Chap4 - EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
Chap4 - EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
Chap4 - EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN
P3
A clear statement of the experimental objectives will
answer questions such as the following
1. What factors (variables) do you think are important? Are there other
factors that might be important, or that need to be controlled? Is the
experiment intended to show which variables are important or to estimate
the effect of variables that are known to be important?
2. Can the experimental factors be set precisely at levels and times of
your choice? Are there important factors that are beyond your control but
which can be measured?
3. What kind of a model will be fitted to the data? Is an empirical model (a
smoothing poly- nomial) sufficient, or is a mechanistic model to be used?
How many parameters must be estimated to fit the model? Will there be
interactions between some variables?
4. How large is the expected random experimental error compared with
the expected size of the effects? Does my experimental design provide a
good estimate of the random experimental error? Have I done all that is
possible to eliminate bias in measurements, and to improve precision?
5. How many experiments does my budget allow? Shall I make an initial
commitment of the full budget, or shall I do some preliminary
experiments and use what I learn to refine the work plan?
Principles of Experimental Design
Four basic principles of good experimental design are direct
comparison, replication, randomization, and blocking.
Comparative Designs:
If we add substance X to a process and the output improves, it is tempting
to attribute the improvement to the addition of X. But this observation may be
entirely wrong => X may have no importance in the process.
Replication:
Replication provides an internal estimate of random experimental error.
The influence of error in the effect of a factor is estimated by calculating
the standard error. All other things being equal, the standard error will decrease
as the number of observations and replicates increases.
This means that the precision of a comparison (e.g., difference in two means)
can be increased by increasing the number of experimental runs.
Increased precision leads to a greater likelihood of correctly detecting
small differences between treatments. It is sometimes better to increase
the number of runs by replicating observations instead of adding observations
at new settings
Randomization
To assure validity of the estimate of experimental error, we rely on the principle
of randomization. It leads to an unbiased estimate of variance as well as
an unbiased estimate of treatment differences. Unbiased means free of
systemic influences from otherwise uncontrolled variation
Blocking
Blocking is a means of reducing experimental error. The basic idea is to partition
the total set of experimental units into subsets (blocks) that are as homogeneous
as possible. In this way the effects of nuisance factors that contribute systematic
variation to the difference can be eliminated.
This will lead to a more sensitive analysis because the experimental error will be
evaluated in each block and then pooled over the entire experiment
Attributes of a Good Experiment
2500
2000
Average
1500 Max.
Safety
1000
500
0
0 5 10 15 20
Lead-time
Overtime
Variable
Constant Production
Production
Inventories
4.2. SCREENING OF IMPORTANT VARIABLES
• A structured, organized method
– To determine whether some program or treatment
causes some outcome or outcomes to occur.
• If X, then Y
– Because there may be lots of reasons, other than
the program, for why you observed the outcome,
• If not X, then not Y needs to be addressed, too
• Identify the variable you will change – the independent variable
• Identify the variable you will measure – the dependent variable
• Write an experimental hypothesis – a statement predicting how
the X will affect the Y
• Is it directional or bidirectional? Why?
• Write a null hypothesis – a statement predicting that the Y will
have no effect on the X
Key terms
• Experiment: Process of collecting sample data
Stratification
• Suppose that some Y measurements will be made in the morning and some in
the afternoon.
• If you anticipate a difference between morning and afternoon measurements:
– Ensure that within each period, there are equal numbers of subjects
in each treatment group.
– Take account of the difference between periods in your analysis.
• This is sometimes called “blocking”.
4.4. Experimental Design
Factorial experiments
Suppose we are interested in the effect of both salt water and a
high-fat diet on blood pressure.
Ideally: look at all 4 treatments in one experiment.
Plain water Normal diet
Salt water High-fat diet
Interactions
– We can learn more.
– More efficient than
doing all single-
factor experiments.
Data presentation
Good plot Bad plot
40
35
30
25
20
15
10
5
0
A B
Group
If salt
has an effect
P-values
• A P-value is the probability of obtaining data as extreme as was
observed, if the null hypothesis were true (i.e., if the treatment
has no effect).
• = true difference in
average BP (the treatment
effect).
• H0: = 0 (i.e., no effect)
• Test statistic, D.
• If |D| > C, reject H0.
• C chosen so that the chance
you reject H0, if H0 is true, is
5%
Statistical power
6 per
group:
12 per
group:
Effect of the effect
= 8.5:
= 12.5:
Various effects
• Desired power sample size
Final conclusions
• Experiments should be designed.
• Good design and good analysis can lead to reduced sample
sizes.
• Consult an expert on both the analysis and the design of your
experiment.