Chapter 05
Chapter 05
Chapter 05
Editors: Beth Shook, Katie Nelson, Kelsie Aguilera and Lara Braff
2019
ISBN – 978-1-931303-63-7
www.explorations.americananthro.org
Chapter 5: Meet the Living Primates
Stephanie Etting, Ph.D., Sacramento City College
Learning Objectives
One of the best parts of teaching anthropology for me is getting to spend time at zoos watching primates. What I also
find interesting is watching people watch primates. I have very often heard a parent and child walk up to a chimpanzee
enclosure and exclaim “Look at the monkeys!” The parent and child often don’t know that a chimpanzee is not a monkey,
nor are they likely to know that chimpanzees share more than 98% of their DNA with us. What strikes me as significant
is that, although most people do not know the difference between a monkey, an ape, and a lemur, they nonetheless
recognize something in the animals as being similar to themselves. What people probably mean when they say “monkey”
is actually “primate,” a term that refers to all organisms classified within the Order Primates and also the subject of
this chapter. You may be wondering why a field dedicated to the study of humans would include the study of non-
human animals. Because humans are primates, we share a wide range of behavioral and morphological traits with the
other species who also fall into this group. In Chapter 2, you learned about the nature of Linnaean classification, the
system we use for organizing life-forms. Here, we focus on the organization and diversity within the Order Primates.
The term Order Primates dates back to 1758 when, in his tenth edition of Systema Naturae, Carolus Linnaeus put humans,
“simia” monkeys and apes), “lemurs” lemurs and colugos), and some bats into one of eight groups of mammals. Linnaeus
was wrong in including colugos now in Order Dermoptera) and bats now in Order Chiroptera), but the grouping of
humans with the then-known non-human primates was significant in that by doing so Linnaeus formally recognized
the affinities between humans and these non-human taxa. In fact, acknowledgment of similarities between humans and
non-humans dates back far earlier than Linnaeus see the Special Topic box), yet it was only more recently that we
attained the genetic data to back up our intuition.
WHAT IS A PRIMATE?
Primates are one of at least twenty Orders belonging to the Class Mammalia. All members of this class share certain
characteristics, including, among other things, having fur or hair, producing milk from mammary glands, and being
warm-blooded. There are three types of mammals: monotremes, marsupials, and placental mammals. Monotremes
are the most primitive of the mammals, meaning they have retained more ancient traits than marsupials or placental
mammals, and so, monotremes are characterized by some unusual traits. Monotremes, which include echidnas and
duck-billed platypuses, lay eggs rather than give birth to live young. Once the young hatch, they lap up milk produced
Types of Traits
When evaluating relationships between different groups of primates, we use key traits that allow us to determine which
species are most closely related to one another. Traits can be either primitive or derived. Primitive traits are those that
a taxon has because it has inherited the trait from a distant ancestor. For example, all primates have body hair because
we are mammals and all mammals share an ancestor hundreds of millions of years ago that had body hair. This trait
has been passed down to all mammals from this shared ancestor, so all mammals alive today have body hair. Derived
traits are those that have been more recently altered. This type of trait is most useful when we are trying to distinguish
one group from another because derived traits tell us which taxa are more closely related to each other. For example,
humans walk on two legs. The many adaptations that humans possess which allow us to move in this way evolved after
humans split from the Genus Pan. This means that when we find fossil taxa that share derived traits for walking on two
legs, we can conclude that they are likely more closely related to humans than to chimpanzees and bonobos.
There are a couple of other important points about primitive and derived traits that will become apparent as we discuss
primate diversity. First, the terms primitive and derived are relative terms. This means that depending on what taxa
are being compared, a trait can be either one. For example, in the previous section, body hair was used as an example
for a primitive trait among primates. A ll mammals have body hair because we share a distant ancestor who had this
trait. The presence of body hair therefore doesn’t allow you to distinguish whether monkeys are more closely related
to apes or lemurs because they all share this trait. However, if we are comparing mammals to birds and fish, then body
hair becomes a derived trait of mammals. It evolved after mammals diverged from birds and fish, and it tells us that
all mammals are more closely related to each other than they are to birds or fish. The second important point is that
very often when one lineage splits into two, one taxon will stay more similar to the last common ancestor in retaining
more primitive traits, whereas the other lineage will usually become more different from the last common ancestor
by developing more derived traits. This will become very apparent when we discuss the two suborders of primates,
Strepsirrhini and Haplorrhini. When these two lineages diverged, strepsirrhines retained more primitive traits
those present in the ancestor of primates) and haplorrhines developed more derived traits became more different
from the ancestor of primates).
There are two other types of traits that will be relevant to our discussions here: generalized and specialized traits.
Generalized traits are those characteristics that are useful for a wide range of things. Having opposable thumbs that
go in a different direction than the rest of your fingers is a very useful, generalized trait. You can hold a pen, grab a
branch, peel a banana, or text your friends all thanks to your opposable thumbs. Specialized traits are those that have
been modified for a specific purpose. These traits may not have a wide range of uses, but they will be very efficient at
their job. Hooves in horses are a good example of a specialized trait. Horses cannot grasp objects with their hooves, but
hooves allow horses to run very quickly on the ground on all fours. You can think of generalized traits as a Swiss Army
The Order Primates is distinguished from other groups of mammals in having a suite of characteristics. This means that
there is no individual trait that you can use to instantly identify an animal as a primate; instead, you have to look for
animals that possess a collection of traits. What this also means is that each individual trait we discuss may be found in
non-primates, but if you see an animal that has most or all of these traits, there is a good chance it is a primate.
One area in which the Order Primates is most distinguished from other organisms regards traits related to our senses,
especially our vision. Compared to other animals, primates rely on vision as a primary sense. Our heavy reliance on
vision is reflected in many areas of our anatomy and behavior. All primates have eyes that face forward with convergent
overlapping) visual fields. This means that if you cover one eye with your hand, you can still see most of the room with
your other one. This also means that we cannot see on the sides or behind us as well as some other animals can. In
order to protect the sides of the eyes from the muscles we use for chewing, all primates have at least a postorbital
bar, a bony ring around the outside of the eye Figure 5.1). Some primate taxa have more convergent eyes than others,
so those primates need extra protection for their eyes. As a result, animals with greater orbital convergence will have
a postorbital plate or postorbital closure in addition to the bar Figure 5.1). The postorbital bar is a derived trait of
primates, appearing in our earliest ancestors, which you will read more about in Chapter 8.
Figure 5.1 All primates have some form of bony protection around their eyes. Some have a postorbital bar only (right),
but many have full postorbital closure, also called a postorbital plate, that completely protects the back of the eye socket
(left).
Primates also differ from other mammals in the size and complexity of our brains. All primates have brains that are
larger than you would expect when compared to other mammals of the same size. On average, primates have brains
that are twice as big for their body size as you would expect when compared to other mammals. Not unexpectedly, the
visual centers of the brain are larger in primates and the wiring is different from that in other animals, reflecting our
reliance on this sense. The neocortex, which is used for higher functions like consciousness and language in humans, as
well as sensory perception and spatial awareness, is also larger in primates relative to other animals. In non-primates
this part of the brain is often smooth, but in primates it is made up of many folds which increase the surface area. It
has been proposed that the more complex neocortex of primates is related to diet, with fruit-eating primates having
larger relative brain sizes than leaf-eating primates, due to the more challenging cognitive demands required to find and
process fruits Clutton‐Brock and Harvey 1980). An alternative hypothesis argues that larger brain size is necessary for
navigating the complexities of primate social life, with larger brains occurring in species who live in larger, more complex
groups relative to those living in pairs or solitarily Dunbar 1998). There seems to be support for both hypotheses, as
large brains are a benefit under both sets of selective pressures.
