Hare, R. Psychopathy
Hare, R. Psychopathy
Hare, R. Psychopathy
Psychopaths have been described as “social predators who charm, manipulate, and
ruthlessly plow their way through life.…Completely lacking in conscience and feeling for
others, they selfishly take what they want and do as they please, violating social norms
and expectations without the slightest sense of guilt or regret” (Hare, 1999, p. xi). The
diagnosis of psychopathic personality disorder has a rich historical tradition. Psychopathy
is perhaps even the prototypic personality disorder. The term “psychopath” at one time
referred more generally to all personality disorders (i.e., pathologies of the psyche) in
Schneider’s (1923) influential nomenclature of 10 distinct “psychopathic” personalities.
Only Schneider’s affectionless psychopathic was aligned with the current concept of
psychopathy: “Affectionless psychopathic persons are personalities who are lacking or
almost lacking in compassion, shame, honor, remorse, and conscience” (Schneider, 1950,
p. 25). It was subsequent to the work of Schneider that the term “psychopath” became
confined to the particular personality disorder, albeit with aliases that are misaligned
with, or do not capture, the traditional construct (e.g., sociopath, antisocial personality
disorder).
Page 1 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
There has long been an interest and effort in providing an adequate description of the
personality structure of psychopathy. The most influential clinical work was clearly
provided by Hervey Cleckley (1941, 1976) in his seminal text, The Mask of Sanity
(Cleckley also coauthored the text, The Three Faces of Eve with Corbett H. Thigpen;
Thigpen & Cleckley, 1954). In the original version of his text, Cleckley (1941) identified
21 characteristics of psychopathy: (1) usually very attractive person superficially, more
clever than average, superior general objective intelligence; (2) free from demonstrable
symptoms of psychosis, free from any marked nervousness of other symptoms of a
psychoneurosis; (3) no sense (p. 479) of responsibility, not concerned about irresponsible
behavior; (4) total disregard for the truth; (5) does not accept blame for actions; (6) no
sense of shame; (7) undependable, cheats and lies without compunction, commits
antisocial acts without adequate motivation; (8) execrable judgment; (9) inability to learn
or profit from experience; (10) egocentricity, incapacity for object-love; (11) general
poverty of affect, readiness of expression rather than depth of feeling; (12) lacks insight,
cannot see self as others see him; (13) no appreciation for kindness or consideration
shown by others; (14) alcoholic indulgences; (15) when drinking, readily places self in
disgraceful or ignominious position, bizarre behavior when drinking, seeking a state of
stupefaction; (16) does not choose to attain permanent unconsciousness by taking own
life; (17) sex life shows peculiarities, casual sex; (18) no evidence of adverse heredity,
familial inferiority; (19) often no evidence of early maladjustment; (20) inability to follow
any life plan consistently; and (21) goes out of way to make a failure of life.
Some of these features are a bit curious (e.g., no evidence of adverse heredity, familial
inferiority, often no evidence of early maladjustment, and goes out of way to make a
failure of life). This was perhaps a reflection of the lack of information in the 1930s about
behavioral genetics and developmental psychopathology, a psychodynamic orientation to
understanding abnormal behavior, and an emphasis on case studies rather than on
empirical investigation (Hare & Neumann, 2006, 2008). In any event, these more esoteric
features were short lived. Cleckley revised and expanded his work with each edition
published over the course of his life. By the time of the most frequently cited fifth edition
(1976), one feature from 1941 (free from demonstrable symptoms of psychosis or marked
nervousness) had been split into two characteristics, several (e.g., execrable judgment
and inability to learn or profit from experience) had been pooled into single items, and
Page 2 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
the three just noted (i.e., features 18, 19, and 21) had been deleted. Cleckley (1976)
listed 16 features in this edition of his text: (1) superficial charm and good “intelligence”;
(2) absence of delusions and other signs of irrational thinking; (3) absence of
“nervousness” or psychoneurotic manifestations; (4) unreliability; (5) untruthfulness and
insincerity; (6) lack of remorse or shame; (7) inadequately motivated antisocial behavior;
(8) poor judgment and failure to learn by experience; (9) pathologic egocentricity and
incapacity for love; (10) general poverty in major affective reactions; (11) specific loss of
insight; (12) unresponsiveness in general interpersonal relations; (13) fantastic behavior
with drink and sometimes without; (14) suicide rarely carried out; (15) sex life
impersonal, trivial, and poorly integrated; and (16) failure to follow any life plan.
Psychopathy Checklist-Revised
While Cleckley’s clinical writings influenced the way in which early researchers viewed
psychopathy, they did not lend themselves readily to empirical measurement (Hare, 1968,
1986). The Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) and its predecessor, the PCL (Hare,
1980), arose because of the concern in the 1970s about the lack of a reliable, valid, and
generally accepted tool for the assessment of psychopathy. The PCL-R is a clinical
construct rating scale that uses a semi-structured interview, case history information,
and specific scoring criteria to rate each of 20 items on a 3-point scale (i.e., 0, 1, and 2).
The 20 features are as follows: glibness/superficial charm; grandiose sense of self-worth;
need for stimulation/proneness to boredom; pathological lying; conning/manipulative;
lack of remorse or guilt; shallow affect; callous/lack of empathy; parasitic lifestyle; poor
behavioral controls; promiscuous sexual behavior; early behavior problems; lack of
realistic, long-term goals; impulsivity; irresponsibility; failure to accept responsibility for
own actions; many short-term marital relationships; juvenile delinquency; revocation of
conditional release; and criminal versatility (Hare, 2003).
Based on analyses of very large samples of offenders, the evidence supports a model in
which 18 of the 20 items form four factors or dimensions (Hare, 2003; Neumann, 2007;
Neumann, Hare, & Newman, 2007). These are as follows: Interpersonal (glibness
superficial charm, grandiose sense of self-worth, pathological deception, conning
manipulative); Affective (lack of remorse or guilt, shallow affect, callous lack of empathy,
failure to accept responsibility for actions); Lifestyle (need for stimulation, proneness to
boredom, parasitic lifestyle, lack of realistic long-term goals, impulsivity, irresponsibility);
and Antisocial (poor behavioral controls, early behavior problems, juvenile delinquency,
revocation of conditional release, criminal versatility). Two other items (promiscuous
sexual behavior, many short-term relationships) do not load on any factor but do
contribute to the total PCL-R score. The Interpersonal Affective dimensions and the
Page 3 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Lifestyle Antisocial dimensions comprise, respectively, the original PCL-R Factors 1 and 2
(see Table 22.1) described by Hare (2003; also see Hare & Neumann, 2008). Total PCL-R
(p. 480) scores can vary from 0 to 40 and reflect the degree to which the individual
matches the prototypical psychopath. This is in line with recent evidence that, at the
measurement level, the structure of psychopathy is dimensional, whether assessed by the
PCL-R (Edens, Marcus, Lilienfeld, & Poythress, 2006; Guay, Ruscio, Knight, & Hare,
2007; Walters, Duncan, & Mitchell-Perez, 2007), the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening
Version (PCL: SV; Hart, Cox, & Hare, 1995; Walters, Gray, et al., 2007), the Psychopathy
Checklist: Youth Version (PCL: YV; Forth, Kosson, & Hare, 2003; Murrie et al., 2007), the
Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick & Hare, 2001; Murrie et al., 2007), or
by self-report (Marcus, Lilienfeld, Edens, & Poythress, 2006).
Page 4 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
F1 P1
F2 P2
Page 5 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Note: The PCL-R, PCL: YV, and PCL: SV items are from Hare (1991, 2003), Forth,
Kosson, and Hare (2003), and Hart, Cox, and Hare (1995), respectively. Note that the
item titles cannot be scored without reference to the formal criteria contained in the
published manuals. PCL-R items 11, Promiscuous sexual behavior, and 17, Many short-
term marital relationships, contribute to the Total score but do not load on any factors.
PCL: YV items 11, Impersonal sexual behavior, and 17, Unstable interpersonal
relationships, contribute to the Total score but do not load on any factor. F1 and F2
are the original PCL-R factors, but with the addition of item 20. P1 and P2 are Parts 1
and 2 described in the PCL: SV Manual.
Page 6 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Derivatives
A derivative of the PCL-R, the PCL: SV (Table 22.1) was constructed for use in
nonforensic contexts. It is used as a screen for psychopathy or as a stand-alone
instrument for assessing psychopathy in civil psychiatric and community populations
(Guy & Douglas, 2006; Hare, 2007). It is closely related to the PCL-R, both conceptually
and empirically (Cooke, Michie, Hart, & Hare, 1999; Guy & Douglas, 2006). The
Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL: YV; Forth et al., 2003) is an age-appropriate,
downward extension of the PCL-R (see Table 22.1). Both the PCL: SV and the PCL: YV
have much the same conceptual, psychometric, structural, and predictive properties as
the PCL-R (e.g., Book, Clark, Forth, & Hare, 2006; Neumann, Kosson, Forth, & Hare,
2006; Vitacco, Neumann, Caldwell, Leistico, & Van Rybroek, 2006; Vitacco, Neumann, &
Jackson, 2005).
There is little doubt that the PCL-R and its derivatives have become the dominant
instruments for the assessment of psychopathy and that their use has resulted in the
accumulation of a large body of replicable findings, both basic and applied. Although
some might view such a situation as felicitous, others (e.g., Cooke & Michie, 2001) have
expressed concerns that the PCL-R has become the construct. The proceedings of the
first two meetings of the new Society for the Scientific Study of Psychopathy (SSSP) in
2005 and 2007 made it clear that although the PCL-R might be the dominant measure of
psychopathy, it has encouraged, not impeded, attempts by researchers to devise and
validate other measurement tools, a healthy development for the field. Indeed, efforts
over the past decade have expanded the assessment repertoire to include a variety of
behavioral rating scales, specialized self-report scales, and omnibus personality
inventories (e.g., Frick & Hare, 2001; Lilienfeld & Fowler, 2006; Livesley 2007; Lynam &
Gudonis, 2005; Lynam & Widiger, 2007; Paulus, Neumann, & Hare, in press; Williams,
Paulhus, & Hare, 2007). Many of these measures are conceptually related to the PCL-R;
others have their origins in empirical research on psychopathology and general
personality.
FFM
For example, Widiger and Lynam (1998) translated the PCL-R description of psychopathy,
on an item-by-item basis, into the language of the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of general
personality structure. At the domain level, the FFM includes neuroticism, which assesses
emotional adjustment and stability; extraversion, which assesses an individual’s
proneness to positive emotions and sociability; openness to experience, which refers to
Page 7 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
an individual’s interest in culture and to the preference for new activities and emotions;
agreeableness (versus antagonism), which is concerned with an individual’s interpersonal
relationships and strategies; and conscientiousness (versus undependability), which
relates to self-control, ability to plan, organization, and completion of behavioral tasks.
McCrae and Costa (2003) have further differentiated each of the five broad domains of
the FFM into six more specific facets. The facets of agreeableness versus antagonism
include trust versus mistrust, straightforwardness versus deception and manipulation,
altruism versus exploitation, compliance versus aggression, modesty versus arrogance,
and tender-mindedness versus callousness.
Miller, Lyman, Widiger, and Leukefeld (2001) surveyed 23 psychopathy researchers and
asked each to rate the prototypical, classic Cleckley psychopath on each of 30 bipolar
scales that corresponded to the 30 facets of the FFM. The psychopath was described as
being low in all facets of agreeableness (i.e., mistrustful, deceptive and manipulative,
exploitative, aggressive, arrogant, and callous), low on three facets of conscientiousness
(i.e., immoral and irresponsible, negligent, and reckless), low in the anxiousness,
vulnerability (i.e., fearlessness), and self-consciousness facet of neuroticism (i.e,. glib
charm), low in the warmth facet of extraversion (i.e., interpersonally cold), and high in
the neuroticism facet of impulsiveness, and the extraversion facets of assertiveness and
excitement-seeking, consistent largely with the PCL-R description of psychopathy (p. 482)
(Lynam & Widiger, 2007). As we show later, it also is possible to translate FFM traits in
terms of the PCL-based four-factor model of psychopathy.
