Gender and Psychotherapy
Gender and Psychotherapy
Gender and Psychotherapy
9
GENDER AND PSYCHOTHERAPY
An evolutionary perspective
Introduction
Although the few reported studies of gender–psychotherapy interactions have failed
to establish clearly that females and males respond differently to psychotherapy,
there are good psychological, clinical, social, and evolutionary reasons for expecting
that they do (e.g., Pajer, 1995; Shear, 1997; McGuire & Troisi, 1998b; Rasgon,
McGuire & Troisi, in press; Godfroid, 1999; Glantz & Moehl, Chapter 8, this
volume). This chapter addresses the topic of gender–psychotherapy interactions. It
begins with a sampling of clinical experience. Selected research and theoretical
findings are then reviewed, followed by a discussion of reasons for expecting
gender-specific responses to psychotherapy. The chapter closes with a set of
predictions based on evolutionary models.
Clinical experience
In the process of writing this chapter, the authors asked clinicians about
those female–male differences they believed influenced psychotherapy outcome.
The majority of those queried agreed on the following points. Compared to males,
females are: (1) more verbal; (2) value emotional support more; (3) connect with
their feelings more rapidly; (4) develop ‘transferences’ earlier; (5) somatize their
feelings more; (6) use less alcohol and other substances to cope with adverse
emotional states; (7) stay in psychotherapy longer; (8) more easily accept the idea
of therapy; (9) are less stigmatized by therapy; (10) less often organize their sense
of worth around personal autonomy; (11) more often use self-blame as a submissive
and non-aggressive strategy; (12) more often seek out supportive others during
periods of stress; (13) more often reflect on and seek out therapy for feelings of
vulnerability, lack of affection, and lack of closeness; (14) more often co-operate
in groups and build networks outside the therapy sessions; (15) more often present
with self-harming and self-attacking behaviours rather than outward aggression;
(16) more often reveal shame. Reports addressing many of these points can be found
196
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 197
in the clinical and social psychology literature (e.g., Barnett & Gotlib, 1988; Brown
& Harris, 1978; Surtees, 1980).
The preceding points are consistent with the view that there are female–male
differences in response to psychotherapy. For example, because females are more
verbal than males, have quicker access to their feelings, and accept psychotherapy
more readily, a reasonable expectation is that, on average, females will have a higher
percentage of successful therapeutic outcomes compared to males (see Troisi &
McGuire, Chapter 2, this volume).
197
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 198
(Jang, Lam, Livesley & Vernon, 1997). Shear (1997) has suggested that differences
in the male and female brains are likely contributing factors to both prevalence and
outcome differences although such differences and their associated effects remain
to be demonstrated. Ethnicity can also be a factor both in diagnosis and response
to treatment (e.g., Kosch, Burg & Podikuju, 1998) and therapist gender has been
implicated in therapeutic outcome (e.g., Waller & Katzman, 1998; Fowers,
Applegate, Tredinnick & Slusher, 1996).
Psychological studies of male–female differences among normal populations
consistently report sex-related differences in a variety of behaviours, many of which
seem likely to influence psychotherapy. For example, women’s sociality is oriented
toward dyadic close relationships, whereas men’s sociality is oriented towards
a larger group (Baumeister & Sommer, 1997). Emotional responses also differ. In
studies of undergraduates’ responses to emotional films, women are more expressive
of their emotions and demonstrate different patterns of skin response conductance
(Kring & Gordon, 1998). Links between the body, self-perceived attractiveness,
and self-esteem also distinguish men and women with studies showing links for
women’s attractiveness but not for men’s (Wade & Cooper, 1999). A meta-analysis
of the literature (Feingold, 1994) finds that males are more assertive and have slightly
higher self-esteem than females while females have higher measures of extraversion,
trust, anxiety, and tender-mindedness.
