densities, we found that for
every measure, the models including PDS (without the PDS-sex interac-
tion) showed the lowest AIC for the vast majority of connectivity densi-
ties (Fig. 4).
Taking a representative connectivity density of 25%, we can observe
the neurodevelopmental trajectories via GAMM LOESS, for the four net-
work measures the trend along the PDS was nonlinear, with an inflec-
tion point around the level 2 of the pubertal scale (Fig. 5). In particu-
lar, the degree, clustering coefficient, and efficiency showed a nonlinear
pattern, where these measures increase in value after the turning point,
while the path-length showed the inverse pattern (Fig. 5). The PDS term
was significant for each network measure, as well as the random-effect
term for the degree, clustering, and path-length (Table 1).
At the functional network level, neurodevelopmental effects along
the pubertal stage were found mainly in frontal and parietal related net-
works for every graph theory measure (Fig. 6). Particularly, for the de-
gree, stronger effects were found in the default mode (DMN), showing a
nonlinear pattern similar to the whole-brain trend (Table 2; Fig. 7). The
clustering coefficient showed a similar spatial pattern, but with no sig-
nificant effects after correcting for multiple comparisons. For efficiency,
several frontal and parietal related functional networks such as the de-
fault mode (DMN), dorsal attention (DAN), and frontoparietal (FPN),
plus the cerebellar (CBL), subcortical (SUB), and visual (VIS) networks
showed a strong effect along the PDS (Table 2; Fig. 7). While for the
path-length, as expected, the effects were inverse to those of efficiency
Fig. 2. A) Density plots of the linear mixed-effects (LME) t-scores of the rela- (Fig. 7), being the stronger effects in the cerebellar (CBL), default mode
tionship between the head motion (measured by the average FD-RMS) and the (DMN), frontoparietal (FPN), salience (SAL), ventral attention (VAN),
functional connectivity of edgewise and nodal graph theory variables, for both and visual (VIS) networks (Table 2).
preprocessing approaches: with and without Global Signal Regression (GSR).
B) Hexbin plots of the Euclidean distance between pairs of nodes and the as-
sociation of head motion with functional connectivity (LME t-scores), for both
preprocessing strategies: with and without GSR. Colormap expresses the count 3.3. Brain-cognition relationships
of edges with that effect. The fitted line of this relationship is depicted in red.
Regarding the relationship of the functional connectome and cogni-
tive performance, NBS identified a significant set of network interac-
tions that relate with performance in the Card Sorting Task (CST) as-
3.2. Neurodevelopmental trajectories sessment (Fig. 8). A widespread inter- and intra-network connectivity
was associated with higher scores in the CST (pFWE = 0.013), where
Regarding the pubertal scale along chronological age, both sex the cingulo-opercular network (CON), dorsal attention (DAN), default
groups showed a sigmoid shape with faster growth in females than mode (DMN), and fronto-parietal (FPN) networks are those with greater
males. There was a significant effect for the age (LRT = 349.58; number of positive connections. Additionally, two connections of the
DOF = 15.1; p < 2.2e-16), age-sex interaction (LRT = 52.81; sensorimotor-hand network (SMH) showed a concave pattern. No sig-
DOF = 14.99; p = 4.11e-06), and random-effects terms (LRT = 24.88; nificant components were found for the perseveration nor recursive per-
DOF = 10.56; p = 0.0076; Fig. 3). severation scores after the permutation test correction.
4
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
4. Discussion
5
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
Table 1
For every graph theory measure computed at the 25% connectivity density, the Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT), its degrees of
freedom (DOF), and significance (p) of the Pubertal Development Scale LOESS (PDS) and random-effects intercepts (RE) terms
within the Generalized Additive Mixed Model (GAMM).
Table 2
Graph theory measures with significant LOESS term for Pu-
bertal Development Scale (PDS) within the additive model
(GAMM) at the functional network level, corrected for FDR q
< 0.05. Values for PDS Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT), its degrees
of freedom (DOF), and p-value (p).
