Introduction To Psychology - 1St Canadian Edition: 7.3 Adolescence: Developing Independence and Identity
Introduction To Psychology - 1St Canadian Edition: 7.3 Adolescence: Developing Independence and Identity
Introduction To Psychology - 1St Canadian Edition: 7.3 Adolescence: Developing Independence and Identity
Toggle Menu
Search in book: SEARCH
CONTENTS
Learning Objectives
1.Summarize the physical and cognitive changes that occur for boys and girls during adolescence.
2.Explain how adolescents develop a sense of morality and of self-identity.
Adolescence is defined as the years between the onset of puberty and the beginning of
adulthood. In the past, when people were likely to marry in their early 20s or younger, this
period might have lasted only 10 years or less — starting roughly between ages 12 and 13 and
ending by age 20, at which time the child got a job or went to work on the family farm, married,
and started his or her own family. Today, children mature more slowly, move away from home
at later ages, and maintain ties with their parents longer. For instance, children may go away to
university but still receive financial support from parents, and they may come home on weekends
or even to live for extended time periods. Thus the period between puberty and adulthood may
well last into the late 20s, merging into adulthood itself. In fact, it is appropriate now to consider
the period of adolescence and that of emerging adulthood (the ages between 18 and the middle
or late 20s) together.
During adolescence, the child continues to grow physically, cognitively, and emotionally,
changing from a child into an adult. The body grows rapidly in size, and the sexual and
reproductive organs become fully functional. At the same time, as adolescents develop more
advanced patterns of reasoning and a stronger sense of self, they seek to forge their own
identities, developing important attachments with people other than their parents. Particularly in
Western societies, where the need to forge a new independence is critical (Baumeister & Tice,
1986; Twenge, 2006), this period can be stressful for many children, as it involves new
emotions, the need to develop new social relationships, and an increasing sense of responsibility
and independence.
Although adolescence can be a time of stress for many teenagers, most of them weather the trials
and tribulations successfully. For example, the majority of adolescents experiment with alcohol
sometime before high school graduation. Although many will have been drunk at least once,
relatively few teenagers will develop long-lasting drinking problems or permit alcohol to
adversely affect their school or personal relationships. Similarly, a great many teenagers break
the law during adolescence, but very few young people develop criminal careers (Farrington,
1995). These facts do not, however, mean that using drugs or alcohol is a good idea. The use of
recreational drugs can have substantial negative consequences, and the likelihood of these
problems (including dependence, addiction, and even brain damage) is significantly greater for
young adults who begin using drugs at an early age.
Adolescence begins with the onset of puberty, a developmental period in which hormonal
changes cause rapid physical alterations in the body, culminating in sexual maturity. Although
the timing varies to some degree across cultures, the average age range for reaching puberty is
between nine and 14 years for girls and between 10 and 17 years for boys (Marshall & Tanner,
1986).
Puberty begins when the pituitary gland begins to stimulate the production of the male sex
hormone testosterone in boys and the female sex hormones estrogen and progesterone in girls.
The release of these sex hormones triggers the development of the primary sex
characteristics, the sex organs concerned with reproduction (Figure 7.8, “Sex Characteristics”).
These changes include the enlargement of the testicles and the penis in boys and the
development of the ovaries, uterus, and vagina in girls. In addition, secondary sex
characteristics (features that distinguish the two sexes from each other but are not involved in
reproduction) are also developing, such as an enlarged Adam’s apple, a deeper voice, and pubic
and underarm hair in boys, and enlargement of the breasts and hips and the appearance of pubic
and underarm hair in girls (Figure 7.8, “Sex Characteristics”). The enlargement of breasts is
usually the first sign of puberty in girls and, on average, occurs between ages 10 and 12
(Marshall & Tanner, 1986). Boys typically begin to grow facial hair between ages 14 and 16, and
both boys and girls experience a rapid growth spurt during this stage. The growth spurt for girls
usually occurs earlier than that for boys, with some boys continuing to grow into their 20s.
Figure 7.8 Sex Characteristics.
Puberty brings dramatic changes in the body, including the development of primary and secondary sex
characteristics.
A major milestone in puberty for girls is menarche, the first menstrual period, typically
experienced at around 12 or 13 years of age (Anderson, Dannal, & Must, 2003). The age of
menarche varies substantially and is determined by genetics, as well as by diet and lifestyle,
since a certain amount of body fat is needed to attain menarche. Girls who are very slim, who
engage in strenuous athletic activities, or who are malnourished may begin to menstruate later.