Turning to intervention outcome, Pajer (1995) has suggested that there are four
factors that may contribute to the greater longitudinal course of depression among
females. These include (1) sex differences in pharmacokinetics and responsiveness
to medications, (2) higher rates of disorder co-morbidity among females, (3) normal
female hormone changes possibly serving as ‘triggers’ for mental disorders among
genetically vulnerable females, and (4) the possibility that females are subject to
unique psychosocial stressors that impede recovery. Mann et al. (1996) have noted
that when male and female alcoholics are treated with group psychotherapy, females
have a better response to treatment as measured by the Giessen test (a test assessing
psychosocial features of personality). Zlotnick et al. (1996) conducted a naturalistic
follow-up study involving a large number of both depressed males and females
in which the authors investigated possible gender interactions with (1) type of
treatment received, (2) dysfunctional attitudes, (3) life events, and (4) social support.
In addition, psychosocial factors (e.g., need for approval) that were thought to
be more important among females than males were evaluated to determine if they
had a different impact on symptoms of depression and treatment outcome. Over
the 18-month study they were unable to identify any main effects for gender or any
significant interactions involving any of the variables of interest. Findings from
this and related studies (e.g., Yanovski, Menduke & Albertson, 1995) raise obvious
questions about gender–psychotherapy interactions.
At best, the preceding review is suggestive of possible gender–psychotherapy
interactions; at worst it suggests that such interactions are either absent or so
minimal that they will elude detection.
198
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 199
An evolutionary perspective
Can an evolutionary perspective serve as the source of research questions relevant
to sex-related response differences in psychotherapy? As noted, there are few
empirical studies in the literature addressing this question. Thus, it is not possible
to test evolutionary-based hypotheses against empirical data. What is possible is
to use an evolutionary perspective to make specific predictions about response
differences.
As a way of introducing the evolutionary perspective, a second look at the
Zlotnick et al. (1996) study will be helpful. This study did not demonstrate gender-
specific differences in response to psychotherapy. A possible reason is that the
investigators grouped study subjects by disorder type (e.g., depression) rather than
by specific stressors that may elicit disorders (e.g., reproductive failure). From an
evolutionary perspective, some stressors can be ameliorated by a therapist who is
aware of their adaptive importance (reviewed in McGuire & Troisi, 1998a) while
others cannot be ameliorated regardless of a patient’s skills in using the tools of
psychotherapy or a therapist’s skills or sex. This view derives from an evolutionary
analysis of the aetiology of disorders and implies that future studies of differential
psychotherapy responses should address gender-specific vulnerabilities to different
stressors that have adaptive relevance.
199
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 200
200
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 201
are often at the very centre of psychotherapy and the substance and intensity of
conflicts will differ from patient to patient. (See Archer, 1996, for a comparison of
the explanatory power of evolutionary theory versus social role theory; McGuire
& Troisi, 1998a, and Mealey, 1995 for examples of evolutionary models explaining
female–male disorder prevalence differences.)
This brings us to examples of how an evolutionary perspective can be used to
predict female–male outcome differences in psychotherapy.
Physical attractiveness
Will self-perceived physical unattractiveness result in different psychotherapy
outcomes among males and females? Yes. Females are more likely to focus on and
be sensitive to physically attractive traits than are males (Buss, 1985, 1987, 1988,
1989, 1994). Further, links between self-perceived attractiveness and self-esteem
have been shown to be greater in females than in males (Wade & Cooper, 1999).
From an evolutionary perspective, physical attractiveness and self-perceived
reproductive potential positively correlate, e.g., the more beautiful a female, the
more she will interest males, and the more mating options she will have. Physical
unattractiveness thus is more often likely to be a concern of females than males
seeking psychotherapy and in situations in which cross-sex unattractiveness is
roughly equivalent, males would be expected to experience a higher frequency of
successful psychotherapy outcomes.