4.2. Puberty and neurodevelopmental trajectories Notably, turning points coincide with the start of puberty. No pre-
vious work has focused on the functional organization of the brain net-
Pubertal status showed a faster growth rate in females compared to work; however, animal studies have demonstrated how the start of the
males. Although self-reported questionnaires are sensitive to inaccura- hormone events of puberty provoke plastic changes in the structure
cies (Dorn et al., 2006), the pattern observed has been replicated in other and function of the brain (Laube et al., 2020). Pubertal hormones at
populations (Braams et al., 2015; Wierenga et al., 2018; van Duijvenvo- the onset of puberty are related to an increase of the synaptic pruning
orde et al., 2019). Pubertal scale scores showed a faster growth rate in in frontal areas (Drzewiecki et al., 2016), as well as a growth in in-
females around level 2 and 12 years of age compared to males. Also the hibitory neurotransmission in these areas while not in sensory regions
PDS fits better to brain functional organization measures than chrono- (Piekarski et al., 2017). These results evince the changes in the brain
logical age, with inflection points at the first signs of puberty (level 2 connectivity patterns at lower scales following the onset of puberty, par-
of the PDS). Functional centrality, segregation, and efficiency increases ticularly in association areas. In addition, human studies have shown the
after the inflection, while path-length decreases after such a point, being relevance of pubertal status in gray matter (Goddings et al., 2014), white
more prominent in frontal and parietal functional networks in all cases. matter microstructure (Genc et al., 2017), and even fronto-subcortical
6
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
7
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
control networks. In addition, higher connectivity between those re- Berg, E.A., 1948. A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J.
gions was associated with better cognitive flexibility performance. In Gen. Psychol. 39 (1), 15–22. doi:10.1080/00221309.1948.9918159.
Betzel, R.F., Byrge, L., He, Y., Goñi, J., Zuo, X.N., Sporns, O., 2014. Changes in structural
summary, pubertal status along with longitudinal and nonlinear pat- and functional connectivity among resting-state networks across the human lifespan.
terns contribute to better description of the developmental trajectories Neuroimage 102, 345–357. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.067.
of the functional connectome, even converging previous discrepancies Blakemore, S.J., Burnett, S., Dahl, R.E., 2010. The role of puberty in the developing ado-
lescent brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31 (6), 926–933. doi:10.1002/hbm.21052.
regarding both segregation and integration increase in adolescence. Braams, B.R., van Duijvenvoorde, A.C., Peper, J.S., Crone, E.A., 2015. Longitudinal
changes in adolescent risk-taking: a comprehensive study of neural responses to re-
wards, pubertal development, and risk-taking behavior. J. Neurosci. 35 (18), 7226–
Data and code accessibility
7238. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4764-14.2015.
Bramen, J.E., Hranilovich, J.A., Dahl, R.E., Forbes, E.E., Chen, J., Toga, A.W., Sowell, E.R.,
The preprocessed data and the code/software described in the paper 2011. Puberty influences medial temporal lobe and cortical gray matter maturation
is freely available online at https://github.com/BrainMapINB/Pubertal_ differently in boys than girls matched for sexual maturity. Cereb. Cortex 21 (3), 636–
646. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq137.
Connectome. Casey, B.J., Cannonier, T., Conley, M.I., Cohen, A.O., Barch, D.M., Heitzeg, M.M.,
Orr, C.A., 2018. The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD)
study: imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 43–54.
Author statement doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001.
Chai, L.R., Khambhati, A.N., Ciric, R., Moore, T.M., Gur, R.C., Gur, R.E., Bassett, D.S.,
Zeus Gracia-Tabuenca: Methodology, Software, Validation, Formal 2017. Evolution of brain network dynamics in neurodevelopment. Netw. Neurosci. 1
(1), 14–30. doi:10.1162/NETN_a_00001.
analysis, Investigation, Data Curation, Writing – Original Draft. Martha Chai, X.J., Castañón, A.N., Öngür, D., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., 2012. Anticorrelations in
Beatriz Moreno: Methodology, Investigation, Data Curation. Fernando resting state networks without global signal regression. Neuroimage 59 (2), 1420–
A. Barrios: Methodology, Resources. Sarael Alcauter: Conceptualization, 1428. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048.
Ciric, R., Wolf, D.H., Power, J.D., Roalf, D.R., Baum, G.L., Ruparel, K., Gur, R.C., 2017.
Methodology, Investigation, Resources, Writing - Review & Editing
Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of
motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. Neuroimage 154, 174–187.
Acknowledgments doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020.
Coupé, P., Catheline, G., Lanuza, E., Manjón, J.V.Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimag-
ing Initiative, 2017. Towards a unified analysis of brain maturation and aging
We are extremely grateful to the children and parents that volun- across the entire lifespan: a MRI analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38 (11), 5501–5518.
tarily participated in this study. We are grateful to Nuri Aranda López, doi:10.1002/hbm.23743.