The primate visual system uses a lot of energy, so primates have compensated by cutting back on other sensory systems,
particularly our sense of smell. Compared to other mammals, primates have relatively reduced snouts. This is another
derived trait of primates that appears even in our earliest ancestors. As we will discuss, there is variation across primate
taxa in how much snouts are reduced. Those with a better sense of smell usually have poorer vision than those with
a relatively dull sense of smell. The reason for this is that all organisms have a limited amount of energy to spend on
running our bodies, so we make evolutionary trade-offs, because energy spent on one trait must mean cutting back
on energy spent on another. With regards to primate senses, primates with better vision more convergent eyes, better
visual acuity, etc.) are spending more energy on vision and thus will have poorer smell and a shorter snout). Primates
who spend less energy on vision less convergent eyes, poorer visual acuity, etc.) will have a better sense of smell and a
longer snout).
Lastly, primates share some behavioral and ecological traits. Primates are very social animals, and all primates, even
those that search for food alone, have strong social networks with others of their species. Indeed, social networks in
primates have been shown to be crucial in times of stress and to enhance reproductive success Silk et al. 2009). Unlike
many animals, primates do not migrate. This means that primates stay in a relatively stable area for their whole life, often
interacting with the same individuals for their long lives. The long-term relationships that primates form with others
of their species lead to complex and fascinating social behaviors, which you will read about in Chapter 6. Finally, non-
human primates show a clear preference for tropical regions of the world. Most primates are found between the Tropic
of Cancer and the Tropic of Capricorn, with only a few taxa living outside of these regions. You can see a summary of
the primate suite of traits in Figure 5.3.
Convergent eyes
Post-orbital bar
Short snouts
Pentadactyly
Flattened nails
Tactile pads
Highly arboreal
Large brains
Figure 5.3 Primate Traits at a Glance: This table summarizes the suite of traits that differentiate primates from other
mammals.
When trying to place primate species into specific taxonomic groups, we use a variety of dental characteristics,
locomotor adaptations, and behavioral adaptations. Differences in these characteristics across groups reflect
constraints of evolutionary history as well as variation in adaptations.
Dental Characteristics
Teeth may not seem like the most exciting topic with which to start, but we can learn a tremendous amount of
information about an organism from its teeth. First, teeth are vital to survival. Wild animals do not have the benefit of
knives and forks, and so rely primarily on their teeth to process their food. Because of this, teeth of any species have
evolved to reflect what that organism eats and so tell us directly about their diet. Second, variation in tooth size, shape,
and number tells us a lot about an organism’s evolutionary history. Some taxa have more teeth than others or different
forms of teeth than others. Furthermore, differences in teeth between males and females can tell us about competition
over mates see Chapter 6). Lastly, teeth preserve really well in the fossil record. Enamel is hard, and there is little meat
on jaws so carnivores and scavengers often leave them behind. Because of this, very often we find a lot of fossil jaws and
teeth, and so we need to be able to learn as much as we can from those pieces.
As discussed before, primate taxa can vary in the numbers and forms of teeth they have.
We determine the number of each type of tooth an organism has by its dental formula.
The dental formula tells you how many incisors, canines, premolars, and molars are in
each quadrant of the mouth half of the top or bottom). For example, Figure 5.5 shows
half of the lower teeth of a human. You can see that in half of the mandible, there are two
incisors, one canine, two premolars, and three molars. This dental formula is written as
2:1:2:3. The first number represents the number of incisors, followed by the number of
canines, premolars, and molars). Some early fossil primates had a dental formula of
2:1:4:3, but among the living primates, none have more teeth than can be found in a 2:1:3:3
dental formula. Many have fewer teeth, however, and some have a different dental
formula on the top than they do on the bottom.
To determine the dental formula, you need to be able to identify the different types of
teeth. You can recognize incisors because they often look like spatulas with a flat, blade-
like surface. Premolars and molars can be differentiated by the number of cusps that they
have. Cusps are the little bumps which in some species can be quite sharp) that you can
feel with your tongue on the surface of your back teeth. Premolars are smaller than
Figure 5.5 This drawing shows molars and, in primates, often have one or two cusps on them. Molars are bigger, with a
half of the human mandible.
With the four types of teeth larger chewing surface, and so have more cusps. Depending on the species of primate
labeled, you can determine that and whether you’re looking at upper or lower teeth, molars can have between three and
the dental formula is 2:1:2:3.
five cusps. There is even one extinct primate Oreopithecus) who had six cusps on its
molars. Molar cusps can also vary between taxa in how they are arranged, as you will
learn more about later in this chapter. Canines are often easy to distinguish because they are usually much longer and
more conical than the other teeth. This is not always the case, however, as you will see when you read about the teeth
of lemurs and lorises.
Teeth also tell us directly about an organism’s diet. Primates are known to eat a wide range of plant parts, insects,
gums, and, rarely, meat. While all primates eat a variety of foods, what differs among primates are the proportions
Frugivores
Plants want animals to eat their fruits because, in doing so, animals eat the seeds of the fruit and then disperse them far
away from the parent plant. Because plants want animals to eat the fruit, plants often “advertise” fruits by making them
colorful and easy to spot, full of easy-to-digest sugars that make them taste good—and, often, easy to chew and digest
not being too fibrous or tough). For these reasons, frugivores often do not need a lot of specialized traits to consume a
diet rich in fruits Figure 5.6a). Their molars usually have a broad chewing surface with low, rounded cusps referred to
as bunodont molars). Frugivores also often have large incisors for slicing through the outer coatings on fruit. Primates
that eat fruit tend to have stomachs, colons, and small intestines that are intermediate in terms of size and complexity
between insectivores and folivores Chivers and Hladik 1980). They are also usually of intermediate body size between
the other two dietary types. Because fruit does not contain protein, frugivores must supplement their diet with protein
from insects and/or leaves. Some frugivorous primates get protein by eating seeds and so have evolved to have thicker
enamel on their teeth to protect them from excessive wear.
Figure 5.6a Frugivores are characterized by large incisors, bunodont molars, and digestive tracts that are intermediate in
complexity between the other two dietary types.
Insectivores
Insects can be difficult to find and catch but are not typically difficult to chew. As a result, insectivorous primates usually
have small molars with pointed cusps that allow them to puncture the exoskeleton of the insects Figure 5.6b). Once the
outer shells of the insects are punctured, insects are not difficult to digest, so insectivores have simple stomachs and
colons and a long small intestine. Nutritionally, insects provide a lot of protein and fat but are not plentiful enough in
the environment to support large-bodied animals, so insectivores are usually the smallest of the primates.
Figure 5.6b Insectivores need sharp, pointed molar cusps in order to break through the exoskeletons of insects. Insects are
easy to digest, so these primates have simple digestive tracts.
Folivores
Unlike with fruits, plants do not want animals to eat their leaves. Leaves are the way plants get their energy from
the sun, therefore, plants evolved to make their leaves very difficult for animals to eat. Leaves often have toxins in
them, taste bitter, are very fibrous and difficult to chew, and are made of large cellulose molecules that are difficult
to break down into usable sugars. Because of these defenses, animals who eat leaves need a lot of specialized traits
Figure 5.6c). Folivorous primates have broad molars with high, sharp cusps connected by shearing crests. These molar
traits allow folivores to physically break down fibrous leaves when chewing. Folivores then have to chemically break
down cellulose molecules into usable energy, so these animals need specialized digestive systems. Some folivores have
complex stomachs with multiple compartments, but all leaf eaters have large, long intestines and special gut bacteria
that can break up cellulose. Folivores are usually the largest bodied of all primates, and they spend a large portion of
their day digesting their food, so they are often less active than frugivores or insectivores.
High, sharp
Smaller Complex
molar cusps with
incisors digestive tract
shearing crests
Figure 5.6c In order to derive energy from leaves, folivores have smaller incisors, high, sharp molar cusps connected by
shearing crests and complex digestive tracts filled with specialized bacteria.
Behavioral Adaptations
Chapter 6 is entirely dedicated to primate behavior, so only broad differences related to taxonomic classification will
Locomotor Adaptations
Figure 5.7 An example of a vertical clinger and leaper. Note the longer legs than arms, long
lower back and long fingers and toes. This vertical clinger and leaper doesn’t have a tail, but
most have long tails as well.