Studies that conceptually relate their measures to the PCL-R benefit from the large body
of theory and research that resulted from widespread adoption of the PCL-R family of
instruments. Rather than being concerned about its popularity, clinicians might better
view the PCL-R as an “anchor for the burgeoning nomological network of
psychopathy” (Benning, Patrick, Salekin, & Leistico, 2005, p. 271). This network not only
includes diverse measurement tools but also input from behavioral genetics,
developmental psychopathology, personality theory, cognitive neuroscience, and
community studies.
The enormous increase in theory and research on psychopathy over the past two decades
owes much to the development and adoption of the PCL-R as a common metric for
assessing the construct (Hare & Neumann, 2006, 2008). Its impact has been felt by
researchers who conduct basic research on the etiology and nature of psychopathy (e.g.,
Blair, Mitchell, & Blair, 2005; Gao, Glenn, Schug, Yang, & Raine, 2009; Kiehl, 2006;
Newman, Curtin, Bertsch, & Baskin-Sommers, 2010; Patrick, 2006; Viding, Larsson, &
Jones, 2008), and by those more concerned with the implications of psychopathy for the
mental health and criminal justice systems (e.g., Felthous & Saß, 2000; Gacono, 2000;
Page 8 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Hervé & Yuille, 2007). The Buros Mental Measurements Yearbooks described the PCL-R
as the standard tool for the assessment of psychopathy (Acheson, 2005; Fulero, 1995).
While the empirical support for this view is strong, some investigators believe that the
PCL-R has drifted from Cleckley’s clinical accounts, and that low anxiety and fearlessness
should be included in, and antisociality excluded from, the list of PCL-R items. Hare and
Neumann (2008, 2010a) have discussed these and related issues in detail elsewhere.
Here, we offer a few comments on the role of low anxiety, fearlessness, and antisociality
in the psychopathy construct.
Low Anxiety
Some theorists have suggested that the PCL-R should have included low anxiety, in large
part because it was said to be included within Cleckley’s (1941, 1976) description of
psychopathy (Brinkley, Schmitt, & Newman, 2005; Lykken, 1995; Salekin, Rogers, &
Machin, 2001). Low anxiousness was reported in the Miller et al. (2001) FFM survey of
psychopathy researchers. However, Cleckley was rather unclear and inconsistent
concerning the presence of anxiety. In the first edition he devoted only half a sentence to
the topic, stating that the psychopath is “usually free from any marked nervousness or
other symptoms of psychoneurosis” (Cleckley 1941, p. 239). However, this statement
would apply to the average person, and not just the psychopathic. It suggests an absence
of problematic anxiety, rather than a problematically low level of anxiousness, which are
really very different in their implications for personality disorder. Coverage of
anxiousness in later editions increased to about half a page, although there are
references throughout the text to anxiety of one form or another. Cleckley (1976) did say,
“Within himself he appears almost as incapable of anxiety as of profound remorse” (p.
340), a statement oft quoted by those who believe that lack of anxiety should have been
included in the PCL-R. However, in the previous sentence, Cleckley had also commented
that psychopaths experience tension or uneasiness but that it “seems provoked entirely
by external circumstances, never by feelings of guilt, remorse, or intrapersonal
insecurity.” This psychodynamic perspective suggests that it is not so much a lack of
anxiety that differentiates psychopaths from others as it is the source of the anxiety
(intra- or extrapsychic).
A good deal of empirical literature has indicated that psychopathy, measured with the
PCL-R or self-report, is at best only weakly related to various measures of anxiousness
(Hare, 2003). Hale, Goldstein, Abramowitz, Calamari, and Kosson (2004) concluded that
the PCL-R was unrelated to contemporary measures of anxiety and that the “finding
raises questions about traditional conceptualizations of psychopathy that posit an
attenuated capacity for anxiety” (p. 705).
Page 9 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition, text revision
(DSM-IV-TR) states that “individuals with this disorder [ASPD] may also experience
dysphoria, including complaints of tension, inability to tolerate boredom, and depressed
mood” (APA, 2000, p. 702) and notes more specifically that “they may have associated
anxiety disorders [and] depressive disorders” (APA, 2000, p. 702). The suggestion in
DSM-IV-TR that ASPD is associated with anxiety disorders may be attributed in part to
the confinement of many of the ASPD studies to clinical populations (Lilienfeld, 1994).
Anxiousness is common among persons in treatment for mental disorders. However,
increased prevalence rates of panic disorder, agoraphobia, social phobia, and obsessive-
compulsive personality have also been (p. 483) reported among persons diagnosed with
ASPD in the National Institute of Mental Health (Robins, Tipp, & Przybeck, 1991) and
Edmonton (Swanson, Bland, & Newman, 1994) epidemiologic, community studies. Dahl
(1998) suggested that “these findings clearly demonstrate that Cleckley (1941) was
wrong when he stated that psychopaths did not show manifest anxiety” (p. 298). An
association of ASPD with anxiety disorders could reflect, though, in part, the reliance of
the DSM-IV-TR criteria for ASPD on the epidemiologic studies rather than the PCL-R
criterion set. The callous-unemotional traits of psychopathy have at times correlated
negatively with measures of anxiousness (e.g., Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989), but
psychopathic persons will also report clinically high levels of anxiousness (Schmitt &
Newman, 1999).
Fearlessness
There is an extensive research literature (to which the first author has contributed)
indicating that the concepts of “low fear arousal” or “fearlessness” (e.g., Lykken, 1995)
may appear to explain the psychopath’s apparent social poise and difficulty in staying out
of trouble. However, in their meta-analytic review of the literature, Sylvers, Lilienfeld,
and LaPrairie (2011, p. 134) commented that the argument by Lykken and others “that
psychopathy is characterized by low trait fear remains controversial,” and that it is
“unclear which, if any, psychopathological syndromes are characterized by low trait
anxiety.” The measures of fearlessness used in this research are often heavily laden with
excitement seeking, sensation seeking, and impulsivity, rather than with a lack of
anxiousness. Fearfulness and anxiousness can appear on the surface to be quite similar
constructs, but they may in fact be very different, or at least they are understood to be
different by some researchers. Fearfulness involves a sensitivity to cues or signs of
impending danger, whereas anxiousness is distress associated with the perception that
impending danger is imminent or inevitable (Frick et al., 2000; Sylvers et al., 2011). The
opposite of fearfulness would perhaps be a fearlessness that some suggest is in fact
central to the construct of psychopathy (Lykken, 1995). Persons who are high in
fearlessness engage in substantial risk taking and may then often experience anxiousness
Page 10 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
secondary to their producing and encountering highly stressful events, yet nevertheless
they still engage in the high-risk behavior (Frick et al., 2000; Lilienfeld, 1994). The
assessment of fearlessness often involves measures of thrill seeking, sensation seeking,
and adventure seeking, which generally load on the broad personality domain of
constraint rather than on a negative affectivity domain that would include anxiousness. In
sum, it is not entirely clear whether this fearless, thrill-seeking behavior is best
understood as reflecting fearlessness or an impulsive disinhibition. Furthermore,
Newman et al. (2010) suggest that psychopathy may be characterized less by
fearlessness than by idiosyncrasies in attention that limit the processing of emotion-
related cues associated with response modulation.
There are at least two issues here: the role of anxiety and fearlessness in the
conceptualization of psychopathy; and whether the PCL-R is compromised by not having
items that specifically measure these traits. A recent study by Neumann, Hare, and
Johansson (2012) addressed each issue by adding to the PCL-R items written specifically
to provide clinical ratings of low anxiety and fearlessness (LAF). These items were
administered and scored according to the standard PCL-R protocol. A series of
confirmatory factor analyses revealed that the LAF items could be placed on any of the
four PCL-R factors without any reduction in model fit. Structural equation modeling
indicated that a PCL-R superordinate factor was able to account for most of the variance
of a separate LAF factor. The results indicate that low anxiety and fearlessness may be
part of the PCL-R psychopathy construct but that they are comprehensively accounted for
by extant PCL-R items.
Antisociality
Some investigators assert that psychopathy can be conceptualized without reference to
antisociality (Cooke & Michie, 2001), largely on the misconception that antisociality is
inconsistent with the conception of psychopathy provided by Cleckley and other early
clinicians (see Hare & Neumann, 2008). However, inspection of the items that comprise
Cleckley’s 1941 and 1976 descriptions of psychopathy clearly conveys the important role
played by antisocial behavior. His patients could not be considered prosocial, or even
simply asocial, without stretching the meanings of these terms. In 1941 Cleckley placed
considerable emphasis on alcohol abuse and the problems it caused for the individual and
for those around him (or her). Later editions also described at length the socially
disruptive behaviors exhibited by psychopathic persons under the effects of alcohol.
Indeed, one can argue that most of the features of psychopathy fundamentally are
antisocial in nature (Hare & Neumann, 2010b). Cleckley (1976) stated that he was “in
complete (p. 484) accord” with the description of the psychopath as “simply a basically
asocial or antisocial individual” (p. 370). “Not only is the psychopath undependable, but
also in more active ways he cheats, deserts, annoys, brawls, fails, and lies without any
Page 11 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
apparent compunction. He will commit theft, forgery, adultery, fraud, and other deeds for
astonishingly small stakes, and under much greater risks of being discovered than will
the ordinary scoundrel” (p. 343).
Page 12 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
lack of empathy, shallow emotions) also make it relatively easy for them to engage in
aggression and violence that is more predatory, premeditated, instrumental, or “cold
blooded” in nature (Cornell et al., 1996; Hare, 2003; Meloy, 2002; Porter & Woodworth,
2006; Williamson, Hare, & Wong, 1987; Vitacco, Neumann, & Caldwell, 2010; Woodworth
& Porter, 2002).
Assessment of Risk
Extensive discussions of the theories and methodologies of risk assessment are provided
elsewhere (Monahan & Steadman, 1994; Monahan et al., 2001; Quinsey, Harris, Rice, &
Cormier, 2006). The latest generation of risk assessment instruments largely has
dispelled the belief that useful predictions cannot be made about criminal behavior
(Harris & Rice, 2007; Monahan et al., 2001). Empirical evidence indicates that actuarial
risk instruments and structured clinical assessments perform about equally well. The
former are empirically derived sets of static (primarily criminal history, demographic)
risk factors and include the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide (VRAG; Quinsey, Harris et al.,
2006), the Sex Offender Risk Appraisal Guide (SORAG; Quinsey, Rice, & Harris, 1995),
and the Domestic Violence Risk Appraisal Guide (DVRAG; Hilton, Harris, Rice, Houghton,
& Eke, 2008), instruments that improve considerably on unstructured clinical judgments
or impressions. Procedures that include structured clinical decisions based on specific
criteria also are proving to be useful. For example, the Historical-Clinical-Risk 20
(HCR-20; Webster, Douglas, Eaves, & Hart, 1997) assesses ten historical (H) variables,
five clinical (C) variables, and five risk management (R) variables. Because of its
importance in the assessment of risk, psychopathy, as measured by the PCL-R or the PCL:
SV is included in the VRAG, SORAG, DVRAG, and HCR-20, as well as in the Sexual
Violence Risk 20 (SVR-20; Boer, Hart, Kropp, & Webster, 1997). We note that the PCL-R
and its derivatives reflect static risk factors and are properly used as (p. 485)
supplements to more general risk evaluations. In addition to the instruments described
earlier, there is increasing interest in the role of dynamic (changeable) risk factors in risk
assessment (Quinsey, Jones, Book, & Bar, 2006).