Even after menstruation begins, girls whose level of body fat drops below the critical level may
stop having their periods. The sequence of events for puberty is more predictable than the age at
which they occur. Some girls may begin to grow pubic hair at age 10 but not attain menarche
until age 15. In boys, facial hair may not appear until 10 years after the initial onset of puberty.
The timing of puberty in both boys and girls can have significant psychological consequences.
Boys who mature earlier attain some social advantages because they are taller and stronger and,
therefore, often more popular (Lynne, Graber, Nichols, Brooks-Gunn, & Botvin, 2007). At the
same time, however, early-maturing boys are at greater risk for delinquency and are more likely
than their peers to engage in antisocial behaviours, including drug and alcohol use, truancy, and
precocious sexual activity. Girls who mature early may find their maturity stressful, particularly
if they experience teasing or sexual harassment (Mendle, Turkheimer, & Emery, 2007; Pescovitz
& Walvoord, 2007). Early-maturing girls are also more likely to have emotional problems, a
lower self-image, and higher rates of depression, anxiety, and disordered eating than their peers
(Ge, Conger, & Elder, 1996).
Although the most rapid cognitive changes occur during childhood, the brain continues to
develop throughout adolescence, and even into the 20s (Weinberger, Elvevåg, & Giedd,
2005). During adolescence, the brain continues to form new neural connections, but also casts
off unused neurons and connections (Blakemore, 2008). As teenagers mature, the prefrontal
cortex, the area of the brain responsible for reasoning, planning, and problem solving, also
continues to develop (Goldberg, 2001). And myelin, the fatty tissue that forms around axons and
neurons and helps speed transmissions between different regions of the brain, also continues to
grow (Rapoport et al., 1999).
Adolescents often seem to act impulsively, rather than thoughtfully, and this may be in part
because the development of the prefrontal cortex is, in general, slower than the development of
the emotional parts of the brain, including the limbic system (Blakemore, 2008). Furthermore,
the hormonal surge that is associated with puberty, which primarily influences emotional
responses, may create strong emotions and lead to impulsive behaviour. It has been hypothesized
that adolescents may engage in risky behaviour, such as smoking, drug use, dangerous driving,
and unprotected sex, in part because they have not yet fully acquired the mental ability to curb
impulsive behaviour or to make entirely rational judgments (Steinberg, 2007).
The new cognitive abilities that are attained during adolescence may also give rise to new
feelings of egocentrism, in which adolescents believe that they can do anything and that they
know better than anyone else, including their parents (Elkind, 1978). Teenagers are likely to be
highly self-conscious, often creating an imaginary audience in which they feel that everyone is
constantly watching them (Goossens, Beyers, Emmen, & van Aken, 2002). Because teens think
so much about themselves, they mistakenly believe that others must be thinking about them, too
(Rycek, Stuhr, McDermott, Benker, & Swartz, 1998). It is no wonder that everything a teen’s
parents do suddenly feels embarrassing to them when they are in public.
Some of the most important changes that occur during adolescence involve the further
development of the self-concept and the development of new attachments. Whereas young
children are most strongly attached to their parents, the important attachments of adolescents
move increasingly away from parents and increasingly toward peers (Harris, 1998). As a result,
parents’ influence diminishes at this stage.
One approach to assessing identity development was proposed by James Marcia (1980). In his
approach, adolescents are asked questions regarding their exploration of and commitment to
issues related to occupation, politics, religion, and sexual behaviour. The responses to the
questions allow the researchers to classify the adolescent into one of four identity categories (see
Table 7.4, “James Marcia’s Stages of Identity Development”).
Table 7.4 James Marcia’s Stages of Identity Development. Adapted from Marcia (1980).</caption
[Skip Table]
Identity-diffusion The individual does not have firm commitments regarding the issues in
status question and is not making progress toward them.
The individual has not engaged in any identity experimentation and has
Foreclosure status
established an identity based on the choices or values of others.
The individual is exploring various choices but has not yet made a clear
Moratorium status
commitment to any of them.
Identity- The individual has attained a coherent and committed identity based on
achievement status personal decisions.
Studies assessing how teens pass through Marcia’s stages show that, although most teens
eventually succeed in developing a stable identity, the path to it is not always easy and there are
many routes that can be taken. Some teens may simply adopt the beliefs of their parents or the
first role that is offered to them, perhaps at the expense of searching for other, more promising
possibilities (foreclosure status). Other teens may spend years trying on different possible
identities (moratorium status) before finally choosing one.