Finally, primate groups vary in their adaptations for different forms of locomotion, or how they move around. Living
primates are known to move by vertical clinging and leaping, quadrupedalism, brachiation, and bipedalism. Vertical
clinging and leaping is when an animal grasps a vertical branch with its body upright, pushes off with long hind legs and
then lands on another vertical support branch Figure 5.7). Animals who move in this way usually have longer legs than
arms, long fingers and toes, and smaller bodies. Vertical clinger leapers also tend to have elongated ankle bones, which
serve as a lever to help them push off with their legs and leap to another branch.
Figure 5.8 Here are examples of a typical quadrupedal primate. Note that the arms and legs are about the same length and
the back is long and flexible. This is a terrestrial quadruped so the arms and legs are relatively long and the tail is shorter.
Quadrupedalism is the most common form of locomotion among primates Figure 5.8). The term quadrupedal means
to walk on all fours. Animals that move in this way usually have legs and arms that are about the same length and
typically have a tail for balance. Arboreal quadrupeds usually have shorter arms and legs and longer tails, while terrestrial
Figure 5.9 These are examples of a typical brachiator. Note the longer arms than legs, short back, and lack of a tail. You will
read about more details of their anatomy later in the chapter.
Lastly, humans move around on two feet, called bipedalism. Some primates will occasionally travel on two feet but do
so awkwardly and never for long distances. Among mammals, only humans have evolved to walk with a striding gait on
two legs as a primary form of locomotion. To move bipedally, humans need many specialized adaptations that will be
discussed in detail in later chapters.
As we begin exploring the different taxa of primates, it is important to keep in mind the hierarchical nature of taxonomic
classification discussed in Chapter 2) and how this relates to the key characteristics that will be covered. Figure 5.11
summarizes the major taxonomic groups of primates. If you locate humans on the chart, you can trace our classification
and see all of the categories getting more and more inclusive as you work your way up to the Order Primates. What this
means is that humans will have the key traits of each of those groups. It is a good idea to refer to the figure to orient
yourself as we discuss each taxon.
Figure 5.11 This taxonomy chart shows the major groups of primate taxa. Be sure to refer back to this chart as you read through the
primate groups so that you can see how each group is related to one another.
Our goal in taxonomic classification is to place taxa into categories that reflect their clade relationships. A clade
is a grouping of organisms that reflect a branch of the evolutionary tree, a grouping based on relatedness. Clade
relationships are determined using derived traits shared by groups of taxa as well as genetic similarities. An example
of a clade would be a grouping that includes humans, chimpanzees, bonobos, and gorillas. These taxa are in what is
referred to as the African clade of hominoids. The African clade grouping reflects the fact that humans, chimpanzees,
bonobos, and gorillas all share a more recent ancestor with each other than any of them do with other species—that
is, we are on the same branch of the evolutionary tree.We know members of the African clade are most closely related
based on derived morphological traits as well as genetic similarities. In this grouping, we exclude the orangutan, which
is considered a member of the Asian clade of hominoids.
In contrast, grades are groupings that reflect levels of adaptation or overall similarity and not necessarily actual
evolutionary relationships. An example of a grade would be placing orangutans, gorillas, bonobos, and chimpanzees into
a group, and excluding humans. Grouping in this way is based on the superficial similarities of the apes in being large-
bodied, having lots of body hair, living in tropical forests, using trees, and so on. According to these criteria, humans
seem to be the unusual ones in that we differ in our morphology, behavior, and ecology. Separating humans from the
other large-bodied apes is the system that was used historically. We now know that grouping orangutans, gorillas,
bonobos, and chimpanzees and excluding humans does not accurately reflect our true evolutionary relationships Figure
5.12), and because our goal in taxonomic classification is to organize animals to reflect their evolutionary relationships,
we prefer to use clade classifications.
Suborder Strepsirrhini
Compared to haplorrhines, strepsirrhines rely more on nonvisual senses. Strepsirrhines have longer snouts than
haplorrhines and get their name because they all have wet noses rhinariums) like cats and dogs. The long snout and
rhinarium reflect strepsirrhines’ greater reliance on olfaction relative to haplorrhines. Indeed, many strepsirrhines use
scent marking, rubbing scent glands or urine on objects in the environment to communicate with others. Additionally,
many strepsirrhines have mobile ears that they use to locate insect prey and predators. As discussed earlier, there
are trade-offs in sensory systems, so while strepsirrhines have a better sense of smell than haplorrhines, their visual
adaptations are more primitive. Strepsirrhines have less convergent eyes than haplorrhines, and therefore all have
postorbital bars whereas haplorrhines have full postorbital closure Figure 5.1). All strepsirrhines have a tapetum
lucidum, a reflective layer at the back of the eye that reflects light and thereby enhances the ability to see in low-light
conditions. It is the same layer that causes your dog or cat to have “yellow eye” when you take photos of them with the
flash on. It is thought to be primitive among mammals as a whole.
Madagascar is an island off the east coast of Africa, and it is roughly the size of California, Oregon, and Washington
combined. It has been separated from Africa for about 130 million years and from India for about 85 million years, which
means it was already an island when strepsirrhines got there approximately 60–70 million years ago. Only a few mammal
species ever reached Madagascar, and so when lemurs arrived they were able to flourish into a variety of forms.
Lemurs are also diverse in terms of behavior. Many Malagasy strepsirrhines are solitary foragers, but some live in pairs,
others in small groups, some in larger groups, and some, like the red-ruffed lemur, are now known to live in complex
social groups that are unlike what we see in any other primates Vasey 2006). It is also among the lemurs that we see
some of the best vertical clingers and leapers. Many lemurs are quadrupedal, but even the quadrupedal lemurs are quite
adept at leaping. Malagasy strepsirrhines also exhibit a few unusual traits. They are highly seasonal breeders, often
mating only during a short window, once a year Wright 1999). Female ring-tailed lemurs, for example, only come into
estrus one day a year for a mere six hours. Malagasy strepsirrhines are also unusual in that females are socially dominant.
In most primates, males dominate females because they are typically larger and exhibit greater aggression, but in lemur
groups, males and females are usually the same size and females have priority access to resources over males.
Unlike the lemurs of Madagascar, lorises, pottos, and galagos live in areas where they share their environments with
monkeys and apes, who often eat similar foods. Lorises live across South and Southeast Asia, while pottos and galagos
live across Central Africa. Because of competition with larger-bodied monkeys and apes, mainland strepsirrhines are
more restricted in the niches they can fill in their environments and so are not as diverse as the lemurs of Madagascar.
All strepsirrhines in Africa and Asia are nocturnal and solitary. Their body sizes
don’t range as greatly as the lemurs, and neither do their diets. For the most part,
the diet of lorises, pottos, and galagos consist of fruits and insects. A couple of
species eat more gum, but overall the diet of this group is fairly narrow when
compared to the Malagasy lemurs. Lorises and pottos are known for being slow,
quadrupedal climbers, moving quietly through the forests to avoid being detected
by predators Figure 5.18). Because they are not fast moving, these strepsirrhines
have developed alternative defenses against predators. Lorises, for example, eat a
lot of caterpillars, which makes their saliva slightly toxic. Loris mothers will then
bathe their young in this toxic saliva, thus making the babies unappealing to
predators. In comparison to the slow-moving lorises and pottos, galagos are
active quadrupedal runners and leapers that scurry about the forests at night.
Galagos make distinctive calls that sound like a baby crying, which has led to their
nickname “bushbabies.” Figure 5.19 summarizes the key differences between
these two groups of strepsirrhines.
Figure 5.19 Strepsirrhini at a glance: This table summarizes the key differences between the two groups of strepsirrhines.
Suborder Haplorrhini
When the strepsirrhini and haplorrhini split from one another, strepsirrhines retained more primitive traits those likely
present in the last common ancestor), while haplorrhines became quite different, developing many derived traits. Thus,
all of the traits discussed below are considered derived traits.
As mentioned earlier, the visual systems of haplorrhines are more developed than those of strepsirrhines. Many
haplorrhines are trichromatic and, with one exception that will be discussed shortly, all have full postorbital closure
Figure 5.1). This increase in bony closure around the eye protects the more convergent eyes that haplorrhines possess.