Csardi, G., Nepusz, T., 2006. The igraph software package for complex network research.
Leonor Casanova Rico, Luis Concha, César Arturo Domínguez Frausto, InterJ. Complex Syst. 1695 (5), 1–9. http://igraph.org.
Deisy Gasca Martínez, Leopoldo González-Santos, Ma. de Lourdes Lara Di Martino, A., Fair, D.A., Kelly, C., Satterthwaite, T.D., Castellanos, F.X., Thomason, M.E.,
Ayala, Juan J. Ortiz, and Erick H. Pasaye for their technical support. Milham, M.P., 2014. Unraveling the miswired connectome: a developmental perspec-
tive. Neuron 83 (6), 1335–1353. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.050.
We are also grateful to Rober Mercadillo for his support in the psycho- Dijkstra, E.W., 1959. A note on two problems in connexion with graphs. Numer. Math. 1
logical assessment and Michael C. Jeziorski for editing the manuscript. (1), 269–271. doi:10.1007/BF01386390.
The authors thankfully acknowledge the imaging resources and support Dorn, L.D., Dahl, R.E., Woodward, H.R., Biro, F., 2006. Defining the boundaries of early
adolescence: a user’s guide to assessing pubertal status and pubertal timing in research
provided by the “Laboratorio Nacional de Imagenología por Resonancia
with adolescents. Appl. Dev. Sci. 10 (1), 30–56. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads1001_3.
Magnética”, CONACYT network of national laboratories. Zeus Gracia Drzewiecki, C.M., Willing, J., Juraska, J.M., 2016. Synaptic number changes in the medial
Tabuenca is a doctoral student of “Programa de Doctorado en Ciencias prefrontal cortex across adolescence in male and female rats: a role for pubertal onset.
Synapse 70 (9), 361–368. doi:10.1002/syn.21909.
Biomédicas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México” (UNAM) and
Fair, D.A., Cohen, A.L., Power, J.D., Dosenbach, N.U., Church, J.A., Miezin, F.M., Pe-
received a fellowship (330142) from “Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y tersen, S.E., 2009. Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” or-
Tecnología” (CONACYT). CONACYT had no role in study design, data ganization. PLoS Comput. Biol. 5 (5), e1000381. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381.
collection, analyses nor writing the manuscript. This research was par- Fonov, V., Evans, A.C., Botteron, K., Almli, C.R., McKinstry, R.C., Collins, D.L.Brain Devel-
opment Cooperative Group, 2011. Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pedi-
tially supported by grant UNAM-DGAPA-PAPIIT IN212219 to SA. atric studies. Neuroimage 54 (1), 313–327. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033.
Fornito, A., Zalesky, A., Bullmore, E.T., 2010. Network scaling effects in graph analytic
studies of human resting-state FMRI data. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4, 22. doi:10.3389/fn-
Supplementary materials sys.2010.00022.
Gao, W., Alcauter, S., Smith, J.K., Gilmore, J.H., Lin, W., 2015. Development of human
Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in brain cortical network architecture during infancy. Brain Struct. Funct. 220 (2), 1173–
1186. doi:10.1007/s00429-014-0710-3.
the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117769.
Genc, S., Seal, M.L., Dhollander, T., Malpas, C.B., Hazell, P., Silk, T.J., 2017.
White matter alterations at pubertal onset. Neuroimage 156, 286–292.
References doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.017.
Goddings, A.L., Mills, K.L., Clasen, L.S., Giedd, J.N., Viner, R.M., Blakemore, S.J., 2014.
Akaike, H., 1974. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Autom. The influence of puberty on subcortical brain development. Neuroimage 88, 242–251.
Control 19 (6), 716–723. doi:10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.073.
Alcauter, S., Lin, W., Smith, J.K., Short, S.J., Goldman, B.D., Reznick, J.S., Gao, W., 2014. Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A.C.,
Development of thalamocortical connectivity during infancy and its cognitive corre- Rapoport, J.L., 2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during
lations. J. Neurosci. 34 (27), 9067–9075. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0796-14.2014. childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 101 (21), 8174–8179.