A detailed account of psychopathy as a risk for recidivism and violence is beyond the
scope of this article. However, its significance as a robust risk factor for institutional
problems, for recidivism in general, and for violence in particular, is now well established
(see the large-scale meta-analysis by Leistico, Salekin, DeCoster, & Rogers, 2008; also
see Campbell, French, & Gendreau, 2009). The predictive value of psychopathy applies
not only to adult male offenders but also to adult female offenders (Jackson & Richards,
2007; Verona & Vitale, 2006); adolescent offenders (Flight & Forth, 2007; Forth et al.,
Page 13 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
2003; Gretton, Hare, & Catchpole, 2004; Stafford & Cornell, 2003); forensic psychiatric
patients, including those with schizophrenia (Dolan & Davies, 2006; Doyle, Dolan, &
McGovern, 2002; Heilbrun et al., 1998; Hill, Neumann, & Rogers, 2004; Hill, Rogers, &
Bickford, 1996; Lincoln & Hodgins, 2008; Rice & Harris, 1992; Tengström, Grann,
Långström, & Kullgren, 2000; Tengström, Hodgins, Grann, Långström, & Kullgren,
2004; Tengström et al., 2006); offenders with intellectual difficulties (Gray, Fitzgerald,
Taylor, MacCulloch, & Snowden, 2007); and civil psychiatric patients (Steadman et al.,
2000; Vitacco et al., 2005). Psychopathy also is increasingly being seen as an important
factor in explaining domestic violence (Spidel et al., 2007; Swogger, Walsh, & Kosson,
2007), with the PCL-R being an integral component in the DVRAG (Hilton et al., 2008). In
some cases, the predictive utility of the PCL-R and PCL: SV is at least as good as the
purpose-built instruments, including those of which they are a part (Dahle, 2006; Dolan &
Davies, 2006; Doyle et al., 2002; Edens, Skeem, & Douglas, 2006; Hare, 2003; Kroner,
Mills, & Reddon, 2005; Pham, Ducro, Maghem, & Réveillère, 2005; Sjöstedt & Långström,
2002; Tengström, 2001). For example, in the MacArthur Risk Study (Monahan et al.,
2001) the VRAG predicted violence in civil psychiatric patients, but the effect was due
entirely to the inclusion in the VRAG of the PCL: SV (Edens, Skeem et al., 2006).
The last few years have seen a sharp increase in public and professional attention paid to
sex offenders, particularly those who commit a new offense following release from a
treatment program or prison. It has long been recognized that psychopathic sex offenders
present special problems for therapists and the criminal justice system (Knight & Guay,
2006). In general, the prevalence of psychopathy, as measured by the PCL-R, is lower in
child molesters than in rapists or “mixed” offenders (Hare, 2003; Porter et al., 2000;
Porter, ten Brinke, & Wilson, 2009). However, child molesters with high PCL-R scores are
at increased risk for sexual reoffending (Porter et al., 2009). Quinsey et al. (1995)
concluded from their extensive research that psychopathy functions as a general
predictor of sexual and violent recidivism. Although psychopathy appears to be more
predictive of general violence than sexual violence (Hare, 2003; Porter et al., 2009), its
relationship with the latter may be underestimated because many sexually motivated
violent offences are officially recorded as nonsexual violent offences (Rice, Harris, Lang,
& Cormier, 2006). Not only are the offenses of psychopathic sex offenders likely to be
more violent than those of other sex offenders, they tend to be more sadistic (Hare, 2003;
Harris et al., 2003; Mokros, Osterheider, Hucker, & Nitschke, 2011; Porter, Woodworth,
Earle, Drugge, & Bower, 2003). In their PCL-R study of murderers, Porter et al. (2003)
concluded that “not only are psychopathic offenders disproportionately more likely to
engage in sexual homicide (than are other murderers), but, when they do, they use
significantly more gratuitous and sadistic violence” (p. 467).
Page 14 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
One of the most potent combinations to emerge from the recent research on sex
offenders is psychopathy coupled with evidence of deviant sexual arousal. Rice and
Harris (1997) reported that sexual recidivism was strongly predicted by a combination of
a high PCL-R score and deviant sexual arousal, defined by phallometric evidence of a
preference for deviant stimuli, such as children, rape cues, or nonsexual violence cues.
Several studies indicate that psychopathy and behavioral or structured clinical evidence
of deviant sexual arousal also is a strong predictor of sexual violence (Harris & Hanson,
1998; Hildebrand, de Ruiter, & de Vogel, 2004; Serin, Mailloux, & Malcolm, 2001).
Gretton, McBride, Hare, O’Shaughnessy, and Kumka (2001) (p. 486) found that this
combination was highly predictive of general and violent reoffending in adolescent sex
offenders. Recently, Harris and colleagues (2003) reported that in a large-sample study
involving four independent sites the psychopathy–sexual deviance combination was
predictive of violent recidivism in general, both sexual and nonsexual. The authors
commented, “Because of the robustness of this (psychopathy × sexual deviance)
interaction and its prognostic significance, its inclusion in the next generation of actuarial
instruments for sex offenders should increase predictive accuracy” (p. 421) of general
violent recidivism. Deviant fantasies no doubt play an important role in facilitating this
psychopathy-deviance pattern (Logan & Hare, 2008; Williams, Cooper, Howell, Yuille, &
Paulhus, 2009).
The literature on psychopathy and violence is compelling, but the emphasis has been on
classical psychometric approaches (i.e., not formally accounting for measurement error),
likely underestimating the role of psychopathy in violence. Modern model-based
approaches, including structural equation modeling (SEM), are beginning to prove
fruitful in elucidating the associations between the PCL scales and violence. For instance,
based on a sample of 149 male psychiatric patients within a maximum security forensic
state hospital, Hill et al. (2004) found that the four-factor model accounted for 31% of the
variance in patients’ aggression across a 6-month follow-up. The Interpersonal (.56) and
Antisocial (.35) factors were the strongest predictors. Similarly, using a very large sample
(N = 840) of civil psychiatric outpatients, Vitacco et al. (2005) found that the four-factor
Page 15 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
model accounted for 21% of violent and aggressive behavior within the community at 20-
week follow-up. In this study, both the Affective (.41) and Antisocial (.40) factors were the
strongest predictors. Noteworthy is that these and other studies (see discussions by Hare
& Neumann, 2008; Hare & Neumann, 2010b) indicate that each of the PCL dimensions
plays an important role in the prediction of aggression and violence.
As discussed previously, taking into account the type of violence involved—that is,
reactive versus instrumental—facilitates understanding the link between psychopathy
and violent behavior. A more general issue concerns the severity and temporal aspects of
the violence. We have begun to use modern statistical methods of growth modeling to
provide a better sense of how psychopathy might be associated with violent behavior over
time. This approach has the advantage of separating the level of some phenomenon
(violence) at any given time from the rate of change or growth of the phenomenon over
time (Muthen & Muthen, 2001). Neumann and Vitacco (2004), using a latent growth
model, found that the absolute level of violence was primarily explained by the Antisocial
psychopathy factor and a psychotic symptom factor in a sample of civil psychiatric
outpatients. In contrast, the Interpersonal psychopathy factor predicted the growth in
violent acts over a 30-week follow-up. This latent growth modeling research is notably
different from previous prediction research, which has been primarily concerned with
predicting a single event (e.g., the first violent act after release from custody). A more
dynamic picture can be provided by modeling the growth of a phenomenon over time,
rather than simply trying to predict a single event.
Page 16 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
samples (Babiak et al., 2010). Similar support for the four-factor (PCL-based) model of
psychopathy has also been found with European samples (e.g., Žukauskienė,
Laurinavičius, & Čėsnienė, 2010; Mokros, Stadtland, Osterheider, & Nedopil, 2010;
Neumann, 2007), and new research suggests such support with samples from across the
world (Hare, 2010). In addition, a newly revised PCL-based self-report measure (Paulhus
et al., in press; Williams et al., 2007) has also been shown to conform to the four-factor
model in line with the PCL Scales. Thus, the domains of the four-factor model
(Interpersonal, Affective, Lifestyle, and Antisocial) appear to provide an adequate
delineation of psychopathic traits across an impressive range of populations.
In line with this proposal, new research by Neumann (2011), based on independent
community and psychiatric samples, suggests that a four-factor model, composed of FFM
agreeableness and conscientiousness items set to load on specific interpersonal,
affective, lifestyle, and antisocial factors, provides very good fit to the data. Moreover,
latent SRP and PCL: SV-based psychopathy factors show good correspondence in
predicting their respective FFM factors. The results of this research suggests that it may
Page 17 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
be possible to provide an integration across the PCL and FFM approaches, one based on
clinical tradition and the other on general personality theory, via a common latent
variable model of psychopathy.
Page 18 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
al. criteria were subsequently revised by Spitzer, Endicott, and E. Robins (1978) for their
Research Diagnostic Criteria, which were in turn revised for DSM-III (APA, 1980). Robins
was a member of the DSM-III Personality Disorders Work Group.
The DSM-III criterion set for ASPD proved to be quite successful in obtaining
(p. 488)
The authors of DSM-IV ASPD were concerned with two issues, the complexity of the
criterion set and the apparent preference of many researchers for the PCL-R) (Widiger &
Corbitt, 1993). A commonly reported finding was an overdiagnosis of ASPD within prison
settings and that the ASPD criterion set correlated more highly with PCL-R Factor 2 than
with with PCL-R Factor 1 (e.g., Hare, 2003; Ogloff, 2007; Shine & Hobson, 1997; Sturek,
Loper, & Warren, 2008; Warren & South, 2006), suggesting perhaps that the ASPD
criterion set was not identifying the core, personality features of psychopathy and was
identifying instead simply the tendency to be aimless, impulsive, irresponsible,
delinquent, or criminal (Hare, 1996). “Research that uses a DSM diagnosis of [ASPD] taps
the social deviance component of psychopathy but misses much of the personality
component, whereas each component is measured by the PCL-R” (Hare, 2003, p. 92).
A comparison of the DSM-III-R criteria for ASPD with the PCL-R criteria for psychopathy
was the focus of the DSM-IV field trial, the results of which were mixed (Widiger et al.,
1996). Number of arrests and convictions correlated significantly with both ASPD and
psychopathy in the drug-homelessness clinic, the methadone maintenance clinic, and the
Page 19 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
psychiatric inpatient hospital but not with either ASPD or psychopathy within the prison
setting. Items that were unique to the PCL-R (e.g., lacks empathy; inflated and arrogant
self-appraisal; and glib, superficial charm) correlated more highly with interviewers’
ratings of ASPD and psychopathy within the prison setting, but not within the clinical
settings. The PCL-R items that were most predictive of clinician’s impressions of
psychopathy within a drug treatment and homelessness site included adult antisocial
behavior. Within a psychiatric inpatient site, the most predictive items were adult
antisocial behavior and early behavior problems, along with glib, superficial charm. In
contrast, the most predictive items within the prison site were inflated, arrogant self-
appraisal; lack of empathy; irresponsibility; deceitfulness; and glib, superficial charm.
A revision of the criterion set for ASPD to include the additional traits of glib charm, lack
of empathy, and arrogance was also opposed by the authors of the criterion set for
narcissistic personality disorder (Widiger, 2006; Widiger & Corbitt, 1993). These features
are also central to the diagnosis of narcissistic personality disorder, and their inclusion
within the criterion set for ASPD would have increased markedly their diagnostic co-
occurrence and undermined their differential diagnosis (Widiger & Corbitt, 1995). The
authors of the DSM-IV criterion set for narcissistic personality disorder (Gunderson,
Ronningstam, & Smith, 1991) considered these personality disorders to be qualitatively
distinct conditions, and they felt that the criterion sets should increase the ability of
clinicians to differentiate among these distinct disorders rather than complicate this
effort through criterion set overlap (Gunderson, 1992). “The high comorbidity of
narcissistic personality disorder with other personality disorders makes differential
diagnosis essential” (Ronningstam, 1999, p. 681).
In the end, no revisions were made to the criterion set for DSM-IV ASPD to increase its
coordination with PCL-R psychopathy. However, the text was modified to indicate that
lack of empathy; callousness; cynicism; contemptuousness; arrogance; and glib,
superficial charm were also important features of ASPD. It was further noted that “lack of
empathy, inflated self-appraisal, and superficial charm are features that have been
commonly (p. 489) included in traditional conceptions of psychopathy that may be
particularly distinguishing of the disorder and more predictive of recidivism in prison or
forensic settings where criminal, delinquent, or aggressive acts are likely to be non-
specific” (APA, 1994, p. 647).