Haplorrhines also evolved to have a fovea, a depression in the retina at the back of the eye containing concentrations of
cells that allow us to see things very close up in great detail. The heavier reliance on vision over olfaction is also reflected
in the shorter snouts ending with the dry nose no rhinarium) of haplorrhines. All but two genera of living haplorrhines
are active during the day, so this group lacks the tapetum lucidum which is so useful to nocturnal species. On average,
haplorrhines also have larger brains relative to their body size when compared with strepsirrhines.
The Haplorrhini differ from the Strepsirrhini in aspects of ecology and behavior as well. Haplorrhines are generally
larger than strepsirrhines, and so we see many more species that are folivorous and frugivorous, and fewer that are
insectivorous. This dietary difference is reflected in the teeth of haplorrhines, which are broader with more surface
area for chewing. The larger body size of this taxon also influences locomotion. Only one haplorrhine is a vertical
clinger and leaper. Most members of this suborder are quadrupedal, with one subgroup specialized for brachiation. A
few haplorrhine taxa are monomorphic, meaning males and females are the same size, but many members of this group
show moderate to high sexual dimorphism in body size and canine size. Haplorrhines also differ in social behavior.
All but two haplorrhines live in groups, which is very different from the primarily solitary strepsirrhines. Differences
between the two suborders are summarized in Figure 5.20.
No rhinarium
Rhinarium
Short snout
Longer snout
Eyes more convergent
Eyes less convergent
Sensory
Post-orbital plate
adaptations Post-orbital bar
No tapetum lucidum
Tapetum lucidum
Many are trichromatic
Mobile ears
Fovea
Dietary
Mostly insectivores and frugivores, few folivores Few insectivores, mostly frugivores and folivores
differences
Activity Mostly nocturnal, few diurnal or cathemeral Only two are nocturnal, rest are diurnal
patterns and
Ecology Almost entirely arboreal Many arboreal taxa, also many terrestrial taxa
Social Only two are solitary, all others live in pairs, small to very
Mostly solitary, some pairs, small to large groups
groupings large groups
Figure 5.20 Suborders at a glance: This table summarizes the key differences between the two primate suborders.
Suborder Haplorrhini is divided into three infraorders: Tarsiiformes, which includes the tarsiers of Asia; Platyrrhini,
which includes the New World monkeys of Central and South America; and Catarrhini, a group that includes the Old
World monkeys and apes of Asia and Africa, as well as humans. According to molecular estimates, tarsiers split from the
other haplorrhines close to 70 million years ago, and platyrrhini split from catarrhini close to 46 million years ago Pozzi
et al. 2014).
Almost full PO
Very small
Two grooming closure
Nocturnal claws
More
Highly 2:1:3:3/1:1:3:3 dental convergent eyes
insectivorous formula
No tapetum
Solitary Do not eat lucidum
vegetation
Vertical clinger- No rhinarium
leapers Can rotate their
Genetic
heads nearly 180
Little/no sexual evidence
degrees
dimorphism
Fovea
Figure 5.24 Tarsiers at a glance: Tarsiers have a mix of traits that lead to debate about their classification. Some of their
traits superficially resemble strepsirrhines, but they share many derived traits with haplorrhines. They also possess unique
characteristics that are unlike any other primates.
The platyrrhines, also commonly called New World monkeys, are the only
non-human primates in Central and South America Figure 5.25) and so, like
the lemurs of Madagascar, have diversified into a variety of forms in the
absence of competition. Infraorder Platyrrhini get their name from their
distinctive nose shape. “Platy” means flat and “rhini” refers to noses and,
indeed, New World monkeys have noses that are flat and wide, with nostrils
that are far apart, facing outward, and usually round in shape Figure 5.26).
This nose shape is very different from what we see in catarrhines, the group
that includes Old World monkeys, apes, and humans.
individuals to distinguish reds and yellows from blues and greens are on the X
Platyrrhines include the smallest of the monkeys, the marmosets and tamarins Figure 5.27). These small monkeys, all
of which weigh less than 1 kilogram, live in cooperative family groups, wherein usually only one female reproduces and
everyone else helps carry and raise the offspring. They are unusual primates in that they regularly produce twins. The
diet of marmosets and tamarins largely consists of gums and saps, so these monkeys have evolved claw-like nails that
enable them to cling to the sides of tree trunks like squirrels as well as special teeth that allow them to gnaw through
bark. They also have one fewer molar than other platyrrhines, giving them a dental formula of 2:1:3:2.
The largest of the platyrrhines are a family that include spider monkeys, woolly spider monkeys, woolly monkeys, and
howler monkeys Figure 5.28). This group of monkeys can weigh up to 9–15 kg and have evolved prehensile tails that
can hold their entire body weight. It is among this group that we see semi-brachiators, like the spider monkey Figure
5.10). To make them more efficient in this form of locomotion, spider monkeys evolved to not have thumbs so that their
hands work more like hooks that can easily let go of branches while swinging. Howler monkeys are another well-known
member of this group, earning their name due to their loud calls, which can be heard for miles away. To make these
loud vocalizations, howler monkeys have a specialized vocal system that includes a large larynx and hyoid bone. Howler
monkeys are the most folivorous of the platyrrhines and are known for spending a large portion of their day digesting
their food.
There are many other monkeys in the New World, including the gregarious capuchins Figure 5.26) and squirrel
monkeys, the pair-living titi monkeys, and the nocturnal owl monkeys. There are also the seed-eating monkeys such
as saki monkeys and uakaris. In many areas across Central and South America, multiple different species of platyrrhine
will share the forests, and some species will even travel together in associations that you will learn about in Chapter
6. According to molecular evidence, the diversity of platyrrhines that we see today seems to have originated about 25
million years ago Schneider and Sampaio 2015). Figure 5.29 summarizes the key traits of platyrrhines relative to the
other infraorders of Haplorrhini.
Platyrrhini traits
Highly arboreal
Figure 5.29 Platyrrhini at a glance: Summary of the key traits we use to distinguish platyrrhines. Traits indicated with an
are those with exceptions detailed in the text.
Infraorder Catarrhini includes Old World monkeys, apes, and humans. Non-human catarrhines are found all over
A frica and South and Southeast A sia, with some being found as far north as Japan. The most northerly and southerly
catarrhines are from the superfamily that includes the Old World monkeys. In contrast, apes are less tolerant of drier,
more seasonal environments and so have a relatively restricted geographic range.
Figure 5.31 Catarrhines are distinguished in that they only have two premolars compared to the three premolars seen in
most other primate taxa, including the platyrrhines shown here for comparison. In these images you can also see one of
the derived traits of cercopithecoids, their bilophodont molars, which differ from the more primitive Y-5 molars that apes
and humans have.
Infraorder Catarrhini is divided into two superfamilies: Superfamily Cercopithecoidea, which includes Old World
monkeys, and Superfamily Hominoidea, which includes apes and humans. Molecular estimates place the split between
cercopithecoids and hominoids at about 32 million years ago Pozzi et al. 2014), which fits well with the fossil record
showing evidence of the lineages by about 25 million years ago see Chapter 8 on primate evolution).
Compared to hominoids, Old World monkeys have a more primitive quadrupedal body plan discussed later in Figure
5.39), but they do have a couple of derived traits shared by all members of this group. Cercopithecoidea have
bilophodont molars “bi” meaning two, “loph” referring to ridge, and “dont” meaning tooth). Referring back to Figure
5.31, you will see how the molars of cercopithecoids have four cusps arranged in a square pattern and have two ridges
connecting them. It is thought that this molar enabled Old World monkeys to eat a wide range of foods, thus allowing
them to live in habitats that apes cannot. The other key derived trait that all cercopithecoids share is having ischial
callosities Figure 5.32). The ischium is the part of your pelvis that you are sitting on right now see Appendix A:
Osteology). In Old World monkeys, this part of the pelvis has a flattened surface that, in living animals, will have callused
skin over it. These function as seat pads for cercopithecoids, who often sit above branches when feeding and resting.
