Reemergent Scientism, Postmodernism, and Dialogue Across Differences
Reemergent Scientism, Postmodernism, and Dialogue Across Differences
Reemergent Scientism, Postmodernism, and Dialogue Across Differences
1177/1077800403259492
QUALITATIVE
Maxwell / REEMERGENT
INQUIRY / SCIENTISM
February 2004 ARTICLE
Reemergent Scientism, Postmodernism,
and Dialogue Across Differences
Joseph A. Maxwell
George Mason University
I do not think there is a theoretical position from which we can reconcile their
differences, their otherness to each other—nor do I think we should smooth out
their “aversions and attractions.” The nasty questions that they raise about each
other’s “project” need to be relentlessly pursued. One of the primary lessons of
“modernity/postmodernity” is a radical skepticism about the possibility of a
reconciliation—an aufhebung, without gaps, fissures, and ruptures. However,
together, Habermas/Derrida provide us with a force-field that constitutes the
“dynamic, transmutational structure of a complex phenomenon”—the phe-
nomenon I have labeled “modernity/postmodernity.” (p. 225)
35
Downloaded from qix.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on May 25, 2015
36 QUALITATIVE INQUIRY / February 2004
Downloaded from qix.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on May 25, 2015
Maxwell / REEMERGENT SCIENTISM 37
pay more explicit attention to issues of validity—the criteria we use for decid-
ing between alternative interpretations, explanations, and theories of the
things we study. Lather and I have both addressed this issue, from our own
perspectives (Lather, 1993; Maxwell, 1992), and it is a topic of renewed con-
cern in qualitative research. I see this concern for validity as the essential char-
acteristic of science; as Donald Campbell (1986) argued, “The social crux of
science . . . is the ability to render implausible rival hypotheses. . . . The central
mode of argument involved is closer to the hermeneutic methods than to
some idealizations of scientific certainty” (pp. 125-126).
I also think that this is an area where qualitative researchers can learn
something from a dialogue with the more open-minded advocates of “scien-
tific inquiry.” A particularly salient “cautionary tale” for qualitative research-
ers is the rise and fall of facilitated communication as a method for working
with autistic and severely retarded persons. This technique, promoted as a
communication breakthrough for individuals who had generally been seen
as incapable of any sort of linguistic communication with others, involves
the use of a “facilitator” who assists the client with typing messages using a
keyboard—messages that often contrast strikingly with the client’s previ-
ously assumed communication ability. Facilitated communication was
initially advocated on the basis of qualitative studies that were claimed to
demonstrate the technique’s validity and effectiveness (e.g., Biklen, 1993;
Crossley, 1992).1 However, subsequent research using controlled testing has
almost uniformly failed to support the view that these messages were actu-
ally produced by the client and has strongly suggested that the messages
were consciously or unconsciously “cued” by the facilitator (Jacobson,
Mulick, & Schwartz, 1995; Mostert, 2001). This controversy gained consider-
able notoriety when some facilitators began generating messages alleging
sexual abuse of the client, allegations that were almost invariably rejected by
the courts because of the absence of any corroborating information.
One of the outcomes of this controversy was a methodological polariza-
tion of the debate; proponents of facilitated communication rejected quantita-
tive and experimental assessments as artificial, intrusive, and disrespectful of
clients, whereas critics dismissed qualitative studies as “anecdotal” and sub-
ject to irreparable biases. (An important exception is a study by Twachtman-
Cullen [1997] involving in-depth qualitative case studies of three facilitators
and their clients. This study paid particular attention to alternative expla-
nations and validity threats and supported the claim of experimentalists that
the facilitated messages were being unconsciously shaped by the facilitator.
The study also documented serious ethical problems in the use of facilitated
communication.) This polarization led to an almost complete absence of
meaningful dialogue between the two positions on how to investigate the
claims regarding facilitated communication and has undoubtedly exacer-
bated the disdain for qualitative research displayed by many researchers in
this field.
Downloaded from qix.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on May 25, 2015
38 QUALITATIVE INQUIRY / February 2004
Downloaded from qix.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on May 25, 2015
Maxwell / REEMERGENT SCIENTISM 39
NOTE
1. My initial reaction to this literature, shortly after it appeared, was that the “ethno-
graphic” accounts that were presented in support of facilitated communication were
Downloaded from qix.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on May 25, 2015
40 QUALITATIVE INQUIRY / February 2004
quite inadequate as ethnography and incapable of credibly supporting the claims that
were being made for the method. I am not claiming that facilitated communication is
never a useful approach or that the messages generated never represent valid communi-
cations from the client; I think the jury is still out on this issue. However, it seems clear
that the technique was grossly oversold, that major validity threats were not addressed
by proponents, and that its promotion raised and then crushed the hopes of many par-
ents of children with severe communication disabilities.
REFERENCES
Bernstein, R. (1992). The new constellation: The ethical-political horizons of modernity-
postmodernity. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Biklen, D. (1993). Communication unbound. New York: Teachers College Press.
Campbell, D. T. (1986). Science’s social system of validity-enhancing collective belief
change and the problems of the social sciences. In D. W. Fiske & R. A. Shweder
(Eds.), Metatheory in social science: Pluralisms and subjectivities (pp. 108-135). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Crossley, R. (1992). Getting the words out: Case studies in facilitated communication
training. Topics in Language Disorders, 12, 46-59.
Jacobson, J. W., Mulick, J. A., & Schwartz, A. A. (1995). A history of facilitated communi-
cation: Science, pseudoscience, and antiscience. American Psychologist, 50, 750-765.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Lather, P. (1993). Fertile obsession: Validity after poststructuralism. Sociological Quar-
terly, 34(4), 673-693.
Lather, P. (2004). This is your father’s paradigm: Government intrusion and the case of
qualitative research in education. Qualitative Inquiry, 10(1).
Maxwell, J. (1992). Understanding and validity in qualitative research. Harvard Educa-
tional Review, 62(Fall), 279-300.
Maxwell, J. A. (1993). Gaining acceptance for qualitative methods from clients, policy-
makers, and participants. In D. M. Fetterman (Ed.), Speaking the language of power
(pp. 105-115). Washington, DC: Falmer.
Maxwell, J. A. (1996). Qualitative research design: An interactive approach. Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Maxwell, J. A. (2002). Realism and the role of the researcher in qualitative psychology.
In M. Kiegelmann (Ed.), The role of the researcher in qualitative psychology (pp. 11-30).
Tübingen, Germany: Verlag Ingeborg Huber.
Maxwell, J. A. (in press-a). Causal explanation, qualitative research, and scientific in-
quiry in education. Educational Researcher.
Maxwell, J. A. (in press-b). Using qualitative methods for causal explanation. Field
Methods.
Mostert, M. (2001). Facilitated communication since 1995: A review of published stud-
ies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(3), 287-313.
Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diffusion of innovations (4th ed.). New York: Free Press.
Shavelson, R. J., & Towne, L. (Eds.). (2002). Scientific research in education. Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Downloaded from qix.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on May 25, 2015
Maxwell / REEMERGENT SCIENTISM 41
Downloaded from qix.sagepub.com at Bobst Library, New York University on May 25, 2015