The current proposal for DSM-5 ASPD (also titled “dissocial” personality disorder)
consists of two components: four impairments in self and interpersonal functioning (e.g.,
ego-centrism: self-esteem derived from personal gain, power, or pleasure), and seven
maladaptive personality traits, such as callousness, deceitfulness, and impulsivity (APA,
2011). One concern with respect to this new criterion set is that it is untested and not
Page 20 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
clearly tied empirically to any prior criterion set for ASPD or psychopathy. It resembles
prior criterion sets in many respects but given the substantial empirical foundation for
DSM-IV ASPD and PCL-R psychopathy one might have expected a more conservative
approach for this well-established diagnosis by building upon prior research rather than
creating a whole new criterion set. In addition, the distinction between the impairments
of self-interpersonal functioning and the traits is unclear. For example, it would seem that
there will be little difference between the interpersonal impairment of lack of empathy
(suggested by a lack of concern for feelings, needs, or suffering of others; lack of remorse
after hurting or mistreating another) (impairment in interpersonal functioning) and the
personality trait of callousness (suggested by a lack of concern for feelings or problems of
others; lack of guilt or remorse about the negative or harmful effects of one’s actions on
others) (APA, 2011). It might also be worth noting that, at least so far, there is no
proposal to include traits of fearlessness or low anxiousness.
Etiology
There is increasing evidence that broad genetic factors may account for a substantial
portion of the variance and covariance of diverse sets of psychopathy traits. For instance,
investigators have reported bivariate analyses that suggest that there are genetic
influences on the covariance of psychopathy scales reflecting emotional detachment and
antisocial tendencies (Viding, Blair, Moffitt, & Plomin, 2005). Relatedly, both Minnesota
Personality Questionnaire (MPQ) dimensions (fearless-dominance and impulsive-
antisociality) show genetic covariation with externalizing psychopathology in men
(Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, Patrick, & Iacono, 2005). In a large sample of 9- to 10-year-old
twins, Baker, Jacobson, Raine, Lozano, and Bezdjian (2007) found that a common
antisocial behavior factor (composed of child psychopathy traits, aggression, and
delinquency) across informants was strongly heritable. Recently, Viding, Frick, and
Plomin (2007) found a common genetic component to the covariation between callous-
unemotional traits and antisocial tendencies in children. Finally, based on a large
adolescent twin sample, Larsson et al. (2007) reported that the same general four factors
present in the four-factor model of psychopathy (e.g., Hare & Neumann, 2005; Neumann
et al., 2006; Vitacco et al., 2005) all loaded onto a single genetic factor. The variance in
the male psychopathic traits in each factor accounted for by the common genetic factor
was 25% for grandiose manipulative, 20% for callous unemotional, 42% for impulsive
irresponsible, 19% for antisocial behavior (ages 13 to 14), and 30% for antisocial
behavior (ages 16 to 17). For females, the variance accounted for by the common genetic
factor was 37% for grandiose manipulative, 22% for callous unemotional, 45% for
impulsive irresponsible, 21% for antisocial behavior (ages 13 to 14), and 41% for
Page 21 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
antisocial behavior (ages 16 to 17). Notably, in both sexes the impulsive irresponsible and
antisocial facets showed some of the strongest genetic components, consistent with the
very early conceptions of psychopathy.
The results of twin and adoption studies indicate a strong genetic component for
antisocial-psychopathic behavior. Generally speaking, genetic factors are believed to
account for approximately 50% of variation in antisocial behavior, although this estimate
may be influenced by the interaction among genes, or between genes and environment
(Moffitt, 2005; Raine, 2008; Rhee & Waldman, 2002). However, when additive
(interactive) and nonadditive (singular) genetic contributions are assessed, the genetic
contribution remains resilient. Waldman and Rhee (2006) provided results of a meta-
analysis of 51 twin and adoption studies of antisocial behavior that indicated a substantial
contribution of both additive genetic factors (effect size = .32) and nonadditive genetic
factors (effect size = .09). The heritability of antisocial behavior is also supported by
animal studies of temperament. Selection studies (where brother-sister matings are
carried out over many generations) have been successful in breeding rats for specific
traits, including aggression, indicating that part of what is genetically transmitted is
temperament (Chiavegatto, 2006; DeVries, Young, & Nelson, 1997). These results
indicate that (p. 490) specific, heritable genes may be important contributors to
antisocial, psychopathic behavior.
Numerous environmental factors have also been implicated in the etiology of antisocial
and psychopathic behavior. Shared, or common, environmental influences account for
Page 22 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
15% to 20% of variation in criminality or delinquency (Rhee & Waldman, 2002). This
finding is remarkably robust even when compared to other psychiatric disorders with
known environmental components such as affective and substance use disorders
(Kendler, Prescott, Myers, & Neale, 2003), indicating something distinct about the shared
environmental influence on antisocial, psychopathic behavior. The modeling or learning
of psychopathic behaviors is more likely to occur in environments that have higher
incidents of this type of behavior, or that condone antisociality and violence (Eron,
1997). Not surprisingly, shared environmental factors such as low family income, inner-
city residence, poor parental supervision, single-parent households, rearing by antisocial
parents, delinquent siblings, parental conflict, harsh discipline, neglect, large family size,
young mother, and depressed mother have all been implicated as risk factors for
psychopathic behavior (Farrington, 2006). The effects of these factors are not limited to
learning, however. For instance, neglect and physical abuse can generate several
possible courses to antisocial and aggressive behavior, such as desensitization to pain,
impulsive coping styles, changes in self-esteem, and early contact with the justice system
(Widom, 1994). Nonshared environmental influences are also substantial contributors.
Factors specific to the individual appear to account for fully 30% of antisocial behavior
variance (Moffitt, 2005). In short, this is the remaining variance not accounted for by
genetic (50%) or shared environmental (20%) influences. Nonshared environmental
factors may include delinquent peers, individual social and academic experiences, or
physical abuse.
Unfortunately, the interactive effects of genetic and environmental influences are difficult
to tease apart and likely create confusion about what these estimates mean in terms of
causation. For example, the individual who is genetically predisposed to psychopathic
behavior will subsequently elicit environmental factors associated with antisocial
outcomes, such as peer problems, academic difficulty, and harsh discipline from parents
(Beaver, Barnes, May, & Schwartz, 2011; Larsson, Viding, Rijsdijk, & Plomin, 2008). In
addition, psychopathic individuals may receive their genes from psychopathic parents
who also exhibit delinquent and irresponsible behavior, thus creating an immediate home
environment that is likely to model instability and criminality. Concerns surrounding the
interaction of environmental and genetic factors have led to research designs that have
focused more directly at making these distinctions. Studies that explicitly address this
issue have found that environmental factors continue to play a large part in etiology of
antisocial behavior beyond genetic factors alone. For instance, after controlling for the
genetic component of physical maltreatment, Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, and Taylor (2004)
found that the environmental etiological effect of physical maltreatment remained. Thus,
independent of one another, genes and environment account for important variance in
criminal and delinquent outcomes. However, due to the strong interaction between these
components, the significance of either etiological course remains difficult to quantify.
Page 23 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Epidemiology
The prevalence of ASPD in the general population indicates strong gender differences,
with higher incidence in men than in women. Using the (p. 491) Diagnostic Interview
Schedule, the Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) study estimated ASPD prevalence to
be 4.5% in men and 0.8% in women (Robins & Regier, 1991). Similarly, the National
Comorbidity Survey (NCS) indicated substantial gender differences, with 5.8% of men
and only 1.2% of women meeting ASPD criteria (Kessler et al., 1994). In addition, ASPD
prevalence rates tend to be similar across race. For example, ECA estimates
demonstrated little difference between African American and Caucasian races (2.3% vs.
2.6%, respectively), suggesting that ASPD tends to present with equal incidence across
race and ethnicity (see also Oltmanns & Powers, Chapter 10, and Torgersen, Chapter 9,
this volume).
At this point, there is little evidence that there are significant ethnic differences in the
prevalence of psychopathy (Cooke, Kosson, & Michie, 2001; Hare, 2003; Skeem, Edens,
Camp, & Colwell, 2004; Sullivan & Kosson, 2006). Item response theory (IRT) analyses
indicate that PCL-R scores in the upper range (around 30) appear to reflect much the
same level of psychopathy in North American male offenders as they do European male
offenders and forensic psychiatric patients (Bolt, Hare, & Neumann, 2007; Bolt, Hare,
Page 24 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Vitale, & Newman, 2004; Cooke, Michie, Hart, & Clark, 2005). Similarly, IRT analyses
(Cooke et al., 2001) and a meta-analytic review (Skeem et al., 2004) indicate that the
PCL-R total scores function similarly in 1990American and Caucasian offenders and
patients. Nonetheless, we note that there are ethnic differences in the functioning of
individual PCL-R items (Bolt et al., 2004, 2007; Cooke et al., 2001, 2005) and in the
external correlates of the PCL-R and other measures of psychopathy (Hare, 2003; Hervé
& Yuille, 2007; Kosson, Smith, & Newman, 1990; Patrick, 2006; Sullivan, Abramowitz,
Lopez, & Kosson, 2006). In their report of the use of the PCL-R in the extensive
Pittsburgh Youth Study, Vachon, Lynam, Loeber, and Stouthamer-Loeber (2012, p. 268)
concluded that, “psychopathy behaves similarly across ethnic groups and conviction
status. The implications of these findings are straightforward—research conducted on
Caucasian, African American, convicted, and nonconvicted samples is relevant for a
general understanding of psychopathy. There are also several clinical and forensic
implications of these findings; for example, taking into account race or setting should
have little impact when measuring psychopathy or using it to assess risk. Furthermore,
treatment considerations related to psychopathy will not vary according to the patient’s
race or criminal history.”
Gender differences in the prevalence of psychopathy generally are consistent with the
ASPD findings (Verona & Vitale, 2006), indicating that women are less psychopathic (or
at least have lower psychopathy scores) than men (Bolt et al., 2004; Hare, 2003; Vitale,
Smith, Brinkley, & Newman, 2002). IRT analyses indicate that a given PCL-R score has
much the same meaning, with respect to the underlying trait of psychopathy, in female as
in male offenders (Bolt et al., 2004). There are gender differences in the functioning of
individual PCL-R items (Bolt et al., 2004, 2007), mostly confined to the lifestyle and
antisocial components. There also are similarities and differences in external correlates of
the PCL-R and other measures of psychopathy (Hare, 2003; Hervé & Yuille, 2007;
Kennealy, Hicks, & Patrick, 2007; Patrick, 2006; Sullivan et al., 2006), in part because of
the influence of cultural and biological factors that influence sex-role expectations and
behaviors.
Gender differences in personality disorder have often been attributed to some form of
gender bias in diagnosis or assessment (see Oltmanns & Powers, (p. 492) Chapter 10).
However, well-established gender differences in the facets of the Five-Factor Model of
general personality structure (Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae, 2001) support the gender
differences obtained for psychopathy and ASPD. For example, Costa et al. (2001) report
that women score much higher on all facets of agreeableness and neuroticism than men,
as well as on the warmth facet of extraversion and the dutifulness facet of the
conscientiousness. Additionally, women score lower than men on the excitement seeking
Page 25 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
and assertiveness facets of extraversion. In sum, the facets in which the psychopath is
low are precisely those facets in which men tend to score lower than women (e.g., all
facets of agreeableness; the anxiety, depression, self-consciousness and vulnerability
facets of neuroticism; the warmth facet of the extraversion domain; and the dutifulness
facet of the conscientiousness domain). Likewise, the facets in which the psychopath is
high are facets in which men score higher than women (e.g., the excitement seeking and
assertiveness facets of extraversion). That is, the facets of general personality structures
involved in psychopathy are ones that are more characteristic of men than women. Thus,
from a general personality standpoint large gender differences in psychopathy are to be
expected.
Course
Several studies have supported the temporal stability of psychopathic and antisocial
traits. Frick, Kimonis, Dandreaux, and Farell (2003) found that APSD trait dimensions
were stable over a 4-year period in a sample of nonreferred children in the third, fourth,
sixth, and seventh grades at first assessment. In this study, baseline antisocial behavior,
socioeconomic status, and quality of parenting were significant predictors of stability.
Using a large sample of inner-city boys assessed annually from ages 8 to 16 years and
items from a child behavior checklist to model interpersonal callousness, Obradovic,
Pardini, Long, and Loeber (2007) found evidence of significant stability across a 9-year
period, as well as longitudinal invariance. The latter finding is important because it
suggests that the same construct was being modeled across time. In related research,
Burke, Loeber, and Lahey (2007) reported that the same behavior checklist-based
interpersonal-callousness measure significantly predicted PCL-R scores at age 19 years in
a clinic-referred sample of boys assessed at ages 7 to 12 years.
