Merged
Merged
Merged
2020-21
►An important feature of language which is central to the study of syntax is that
language makes infinite use of a finite set of rules or principles, an observation
which led to the development of generative linguistics in the 20th century (Noam
Chomsky – 1965: Aspects of the Theory of Syntax).
► A language is a system for combining its constituents in infinitely many ways. One
piece of evidence of the system is represented by word-order restrictions. Given 5
words such as man, ball, a, the, kicked – how many possible combinations are there?
Are all of these combinations grammatical? Mathematically, the number of possible
combinations of 5 words is 5 factorial, equalling 120 instances. But among these 120
possible combinations only 6 form grammatical English sentences. Can you identify
them?
All the other 114 combinations are unacceptable/ ungrammatical to native
speakers of English. It is clear then that there are rules in English for combining
words.
More evidence of the finite set of rules and principles can be found in cognitive
creative abilities. Native speakers of a language are unconscious of the rules
they use all the time but have no difficulty in producing and understanding sentences
which they have never heard, seen or talked about before → linguistic competence.
A related part of this competence is that a language speaker can produce an infinite
number of grammatical sentences.
►How can these two facts be explained? Memorization appears
implausible, and this is a fact. Native speakers can generate an infinite
number of infinitely long sentences.
Some sentences can go on, and on, and on…..
In addition, there is a limit to the amount of information the human brain can
process. Consequently, a more plausible hypothesis is this:
→ All native speakers have a grammatical competence (internalized
knowledge of language) which can generate an infinite set of grammatical
sentences from a finite set of rules or principles.
[→compared with performance = the actual usage of this knowledge].
►This hypothesis has been accepted by most linguists and has been taken
as the subject matter of syntactic theory. This grammatical competence is
hypothesized to characterize a generative grammar →
a grammar that can generate an infinite number of well-formed English
sentences from a finite set of rules or principles.
Thus the job of syntax is to discover and formulate these rules. Note that
in generative theory, “rules” does not refer to prescriptive rules but to
“descriptive rules”. Prescriptive rules disfavour or discredit certain usages;
they prescribe forms which are generally in use; descriptive rules
characterize the forms speakers actually use.
Why are we supposed to study syntax and what is it good for?
There are many reasons for studying syntax, from general humanistic or
behavioural motivations to much more specific goals:
→ to help us illustrate the patterns of English more effectively and clearly;
→ to enable us to analyse and interpret the structures of English
sentences in a systematic and explicit way.
The practice of syntactic analysis /parsing /diagramming sentences will
improve students’ understanding of the structure of English and will
facilitate their comprehension and production of the language.
Let us look at slightly different cases. Can you explain why the following examples are
unacceptable?
*Despite of his limited educational opportunities, Abraham Lincoln became one of the
greatest intellectuals in the world.
*A pastor was executed, notwithstanding on many applications in favour of him.
To understand these examples, we first need to recognize that the words despite and
notwithstanding are prepositions, and further that canonical English prepositions
combine only with Noun Phrases. In the two examples they combine with Prepositional
Phrases again (headed by of and on), thus violating the rule.
More subtle examples are the following:
He said that that ‘that’ that that man used was wrong.
--the kind of sentence one can play with when starting to learn English
grammar. What are the differences between the 5 thats?
Again, syntactic knowledge must be used to explain the differences.
DEFINITION OF SYNTAX
Syntax is one of the components of grammar: the other major component is
inflectional morphology. Simply put, syntax is the study of the structure of
sentences. Thus syntactic analysis can be defined as:
a/ determining the relevant component parts of a sentence;
b/ describing these parts grammatically.
► The component parts of sentences are called constituents; so syntax
involves two closely related tasks: a/ breaking down the sentence into its
constituents; b/ assigning some grammatical label to each constituent, stating
what type of constituent (or grammatical category) it is, and what grammatical
function it has.
► In syntactic analysis we start from what is regarded as the largest unit of
description – the sentence, and proceed to the smallest meaningful unit. The
system of English grammar can be described in terms of a rank scale, from
the largest to the smallest unit.
sentence
clause
phrase
word/ lexeme
morpheme → the minimal indivisible unit in syntax
REPRESENTING SENTENCE STRUCTURES
Here we will be looking at two techniques: labelled bracketing and tree diagrams.
LABELLED BRACKETING
[s [NP The terrorists] [VP assassinated [NP the ambassador] VP] s].
NP, VP → category labels
→ Note here the notion of syntax as the description (and study) of constituents,
relations and functions.