Aurich, N.K., Alves Filho, J.O., Marques da Silva, A.M., Franco, A.R., 2015. Evaluating the doi:10.1073/pnas.0402680101.
reliability of different preprocessing steps to estimate graph theoretical measures in Gracia-Tabuenca, Z., Moreno, M.B., Barrios, F.A., Alcauter, S., 2018. Hemispheric
resting state fMRI data. Front. Neurosci. 9, 48. doi:10.3389/fnins.2015.00048. asymmetry and homotopy of resting state functional connectivity correlate
Avants, B.B., Epstein, C.L., Grossman, M., Gee, J.C., 2008. Symmetric diffeo- with visuospatial abilities in school-age children. Neuroimage 174, 441–448.
morphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated label- doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.051.
ing of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med. Image Anal. 12 (1), 26–41. Gracia-Tabuenca, Z., Díaz-Patiño, J.C., Arelio, I., Alcauter, S., 2020. Topological
doi:10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004. data analysis reveals robust alterations in the whole-brain and frontal lobe
Barrat, A., Barthelemy, M., Pastor-Satorras, R., Vespignani, A., 2004. The architec- functional connectomes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. eNeuro 7 (3).
ture of complex weighted networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 101 (11), 3747–3752. doi:10.1523/ENEURO.0543-19.2020.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0400087101. Gracia-Tabuenca, Z., Alcauter, S., 2020. NBR: network-based R-statistics for (unbalanced)
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models longitudinal samples. bioRxiv doi:10.1101/2020.11.07.373019.
using lme4. arXiv:1406.5823. Gu, S., Satterthwaite, T.D., Medaglia, J.D., Yang, M., Gur, R.E., Gur, R.C., Bassett, D.S.,
Baum, G.L., Ciric, R., Roalf, D.R., Betzel, R.F., Moore, T.M., Shinohara, R.T., 2015. Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Cook, P.A., 2017. Modular segregation of structural brain networks supports the Sci. 112 (44), 13681–13686. doi:10.1073/pnas.1502829112.
development of executive function in youth. Curr. Biol. 27 (11), 1561–1572. Gu, Z., Gu, L., Eils, R., Schlesner, M., Brors, B., 2014. circlize implements and enhances
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.051. circular visualization in R. Bioinformatics 30 (19), 2811–2812. doi:10.1093/bioinfor-
Benjamini, Y., Hochberg, Y., 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and matics/btu393.
powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 57 (1), 289– Gur, R.C., Richard, J., Calkins, M.E., Chiavacci, R., Hansen, J.A., Bilker, W.B., Abou-
300. doi:10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x. Sleiman, P.M., 2012. Age group and sex differences in performance on a comput-
8
Z. Gracia-Tabuenca, M.B. Moreno, F.A. Barrios et al. NeuroImage 229 (2021) 117769
erized neurocognitive battery in children age 8− 21. Neuropsychology 26 (2), 251. Petersen, A.C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., Boxer, A., 1988. A self-report measure of puber-
doi:10.1037/a0026712. tal status: reliability, validity, and initial norms. J. Youth Adolesc. 17 (2), 117–133.
Hauser, T.U., Iannaccone, R., Walitza, S., Brandeis, D., Brem, S., 2015. Cognitive flexi- doi:10.1007/BF01537962.
bility in adolescence: neural and behavioral mechanisms of reward prediction error Piekarski, D.J., Boivin, J.R., Wilbrecht, L., 2017. Ovarian hormones organize the matura-
processing in adaptive decision making during development. Neuroimage 104, 347– tion of inhibitory neurotransmission in the frontal cortex at puberty onset in female
354. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.018. mice. Curr. Biol. 27 (12), 1735–1745. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.027.
Herting, M.M., Maxwell, E.C., Irvine, C., Nagel, B.J., 2012. The impact of sex, puberty, Power, J.D., Cohen, A.L., Nelson, S.M., Wig, G.S., Barnes, K.A., Church, J.A., Petersen, S.E.,
and hormones on white matter microstructure in adolescents. Cereb. Cortex 22 (9), 2011. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72 (4), 665–678.
1979–1992. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr246. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006.
Hibberd, E.E., Hackney, A.C., Lane, A.R., Myers, J.B., 2015. Assessing biological ma- Rubinov, M., Sporns, O., 2010. Complex network measures of brain con-
turity: chronological age and the pubertal development scale predict free testos- nectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage 52 (3), 1059–1069.
terone in adolescent males. J. Pediatr. Endocrinol. Metab. 28 (3–4), 381–386. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003.
doi:10.1515/jpem-2014-0187. Satterthwaite, T.D., Wolf, D.H., Ruparel, K., Erus, G., Elliott, M.A., Eickhoff, S.B., Hakonar-
Hidalgo-Lopez, E., Mueller, K., Harris, T., Aichhorn, M., Sacher, J., Pletzer, B., son, H., 2013a. Heterogeneous impact of motion on fundamental patterns of devel-
2020. Human menstrual cycle variation in subcortical functional brain opmental changes in functional connectivity during youth. Neuroimage 83, 45–57.