Loney, Taylor, Butler and Iacono (2007) used a large sample of twins and found that the
Minnesota Temperament Inventory (MTI) detachment and antisocial tendencies showed
good stability (see also Neumann, Wampler, Taylor, Blonigen, & Iacono, 2011). Lynam,
Caspi, Moffitt, Loeber and Stouthamer-Loeber (2007) also found moderate stability from
ages 13 to 24 years, respectively, using the Child Psychopathy Scale (CPS; Lynam, 1997)
and the PCL: SV. This latter study is notable for its use of a hetero-method approach.
Also, Lynam et al. (2007) found that in addition to CPS scores, family structure and SES
also predicted PCL: SV scores, consistent with the Frick et al. (2003) findings.
Importantly, across many of these studies there appear to be fundamental longitudinal
relations between the antisocial-tendencies component of psychopathy and other
psychopathic traits. Similarly, Larsson et al. (2007) found that prior (ages 13 to 14 years)
Page 26 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Although psychopathy and ASPD are considered to be chronic, lifelong disorders, many of
the specific antisocial behaviors associated with these diagnoses do tend to decrease
significantly with age (Cleckley, 1941). Robins’s (1966) longitudinal study of delinquent
children similarly indicated that approximately 40% of antisocial youths show a reduction
in antisocial activity in adulthood, and that the median age of clinical improvement was
35 years. Comparable findings have been reported in the psychopathy research, albeit
with slightly higher age estimates for remission of symptoms (Hare, 2003). In addition,
cross-sectional prevalence estimates in prisoners reflect this trend with a linear decline
in PCL-R and ASPD scores beginning at age 20 years (Hare, 2003; Harpur & Hare, 1994).
Simply put, there appears to be a higher prevalence of ASPD and psychopathy in
prisoners between the ages of 20 to 40 years than after age 40 years. However, the
clinical improvement documented is relative to the group; before the drop in criminal
behaviors, psychopathic individuals participate in more criminal activity, have higher
conviction rates, and serve longer sentences than nonpsychopathic offenders, and (p. 493)
after age 40 years, conviction rates drop but remain comparable for psychopathic and
nonpsychopathic criminals (Hare, 2003; Hare, McPherson, & Forth, 1988; Harpur &
Hare, 1994). Thus, while the reduction of criminal behaviors over time is significant for
the psychopath, this “improvement” merely renders them comparable in criminality to
their nonpsychopathic counterparts.
Interestingly, while the psychopath appears to “age out” of his (or her) criminal activity
over time, there is evidence that the personality characteristics that accompany
psychopathy remain remarkably stable. In their cross-sectional study, Harpur and Hare
(1994) demonstrated that the psychopathy factors were differentially related to age;
while Factor 2, which assesses the “traits and behaviors associated with an unstable and
antisocial lifestyle” (p. 605) was found to have the predicted negative relation with age,
Factor 1, which describes the “affective and interpersonal traits central to the classical
clinical descriptions of the psychopath [including] egocentricity, manipulativeness,
callousness, and lack of empathy” (pp. 604–605) was unrelated to age. In fact, Factor 1
scores of the 15- to 20-year-old age group were strikingly similar to Factor 1 scores of the
46- to 70-year-old age group, indicating that the personality characteristics present in
Factor 1 show no significant age reduction. Thus, although criminal behaviors become
Page 27 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
less prevalent over the life course, the traits associated with psychopathy appear to
continue to cause problems for the psychopath long after his criminal career ends. Hare
(2003) reported similar findings for larger samples of offenders. It is likely that this
pattern of age-related changes in psychopathy applies in the general population, based on
the personality literature, for which similar findings have been reported. Longitudinal
studies of the Five-Factor Model indicate that the factors of agreeableness and
conscientiousness tend to increase across age (Costa, & McCrae, 1992; Soto, John,
Gosling, & Potter, 2011). Importantly, these domains are those particular important to
psychopathy, ASPD, and antisocial behavior in general (Lynam & Widiger, 2007). Thus,
independently of the psychopathy and ASPD research, predictions about the course of
these disorders are supported from the broad personality literature.
Treatment
Unlike most other offenders, psychopaths appear to suffer little personal distress, see
little wrong with their attitudes and behavior, and seek treatment only when it is in their
best interests to do so, such as when seeking probation or parole. They appear to derive
little benefit from prison treatment programs that are emotion based, involve “talk
therapy,” are psychodynamic or insight oriented, or are aimed at the development of
empathy, conscience, and interpersonal skills (Blair, 2008; Harris & Rice, 2006, 2007;
Thornton & Blud, 2007; Wong & Burt, 2007). This is hardly surprising, given recent
findings from behavioral genetics, developmental psychopathology, and neurobiology
(Frick, 2009; Gao et al., 2009; Harenski, Hare, & Kiehl, 2010; Harris & Rice, 2006; Harris,
Skilling, & Rice, 2001; Juárez, Kiehl, & Calhoun, 2012; Kiehl, 2006; Larsson, Viding, &
Plomin, 2008) that psychopathy is characterized by personality and behavioral
propensities that are strongly entrenched and presumably difficult to change. Some
authors recently have argued for programs primarily geared toward a reduction in risk
for recidivism and violence. Wong and colleagues (Wong & Burt, 2007; Wong, Gordon, &
Gu, 2007; Wong & Hare, 2005) have proposed that such risk management and “harm
reduction” programs should involve an integration of relapse-prevention techniques and
risk/needs/responsively principles (Andrews & Bonta, 2003) with elements of the best
available cognitive-behavioral correctional programs. The programs should be less
concerned with developing empathy and conscience or effecting changes in personality
than with convincing participants that they alone are responsible for their behavior, and
that there are more prosocial ways of using their strengths and abilities to satisfy their
needs and wants. Early indications are that such programs may help to reduce the
seriousness of postrelease offending (Wong et al., 2007). There also is some recent
Page 28 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
There now is an extensive literature indicating that some key clinical and behavioral
attributes of psychopathy, including impulsivity, poor executive functioning and response
inhibition, difficulty in processing emotional material, and poor moral decision making,
appear to be related to “anomalies” (some would say deficits) in various autonomic,
Page 29 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Perhaps the most interesting and controversial research on psychopathy has to do with
the use of imaging techniques to investigate brain function and structure. The first study
used single photon computed tomography (SPECT) to study functional differences during
processing of semantic and affective words by psychopathic substance abusers at the
Bronx VA Center (Intrator et al., 1997). Psychopathic individuals showed less anterior
activation, and less differentiation between neutral and emotional words, than did other
individuals. A curious finding was that when viewing emotional words the psychopathic
substance abusers showed increased activation in lateral prefrontal regions, which have
been implicated in semantic processing. This study was followed at the University of
British Columbia by a series of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of
processing of neutral and emotional words and pictures (Kiehl et al., 2001, 2004).
Psychopathic offenders showed less activity than did nonpsychopathic offenders in
several brain regions, including the amygdala, dorsal and ventral anterior cingulate,
posterior cingulate, and ventral striatum. However, among the psychopathic offenders
there was increased activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex, a finding similar to that
reported in the SPECT study by Intrator et al. (1997). The authors suggested that the
lateral prefrontal regions were engaged by psychopathic individuals as a compensatory
response to decreased input from limbic regions. That is, what was an emotional task for
most people appeared to be a linguistic one for psychopaths (think Spock on Star Trek).
There now is a large literature on functional brain differences between psychopathic and
other individuals, as well as a rapidly developing literature on differences in brain
structure (see reviews by Blair, 2006; Blair et al., 2005; Gao et al., 2009; Glenn, Raine, &
Schug, 2009; Hare, 2003; Harenski et al., 2010; Kiehl, 2006; Patrick, 2006; Wahlund &
Kristiansson, 2009; Yang & Raine, 2009). The trend is to view psychopathy in terms of
general brain models, rather than as localized problems within single structures (e.g.,
amygdala, hippocampus, frontal cortex). Interestingly, these brain models of psychopathy
bear a strong (p. 495) resemblance to models of the “moral” (De Oliveira-Souza et al.,
2008; Glenn et al., 2009; Moll, Zahn, de Oliveira-Souza, Krueger, & Grafman, 2005) and
the “social” brain (Adolphs, 2001; Harenski et al., 2010). With respect to the latter,
Harenski et al. (2010, p. 141) commented, “A network of brain regions that [is]
Page 30 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
consistently implicated in social cognition has been identified. These include the medial
prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate/precuneus, the amygdala, anterior insula, and the
anterior and posterior temporal cortex (anterior cingulate). Collectively, these regions
demonstrate a remarkable convergence with the brain regions that have been implicated
in psychopathy.” Sarkar, Clark, and Deeley (2011) have provided a recent overview of
brain function and structure in personality disorders and psychopathy, including evidence
that ASPD and psychopathy exhibit abnormalities in the white matter pathway that
connects limbic and ventral frontal brain regions (Sundram et al., 2011).
Although imaging studies have gained a great deal of attention in recent years, the
traditional psychophysiological paradigms continue to add greatly to our understanding
of psychopathy (e.g., Fung et al., 2005; Hare, 2003; Isen et al., 2010; also see the
chapters in Patrick, 2006). Startle probe methodology and ERPs are proving particularly
useful in the elucidation of learning, motivational, emotional, cognitive, and attentional
processes in psychopathy. For example, psychopaths give relatively small startle
responses to loud noises in the presence of negatively valenced pictures (Benning,
Patrick, & Iacono, 2005; Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 1993; Vaidyanathan, Hall, Patrick, &
Bernat, 2011). These findings typically are interpreted as evidence for a lack of fear
arousal in psychopathy. However, Newman et al. (2010) have provided evidence that
“higher order cognitive processes” moderate the fear responses of psychopathic
individuals. “These findings suggest that psychopaths’ diminished reactivity to fear
stimuli, and emotion-related cues more generally, reflect idiosyncrasies in attention that
limit their processing of peripheral information” (Newman et al., 2010, p. 66).
ERPs have long been used in the study of cognitive and attentional processes in
psychopathy (e.g., Jutai & Hare, 1983; Kiehl, Bates, Laurens, Hare, & Liddle, 2006; Raine,
1989; Williamson et al., 1991). In a recent review Harenski et al. (2010) summarized
some of the more interesting ERP findings and their interpretation. For example, they
described a series of linguistic and target-detection tasks in which “psychopaths showed
an abnormal late negativity across fronto-central sites in the 300–500-ms time
window” (p. 138). Each of the tasks required the participant to attend and respond to a
target stimulus. The authors viewed these ERP findings as an indication that psychopaths
give unusually large orienting responses to stimuli of interest. This interpretation, based
on an unusual ERP waveform, is consistent with the speculation by Hare (1986, p. 13)
that the biological anomalies observed in psychopaths “are more likely a reflection of the
particular motivational and cognitive demands placed on them, than of an autonomic
nervous system that does not function properly…(P)sychopaths may have difficulty in
allocating their attentional and processing resources between competing demands of two
tasks. It appears that rather than distributing resources between tasks they focus
attention on the one that is most interesting to them.” The ERP findings also are
Page 31 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
consistent with the influential body of research by Newman and his colleagues on
response modulation in psychopathy (Hiatt & Newman, 2006; Newman et al., 2010;
Newman, Hiatt, & MacCoon, in press; Newman, Patterson, & Kosson, 1987; Zeier,
Maxwell, & Newman, 2009). Newman defines response modulation as a brief and
relatively automatic shift of attention from the organization and implementation of goal-
directed action to its evaluation. The hypothesis is that psychopaths are deficient in
modulation of their attentional responses, a deficit that interferes with their ability to
accommodate the meaning of contextual cues while actively engaged in goal-directed
behavior. That is, they attend selectively to the primary demands of a situation but are
less likely than others to process a range of incidental information that normally provides
perspective on behavior and guides interpersonal interactions and response strategies.
Evolutionary Psychology
Page 32 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
This is more than an academic issue. The interpretations placed on the cognitive/affective
neuroscience of criminality and psychopathy will have a major impact on determinations
of legal culpability (e.g., Gazzaniga, 2008; Mobbs, Lau, Jones, & Frith, 2007). Discussions
of the clinical, philosophical, ethical, and legal issues related to the psychopathy and
legal responsibility are available in Malatesti and McMillan (2010).