To help them work through the process of developing an identity, teenagers may well try out
different identities in different social situations. They may maintain one identity at home and a
different type of persona when they are with their peers. Eventually, most teenagers do integrate
the different possibilities into a single self-concept and a comfortable sense of identity (identity-
achievement status).
For teenagers, the peer group provides valuable information about the self-concept. For instance,
in response to the question “What were you like as a teenager? (e.g., cool, nerdy, awkward?),”
posed on the website Answerbag, one teenager replied in this way:
I’m still a teenager now, but from 8th-9th grade I didn’t really know what I wanted at all. I was smart, so I
hung out with the nerdy kids. I still do; my friends mean the world to me. But in the middle of 8th I
started hanging out with whom you may call the “cool” kids…and I also hung out with some stoners, just
for variety. I pierced various parts of my body and kept my grades up. Now, I’m just trying to find who I
am. I’m even doing my sophomore year in China so I can get a better view of what I want. (Answerbag,
2007)
Responses like this one demonstrate the extent to which adolescents are developing their self-
concepts and self-identities and how they rely on peers to help them do that. The writer here is
trying out several (perhaps conflicting) identities, and the identities any teen experiments with
are defined by the group the person chooses to be a part of. The friendship groups (cliques,
crowds, or gangs) that are such an important part of the adolescent experience allow the young
adult to try out different identities, and these groups provide a sense of belonging and acceptance
(Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 2006). A big part of what the adolescent is learning is social
identity, the part of the self-concept that is derived from one’s group memberships. Adolescents
define their social identities according to how they are similar to and differ from others, finding
meaning in the sports, religious, school, gender, and ethnic categories they belong to.
The independence that comes with adolescence requires independent thinking as well as the
development of morality — standards of behaviour that are generally agreed on within a
culture to be right or proper. Just as Piaget believed that children’s cognitive development
follows specific patterns, Lawrence Kohlberg (1984) argued that children learn their moral
values through active thinking and reasoning, and that moral development follows a series of
stages. To study moral development, Kohlberg posed moral dilemmas to children, teenagers, and
adults, such as the following:
In Europe, a woman was near death from a special kind of cancer. There was one drug that the
doctors thought might save her. It was a form of radium that a druggist in the same town had
recently discovered. The drug was expensive to make, but the druggist was charging 10 times
what the drug cost him to make. He paid $400 for the radium and charged $4,000 for a small
dose of the drug. The sick woman’s husband, Heinz, went to everyone he knew to borrow the
money and tried every legal means, but he could only get together about $2,000, which is half of
what it cost. He told the druggist that his wife was dying and asked him to sell it cheaper or let
him pay later. But the druggist said, “No, I discovered the drug and I’m going to make money
from it.” So, having tried every legal means, Heinz gets desperate and considers breaking into
the man’s store to steal the drug for his wife.
As you can see in Table 7.5, “Lawrence Kohlberg’s Stages of Moral Reasoning,” Kohlberg
concluded, on the basis of their responses to the moral questions, that, as children develop
intellectually, they pass through three stages of moral thinking: the preconventional level,
the conventional level, and the postconventional level.
[Skip Table]
Although research has supported Kohlberg’s idea that moral reasoning changes from an early
emphasis on punishment and social rules and regulations to an emphasis on more general ethical
principles, as with Piaget’s approach, Kohlberg’s stage model is probably too simple. For one,
children may use higher levels of reasoning for some types of problems, but revert to lower
levels in situations where doing so is more consistent with their goals or beliefs (Rest,
1979). Second, it has been argued that the stage model is particularly appropriate for Western,
rather than non-Western, samples in which allegiance to social norms (such as respect for
authority) may be particularly important (Haidt, 2001). And there is frequently little correlation
between how children score on the moral stages and how they behave in real life.
Perhaps the most important critique of Kohlberg’s theory is that it may describe the moral
development of boys better than it describes that of girls. Carol Gilligan (1982) has argued that,
because of differences in their socialization, males tend to value principles of justice and rights,
whereas females value caring for and helping others. Although there is little evidence that boys
and girls score differently on Kohlberg’s stages of moral development (Turiel, 1998), it is true
that girls and women tend to focus more on issues of caring, helping, and connecting with others
than do boys and men (Jaffee & Hyde, 2000). If you don’t believe this, ask yourself when you
last got a thank-you note from a man.
Key Takeaways
Adolescence is the period of time between the onset of puberty and emerging adulthood.