TREE DIAGRAMS
S → contains as immediate constituents NP & VP
NP VP
Det N V NP
Det N
The tree diagram provides the analysis of our sentence down to word level. Analysis can go
beyond word level → morphemes
REPRESENTING SENTENCE STRUCTURES
►The syntactic information provided by the tree diagram is the same as
that provided by the labelled bracketing.
With tree diagrams, the system of expanding one unit into other units is
expressed as a set of instructions, called Phrase Structure Rules (PS
rules): S→ NP–VP
VP → V- NP → expand/ rewrite
NP → Det –N
The category labels in the tree diagram (S, NP, VP, V etc) are attached to the
nodes of the tree, and the lines connecting these nodes are called branches.
The node labelled S dominates the nodes labelled NP and VP as well as all the
other nodes further down. → the notion of immediate dominance.
The words are attached to the so-called terminal nodes. Thus the tree diagram
shows constituency very clearly: Det and N combine to form a NP constituent.,
V and NP form the VP constituent and so on. We have been using category labels;
you will recall that there are two main category classes: lexical and functional.
In traditional grammar, lexical categories are known as parts of speech: noun, verb,
adjective, adverb, preposition.
CATEGORIES
Let us briefly examine some of the criteria which are used in determining the
category a word belongs to. One less reliable type of criterion for categories is
semantic, e.g. a noun denotes a person, thing or place; a verb denotes an activity
or state; an adjective, a property. Such semantic generalisations are only
tendencies, not absolute rules. There are nouns which denote activities- the
hammering, events- recital, states- drunkenness and properties- foolishness.
► More reliable evidence for determining the category of words comes from
morphological and distributional criteria.
Examples of morphological criteria: nouns, but no other category, can take
a plural affix; most verbs change their morphological form according to the
requirements of tense and agreement;
Examples of distributional (syntactic) criteria: nouns are often preceded by
Determiners; nouns may be modified by adjectives; verbs may be modified by
adverbs.
In English nouns can be easily used as verbs and verbs as nouns; it is therefore
necessary to look at the context in which a word occurs.
CATEGORIES
In Shakespeare’s play Richard II [First Folio Edition] there is the following sentence:
Grace me no grace, nor Vunckle me, I am no Traytors Vunckle….
Using the criteria from the previous slide, the first instance of ‘uncle’ must be a
verb, because the noun following it does not need a preposition (by contrast, nouns
can only be followed by preposition and noun). The second ‘uncle’ is a noun
because ‘traitor’ has the possessive ‘s.
► Words which can appear in the same context are said to have the same
distribution: they belong to the same lexical category.
Syntax uses distribution frames, empty slots in a phrase that can be filled by a
certain category e.g. for nouns [the___]; [a___]; [of____].
► The main grammatical categories are Determiner [Det], Auxiliary [Aux],
coordinator and complementizer [Comp].
Grammatical categories determine the syntax of the sentence rather than
contribute to the meaning.
Pronouns are a hybrid category; they do not carry much lexical meaning, but they
can function on their own to stand for a Noun Phrase, unlike articles and
complementizers, which need something to follow them.
GRAMMATICAL CATEGORIES
►►The Det category includes articles as well as demonstratives, possessive
pronouns, possessive nouns, some quantifiers (any; many; much; all), some
interrogatives and some numerals; Det is an umbrella term for all these.
Determiners precede a noun to specify which noun is meant or whose it is. Some
are used before other determiners: all, both and half, e.g. all the books; half that
man’s money; both those problems; these quantifiers are called Pre-Determiners
►Auxiliaries will be dealt with in more detail later.
► Coordinators (sometimes called coordinating conjunctions)
join similar categories or phrases, e.g. and/ or.
John and Paul went to Madrid or Barcelona.
There are also two-part coordinators: both….and; either…..or; neither…nor.
►Complementizers such as that, because, whether, if, since join two clauses
where one is subordinate to the other:
[ S1 His aunt feared [X that [S2 he might catch something ]]].
S2 is a sentence inside S1: it is a constituent of S1.
The role of that is that of linking S2 (also called the embedded clause) to the
sentence inside which it occurs and it occupies the so-called complementizer slot
(COMP slot) in front of S2. Elements which occupy the COMP slot are usually
referred to as complementizers (also called subordinators or subordinating
conjunctions).
GRAMMATICAL CATEGORIES
Complementizers Other uses
After he left, it rained. after him – preposition
Fair as the moon is, it…. as nice – preposition
Before it snowed, it was cold. before me – preposition
I expect for you to do that. for John – preposition
She was tired, so she went to sleep. so tired - adverb
[S1 I expect [X for [S2 you to do that ]]]. S2→ non-finite embedded clause/ sentence
[S1 I expect [X that [S2 you should do that ]]]. S2 → finite embedded clause
Is there any difference in meaning? What about structure?