Author’s Note
Correspondence concerning this manuscript should be addressed to Robert D. Hare,
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, 2136 West Mall, Vancouver,
Canada, V6T 1Z4. E-mail: [email protected]. Robert Hare receives royalties from
the sale of the PCL-R and its derivatives. Portions of this chapter are based on Hare and
Neumann (2008, 2010a). We thank Kylie Neufeld for her assistance in preparation of this
chapter.
References
Acheson, S. K. (2005). Review of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, 2nd edition. In
R. A. Spies & B. S. Plake (Eds.), The sixteenth mental measurements yearbook (pp. 429–
431). Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental Measurements.
Page 33 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2003). The psychology of criminal conduct (3rd ed.).
Cincinnati, OH: Anderson.
Archer, R. P., Buffington-Vollum, J. K., Stredny, R. V., & Handel, R. W. (2006). A survey of
psychological test use patterns among forensic psychologists. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 87, 84–94.
Babiak, P., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2010). Corporate psychopathy: Talking the
walk. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 28, 174–193.
Baker, L. A., Jacobson, K. C., Raine, A., Lozano, D. I., & Bezdjian, S. (2007). Genetic and
environmental bases of childhood antisocial behavior: A multi-informant twin study.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 219–235.
Beaver, K. M., Barnes, J. C., May, J. S., & Schwartz, J. A. (2011). Psychopathic personality
traits, genetic risk, and gene-environment correlations. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38,
896–912.
Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Psychopathy, startle blink
modulation, and electrodermal reactivity in twin men. Psychophysiology, 42, 753–762.
Benning, S. B., Patrick, C. J., Salekin, R. T., & Leistico, A. M. R. (2005). Convergent and
discriminant validity of psychopathy factors assessed via self-report: A comparison of
three instruments. Assessment, 12, 270–289. (p. 497)
Page 34 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Blair, R. J. R., Mitchell, D., & Blair, K. (2005). The psychopath: Emotion and the brain.
New York: Blackwell.
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., & Iacono, W. G. (2005).
Psychopathic personality traits: Heritability and genetic overlap with internalizing and
externalizing psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 637–648.
Boer, D. P., Hart, S. D., Kropp, P. R., & Webster, C. D. (1997). Manual for the Sexual
Violence Risk-20. Professional guidelines for assessing risk of sexual violence. Vancouver:
British Columbia Institute on Family Violence.
Bolt, D. M., Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2007). Score metric equivalence of the
Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) across criminal offenders in North America and
the United Kingdom: A critique of Cooke, Michie, Hart, and Clark (2005) and new
analyses. Assessment, 14, 44–56.
Bolt, D. M., Hare, R. D., Vitale, J. E., & Newman, J. P. (2004). A multigroup item response
theory analysis of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Psychological Assessment, 16,
155–168.
Book, A., Clark, H., Forth, A. E., & Hare, R. D. (2006). The PCL-R assessment of
psychopathy. In R. Archer (Ed.), Forensic uses of clinical assessment instruments (pp.
147–179). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Burke, J. D., Loeber, R., & Lahey, B. B. (2007). Adolescent conduct disorder and
interpersonal callousness as predictors of psychopathy in young adults. Journal of Clinical
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 334–346.
Buss, D. M. (2009). How can evolutionary psychology successfully explain personality and
individual differences? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 359–366.
Page 35 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Caldwell, M. F., McCormick, D. J., Umstead, D., & Van Rybroek, G. J. (2007). Evidence of
treatment progress and therapeutic outcomes among adolescents with psychopathic
features. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34, 573–587.
Campbell, M. A., French, S., & Gendreau, P. (2009). The prediction of violence in adult
offenders: A meta-analytic comparison of instruments and methods of assessment.
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 567–590.
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W.,…Poulton, R. (2002). Role
of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851–854.
Catchpole, R. E. H., & Gretton, H. M. (2003). The predictive validity of risk assessment
with violent young offenders: A 1-year examination of criminal outcome. Criminal Justice
and Behavior, 30, 688–708.
Cleckley, H. (1976). The mask of sanity (5th ed.). St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
Coid, J., Yang, M., Ullrich, S., Roberts, A., & Hare, R. D. (2009). Prevalence and
correlates of psychopathic traits in the household population of Great Britain.
International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32, 65–73.
Cooke, D. J., Kosson, D. S., & Michie, C. (2001). Psychopathy and ethnicity: Structural,
item, and test generalizability of the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (PCL-R) in
Caucasian and African American participants. Psychological Assessment, 13, 531–542.
Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of psychopathy: Towards a
hierarchical model. Psychological Assessment, 13, 171–188.
Cooke, D. J., Michie, C., Hart, S. D., & Clark, D. (2005). Searching for the pan-cultural
core of psychopathic personality disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 39,
283–295.
Page 36 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Cooke, D. J., Michie, C., Hart, S. D., & Hare, R. D. (1999). Evaluation of the screening
version of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL: SV): An item response theory
analysis. Psychological Assessment, 11, 3–13.
Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., Stafford, E., Oram, G., & Pine, D. (1996).
Psychopathy in instrumental and reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 64, 783–790.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The five-factor model of personality and its
relevance to personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 6, 343–359.
Costa, P. A., Terracciano, A., & McCrae, R. R. (2001). Gender differences in personality
traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 8, 322–331.
De Boer, J., Whyte, S., & Maden, T. (2008). Compulsory treatment of dangerous offenders
with severe personality disorders: A comparison of the English DSPD and Dutch TBS
systems. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 19, 148–163.
DeLisi, M., Beaver, K. M., Vaughn, M. G., & Wright, J. P. (2009). All in the family: Gene x
environment interaction between DRD2 and criminal father is associated with five
antisocial phenotypes. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 1187–1197.
De Oliveira-Souza, R., Hare, R. D., Bramati, I. E., Garrido, G. J., Ignácio, F. A., Tovar-Moll,
F., & Moll, J. (2008). Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: Fronto-temporo-limbic
grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 40,
1202–1213.
DeVries, A. C., Young, W. S., & Nelson, R. J. (1997). Reduced aggressive behavior in mice
with targeted disruption of the oxytocin gene. Journal of Neuroendrocrinology, 9, 363–
368.
Page 37 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Dolan, M., & Davies, G. (2006). Psychopathy and institutional outcome in patients with
schizophrenia in forensic settings in the UK. Schizophrenia Research, 81, 277–281.
Doren, D. M., & Yates, P. M. (2008). Effectiveness of sex offender treatment for
psychopathic sexual offenders. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
Comparative Criminology, 52, 234–245.
Doyle, M., Dolan, M., & McGovern, J. (2002). The validity of North American risk
assessment tools in predicting in-patient violent behaviour in England. Legal and
Criminological Psychology, 7, 141–154.
Edens, J. F., Marcus, D. K., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Poythress, N. G. (2006). Psychopathic, not
psychopath: Taxometric evidence for the dimensional structure of psychopathy. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 115, 131–144.
Edens, J. F., Skeem, J. L., & Douglas, K. S. (2006). Incremental validity analyses of the
Violence Risk Appraisal Guide and the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version in a civil
psychiatric sample. Assessment, 13, 368–374.
Feighner, J. P., Robins, E., Guze, S. B., Woodruff, R. A., Winokur, G., & Munoz, R. (1972).
Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26, 57–
63.
Felthous, A. R., & Saß, H. (2000). Introduction to this issue: International perspectives on
psychopathic disorders. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 18, 557–565.
Flight, J. I., & Forth, A. E. (2007). Instrumentally violent youths: The roles of
psychopathic traits, empathy, and attachment. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34, 739–
751.
Forth, A. E., & Book, A. S. (2007). Psychopathy in youth: A valid construct. In H. Hervé &
J. C. Yuille (Eds.), The psychopath: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 369–387).
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Forth, A. E., Kosson, D., & Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare PCL: Youth Version. Toronto,
ON: Multi-Health Systems.
Page 38 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Frick, P. J., & Hare, R. D. (2001). The Antisocial Processes Screening Device. Toronto,
ON: Multi-Health Systems.
Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., Dandreaux, D. M., & Farell, J. M. (2003). The 4 year stability
of psychopathic traits in non-referred youth. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 21, 713–
736.
Frick, P. J., Lilienfeld, S. O., Edens, J. F., Poythress, N. G., Ellis, M., & McBurnett, K.
(2000). The association between anxiety and antisocial behavior. Primary Psychiatry, 7,
52–57.
Fung, M. T., Raine, A., Loeber, R., Lynam, D. R., Steinhauer, S. R., Venables, P. H., &
Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2005). Reduced electrodermal activity in psychopathy-prone
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114, 187–196.
Gao, Y., Glenn, A. L., Schug, R. A., Yang, Y., & Raine, A. (2009). The neurobiology of
psychopathy: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 54,
813–823.
Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., & Schug, R. A. (2009). The neural correlates of moral decision-
making in psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 5–6.
Gray, N. S., Fitzgerald, S., Taylor, J., MacCulloch, M. J., & Snowden, R. J. (2007).
Predicting future reconviction in offenders with intellectual disabilities: The predictive
efficacy of VRAG, PCL:SV and the HCR-20. Psychological Assessment, 19, 474–479.
Page 39 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Gretton, H., Hare, R. D., & Catchpole, R. (2004). Psychopathy and offending from
adolescence to adulthood: A ten-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 72, 636–645.
Gretton, H., McBride, M., Hare, R. D., O’Shaughnessy, R., & Kumka, G. (2001).
Psychopathy and recidivism in adolescent sex offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior,
28, 427–449.
Guay, J. P., Ruscio, J., Knight, R. A., & Hare, R. D. (2007). A taxometric analysis of the
latent structure of psychopathy: Evidence for dimensionality. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 116, 701–716.
Gunderson, J. G., Ronningstam, E., & Smith, L. (1991). Narcissistic personality disorder:
A review of data on DSM-III-R descriptions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 167–177.
Guy, L. S., & Douglas, K. S. (2006). Examining the utility of the PCL:SV as a screening
measure using competing factor models of psychopathy. Psychological Assessment, 18,
225–230.
Hale, L. R. Goldstein, D. S., Abramowitz, C. S., Calamari, J. E., & Kosson, D. S. (2004).
Psychopathy is related to negative affectivity but not to anxiety sensitivity. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 42, 697–710.
Hare, R. D. (1986). Twenty years experience with the Cleckley psychopath. In W. H. Reid,
D. Dorr, J. I Walker, & J. W. Bonner, III (Eds.), Unmasking the psychopath (pp. 3–27).
New York: W.W. Norton.
Page 40 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R). Toronto, ON: Multi-
Health Systems. (p. 499)
Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the psychopaths among
us. New York: Guilford Press.
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (2nd ed.). Toronto, ON:
Multi-Health Systems.
Hare, R. D., McPherson, L. E., & Forth, A. E. (1988). Male psychopaths and their criminal
careers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 710–714.
Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 217–246.
Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2010b). The role of antisociality in the psychopathy
construct: Comment on Skeem and Cooke (2010). Psychological Assessment, 22, 446–454.
Harenski, C., Hare, R. D., & Keihl, K. (2010). Neuroimaging, genetics, and psychopathy:
Implications for the legal system. In L. Malatesti & J. McMillan (Eds.). Interfacing law,
psychiatry and philosophy (pp.125–154). New York: Oxford University Press.
Page 41 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Harris, A. J. R., & Hanson, R. K. (1998, October). Supervising the psychopathic sex
deviant in the community. Paper presented at the 17th Annual Research and Treatment
Conference, The Association for the Treatment of Sexual Abusers, Vancouver, BC.
Harris, G. T., & Rice, M. E. (2007). Characterizing the value of actuarial violence risk
assessments. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 34, 1638–1658.
Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., Hilton, N. Z., Lalumière, M. L., & Quinsey, V. L. (2007).
Coercive and precocious sexuality as a fundamental aspect of psychopathy. Journal of
Personality Disorders, 21, 1–27.
Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., Quinsey, V. L., Lalumiere, M. L., Boer, D., & Lang, C. (2003). A
multisite comparison of actuarial risk instruments for sex offenders. Psychological
Assessment, 15, 413–425.