Infertility
Will infertility result in different psychotherapy outcomes among males and
females? Yes. Considering disorder prevalence, studies demonstrate that knowledge
of infertility is significantly more likely to be associated with a mental disorder
among females than among males (e.g., McEwan, Costello & Taylor, 1987). From
an evolutionary perspective, the fact that women invest more than males in both the
process of reproduction and in the upbringing of offspring easily translates to
the view that reproduction and upbringing are more valued and more meaningful
events to females than males. Further, males may engage in greater degrees of self-
denial when they are the cause of infertility. Successful psychotherapy for infertility
among females thus is likely to require therapy designed to provide insights into
a patient’s somatic condition and her response to lost reproductive options as well
as a suitable substitute for reproduction (e.g., increased investment in non-offspring
kin). For males, a diversion of energy into nonparental activities may be sufficient
to ameliorate an adverse response.
Suboptimal offspring
Will suboptimal offspring result in different psychotherapy outcomes among
males and females? Yes. Compared to males, females invest more in offspring, are
201
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 202
more closely bonded to offspring, and are more responsive to offspring’s needs,
successes, and failures. Clinically, degree of investment does not appear to differ
among females as a function of degree of offspring optimality – indeed, there may
be an inverse correlation between suboptimality and investment. Female certainty
of offspring and male paternity uncertainty are also relevant here. One consequence
of paternity uncertainty appears to be a reduced influence on responses such as self-
esteem, guilt, and ‘sense of responsibility’ in association with suboptimal offspring.
To be successful, psychotherapy with females may require a more intense focus on
bonding and interactions with offspring as well as attempts to uncouple bonding and
investment from the mother’s self-view.
202
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 203
Poverty
Will poverty result in different psychotherapy outcomes among males and
females? Yes and No. If there are offspring, females are more likely to be affected
by poverty because of its adverse effects on offspring development. To the degree
that psychotherapy does not lead to alterations in a patient’s poverty state, it
is unlikely to be effective – in such situations depression is not so much a disorder
as it is an unavoidable response to a chronically stressful social condition. With
increasing age, when reproduction is no longer a possibility for women and unlikely
for men, it is unclear if males and females will be affected differently. As they grow
older, males and females appear to be able to make significant adjustments
to declines in resource availability. Thus, it is during reproductive years when
offspring are present that cross-sex differential psychotherapy outcomes in response
to poverty are most likely.
Loss of spouse
Will loss of spouse result in different psychotherapy outcomes among males
and females? Yes. Clinically, compared to males, females suffer greater adverse
effects from the loss of a spouse to which they have bonded. Their response may
reflect different bonding intensities, the resource-related considerations of daily
living, decline in social status (if status was primarily contingent on the spouse’s
social status), and the consequences of increased management of offspring (if
applicable). Migration may also be a factor. Following the loss of a spouse, males
are more likely to migrate and join new groups than females. Psychotherapy of
females thus would require dealing with loss of spouse, social readjustment, and
practical features of daily living. For males, loss itself is likely to be the most
important factor.
203
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 204
Age
Will age result in different psychotherapy outcomes among males and females?
Yes. Female certainty of genetic relatives and male uncertainty are relevant here,
as is the tendency of grandmothers to be more invested in their grand-offspring
through their daughters compared to grandfathers. Thus, females who are unable
to invest in offspring or grand-offspring or whose investment capacities are compro-
mised are likely to require a more intensive and broader-based psychotherapy.
Conclusion
Available data do not strongly support the view that there are differential gender-
specific responses to psychotherapy. This fact conflicts with clinical intuition, which
is consistent with the idea of cross-sex response differences. We have argued that
the introduction of an evolutionary perspective will help resolve gender-related
psychotherapy related questions.
References
Archer, J. (1996) ‘Sex differences in social behavior’, American Psychologist 51:
909–17.
204
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 205
Barnett, P.A. & Gotlib, I.H. (1988) ‘Psychosocial functioning and depression:
distinguishing among antecedents, concomitants, and consequences’, Psychological
Bulletin 104:97–126.
Baumeister, R.F. & Sommer, K.L. (1997) ‘What do men want? Gender differences and two
spheres of belongingness: Comment on Cross and Madison (1997)’, Psychological
Bulletin 122:38–44.