CONSTITUENTS - BASIC CONCEPTS RELEVANT TO THE
STUDY OF SYNTAX
We have already seen on slide 7, language as a complex system is
analysable in terms of levels; there are formal units at each level;
relations between units, as well as a hierarchy of units.
-- the phonological level: unit: the phoneme
-- the morphological level: units: the morpheme & the word
-- the syntactic level: unit: the phrase
Note the concept of the Sentence as a maximal phrase with degrees
of complexity: simple/ compound/ complex/ compound-complex.
--the (logico) semantic level: units: the sememe & argument structures.
►The function of exclamatives is more restricted than those of any of the other clause types – they
are used to make an exclamation; therefore they have a very expressive or emotional component .
Note that it is possible to make exclamatory messages by simply using the appropriate intonation
on all sorts of clause types: You’re alive, you stupid bastard!
Why can’t he get his dirty stuff into the laundry basket – just once?
Exclamatory meaning can also be expressed by intensifiers, e.g. wow, the hell, surely, so,
such: Get the hell out of here! Look at that fantastic sunset!
Why are you so pathetically stupid?
PHRASES
Phrases are constituents intermediate between the clause and the word.
Each word-level category has a corresponding phrasal category.
►A phrase consists of minimally just a Head (which is obligatory) and several dependents.
The Head of a phrase determines the environment where the phrase can occur
and determines the properties of the whole phrase:
Noun Phrase: [the badgers] [those large badgers from New York state]
[the most important representatives of workers’ interests at the conference]
Verb Phrase: [suddenly die of cancer at a young age]
[blindly rely on the advice of a counsellor]
Adjective Phrase: [interested in the history of theatre to some extent]
[completely and utterly disappointed at the ineptitude of the firm]
[devoid of content] [dull to the extreme]
Prepositional Phrase: [right near the door] [towards the entrance to the building]
[despite the failed attempt by the paramilitary at blowing up the building]
Adverb Phrase: [extremely well]
[completely independently of the approval of his superiors]
The underlined words are the Heads of the respective phrases. In generative grammar the
Head is said to project its phrase, e.g. Noun projects NP or NP is a projection of Noun.
►The other elements are dependent on the Head in two ways: they are either optional or are
included because the head requires them (the Head subcategorizes for them). ©DanaD
CONSTITUENCY TESTS &
GRAMMATICALITY
Seminar 1
Constituency tests are tests for establishing whether a string (a
sequence of words) is a constituent or not.
What did the lady running the group hand in on Friday at noon?
[Her resignation].
When did the lady running the group hand in her resignation?
[On Friday at noon].
3. The Movement Test
If a string can be moved to some other position in the sentence, it is
very likely to be a constituent.
[On Friday at noon], the lady running the group handed in her resignation.
Rover ran out of the house. → [Out of the house] Rover ran.
This type of front movement is known as Fronting/ Preposing.
►Note that due to the fact that word order in English is fixed to a great
extent, only certain constituents can be moved (fronted).
Cleft comes from the old verb cleave: split; thus sentences are divided in two.
Clefting is a device used to focus on a particular constituent of the sentence.
It was/is X that…. X= a constituent
3. In the following sentences, find each instance of the category given in brackets..
Be careful when deciding where each phrase begins and ends.
He drove his brother’s wife’s car from the top of the hill to the beach. (NP and PP)
At the next meeting, the president of the committee called in an expert on
environmental pollution and global warming from America. (NP and AP)
4. You probably know the concept of linguistic ambiguity, which is either lexical
(e.g. glasses) or structural. Can you bracket this sentence to show its two
interpretations?
The British left waffles on the Falkland Islands.
5. Are the sentences below grammatical or ungrammatical? Why?
Think in terms of subcategorization rules and make a clear note of the rule
that has been violated in all the ungrammatical examples.
1. Omar sighed a book.
2. The women were bringing.
3. I gave the car back to him.
4. I gave the car to him back.
5. My car realizes that I’m a lousy cook.
6. John very much Mary loves.
7. Who did we meet at the party?
8. Whom did we meet at the party?
9. They built last year a house.
10. She preferred very much for the students to do their homework.
VERB PHRASES
PREPOSITIONAL PHRASES
NEGATION
Consider this sentence:
[S Her mother [VP read [ NP a book ]]] . Is [read] a constituent?
Tests will help answer this question.
The Question test: What did her mother do? [Read a book].
The Pro-form test: She did so yesterday.
The Pseudo-cleft test: What her mother did was [read a book]. →
→ [read a book] is the VP constituent: read is the Head, followed by a NP which
completes the VP and is a complement of the verb read.