Harris, G. T., Skilling, T. A., & Rice, M. E. (2001). The construct of psychopathy. Crime
and Justice, 28, 197–264.
Hart, S. D., Cox, D. N., & Hare, R. D. (1995). Manual for the Psychopathy Checklist:
Screening Version (PCL:SV). Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.
Heilbrun, K., Hart, S. D., Hare, R. D., Gustafson, D., Nunez, C., & White, A. (1998).
Inpatient and post-discharge aggression in mentally disordered offenders: The role of
psychopathy. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 514–527.
Hervé, H., & Yuille, J.C. (Eds.) (2007). The psychopath: Theory, research, and practice.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hiatt, K. D., & Newman, J. P. (2006). Understanding psychopathy: The cognitive side. In
C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 334–352). New York: Guilford Press.
Page 42 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Hildebrand, M., de Ruiter, C., & de Vogel, V. (2004). Psychopathy and sexual deviance in
treated rapists: Association with sexual and non-sexual recidivism. Sexual Abuse, 16, 1–
24.
Hill, C. D., Neumann, C. S., & Rogers, R. (2004). Confirmatory factor analysis of the
Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version in offenders with Axis I disorders.
Psychological Assessment, 16, 90–95.
Hill, C. D., Rogers, R., & Bickford, M. E. (1996). Predicting aggressive and socially
disruptive behavior in a maximum security forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of
Forensic Sciences, 41, 56–59.
Hilton, N. Z., Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E., Houghton, R. E., & Eke, A. W. (2008). An in depth
actuarial assessment for wife assault recidivism: The Domestic Violence Risk Appraisal
Guide. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 150–163.
Intrator, J., Hare, R., Strizke, P., Brichtswein, K., Dorfman, D., Harpur, T.,…Machac, J.
(1997). A brain imaging (single photon emission computerized tomography) study of
semantic and affective processing in psychopaths. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 96–103.
Isen, J., Raine, A., Baker, L., Dawson, M., Bezdjian, S., & Lozano, D. I. (2010). Sex-specific
association between psychopathic traits and electrodermal reactivity in children. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 216–225.
Jackson, R. L., & Hess, D. T. (2007). Evaluation for civil commitment of sex offenders: A
survey of experts. Sex Abuse, 19, 425–448.
Jackson, R. L., & Richards, H. J. (2007). Psychopathy and the five factor model: Self and
therapist perceptions of psychopathic personality. Personality and Individual Differences,
43, 1711–1721.
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Taylor, A. (2004). Physical maltreatment victim to
antisocial child: Evidence of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 113, 44–55.
Jones, S., Cauffman, E., Miller, J. D., & Mulvey, E. (2006). Investigating different factor
structures of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version: Confirmatory factor analytic
findings. Psychological Assessment, 18, 33–48.
Juárez, M., Kiehl, K. A., & Calhoun, V. D. (2012). Intrinsic limbic and paralimbic networks
are associated with criminal psychopathy. Human Brain Mapping, DOI: 10.1002/hbm.
22037
Page 43 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Jutai, J., & Hare, R. D. (1983). Psychopathy and selective attention during performance of
a complex perceptual-motor task. Psychophysiology, 20, 146–151. (p. 500)
Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The structure of genetic
and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in
men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 929–937.
Kennealy, P. J., Hicks, B. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2007). Validity of factors of the Psychopathy
Checklist-Revised in female prisoners: Discriminant relations with antisocial behavior,
substance abuse, and personality. Assessment, 14, 323–340.
Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Kessler, R., McGonagle, K., Zhao, S. Nelson, C., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S.,…Kendler, K.
S. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the
United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 51, 8–19.
Kiehl, K. A., Bates, A. T., Laurens, K. R., Hare, R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2006). Brain
potentials implicate temporal lobe abnormalities in criminal psychopaths. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 115, 443–453.
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Brink, J., & Liddle, P.
F. (2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as
revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 677–684.
Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Hare, R. D., & Liddle, P. F. (2004).
Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed
by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 130, 27–
42.
Knight, R. A., & Guay, J. P. (2006). The role of psychopathy in sexual coercion against
women. In C. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 512–532). New York: Guilford
Press.
Kosson, D. S., Neumann, C. S., Forth A. E., Salekin, R. T., Hare, R. D., Krischer, M. K., &
Sevecke, K. (in press). Factor structure of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version
(PCL: YV) in adolescent females. Psychological Assessment.
Page 44 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Kosson, D. S., Smith, S. S., & Newman, J. P. (1990). Evaluating the construct validity of
psychopathy in Black and White male inmates: Three preliminary studies. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 99, 250–259.
Kroner, D. G., Mills, J. F., & Reddon, J. R. (2005). A coffee can, factor analysis, and
prediction of antisocial behavior: The structure of criminal risk. International Journal of
Law and Psychiatry, 28, 360–374.
Lally, S. J. (2003). What tests are acceptable for use in forensic evaluations? A survey of
experts. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 34, 491–498.
Langton, C. M., Barbaree, H. E., Harkins, L., & Peacock, E. J. (2006). Sex offenders’
response to treatment and its association with recidivism as a function of psychopathy.
Sexual Abuse, 18, 99–120.
Larsson, H., Tuvblad, C., Rijsdijk, F. V., Andershed, H., Grann, M., & Lichtenstein, P.
(2007). A common genetic factor explains the association between psychopathic
personality and antisocial behavior. Psychological Medicine, 37, 15–26.
Larsson, H., Viding, E., & Plomin, R. (2008). Callous-unemotional traits and antisocial
behavior: Genetic, environmental, and early parenting characteristics. Criminal Justice
and Behavior, 35, 197–211.
Larsson, H., Viding, E., Rijsdijk, F. V., & Plomin, R. (2008). Relationships between
parental negativity and childhood antisocial behavior over time: A bidirectional effects
model in a longitudinal genetically informative design. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology, 36, 633–645.
Leistico, A. R., Salekin, R. T., DeCoster, J., & Rogers, R. (2008). A large-scale meta-
analysis relating the Hare measures of psychopathy to antisocial conduct. Law and
Human Behavior, 32, 28–45.
Lincoln, T. M., & Hodgins, S. (2008). Is lack of insight associated with physically
aggressive behavior among people with schizophrenia living in the community. Journal of
Nervous and Mental Disease, 196, 62–66.
Page 45 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Logan, M., & Hare, R. D. (2008). Criminal psychopathy: An introduction for police. In M.
St-Yves & M. Tanguay (Eds.), Psychology of criminal investigation (pp. 393–442).
Cowansville, QC: Editionsyvonblais.
Loney, B. R., Taylor, J., Butler, M. A., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Adolescent psychopathy
features: 6-year temporal stability and the prediction of externalizing symptoms during
the transition to adulthood. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 242–252.
Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2007).
Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult
psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 155–165.
Lynam, D. R., & Gudonis, L. (2005). The development of psychopathy. Annual Review of
Clinical Psychology, 1, 381–407.
Lynam, D. R., & Miller, J. D. (in press). Fearless Dominance and Psychopathy: Response
to Lilienfeld et al. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment.
Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2007). Using a general model of personality to identify the
basic elements of psychopathy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21, 160–178.
Malatesti, L., & McMillan, J. (Eds.). (2010). Responsibility and psychopathy: Interfacing
law, psychiatry and philosophy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Marcus, D. K., Lilienfeld, S. O., Edens, J. F., & Poythress, N. G. (2006). Is antisocial
personality disorder continuous or categorical? A taxometric analysis. Psychological
Medicine, 36, 1571–1581.
Page 46 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Mellsop, G., Varghese, F. T. N., Joshua, S., & Hicks, A. (1982). Reliability of Axis II of
DSM-III. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1360–1361.
Mercado, C. C., & Ogloff, J. R. P. (2007). Risk and the preventive detention of sex
offenders in Australia and the United States. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry,
30, 49–59.
Miller, J. D., Lyman, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001). Personality disorders as
extreme variants of common personality dimensions: Can the five factor model
adequately represent psychopathy? Journal of Personality, 69, 253–276
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality: DSM-III Axis II. New York: Wiley.
Mobbs, D., Lau, H. C., Jones, O. D., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Law, responsibility, and the
brain. PLoS Biology, 5, 693–700.
Mokros, A., Osterheider, M., Hucker, S. J., & Nitschke, J. (2011). Psychopathy and sexual
sadism. Law and Human Behavior, 35, 188–199.
Mokros, A., Stadtland, C., Osterheider, M., & Nedopil, N. (2010). Assessment of risk for
violent recidivism through multivariate Bayesian classification. Psychology, Public Policy,
and Law, 16, 418–450.
Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2005). The neural
basis of human moral cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 799–809.
Monahan, J., & Steadman, H. J. (1994). Violence and mental disorder: developments in
risk assessment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Page 47 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Monahan, J., Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Appelbaum, P. S., Robbins, P. C., Mulvey, E. P.,…
Banks, S. (2001). Rethinking risk assessment: The McArthur study of mental disorder and
violence. New York: Oxford University Press.
Murrie, D. C., Marcus, D. K., Douglas, K. S., Lee, Z., Salekin, R. T., & Vincent, G. (2007).
Youth with psychopathy features are not a discrete class: A taxometric analysis. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 714–723.
Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2001). Mplus user’s guide (2nd ed.). Los Angeles:
Authors.
Neumann, C. S. (2011). Using measures of the five factor model (FFM) to capture the
four-factor model of psychopathy. Manuscript in preparation.
Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2008). Psychopathic traits in a large community sample:
Links to violence, alcohol use, and intelligence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 76, 893–899.
Neumann, C. S., Hare, R. D., & Newman, J. P. (2007). The super-ordinate nature of the
Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21, 102–117.
Neumann, C. S., Hare, R. D., & Johansson, R. T. (2012). The PCL-R, low anxiety, and
fearlessness: A structural equation modeling analysis. Personality Disorders: Theory,
Research and Treatment. doi: 10.1037/a0027886.
Neumann, C. S., Kosson, D. S., Forth, A. E., & Hare, R. D. (2006). Factor structure of the
Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version in incarcerated adolescents. Psychological
Assessment, 18, 142–154.
Neumann, C. S., Wampler, M., Taylor, J., Blonigen, D. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2011).
Stability and invariance of psychopathic traits from late adolescence to young adulthood.
Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 145–152.
Newman, J. P., Curtin, J. J., Bertsch, J. D., & Baskin-Sommers, A. R. (2010). Attention
moderates the fearlessness of psychopathic offenders. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 66–70.
Page 48 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Newman, J. P., Hiatt, K. D., & MacCoon, D.G. (in press). Cognitive and affective
neuroscience in disinhibitory psychopathology: Summary and integration. In D. Barch
(Ed.), Cognitive and affective neuroscience of psychopathology. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Obradovic, J., Pardini, D. A., Long, J. D., & Loeber, R. (2007). Measuring interpersonal
callousness in boys from childhood to adolescence: An examination of longitudinal
invariance and temporal stability. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36,
276–292.
Olver, M. E., & Wong, S. (2011). Predictors of sex offender treatment dropout:
Psychopathy, sex offender risk, and responsivity implications. Psychology, Crime and
Law, 17, 457–471.
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath:
Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 82–92.
Paulhus, D. L., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (in press). Manual for the Self-Report
Psychopathy scale (SRP). Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.
Pham, T. H., Ducro, C., Maghem, B., & Réveillère, C. (2005). Évaluation du risque de
récidive au sein d’une population de délinquants incarcérés ou internés en Belgique
francophone [Prediction of recidivism among prison inmates and forensic patients in
Belgium]. Annales Médico Psychologiques, 163, 842–845.
Porter, S., Fairweather, D., Drugge, J., Hervé, H., Birt, A., & Boer, D. P. (2000). Profiles of
psychopathy in incarcerated sexual offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 27, 216–
233.
Porter, S., & Porter, S. (2007). Psychopathy and violent crime. In H. Hervé & J. C. Yuille
(Eds.), The psychopath: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 287–300). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Page 49 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Porter, S., ten Brinke, L., & Wilson, K. (2009). Crime profiles and conditional release
performance of psychopathic and non-psychopathic sexual offenders. Legal and
Criminological Psychology, 14, 109–118. (p. 502)
Porter, S., & Woodworth, M. (2006). Psychopathy and aggression. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.),
Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 481–494). New York: Guilford Press.