Baxter, L.R., Schwartz, J.M., Bergman, K.S., Szuba, M.P., Guze, B.H., Mazziotta, J.C.,
Alazaki, A., Selin, C.E., Ferng, H., Munford, P. & Phelps, M.E. (1992) ‘Caudate
glucose metabolic rate changes with both drug and behavior therapy for obsessive–
compulsive disorder’, Archives of General Psychiatry 49:681–9.
Bebbington, P. (1996) ‘The origins of sex differences in depressive disorder: bridging the
gap’, International Review of Psychiatry 8:295–332.
Brady, K.T., Grice, D.E., Dustan, L. & Randall, C. (1993) ‘Gender differences in substance
use disorders’, American Journal of Psychiatry 150: 1707–11.
Brown, G.W. & Harris, T. (1978) Social Origins of Depression, London: Tavistock.
Brown, G.W., Adler, Z. & Bifulco, A. (1980) ‘Life events, difficulty and recovery from
chronic depression’, British Journal of Psychiatry 152:487–98.
Buss, D.W. (1985) ‘Human mate selection’, American Scientist 73:47–51.
Buss, D.W. (1987) ‘Sex differences in human mate selection criteria: An evolutionary
perspective’, in C. Crawford, M. Smith & D. Krebs (eds.), Sociobiology and
Psychology: Ideas, Issues, and Applications, (pp. 335–51), Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Buss, D.W. (1988) ‘The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate
attraction’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54:616–28.
Buss, D.W. (1989) ‘Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses
tested in 37 cultures’, Behavioral Brain Sciences 12:1–49.
Buss, D.W. (1994) The Evolution of Desire, New York: Basic Books.
Crawford, C.C. & Johnston, M.A. (1999) ‘An evolutionary model of courtship and mating
as social exchange: Implications for rape law reform’, Jurimetrics 39:181–200.
Feingold, A. (1994) ‘Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis’, Psychological
Bulletin 116:429–56.
Fowers, B.J., Applegate, B., Tredinnick, M. & Slusher, J. (1996) ‘His and her individual-
isms? Sex bias and individualism in psychologists’ responses to case vignettes’,
Journal of Psychology 130:159–74.
Gilbert, P. (1984) Depression: From Psychology to Brain State, Hove, UK: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates Ltd.
Gilbert, P. (1989) Human Nature and Suffering, Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Ltd.
Gilbert, P. (1992) Depression: The Evolution of Powerlessness, Hove, UK: Psychology
Press.
Godfroid, I.O. (1999) ‘Sex differences relating to psychiatric treatment’, Canadian
Journal of Psychiatry 44:362–7.
Jang, K.L., Lam, R.W., Livesley, W.J. & Vernon, P.A. (1997) ‘Gender differences in the
heritability of seasonal mood change’, Psychiatric Research 70:145–54.
Kessler, R.C., McGonagle, K.A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C.B., Hughes, M., Eshelman, S.,
Wittchen, H.-U. & Kendler, K.S. (1994) ‘Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-
III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States’, Archives of General Psychiatry
51:8–19.
205
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 206
Kornstein, S.G., Schatzberg, A.F., Yonkers, K.A., Thase, M.E., Keitner, G.I., Ryan, C.E.
& Schlager, D. (1995) ‘Gender differences in presentation of chronic major
depression’, Psychopharmacology Bulletin 31:711–18.
Kosch, S.G., Burg, M.A. & Podikuju, S. (1998) ‘Patient ethnicity and diagnosis of
emotional disorders in women’, Family Medicine 30:215–19.
Krawitz, R. & Watson, C. (1997) ‘Gender, race and poverty: bringing the sociopolitical
into psychotherapy’, Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 31:474–9.
Kring, A.M. & Gordon, A.H. (1998) ‘Sex differences in emotion: Expression, experience,
and physiology’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74:686–703.
Loewenthal, K., Goldblatt, V., Gorton, T., Lubitsch, G., Bicknell, H., Fellows, D. &
Sowden, A. (1995) ‘Gender and depression in Anglo-Jewry’, Psychological Medicine
25:1051–63.