connectivity: a multimodal analysis approach. Brain Struct. Funct. 1–15. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.045.
doi:10.1007/s00429-019-02019-z. Satterthwaite, T.D., Elliott, M.A., Gerraty, R.T., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J., Calkins, M.E.,
Hjelmervik, H., Hausmann, M., Osnes, B., Westerhausen, R., Specht, K., 2014. Resting Wolf, D.H., 2013b. An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for
states are resting traits–an FMRI study of sex differences and menstrual cycle effects in control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity
resting state cognitive control networks. PLoS ONE 9 (7), e103492. doi:10.1371/jour- data. Neuroimage 64, 240–256. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.052.
nal.pone.0103492. Satterthwaite, T.D., Wolf, D.H., Roalf, D.R., Ruparel, K., Erus, G., Vandekar, S., Verma, R.,
Hwang, K., Hallquist, M.N., Luna, B., 2012. The development of hub architecture in the 2014. Linked sex differences in cognition and functional connectivity in youth. Cereb.
human functional brain network. Cereb. Cortex 23 (10), 2380–2393. doi:10.1093/cer- Cortex 25 (9), 2383–2394. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu036.
cor/bhs227. Sawyer, S.M., Azzopardi, P.S., Wickremarathne, D., Patton, G.C., 2018.
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., Smith, S., 2002. Improved optimization for the The age of adolescence. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 2 (3), 223–228.
robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neu- doi:10.1016/S2352-4642(18)30022-1.
roimage 17 (2), 825–841. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1132. Somsen, R.J., 2007. The development of attention regulation in the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C.F., Behrens, T.E., Woolrich, M.W., Smith, S.M., 2012. Fsl.. Task. Dev. Sci. 10 (5), 664–680. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00613.x.
Neuroimage 62 (2), 782–790. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. Spielberg, J.M., Forbes, E.E., Ladouceur, C.D., Worthman, C.M., Olino, T.M., Ryan, N.D.,
Kessler, R.C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K.R., Walters, E.E., 2005. Dahl, R.E., 2014. Pubertal testosterone influences threat-related amygdala–
Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the na- orbitofrontal cortex coupling. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10 (3), 408–415.
tional comorbidity survey replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62 (6), 593–602. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu062.
doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. Stam, C.V., Van Straaten, E.C.W., 2012. The organization of physiological brain networks.
Lázaro, J., Ostrosky, F., Lozano, A., 2012. Batería Neuropsicológica de Funciones Ejecuti- Clinical neurophysiology 123 (6), 1067–1087. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.011.
vas y Lóbulos Frontales-2. Manual Moderno, México. Stasinopoulos, D.M., Rigby, R.A., 2007. Generalized additive models for location scale and
Laube, C., van den Bos, W., Fandakova, Y., 2020. The relationship between pubertal hor- shape (GAMLSS) in R. J. Stat. Softw. 23 (7), 1–46. doi:10.18637/jss.v023.i07.
mones and brain plasticity: implications for cognitive training in adolescence. Dev. Taymourtash, A., Schwartz, E., Nenning, K.H., Sobotka, D., Diogo, M., Kasprian, G.,
Cogn. Neurosci. 42, 100753. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100753. Langs, G., 2019. Quantifying residual motion artifacts in fetal fMRI data. In: Smart
Lebel, C., Beaulieu, C., 2011. Longitudinal development of human brain wiring Ultrasound Imaging and Perinatal, Preterm and Paediatric Image Analysis. Springer,
continues from childhood into adulthood. J. Neurosci. 31 (30), 10937–10947. Cham, pp. 171–180. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-32875-7_19.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5302-10.2011. Tomasi, D., Volkow, N.D., 2012. Aging and functional brain networks. Mol. Psychiatry 17
Lenroot, R.K., Gogtay, N., Greenstein, D.K., Wells, E.M., Wallace, G.L., Clasen, L.S., (5), 549. doi:10.1038/mp.2011.81.
Thompson, P.M., 2007. Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajec- Tustison, N.J., Avants, B.B., Cook, P.A., Zheng, Y., Egan, A., Yushkevich, P.A., Gee, J.C.,
tories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage 36 (4), 1065–1073. 2010. N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 29 (6), 1310.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053. doi:10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908.
