Eugenics and Musical Talent
Eugenics and Musical Talent
Eugenics and Musical Talent
The approach to the problem of inheritance of musical talent, from the point of view
of eugenics, divides itself naturally into five stages or tasks: (1) the analysis of what
The development of Seashore’s test fulfilled the first and second stages. The third and fourth stages
were undertaken both through schools and through comparative analysis projects where Seashore and
his colleagues attempted to demonstrate inheritance within racial groups and within families.5 The fifth
step does not appear to have been actively pursued, although Brenton Malin has noted that Seashore’s
subsequent adoption of euthenics to describe his work was a tactic that allowed him to side-step the
increasing push-back against the biological determinism in eugenics while retaining a justification for
dividing up groups of people on racial and/or gender lines.6 Seashore’s basic assumption was that the
sensory capacities he was measuring were innate. The idea that talent is inborn has been widely disproved
for other types of talents or aptitudes based on socio-economic factors (e.g., academic gifted tests for
children). It has also been drawn into question for the sensory capacities Seashore’s test examined.
Experiments with his tests showed correlations of less than 0.5 between the performance of parents and
children on the test, casting doubt on the inheritability of talent.7 Other experiments have shown that
sensory capacities such as pitch discrimination can be improved with training, demonstrating that these
capacities are not necessarily inborn.8 Current critiques of talent tests are largely focused on the process
of the tests, rather than the underlying assumptions.9 As recently as Richard Cowell in 2018, scholars
go as far as to argue that Seashore’s use of simple musical stimuli shielded his testing approach from
being influenced by race or socio-economic status.10 An exception to this is the work of Hoffman who
has explored how talent tests contribute to a construction of musical talent that is similarly biased to the
concept of smartness and who has looked at musical talent testing of the early twentieth century (including
Seashore’s) through the lens of critical race theory.11 Work like Hoffman’s, however, has been limited in
comparison to the continued uncritical use of talent testing in education and academic research.
Seashore’s tests had an impact on music education at both the elementary and post-secondary levels.
Through the Iowa Child Welfare Research Station, founded in 1917 at University of Iowa to study child
development, Seashore had a voice and standing within the local education community. This likely helped
the dissemination of his test and its findings. Interestingly, the work on general intelligence testing at
the Research Station ultimately demonstrated that IQ scores could improve with training and thus were
not an impartial representation of potential.12 This view did not, however, appear to inform Seashore and
his colleagues’ work on musical talent testing. In terms of impact, Patricia Shehan Campbell has argued
that interpretation and dissemination of the results of Seashore’s Measures of Musical Talent test led
elementary school music teachers to minimize efforts to teach rhythm in favor of vocal training, based on
the argument that rhythm was not an important component of musical development.13 Also, the idea that
the musical talent was innate disavowed educators from having to consider the impact of socio-economic
conditions on students’ musical achievement.
At the post-secondary level, the most extensive implementation of Seashore’s test was a ten-plus year
experiment at the University of Rochester’s Eastman School of Music.14 It was directly funded by George
Eastman and began shortly after the School’s founding in 1921. The experiment used the 1919 version
of the test, administered both during admission and during enrollment, in combination with qualitative
information from the School’s faculty. The test scores were also compared to four-year completion rates
with modest results. Alexander Cowan has argued that the use of the test at Eastman contributed to racial-
and class-based stratification.15 Moreover, the use of the test so soon after the School’s founding likely
An earlier version of this paper was presented by the author at the 2017 International Musicological
Society conference in Tokyo, Japan.
Notes
1. For an in-depth discussion of Stumpf’s influence in the development of comparative musicology, see Christensen,
Dieter, “Erich M. von Hornbostel, Carl Stumpf, and the Institutionalization of Comparative Musicology,” in
Comparative Musicology and Anthropology of Music: Essays on the History of Ethnomusicology, ed. Bruno Nettl
and Philip Bohlman (University of Chicago Press, 1991), 201–9.
2. John Grashel, “The Measurement of Musical Aptitude in 20th Century United States: A Brief History,” Bulletin of
the Council for Research in Music Education 176 (Spring 2008): 45–49.