Porter, S., Woodworth, M., Earle, J., Drugge, J., & Bower, D. (2003). Characteristics of
sexual homicides committed by psychopathic and non-psychopathic offenders. Law and
Human Behaviour, 27, 459–470.
Quinsey, V. L., Harris, G. T., Rice, M.E., & Cormier, C. (2006). Violent offenders:
Appraising and managing risk (2nd ed.). Washington: American Psychological
Association.
Quinsey, V. L., Jones, G. B., Book, A. S., & Barr, K. N. (2006). The dynamic prediction of
antisocial behavior among forensic psychiatric patients: A prospective field study. Journal
of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 1539–1565.
Quinsey, V. L., Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1995). Actuarial prediction of sexual
recidivism. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 85–105.
Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial
behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490–
529
Rice, M. E., & Harris, G. T. (1997). Cross-validation and extension of the Violence Risk
Appraisal Guide for child molesters and rapists. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 231–241.
Rice, M. E., Harris, G. T., Lang, C., & Cormier, C. (2006). Violent sex offenses: How are
they best measured from official records? Law and Human Behavior, 30, 525–541.
Robins, L. N. (1966). Deviant children grown up. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Page 50 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Robins, L. N., & Regier, D. A. (Eds.). (1991). Psychiatric disorders in America. New York:
Free Press.
Robins, L. N., Tipp, J., & Przybeck, T. (1991). Antisocial personality. In L. N. Robins & D.
A. Regier (Eds.). Psychiatric disorders in America (pp. 258–280). New York: Free Press.
Salekin, R. T., Rogers, R., & Machin, D. (2001). Psychopathy in youth: Pursuing
diagnostic clarity. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30, 173–194.
Sarkar, S., Clark, B. S., & Deeley, Q. (2011). Differences between psychopathy and other
personality disorders. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 17, 191–200.
Schmitt, W. A., & Newman, J. P. (1999). Are all psychopathic individuals low-anxious?
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 353–358.
Serin, R. C., Mailloux, D. L., & Malcolm, P. B. (2001). Psychopathy, deviant sexual
arousal, and recidivism among sexual offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16,
234–246.
Shine, J. H., & Hobson, J. A. (1997). Construct validity of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist,
Revised, on a UK prison population. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 8, 546–561.
Sjöstedt, G., & Långström, N. (2002). Assessment of risk for criminal recidivism among
rapists: A comparison of four different measure. Psychology, Crime and Law, 8, 25–40.
Skeem, J. L., Edens, J. F. Camp, J., & Colwell, L. H. (2004). Are there ethnic differences in
levels of psychopathy? A meta-analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 505–527.
Soto, C. J., John, O. P., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2011). Age differences in personality
traits from 10 to 65: Big five domains and facets in a large cross-cultural sample. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 330–348.
Spidel, A., Greaves, C., Cooper, B. Hervé, H. Hare, R. D., & Yuille, J. C. (2007). The
psychopath as pimp. Canadian Journal of Police and Security Services, 4, 205–211.
Page 51 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1978). Research diagnostic criteria: Rationale
and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 773–782.
Spitzer, R. L., Forman, J. B., & Nee, J. (1979). DSM-III field trials: I. Initial interrater
diagnostic reliability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 815–817.
Stafford, E., & Cornell, D. (2003). Psychopathy scores predict adolescent inpatient
aggression. Assessment, 10, 102–112.
Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Monahan, J., Appelbaum, P. S., Clark Robbins, P., Mulvey, E.
P.,…Banks, S. (2000). A classification tree approach to the development of actuarial
violence risk assessment tools. Law and Human Behavior, 24, 83–100.
Sturek, J. C., Loper, A. B., & Warren, J. I. (2008). Psychopathy in female inmates: The
SCID-II Personality Questionnaire and the PCL-R. Psychological Services, 5, 309–319.
Sullivan, E. A., Abramowitz, C. S., Lopez, M., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Reliability and
construct validity of the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised for Latino, European American,
and African American male inmates. Psychological Assessment, 18, 382–392.
Sullivan, E. A., & Kosson, D. S. (2006). Ethnic and cultural variations in psychopathy. In
C. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 437–458). New York: Guilford Press.
Sundram, F., Deeley, Q., Sarkar, S., Daly, E., Latham, R., Craig, M.,…Murphy, D. G. M.
(2011). White matter microstructural abnormalities in the frontal lobe of adults with
antisocial personality disorder. Cortex, doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.06.005.
Swanson, M. C. J., Bland, R. C., & Newman, S. C. (1994). Antisocial personality disorders.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 89, 63–70,
Swogger, M. T., Walsh, Z., & Kosson, D. S. (2007). Domestic violence and psychopathic
traits: Distinguishing the antisocial batterer from other antisocial offenders. Aggressive
Behavior, 33, 1–8.
Sylvers, P., Lilienfeld, S. O., & LaPrairie, J. L. (2011). Differences between trait fear and
trait anxiety: Implications for psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 132, 122–
137.
Page 52 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Tengström, A., Grann, M., Långström, N., & Kullgren, G. (2000). Psychopathy (PCL-R) as
a predictor of violent recidivism among criminal offenders with schizophrenia. Law and
Human Behavior, 24, 45–58. (p. 503)
Tengström, A., Hodgins, S., Grann, M., Långström, N., & Kullgren, G. (2004).
Schizophrenia and criminal offending: The role of psychopathy and substance use
disorders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31, 367–391.
Tengström, A., Hodgins, S., Müller-Isberner, R., Jöckel, D., Freese, R., Özokyay, K., &
Sommer, J. (2006). Predicting violent and antisocial behavior in hospital using the
HCR-20: The effect of diagnoses on predictive accuracy. International Journal of Forensic
Mental Health, 5, 39–53.
Thigpen, C. H., & Cleckley, H. (1954) A case of multiple personality. Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 49, 135–151.
Thornton, D., & Blud, L. (2007). The influence of psychopathic traits on response to
treatment. In H. Hervé & J. C. Yuille (Eds.), The psychopath: Theory, research, and
practice (pp. 505–539). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vaidyanathan, U., Hall, J. R., Patrick, C. J., & Bernat, E. M. (2011). Clarifying the role of
defensive reactivity deficits in psychopathy and antisocial personality using startle reflex
methodology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 253–258.
Vachon, D. D., Lynam, D. R., Loeber, R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2012). Generalizing
the nomological network of psychopathy across populations differing on race and
conviction status. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 263–269.
Verona, E., & Vitale, J. (2006). Psychopathy in women: Assessment, manifestations, and
etiology. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 415–436). New York:
Guilford Press.
Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for substantial
genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
46, 592–597.
Viding, E., Frick, P. J., & Plomin, R. (2007). Aetiology of the relationship between callous-
unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190,
s33–s38.
Viding, E., Larsson, H., & Jones, A. P. (2008). Review: Quantitative genetic studies of
antisocial behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 2519–2527.
Page 53 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., & Caldwell, M. F. (2010). Predicting antisocial behavior in
high-risk male adolescents: Contributions of psychopathy and instrumental violence.
Criminal Justice and Behavior, 37, 833–846.
Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., Caldwell, M. F., Leistico, A. M., & Van Rybroek, G. J.
(2006). Testing factor models of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version and their
association with instrumental violence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 74–83.
Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., & Jackson, R. L. (2005). Testing a four-factor model of
psychopathy and its association with ethnicity, gender, intelligence, and violence. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 466–476.
Vitale, J. E., Smith, S. S., Brinkley, C. A., & Newman, J. P. (2002). The reliability and
validity of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised in a sample of female offenders. Criminal
Justice and Behavior, 29, 202–231.
Wahlund, K., & Kristiansson, M. (2009). Aggression, psychopathy and brain imaging—
Review and future recommendations. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32,
266–271.
Walsh, T., & Walsh, Z. (2006). The evidentiary introduction of Psychopathy Checklist-
Revised assessed psychopathy in U.S. Courts: Extent and appropriateness. Law and
Human Behavior, 30, 493–507.
Walters, G. D., Duncan, S. A., & Mitchell-Perez, K. (2007). The latent structure of
psychopathy: A taxometric investigation of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised in a
heterogeneous sample of male prison inmates. Assessment, 14, 270–278.
Walters, G. D., Gray, N. S., Jackson, R. L., Sewell, K. W., Rogers, R., Taylor, J., &
Snowden, R. J. (2007). A taxometric analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening
Version (PCL:SV): Further evidence of dimensionality. Psychological Assessment, 19,
330–339.
Ward, T., & Durrant, R. (2011). Evolutionary behavioural science and crime: Aetiological
and intervention implications. Legal and Criminological Psychology, doi: 10.1111/j.2044–
8333.2011.02020.x.
Page 54 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Warren, J. I., & South, S. C. (2006). Comparing the constructs of antisocial personality
disorder and psychopathy in a sample of incarcerated women. Behavioral Sciences and
the Law, 24, 1–20.
Webster, C. D., Douglas, K. S., Eaves, D, & Hart, S. D. (1997). HCR-20: Assessing risk for
violence, version 2. Burnaby, BC: Mental Health, Law & Policy Institute, Simon Fraser
University.
Widiger, T. A., Cadoret, R., Hare, R., Robins, L., Rutherford, M., Zanarini, M.,…Frances,
A. (1996). DSM-IV antisocial personality disorder field trial. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 105, 3–16.
Widiger, T. A., & Corbitt, E. (1993). Antisocial personality disorder: Proposals for DSM-
IV. Journal of Personality Disorders, 7, 63–77.
Widiger, T. A., Frances, A., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1988). The DSM-III-R
personality disorders: An overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 786–795.
Widiger, T. A., & Lynam, D. R. (1998). Psychopathy and the five-factor model of
personality. In T. Millon, E. Simonson, M. Birket-Smith, & R. D. Davis. (Eds.),
Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior (pp. 171–187). New York: Guilford
Press.
Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (1987). Behavioral indicators, hypothetical constructs, and
personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 1, 82–87.
Page 55 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Williams, K. M., Cooper, B. S., Howell, T. M., Yuille, J. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2009).
Inferring sexually deviant behavior from corresponding fantasies: The role of personality
and pornography consumption. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 36, 198–222.
Williams, K. M., Paulhus, D. L., & Hare, R. D. (2007). Capturing the four-factor structure
of psychopathy in college students via self-report. Journal of Personality Assessment, 88,
205–219.
Williamson, S. E., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1987). Violence: Criminal psychopaths and
their victims. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 19, 454–462.
Williamson, S. E., Harpur, T. J., & Hare, R. D. (1991). Abnormal processing of affective
words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology, 28, 260–273.
Wong, S., & Burt, G. (2007). The heterogeneity of incarcerated psychopaths: Differences
in risk, need, recidivism, and management approaches. In H. Hervé & J. C. Yuille (Eds.),
The psychopath: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 461–484). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wong, S., & Hare, R. D. (2005). Guidelines for a psychopathy treatment program.
Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.
Wong, S. C. P., Gordon, A., & Gu, D. (2007). Assessment and treatment of violence-prone
forensic clients: An integrated approach. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, s66–s74.
Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homicides
as a function of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 436–445.
Yang, Y., & Raine, A. (2009). Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings
in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research:
Neuroimaging, 174, 81–88.
Zeier, J. D., Maxwell, J. S., & Newman, J. P. (2009). Attention moderates the processing of
inhibitory information in primary psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 554–
563.
Zinger, I., & Forth, A. (1998). Psychopathy and Canadian criminal proceedings: The
potential for human rights abuses. Canadian Journal of Criminology, 40, 237–276.
Žukauskienė, R., Laurinavičius, A., & Čėsnienė, I. (2010). Testing factorial structure and
validity of the PCL:SV in Lithuanian prison population. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment, 32, 363–372.
Page 56 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).
Robert D. Hare
Robert D. Hare, Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia,
Vancouver, Canada
Craig S. Neumann
Craig S. Neumann, Department of Psychology, University of North Texas, Denton, TX
Thomas A. Widiger
Thomas A. Widiger, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky.
Page 57 of 57
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com). (c) Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see Privacy Policy).