He [ VP told [NP the girl] [S that she was clever ]]. -- here tell takes two complements
Note that sentences become ungrammatical without their complements; also, it is
impossible to define the Head of a phrase without referring to its complement. A
definition of tell mentions something that is told and a person to whom something is
told.
► Modifiers are always optional constituents, while complements are obligatory.
As we have already noted, modifiers can be added recursively; in principle, there is
no limit to the number of modifiers within a phrase:
He [sometimes ]walked the dog [slowly] [in the park] [on Fridays] [after work]
[for two hours] [to clear his mind].
Now consider the lexical verb dine.
The dictionary definition (the lexical entry in the mental lexicon) for this verb specifies that
the food may appear in the sentence: We dined., or it may appear as a PP headed by the
preposition on: We dined on the chicken., but not as a NP: *We dined the chicken.
► Thus lexical Heads fall into different classes depending on the kind of complements, if any,
they must occur with → we say a Head (in this case a verb) subcategorizes for the
complements (dependents) it takes. Another way of saying this is that a verb occurs inside a
certain distribution frame.
► The main subcategories of lexical verbs are:
--intransitives : dine, work, arrive, die, disintegrate
-- monotransitives – take a single NP complement: see her; open a bottle; read a book
-- ditransitives (double object verbs) take two NP complements, often referred to as
Indirect Objects and Direct Objects, respectively: tell him a lie; show him a book; bake me a cake.
-- prepositional verbs – have a PP as (one of) their complements:
rely on them; deal with them; frown on them.
She interested me in the proposal.
He subjected them to an ordeal.
►►Note that a lexical verb may belong to more than one subcategory:
The silk feels nice. – feel takes a Predicative/ Subject Complement - [nice]
He felt his pulse. -- feel is monotransitive.
In analysis it is important to look at the entire sentence, not just look at the verb.
ELEMENTS BEFORE THE HEAD VERB
If a VP is marked for Tense, it is said to be finite: he laughs/ he laughed/ he is laughing:
the form of the verb is altered from present tense to past tense. Finite VPs contain an
obligatory element, Tense, besides the lexical verb.
►A Tense system is associated with the verb and is used to mark the location in time of
the situation; note that ‘time’ is a semantic notion; we distinguish between duration/
habitual time/ punctual time etc. The tense system is marked by verb inflections and/
or Auxiliaries.
► The question of where Auxiliaries are located in the sentence is a highly debated one
in linguistics. In one analysis, the VP is the complement of the Auxiliary. For several
reasons, including simplicity, we shall adopt the alternative analysis, in which Auxiliary
is part of the VP.
► Auxiliary verbs in English have 4 syntactic properties termed NICE:
Negation/ Inversion/ Code/ Emphasis
1. In negative clauses the negator NOT follows the first Auxiliary. In Modern English
lexical verbs do not allow the negator NOT to follow them. *We live not in Poland.
An Auxiliary is required in front of NOT; if there is no Auxiliary available, DO must be
inserted: We do not live in Poland. [ this syntactic rule is DO -SUPPORT]
2. In certain questions, Auxiliary and Subject invert: Do/ have we live(d) in Poland?
3. In constructions that code (stand for) a previously mentioned VP, the first Auxiliary is
repeated and inverted: We [VP live in Poland], don’t we?
4. In constructions in which the truth of the proposition is emphasized, the first Auxiliary
receives emphatic stress: We do live in Poland. [DO –SUPPORT]
The 4 syntactic properties of Auxiliary serve to illustrate the special status of the first
Auxiliary in English. Note that in English the Auxiliary is a complex formative which is
fundamental for the structure of the English verb system.
Here is the representation of the structure of English Auxiliary in terms of PSRs:
VP → Aux – (M)V
Aux → Aux1 – Aux2
Aux1 → T(ense) – M(odal)
Aux2 → (Perfective) (Progressive)
Tense → Present, Past
Modal → can, may, will, must ….
Perfective → have- en
Progressive → be –ing
► Tense is the obligatory element in a finite VP besides the lexical verb and its
complements, if any; the other elements are optional. Note also that Tense is only
marked once in English: if a Modal is inserted, Perfective is no longer marked for Tense.
Also Tense is always marked on the very first part of the VP (it is always the
first verb, whether lexical or auxiliary that displays Tense marking → Remember
we have adopted the model of analysis in which Auxiliary is part of the VP.
Perfective Aspect Progressive Asp. Passive
VP → T (M) (have + en) (be + ing) (be + en) V
Aux 1
Aux 2
PREPOSITIONAL PHRASES
©DanaD