Figure 5.32 The second derived trait of cercopithecoids are Figure 5.33 Geographic distribution of the Old World
their ischial callosities, shown here on a crested black monkeys. Catarrhines have the widest geographic
macaque. distribution due to the success of cercopithecoid monkeys
who are found all across Africa and Asia.
The cercopithecoid monkeys are the most geographically widespread group of non-human
primates Figure 5.33). Since their divergence from hominoids, this monkey group has
increased in numbers and diversity. In part, their success over hominoids is due to the
faster reproductive rates of cercopithecoids relative to hominoids. On average, Old World
monkeys will reproduce every one to two years, whereas hominoids will reproduce once
every four to nine years, depending on the taxon.
On Madagascar, indris and aye-ayes play roles in the creation myths and omens of local people. There are
many myths regarding the origins of indris and their relationship to humans, including one where two
brothers living in the forest separated, with one brother leaving the forest and becoming a human while the
other stayed in the forest to become the indri. Indris are considered sacred and are therefore protected,
due to their similarities to humans in having long legs, no tail, and upright posture. Unfortunately, the aye-
aye is not treated with the same reverence. Aye-ayes, due to their unusual appearance, are thought to be
omens of death. They are usually killed when encountered because it is believed that someone will die if an
aye-aye points at them.
In India, monkeys play a key role in the Hindu religion. Hanuman, who resembles a monkey, is a key figure in
the Ramayana. Hanuman is thought to be a guardian deity, and so local monkeys like Hanuman langurs and
macaques are protected in India Figure 5.37). In Thailand, where Hinduism is also practiced, the Hindu
reverence for monkeys extends to “monkey feasts,” where large quantities of food are spread out in
gratitude to the monkeys for bringing good fortune.
The people of Japan have coexisted with Japanese macaques for thousands of years, and so monkeys play
key roles in both of the major Japanese religions. In the Shinto religion, macaques are thought of as
messengers between the spirit world and humans and monkey symbols are thought to be good luck. The
other major religion in Japan is Buddhism, and monkeys play a role in symbolism of this religion as well. The
“Three Wise Monkeys” who see no evil, speak no evil, and hear no evil derive from Buddhist iconography of
monkeys.
In the New World, monkeys feature often in Mayan and Aztec stories. In the Mayan creation story, the Popol
Vuh, the “hero brothers” are actually a howler monkey and a spider monkey, who represent ancestors of
humans in the story. In the Aztec religion, spider monkeys are associated with the god of arts, pleasure, and
playfulness. A spider monkey is also represented in a Peruvian Nazca geoglyph, a large design made on the
ground by moving rocks.
In many of these regions today, the relationships between humans and non-human primates are
complicated. The bushmeat and pet trades make these animals valuable at the expense of many animals’
lives, and in some areas, non-human primates have become pests who raid crop fields and consume
valuable foods. All of this has led to the development of a new subarea of anthropology called
Ethnoprimatology, which involves studying the political, economic, symbolic, and practical relationships
between humans and non-human primates. This field highlights the particular challenges for humans of
having to coexist with animals with whom we share so much in common. It also provides insight into some
of the challenges facing primate conservation efforts see Appendix A: Primate Conservation).
Quadrupedalism Brachiation
Arm length vs. leg length About equal Arms are longer
Figure 5.39 Quadrupedalism vs. brachiation: Summary of the key anatomical differences between a quadrupedal primate
and one adapted for brachiation. To view and compare these traits using photos of bones, check out the interactive skeletal
websites listed under the “Further Explorations” section at the end of this chapter.
To successfully swing below branches, many changes to the body needed to occur. The arms of a hominoid are much
longer than the legs in order to increase reach, and the lower back is shorter and less flexible to increase control
when swinging. The torso, shoulders, and arms of hominoids have evolved to increase range of motion and flexibility
Apes and humans also differ from other primates in behavior and life history characteristics. Hominoids all seem to
show varying degrees of female dispersal at sexual maturity. Dispersal refers to leaving the area or group where an
individual was born. As you will learn about in Chapter 6, it is more common that males leave. Indeed, some apes
show males dispersing in addition to females, but the broader tendency for female dispersal in hominoids is a bit
unusual among primates. Our superfamily is also characterized by the most extended life histories of all primates. All
members of this group live a long time and take a long time to grow and start reproducing. Hominoids also reproduce
much less frequently compared to cercopithecoid monkeys. The slow pace of this life history is likely related to why
hominoids have decreased in diversity since they first evolved. In the past, hominoids were tremendously diverse in
both geography and adaptations. Today, there are only five types of hominoids left: gibbons and siamangs, orangutans,
gorillas, chimpanzees and bonobos, and humans.
Infraorder Catarrhini
All trichromatic
Figure 5.40 Catarrhini at a glance: Summary of key traits of the Infraorder Catarrhini as well as the characteristics used to
distinguish between the two superfamilies within this group.
Meet the Living Primates | 175
Family Hylobatidae of Southeast Asia
attachment area for chewing muscles but also in sexual competition Balolia
Gorilla of Africa
There are several species of gorillas that can be found across Central Africa. Gorilla males, like orangutan males,
are about twice the size of female gorillas Figure 5.43). When on the ground, gorillas use a form of
quadrupedalism called knuckle-walking, where the fingers are curled under and the weight is carried on the
knuckles. Male gorillas have a large sagittal crest and larger canines compared with females. A dult male
gorillas are often called “silverbacks” because when they reach about twelve to thirteen years old, the hair on
their backs turns silvery gray. Gorillas typically live in groups of one male and several females. Gorillas are
considered folivorous, although they can be more frugivorous depending on fruit seasonality Remis 1997).
The Genus Pan includes two species: Pan troglodytes the common chimpanzee) and
Pan paniscus the bonobo). These species are separated by the Congo River,
with chimpanzees ranging across West and Central A frica and bonobos located
in a restricted area south of the Congo River. Chimpanzees and bonobos both have
broad, largely frugivorous diets and similar social groups. The two species
differ morphologically in that bonobos are slightly smaller, have their hair parted
down the middle of their foreheads, and are born with dark faces Figure 5.44). In
contrast, chimpanzees do not have the distinctive parted hair and are born with
light faces which darken as they mature Figure 5.45). Chimpanzees and
Figure 5.44 Bonobo, Pan paniscus. bonobos live in a grouping called a fission-fusion community, which you will
You can see the distinctive hair-part learn more about in Chapter 6. Both species are moderately sexually dimorphic,
on this bonobo.
with males about 20%larger than females. When on the ground, chimpanzees and
bonobos knuckle-walk like gorillas do.
Figure 5.45 A common chimpanzee, Pan troglodytes, female and her offspring. Note
the pink face of the youngest individual. Bonobos are born with dark-skinned
faces, but chimpanzees are born with pink faces that darken with age.
Homo
The last member of the Hominoidea to discuss is our own taxon, Genus Homo. Humans differ from apes in many aspects
of our morphology, behavior, and life history, all of which you will be learning about in later chapters. One of the
objectives of this chapter, however, and of biological anthropology in general, is to understand our place in nature. This
means looking for the aspects of human biology that lead us to place humans within the taxonomic diversity we have
just discussed. To accomplish this, we not only consider how humans are different from other species but also examine
the traits that unite us with the other primates, our similarities—that is our focus here.
There are clear similarities between humans and the other apes in our morphology and life history. Like other
hominoids, humans lack a tail and possess upper-body adaptations for brachiation. While our lower body has been
modified for a bipedal gait, we are still able to swing from branches or “monkey bars,” or throw a fastball, all thanks to our
Among the hominoids, humans show particular affinities with other members of the African Clade, Pan and Gorilla.
Humans share over 96% of our DNA with gorillas Scally et al. 2012), and over 98% with Pan Ebersberger et al. 2002).
Even without this strong genetic evidence, the African Clade of hominoids share many morphological similarities. These
shared traits include eye sockets that are slightly farther apart and are more square or rounded compared to the closely
placed, ovoid eyes of orangutans. Also, the cheekbones of the African clade sweep backward compared to the more
flattened orangutan cheekbones. Today, Pan and Gorilla knuckle-walk when on the ground, and it has been suggested
that the last common ancestor of chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas, and humans shared this trait Richmond et al. 2001).