Lisofsky, N., Mårtensson, J., Eckert, A., Lindenberger, U., Gallinat, J., Kühn, S., 2015. Hip- Van Dijk, K.R., Hedden, T., Venkataraman, A., Evans, K.C., Lazar, S.W., Buck-
pocampal volume and functional connectivity changes during the female menstrual ner, R.L., 2010. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connec-
cycle. Neuroimage 118, 154–162. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.012. tomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J. Neurophysiol. 103 (1), 297–321.
Manjón, J.V., Coupé, P., Martí-Bonmatí, L., Collins, D.L., Robles, M., 2010. Adaptive non- doi:10.1152/jn.00783.2009.
local means denoising of MR images with spatially varying noise levels. J. Magn. van Duijvenvoorde, A.C.K., Westhoff, B., de Vos, F., Wierenga, L.M., Crone, E.A, 2019.
Reson. Imaging 31 (1), 192–203. doi:10.1002/jmri.22003. A three-wave longitudinal study of subcortical–cortical resting-state connectivity in
Marek, S., Hwang, K., Foran, W., Hallquist, M.N., Luna, B., 2015. The contribution of adolescence: testing age- and puberty-related changes. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 3769–
network organization and integration to the development of cognitive control. PLoS 3783. doi:10.1002/hbm.24630.
Biol. 13 (12), e1002328. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002328. Vijayakumar, N., de Macks, Z.O., Shirtcliff, E.A., Pfeifer, J.H., 2018. Puberty and the hu-
Mennes, M., Kelly, C., Zuo, X.N., Di Martino, A., Biswal, B.B., Castellanos, F.X., man brain: insights into adolescent development. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 92, 417–
Milham, M.P., 2010. Inter-individual differences in resting-state functional con- 436. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.004.
nectivity predict task-induced BOLD activity. Neuroimage 50 (4), 1690–1701. Wierenga, L.M., Bos, M.G., Schreuders, E., vd Kamp, F., Peper, J.S., Tamnes, C.K.,
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.002. Crone, E.A., 2018. Unraveling age, puberty and testosterone effects on subcorti-
Muschelli, J., Nebel, M.B., Caffo, B.S., Barber, A.D., Pekar, J.J., Mostofsky, S.H., 2014. Re- cal brain development across adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 91, 105–114.
duction of motion-related artifacts in resting state fMRI using aCompCor. Neuroimage doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.02.034.
96, 22–35. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.028. Wood, S., & Scheipl, F. (2014). Gamm4: generalized additive mixed models using mgcv
Paus, T., Keshavan, M., Giedd, J.N., 2008. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge and lme4. R package version 0.2-3.
during adolescence? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9 (12), 947. doi:10.1038/nrn2513. Xia, M., Wang, J., He, Y., 2013. BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human
Paus, T., Nawaz-Khan, I., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G.B., Pitiot, A., Pausova, Z., 2010. brain connectomics. PLoS ONE 8 (7), e68910. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068910.
Sexual dimorphism in the adolescent brain: role of testosterone and androgen receptor Zalesky, A., Fornito, A., Bullmore, E.T., 2010. Network-based statistic: iden-
in global and local volumes of grey and white matter. Horm. Behav. 57 (1), 63–75. tifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage 53 (4), 1197–1207.
doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.08.004. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041.
Peper, J.S., Schnack, H.G., Brouwer, R.M., Van Baal, G.C.M., Pjetri, E., Szekely, E., Zuo, X.N., Ehmke, R., Mennes, M., Imperati, D., Castellanos, F.X., Sporns, O., Mil-
Boomsma, D.I, 2009. Heritability of regional and global brain structure at the on- ham, M.P., 2011. Network centrality in the human functional connectome. Cereb.
set of puberty: a magnetic resonance imaging study in 9-year-old twin pairs. Hum. Cortex 22 (8), 1862–1875. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr269.
Brain Mapp. 30 (7), 2184–2196. doi:10.1002/hbm.20660. Zuo, X.N., He, Y., Betzel, R.F., Colcombe, S., Sporns, O., Milham, M.P., 2017. Human
Perrin, J.S., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G.B., Pitiot, A., Richer, L., Paus, T., 2009. Sex connectomics across the life span. Trends Cogn. Sci. Regul. Ed. 21 (1), 32–45.
differences in the growth of white matter during adolescence. Neuroimage 45 (4), doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.10.005.
1055–1066. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.023.