3. Carl E. Seashore, “Individual and Racial Inheritance of Musical Traits,” Eugenics, Genetics and the Family 1
(1923): 231.
4. Ibid.
5. Zaid D. Lenoir, “Racial Differences in Musical Capacities,” (Ph. D. Thesis, Iowa City: University of Iowa, 1925);
Guy B. Johnson, “Musical Talent and the Negro,” Music Supervisors’ Journal 15, no. 1 (1928): 81–96; Davenport,
Charles B. Davenport, “Race Crossing in Jamaica,” The Scientific Monthly 27, no. 3 (1928): 225–38.
6. Brenton J. Malin, “Not Just Your Average Beauty: Carl Seashore and the History of Communication Research
in the United States,” Communication Theory 21, no. 3 (2011): 299–316. Seashore distinguished between eugenics
as “the science and art of being well born” and euthenics as “the science and art of living well or wise living.” See
Seashore, “The Term ‘Euthenics,’” Science 94, no. 2450 (1941): 561.
8. Micheyl, Christophe, Karine Delhommeau, Xavier Perrot, and Andrew J. Oxenham. “Influence of Musical and
Psychoacoustical Training on Pitch Discrimination,” Hearing Research 219, no. 1–2 (2006): 36–47.
9. For example, see Joanne Haroutounian, “Perspectives of Musical Talent: A Study of Identification Criteria and
Procedures,” High Ability Studies 11, no. 2 (2000): 137–60; and Susan Hallam, “The Power of Music: Its Impact on
10. Specifically, Cowell argued that Seashore “believed that pure tasks from music stimuli matched the pure tasks
in intelligence testing. This was wise, as the concern for the influence of culture (race and socioeconomic status
[SES]) that marred other music talent tests as well as IQ testing is not an issue with discrimination competence.
This interest in ability, aptitude, talent, or musicality dominated testing for more than half a century and continues
to be present in new formats. It remains important, as does Seashore’s foundational research.” Richard Cowell, “An
Overview of Music Tests and Their Uses,” in The Oxford Handbook of Assessment Policy and Practice in Music
Education, Volume 1, ed. Timothy S. Brophy (Oxford University Press, 2019), 537.
11. Adria R. Hoffman, “‘Blessed’: Musical Talent, Smartness, & Figured Identities,” Equity & Excellence in
Education 48, no. 4 (2015): 606–20; Adria R. Hoffman, “Competing Narratives: Musical Aptitude, Race, and
Equity,” in The Palgrave Handbook of Race and the Arts in Education, ed. Amelia M. Kraehe, Rubén Gaztambide-
Fernández, and B. Stephen Carpenter II (Palgrave Macmillan, 2018), 103–17.
12. For an in-depth discussion of this see Henry L. Minton, “The Iowa Child Welfare Research Station and the 1940
Debate on Intelligence: Carrying on the Legacy of a Concerned Mother,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral
Sciences 20, no. 2 (1984): 160–76.
13. Patricia Shehan Campbell, “Rhythmic Movement and Public School Music Education: Conservative and
Progressive Views of the Formative Years,” Journal of Research in Music Education 39, no. 1 (1991): 12–22.
14. Hazel Martha Stanton, “Measurement of Musical Talent: The Eastman Experiment,” University of Iowa Studies
in the Psychology of Music (University of Iowa, 1935).
15. Alexander Cowan, “Eugenics at the Eastman School: Music Psychology and the Racialization of Musical Talent,”
presented at the 2017 American Musicological Society conference in Rochester, NY, https://cowanaw.wordpress.
com/2017/11/07/eugenics-at-the-eastman-school-music-psychology-and-the-racialization-of-musical-talent/.
16. Seashore, “Three New Approaches to the Study of Negro Music,” Annals of the American Academy of Political &
Social Science (1928); Milton Metfessel, “Phonophotography in Folk Music: American Negro Songs in New Notation,”
(1928); Ray E. Miller, “A Strobophotographic Analysis of a Tlingit Indian’s Speech,” International Journal of American
Linguistics 6, no. 1 (1930): 47–68.
18. Dissemination of Seashore’s work and ideas had been aided in a large part through the availability of a low-cost Dover
edition publication of Psychology of Music (1967).