Our closest living relatives today are chimpanzees and bonobos. Because of our close relationship, humans share
many additional traits in common with Pan. Humans, chimpanzees, and bonobos all live in similar social groups that
are characterized by territoriality and male cooperation, among other things. Chimpanzee males are well-known to
cooperate in hunting, a common trait across human societies as well. As you will learn more about in the next chapter,
chimpanzee populations have also been observed to make and use tools for different purposes, not unlike what humans
do.
While primates are fascinating animals in their own right, we study non-human primates in anthropology with the
ultimate goal of understanding more about our own biology and evolutionary history. The close relationship between
humans and non-human primates makes them excellent for studying humans via homology, looking at traits that
are shared between two taxa because they inherited the trait from a common ancestor. Consider, for example, the
characteristics discussed in the previous section that are shared by humans and Pan. Since both taxa exhibit these traits,
they are likely homologous, meaning these shared traits were probably present in the last common ancestor of humans
and Pan approximately 6-8 million years ago .
Non-human primates also make excellent comparators for learning about humans via analogy sometimes called
convergent evolution, parallel evolution, or homoplasy). Many non-human primates live in environments or social
groups similar to those in which our ancestors lived and therefore exhibit similar behavioral and morphological traits as
what we see in humans. For example, baboons and humans share the trait of having long legs. In humans, this is because
about 1.7 million years ago, our ancestors moved into open savanna habitats, like those baboons live in today, and longer
legs enabled them to move over long distances more efficiently. Baboons independently evolved longer arms and legs
for the same reason—to be able to cover more ground, more efficiently, in an open habitat. This means that having long
legs is an analogous trait in baboons and humans—that is, this adaptation evolved independently in the two species but
for the same purpose.
Conclusion
The Order Primates is a diverse and fascinating group of animals united in sharing a suite of characteristics—visual
specialization, grasping hands and feet, large brains, and extended life histories—that differentiates us from other groups
Review Questions
• Why does the field of anthropology, a field dedicated to the study of humans, include the study of
non-human animals? What important things can we learn from non-human primates in
anthropology?
• One of the important goals of an introductory biological anthropology course is to teach you about
your place in nature. What is the full taxonomic classification of humans, and what are some of the
traits we have of each of these categories?
• When you have seen primates in person, did you observe any facial expressions, behaviors, or physical
traits that seemed familiar to you? If so, which ones and why?
• Why is it important to try to place taxa into a clade classification rather than groupings based on
grade? Can you think of an example?
• Draw out a tree showing the major taxonomic group of primates described here, making sure to leave
room in between each level. Underneath each taxon, list some of the key features of this group so that
you can compare traits between groups.
Key Terms
African clade: A grouping that includes gorillas, chimpanzees, bonobos, humans, and their extinct relatives.
Analogy: When two or more taxa exhibit similar traits that have evolved independently, the similar traits evolve due to
similar selective pressures. Also sometimes called convergent evolution, parallel evolution, or homoplasy.)
Arboreal: A descriptor for an organism that spends most of its time in trees.
Asian clade: A grouping that includes orangutans and their extinct relatives.
Bilophodont: Molar pattern of cercopithecoid monkeys in which there are usually four cusps that are arranged in a
square pattern and connected by two ridges.
Brachiation: A form of locomotion in which the organism swings below branches using the forelimbs.
Canines: In most primates, these are the longest of the teeth, often conical in shape and used as a weapon against
predators or others of their species.
Cusps: The bumps on the chewing surface of the premolars and molars, which can be quite sharp in some species.
Dental formula: The number of each type of tooth in one quadrant of the mouth, written as number of incisors:
canines: premolars: molars.
Derived trait: A trait that has been recently modified, most helpful when assigning taxonomic classification.
Diastema: A space between the teeth, usually for large canines to fit when the mouth is closed.
Dry nose: The nose and upper lip are separated and the upper lip can move independently; sometimes referred to as a
“hairy” or “mobile” upper lip.
Ethnoprimatology: A subarea of anthropology that studies the complexities of human-primate relationships in the
modern environment.
Evolutionary trade-off: When an organism, which is limited in the time and energy it can put into aspects of its biology
and behavior, is shaped by natural selection to invest in one adaptation at the expense of another.
Faunivorous: Having a diet consisting of animal matter: insects, eggs, lizards, etc.
Fovea: A depressed area in the retina at the back of the eye containing a concentration of cells that allow us to focus on
objects very close to our face.
Homology: When two or more taxa share characteristics because they inherited them from a common ancestor.
Hone: When primates sharpen their canines by wearing them on adjacent teeth.
Ischial callosities: A flattened area of the ischium on the pelvis over which calluses form; functioning as seat pads for
sitting and resting atop branches.
Knuckle-walking: A form of quadrupedal movement used by Gorilla and Pan when on the ground, where the front
limbs are supported on the knuckles of the hands.
Male bimaturism: Refers to the alternative reproductive strategies in orangutans in which males can delay maturation,
sometimes indefinitely, until a fully mature, “flanged” male disappears.
Molars: The largest teeth at the back of the mouth; used for chewing; in primates, these teeth usually have between
three and five cusps.
Monomorphic: When males and females of a species do not exhibit significant sexual dimorphism.
Natal coat: Refers to the contrasting fur color of baby leaf monkeys compared to adults.
Opposable thumb or opposable big toe: Having thumbs and toes that go in a different direction from the rest of the
fingers, allows for grasping with hands and feet.
Polymorphic color vision: A system in which individuals of a species vary in their abilities to see color. In primates, it
refers to males being dichromatic and females being either trichromatic or dichromatic.
Postorbital bar: A bony ring that surrounds the eye socket, open at the back.
Postorbital closure/plate: A bony plate that provides protection to the side and back of the eye.
Prehensile tail: A tail that is able to hold the full body weight of an organism, which often has a tactile pad on the
underside of the tip for improved grip.
Premolars: Smaller than the molars, used for chewing. In primates, these teeth usually have one or two cusps.
Primitive trait: A trait that has been inherited from a distant ancestor.
Rhinariums: Wet noses; produced when the nose is connected to the upper lip.
Sagittal crest: A bony ridge along the top/middle of the skull, used for attachment of chewing muscles.
Scent marking: The behavior of rubbing scent glands or urine onto objects as a way of communicating with others.
Sexually dimorphic: When a species exhibits sex differences in morphology, behavior, hormones, and/or coloration.
Specialized trait: A trait that has been modified for a specific purpose.
Styloid process of ulna: A bony projection of the ulna at the end near the wrist.
Tactile pads: Sensitive skin at the fingertips for sense of touch. Animals with a prehensile tail have a tactile pad on the
underside of the tail as well.
Tapetum lucidum: Reflecting layer at the back of the eye that magnifies light.
Tetrachromatic: Having the ability to see reds, yellows, blues, greens, and ultraviolet.
Tooth comb or dental comb: A trait of the front, lower teeth of strepsirrhines in which, typically, the four incisors and
canines are long and thin and protrude outward.
Trichromatic color vision: Being able to distinguish yellows and reds in addition to blues and greens.
Vertical clinging and leaping: A locomotor pattern in which animals are oriented upright while clinging to vertical
branches, push off with hind legs, and land oriented upright on another vertical branch.
Y-5 molar: Molar cusp pattern in which five molar cusps are separated by a “Y”-shaped groove pattern.
Stephanie Etting
Stephanie Etting
Fleagle, John G. 2013. Primate Adaptation and Evolution. Third edition. San Diego: Academic Press.
Fuentes, Agustín, and Kimberley J. Hockings. 2010. “The Ethnoprimatological Approach in Primatology.” American
Journal of Primatology 72 10): 841–847.
Rowe, Noel. 1996. Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates. Charlestown, VA: Pogonias Press.
Whitehead, Paul F., William K. Sacco, and Susan B. Hochgraf. 2005. A Photographic Atlas for Physical Anthropology.
Englewood, CO: Morton Publishing.