Mann, K., Ackermann, K., Gunthner, A., Jung, M. & Mundle, G. (1996) ‘Changes in the
self-concept of alcohol dependent women and men during inpatient psychotherapy’,
Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychologie 49(9–10):350–5.
McEwan, K.L., Costello, C.G. & Taylor, P.J. (1987) ‘Adjustment to infertility’, Journal of
Abnormal Psychology 96:108–16.
McGuire, M.T. & Troisi, A. (1998a) Darwinian Psychiatry, New York. Oxford.
McGuire, M.T. & Troisi, A. (1998b) ‘Prevalence differences in depression among males
and females: Are there evolutionary explanations?’, British Journal of Medical
Psychology 71:479-91.
Mealey, L. (1995) ‘The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model’,
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18:523–9.
Nyborg, H. (1994) Hormones, Sex, and Society, Westport, CT: Praeger.
Orlinsky, D.E. & Howard, K.I. (1986) ‘The psychological interior of psychotherapy:
Explorations with the Therapy Session Report’, in L.S. Greenberg & W.M Pinsof (eds.)
The Psychotherapeutic Process: A Research Handbook, (pp. 477–502), New York:
Guilford.
Pajer, K. (1995) ‘New strategies in the treatment of depression in women’, Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry 56(Suppl. 2):30–7.
Price, J. (1967) ‘The dominance hierarchy and the evolution of mental illness’, Lancet
7502:243–6.
Rasgon, N.L., McGuire, M.T. & Troisi, A. (2000) ‘Evolutionary concepts of gender
differences in depressive disorders’. In K. Yonkers & M. Skeiner (eds), Mood Disorders
in Women, (pp. 35–45), New York: Raven Press.
Savin-Williams, R.C. (1987) Adolescence: An Ethological Perspective, New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Shear, M.K. (1997) ‘Anxiety disorders in women: Gender-related modulation of neuro-
biology and behavior’, Seminars in Reproductive Endocrinology 15:69–76.
Surtees, P.G. (1980) ‘Social support, residual adversity and depressive outcome’, Social
Psychiatry 15:71–80.
Thornhill, N.W. & Thornhill, R. (1990a) ‘An evolutionary analysis of psychological
pain following rape: 2. The effects of stranger, friend, and family-member offenders’,
Ethology and Sociobiology 11:177–93.
Thornhill, N.W. & Thornhill, R. (1990b) ‘An evolutionary analysis of psychological pain
following rape: 3. The effects of force and violence’, Aggressive Behavior 16:297–320.
Troisi, A. & Moles, A. (1999) ‘Gender differences in depression: an ethological study of
nonverbal behavior during interviews’, Journal of Psychiatric Research 33:243–50.
206
GC09post 21/9/00 7:20 am Page 207
Wade, T.J. & Cooper, M. (1999) ‘Sex differences in the links between attractiveness, self-
esteem and the body’, Personality and Individual Differences 29:1047–56.
Waller, G. & Katzman, M.A. (1998) ‘Female or male therapists for women with eating
disorders? A pilot study of experts’ opinions’, International Journal of Eating
Disorders 23:117–23.
Whiffen, V.E. & Clark, S.E. (1997) ‘Does victimization account for sex differences in
depressive symptoms?’, British Journal of Clinical Psychology 36(Pt 2):185–93.
Yanovski, A., Menduke, H. & Albertson, M.G. (1995) ‘Analysis of gender and visual
imagery reactivity of conventional and imagery Rorschach’, Perceptual and Motor
Skills 80:1319–40.
Zlotnick, C., Shea, M.T., Pikonis, P.A., Elkin, I. & Ryan, C. (1996) ‘Gender, type of
treatment, dysfunctional attitudes, social support, life events, and depressive symptoms
over naturalistic follow-up’, American Journal of Psychiatry 153:1021–7.
Notes
The authors thank Paul Gilbert, Kent Bailey, and Nancy Brown for their helpful
suggestions in preparing this manuscript.
207