References
Balolia, Katharine L., Christophe Soligo, and Bernard Wood. 2017. “Sagittal Crest Formation in Great Apes and Gibbons.”
Journal of Anatomy 230 6): 820–832.
Bininda-Emonds, Olaf R., Marcel Cardillo, Kate E. Jones, Ross D. E. MacPhee, Robin M. D. Beck, Richard Grenyer,
Samantha A. Price, Rutger A. Vos, John L. Gittleman, and Andy Purvis. 2007. “The Delayed Rise of Present-Day
Mammals.” Nature 446 7,135): 507–512.
Burton, Michael L., Carmella C. Moore, John W. M. Whiting, A. Kimball Romney, David F. Aberle, Juan A. Barcelo, Malcolm
M. Dow, et al.. 1996. “Regions Based on Social Structure.” Current Anthropology 37 1): 87–123.
Changizi, Mark A., Qiong Zhang, and Shinsuke Shimojo. 2006. “Bare Skin, Blood and the Evolution of Primate Colour
Vision.” Biology Letters 2 2): 217–221.
Chivers, David J., and C. M. Hladik. 1980. “Morphology of the Gastrointestinal Tract in Primates: Comparisons With Other
Mammals in Relation to Diet.” Journal of Morphology 166 3): 337–386.
Clutton‐Brock, T. H., and Paul H. Harvey. 1980. “Primates, Brains, and Ecology.” Journal of Zoology 190 3): 309–323.
Dominy, Nathaniel J., and Peter W. Lucas. 2001. “Ecological Importance of Trichromatic Vision to Primates.” Nature 410
6,826): 363–366.
Dunbar, Robin I. M. 1998. “The Social Brain Hypothesis.” Evolutionary Anthropology 6 5): 178–190.
Ebersberger, Ingo, Dirk Metzler, Carsten Schwarz, and Svante Pääbo. 2002. “Genomewide Comparison of DNA
Sequences Between Humans and Chimpanzees.” American Journal of Human Genetics 70 6): 1,490–1,497.
Jameson, Natalie M., Zhoucheng Hou, Kirsten N. Sterner, Amy Weckle, Morris Goodman, Michael E. Steiper, and Derek
Wildman. 2011. “Genomic Data Reject the Hypothesis of a Prosimian Primate Clade.” Journal of Human Evolution 61 3):
295–305.
Kawamura, Shoji, Chihiro Hiramatsu, Amanda D. Melin, Colleen M. Schaffner, Filippo Aureli, and Linda Marie Fedigan.
Matsui, Atsushi, Felix Rakotondraparany, Isao Munechika, Masami Hasegawa, and Satoshi Horai. 2009. “Molecular
Phylogeny and Evolution of Prosimians Based on Complete Sequences of Mitochondrial DNAs.” Gene 441 1–2): 53–66.
Morgan, M. J., A. Adam, and J. D. Mollon. 1992. “Dichromats Detect Colour-Camouflaged Objects That Are Not Detected
by Trichromats.” Proceedings of the Royal Society London B: Biological Sciences 248 1,323): 291–295.
Nater, Alexander, Maja P. Mattle-Greminger, Anton Nurcahyo, Matthew G. Nowak, Marc De Manuel, Tariq Desai, Colin
Groves, et al. 2017. “Morphometric, Behavioral, and Genomic Evidence for a New Orangutan Species.” Current Biology
27 22): 3,487–3,498.
Pessoa, Daniel Marques Almeida, Rafael Maia, Rafael Cavalcanti de Albuquerque Ajuz, Pedro Zurvaino Palmeiro Melo
Rosa De Moraes, Maria Helena Constantino Spyrides, and Valdir Filgueiras Pessoa. 2014. “The Adaptive Value of
Primate Color Vision for Predator Detection.” American Journal of Primatology 76 8): 721–729.
Pozzi, Luca, Jason A. Hodgson, Andrew S. Burrell, Kirstin N. Sterner, Ryan L. Raaum, and Todd R. Disotell. 2014. “Primate
Phylogenetic Relationships and Divergence Dates Inferred From Complete Mitochondrial Genomes.” Molecular
Phylogenetics and Evolution 75: 165–183.
Regan, B. C., C. Julliot, B. Simmen, F. Viénot, P. Charles-Dominique, and J. D. Mollon. 2001. “Fruits, Foliage and the
Evolution of Primate Colour Vision.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences
356 1,407): 229–283.
Remis, Melissa J. 1997. “Western Lowland Gorillas Gorilla gorilla gorilla) as Seasonal Frugivores: Use of Variable
Resources.” American Journal of Primatology 43 2): 87–109.
Richmond, Brian G., David R. Begun, and David S. Strait. 2001. “Origin of Human Bipedalism: The Knuckle‐Walking
Hypothesis Revisited.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 116 S33): 70–105.
Robson, Shannen L., Carel P. van Schaik, and Kristen Hawkes. 2006. “The Derived Features of Human Life History.” In
The Evolution of Human Life History, edited by Richard R. Paine and Kristen Hawkes, 17–44. Santa Fe: SAR Press.
Scally, Aylwyn, Julien Y. Dutheil, LaDeana W. Hillier, Gregory E. Jordan, Ian Goodhead, Javier Herrero, Asger Hobolth, et
al. 2012. “Insights Into Hominid Evolution From the Gorilla Genome Sequence.” Nature 483 7,388): 169–175.
Schneider, Horacio, and Iracilda Sampaio. 2015. “The Systematics and Evolution of New World Primates: A Review.”
Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 82 B): 348–357.
Setchell, Joanna M., Phyllis C. Lee, E. Jean Wickings, and Alan F. Dixson. 2001. “Growth and Ontogeny of Sexual Size
Dimorphism in the Mandrill Mandrillus sphinx).” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 115 4): 349–360.
Silk, Joan B., Jacintha C. Beehner, Thore J. Bergman, Catherine Crockford, Anne L. Engh, Liza R. Moscovice, Roman M.
Wittig, Robert M. Seyfarth, and Dorothy L. Cheney. 2009. “The Benefits of Social Capital: Close Social Bonds Among
Female Baboons Enhance Offspring Survival.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 276
1,670): 3,099–3,104.
Utami, Sri Suci, Benoît Goossens, Michael W. Bruford, Jan R. de Ruiter, and Jan A. R. A. M. van Hooff. 2002. “Male
Bimaturism and Reproductive Success in Sumatran Orang-utans.” Behavioral Ecology 13 5): 643–652.
Vasey, Natalie. 2006. “Impact of Seasonality and Reproduction on Social Structure, Ranging Patterns, and Fission–Fusion
Wright, Patricia C. 1999. “Lemur Traits and Madagascar Ecology: Coping With an Island Environment.” American Journal
of Physical Anthropology 110 S29): 31–72.
Acknowledgements
The author would very much like to thank the editors for the opportunity to contribute to this textbook, along with two
anonymous reviewers who provided useful feedback on earlier drafts of this chapter. She would also like to thank Karin
Enstam Jaffe for her support and encouragement during the writing of this chapter. Most of all, the author would like to
thank all of the Introduction to Biological Anthropology students that she has had over the years who have listened to
her lecture endlessly on these animals that she finds so fascinating and who have helped her to hone her pedagogy in a
field that she loves.
Figure Attributions
Figure 5.1 Post-orbital bar/Post-orbital closure a derivative work original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to
Biological Anthropology by Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 License. [Includes Otolemur crassicaudatus
greater galago) by Animal Diversity Web, CC BY-NC-SA 3.0; Macaca fascicularis long-tailed macaque) by Animal
Diversity Web, CC BY-NC-SA 3.0.]
Figure 5.2 PrimateFeet by Richard Lydekker, original from The Royal Natural History 1:15 1893), is in the public domain.
Figure 5.3 Primate suite of traits table by Stephanie Etting original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.4 Ha,ha,ha …. 14986571843) by Rolf Dietrich Brecher from Germany is under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.
Figure 5.5 Four types of human teeth original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Mary
Nelson is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.6a Frugivore characteristics original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Mary
Nelson is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.6b Insectivore characteristics original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Mary
Nelson is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.6c Folivore characteristics original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Mary
Nelson is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.7 Vertical clinger and leaper locomotion original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology
by Mary Nelson is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.8 Terrestrial quadrupedal primate original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by
Mary Nelson is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.11: Primate taxonomy chart a derivative work original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology by Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License. [Includes Lemur catta Linnaeus, 1759 by Roberto
Díaz Sibaja, CC BY 3.0; Lorisoidea original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Katie
Nelson, CC BY-NC 4.0; Tarsiiformes original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Mary
Nelson, CC BY-NC 4.0; Cebinae Bonaparte, 1831 by Sarah Werning, CC BY 3.0; Colobus guereza Ruppell, 1835 by Yan
Wong from drawing in The Century Dictionary 1911) flipped horizontally), designated to the public domain CC0); Papio
cynocephalus by Owen Jones, designated to the public domain CC0); animals silhouette wolf elephant 2755766) by
mohamed_hassan, Pixabay License.]
Figure 5.12 Grades vs. clades comparison a derivative work original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology by Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License. [Includes Orangutan on a tree Unsplash) by Dawn
Armfield darmfield, public domain CC0 1.0); Gorilla Profile 17997840570) by Charlie Marshall from Bristol UK, United
Kingdom, modified cropped), CC BY 2.0 License; Chimpanzee 14679767561) by Magnus Johansson, modified cropped),
CC BY-SA 2.0; Pointing finger 1922074) by truthseeker08, Pixabay License.]
Figure 5.13 Lemur catta toilet claw by Alex Dunkel Maky) is used under a CC BY 3.0 Licesnse.
Figure 5.14 Extant Strepsirrhini a derivative work by Mark Dumont is under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License. [Includes Katta
család by Veszprémi Állatkert, CC BY-SA 3.0; Aye-aye at night in the wild in Madagascar by Frank Vassen, CC BY 2.0;
Diademed ready to push off by Michael Hogan, designated to the public domain CC0); Juvenile Black-and-White Ruffed
Lemur, Mantadia, Madagascar by Frank Vassen, CC BY 2.0; Microcebus murinus -Artis Zoo, Amsterdam, Netherlands-8a
by Arjan Haverkamp, CC BY 2.0; Slow Loris by Jmiksanek, CC BY-SA 3.0; Slender Loris by Kalyan Varma Kalyanvarma),
CC BY-SA 4.0; Garnett’s Galago Greater Bushbaby) by Mark Dumont, CC BY 2.0].
Figure 5.15 Lemur catta toothcomb by Alex Dunkel Maky) is used under a CC BY 3.0 licesnse.
Figure 5.16 Geographic distribution of living strepsirrhines original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology by Elyssa Ebding at GeoPlace, California State University, Chico is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.17 Indri indri 0003 by Christophe Germain is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.
Figure 5.18 Nycticebus coucang 002 by David Haring / Duke Lemur Center is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.
Figure 5.19 Strepsirrhines at a glance a derivative work original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology by Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 License. [Includes Ringtailed Lemurs in Berenty by David
Dennis, CC BY-SA 2.0; Komba ušatá by Petr Hamerník, CC BY-SA 4.0.]
Figure 5.20 Suborders at a glance a derivative work original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology by Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 License. [Includes Black-and-White Ruffed Lemur,
Mantadia, Madagascar by Frank Vassen, CC BY 2.0; Crab eating macaque face by Bruce89, CC BY-SA 4.0.]
Figure 5.21 Tarsier Sanctuary, Corella, Bohol 2052878890) by yeowatzup is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.
Figure 5.22 Infraorder Tarsiiformes of Asia map original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology
by Elyssa Ebding at GeoPlace, California State University, Chico is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.23 Tarsier skull by Andrew Bardwell from Cleveland, Ohio, USA is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.
Figure 5.25 Infraorder Platyrrhini map original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by Elyssa
Ebding at GeoPlace, California State University, Chico is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.26 CARABLANCA – panoramio by Manuel Velazquez is used under a CC BY 3.0 License.
Figure 5.27 Callitrichinae genus a derivative work by Miguelrangeljris is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License. [Includes
Weißbüschelaffe_ Callithrix_jacchus) by Raymond, CC BY-SA 4.0; Leontopithecus chrysomelas portrait) by Hans
Hillewaert, CC BY-SA 4.0; Emperor_Tamarin_portrait_2_edit1 by Brocken Inaglory, CC BY-SA 4.0;
Dværgsilkeabe_Callithrix_pygmaea by Malene Thyssen User Malene), GNU Free Documentation License;
Mico_argentatus_ portrait) by Hans Hillewaert, CC BY-SA 4.0; Titi Monkey by Jeff Kubina.CC BY-SA 2.0]. ]
Figure 5.28 Atelidae Family a derivative work by User:Miguelrangeljr is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License. [Includes
Ateles marginatus Sao Paulo zoo) by Miguelrangeljr, CC BY-SA 3.0; Alouatta caraya male by Miguelrangeljr, CC BY-SA
3.0; Brachyteles hypoxanthus2 by Paulo B. Chaves, CC BY 2.0; Lagothrix lagotricha walking) by Hans Hillewaert, CC BY-
SA 3.0.]
Figure 5.29 Platyrrhini at a glance table original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by
Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.30 Wolf’s Guenon Picking Up Food 19095137693) by Eric Kilby from Somerville, MA, USA, is used under a CC
BY-SA 2.0 License.
Figure 5.31 Platyrrhini vs. Catarrhini dentition a derivative work original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology by Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC-SA 4.0 License. [Includes Cebus apella brown capuchin)
at Animal Diversity Web by Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor, CC BY-NC-SA 3.0;
Lophocebus albigena gray-cheeked mangaby) at Animal Diversity Web by Phil Myers, Museum of Zoology, University
of Michigan-Ann Arbor, CC BY-NC-SA 3.0; Symphalangus syndactylus siamang) at Animal Diversity Web by Phil Myers,
Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan-Ann Arbor, CC BY-NC-SA 3.0.]
Figure 5.32 Sulawesi trsr DSCN0572 v1 by T. R. Shankar Ramanis used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.
Figure 5.33 Superfamily Cercopithecoidea map original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology
by Elyssa Ebding at GeoPlace, California State University, Chico is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.34 Silverleaf Monkey Kuala Lumpur) by Andrea Lai from Auckland, New Zealand is used under a CC BY 2.0
License.
Figure 5.35 Proboscis monkey Nasalis larvatus) male head by Charles J Sharp creator QS:P170,Q54800218 is used under
a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.
Figure 5.36 Bonnet macaque DSC 0893 by T. R. Shankar Raman is used under a CC BY-SA 4.0 License.
Figure 5.37 Macaque India 4 by Thomas Schoch is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.
Figure 5.38 Superfamily Hominoidea map original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological Anthropology by
Elyssa Ebding at GeoPlace, California State University, Chico is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.39: Quadrupedalism vs. Brachiation table original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Anthropology by Stephanie Etting is under a CC BY-NC 4.0 License.
Figure 5.40 Catarrhini at a glance a derivative work original to Explorations: An Open Invitation to Biological
Figure 5.41 Shout 373310729) by su neko is used under a CC BY-SA 2.0 License.
Figure 5.42a Orang Utan Pongo pygmaeus) female with baby 8066259067) by Bernard DUPONT from FRANCE is used
under a CC BY-SA 2.0 Licence.
Figure 5.42b Orangutan -Zoologischer Garten Berlin-8a by David Arvidsson is used under a CC BY 2.0 License.
Figure 5.43a Enzo naomi echo by Zoostar is used under a CC BY 3.0 License.
Figure 5.43b Male gorilla in SF zoo by Brocken Inaglory is used under a CC BY-SA 3.0 License.
Figure 5.44 Bonobo male Jasongo 15yo Twycross 582a 2014 11 14 01 04 18 UTC) by William H. Calvin is used under a CC
BY-SA 4.0 License.
Figure 5.45 Chimpanzees in Uganda 5984913059) by USAID Africa Bureau uploaded by Elitre is in the public domain.