Emerging adulthood is the period from age 18 years until the mid-20s in which young people
begin to form bonds outside the family, attend university, and find work. Even so, they tend
not to be fully independent and have not taken on all the responsibilities of adulthood. This
stage is most prevalent in Western cultures.
Puberty is a developmental period in which hormonal changes cause rapid physical alterations in
the body.
The cerebral cortex continues to develop during adolescence and early adulthood, enabling
improved reasoning, judgment, impulse control, and long-term planning.
A defining aspect of adolescence is the development of a consistent and committed self-identity.
The process of developing an identity can take time but most adolescents succeed in
developing a stable identity.
Kohlberg’s theory proposes that moral reasoning is divided into the following stages:
preconventional morality, conventional morality, and postconventional morality.
Kohlberg’s theory of morality has been expanded and challenged, particularly by Gilligan, who
has focused on differences in morality between boys and girls.
1.Based on what you learned in this chapter, do you think that people should be allowed to drive
at age 16? Why or why not? At what age do you think they should be allowed to vote and to
drink alcohol?
2.Think about your experiences in high school. What sort of cliques or crowds were there? How
did people express their identities in these groups? How did you use your groups to define
yourself and develop your own identity?
References
Anderson, S. E., Dannal, G. E., & Must, A. (2003). Relative weight and race influence average
age at menarche: Results from two nationally representative surveys of U.S. girls studied 25
years apart. Pediatrics, 111, 844–850.
Answerbag. (2007, March 20). What were you like as a teenager? (e.g., cool, nerdy, awkward?).
Retrieved from http://www.answerbag.com/q_view/171753
Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1986). How adolescence became the struggle for self: A
historical transformation of psychological development. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald
(Eds.), Psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 3, pp. 183–201). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Elkind, D. (1978). The child’s reality: Three developmental themes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Ge, X., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1996). Coming of age too early: Pubertal influences
on girls’ vulnerability to psychological distress. Child Development, 67(6), 3386–3400.
Goldberg, E. (2001). The executive brain: Frontal lobes and the civilized mind. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Goossens, L., Beyers, W., Emmen, M., & van Aken, M. (2002). The imaginary audience and
personal fable: Factor analyses and concurrent validity of the “new look” measures. Journal of
Research on Adolescence, 12(2), 193–215.
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral
judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.
Harris, J. (1998). The nurture assumption — Why children turn out the way they do. New York,
NY: Free Press.
Jaffee, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2000). Gender differences in moral orientation: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 126(5), 703–726.
Marshall, W. A., & Tanner, J. M. (1986). Puberty. In F. Falkner & J. M. Tanner (Eds.), Human
growth: A comprehensive treatise (2nd ed., pp. 171–209). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Mendle, J., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2007). Detrimental psychological outcomes
associated with early pubertal timing in adolescent girls. Developmental Review, 27, 151–171.
Pescovitz, O. H., & Walvoord, E. C. (2007). When puberty is precocious: Scientific and clinical
aspects. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
Rapoport, J. L., Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Hamburger, S., Jeffries, N., Fernandez, T.,…Evans,
A. (1999). Progressive cortical change during adolescence in childhood-onset schizophrenia: A
longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(7), 649–654.
Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Parker, J. G. (2006). Peer interactions, relationships, and
groups. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:
Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 571–645). Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley & Sons.
Rycek, R. F., Stuhr, S. L., McDermott, J., Benker, J., & Swartz, M. D. (1998). Adolescent
egocentrism and cognitive functioning during late adolescence. Adolescence, 33, 746–750.
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral
science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 55–59.
Twenge, J. M. (2006). Generation me: Why today’s young Americans are more confident,
assertive, entitled — and more miserable than ever before. New York, NY: Free Press.
Weinberger, D. R., Elvevåg, B., & Giedd, J. N. (2005). The adolescent brain: A work in progress
[PDF] National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy. Retrieved
from http://www.thenationalcampaign.org/resources/pdf/BRAIN.pdf
Previous: 7.2 Infancy and Childhood: Exploring and Learning
BACK TO TOP
LICENSE
Module 7: Adolescence
Adolescents are now able to analyze situations logically in terms of cause and effect
and to entertain hypothetical situations and entertain what-if possibilities about the
world. This higher-level thinking allows them to think about the future, evaluate
alternatives, and set personal goals. Although there are marked individual differences in
cognitive development among teens, these new capacities allow adolescents to engage
in the kind of introspection and mature decision making that was previously beyond their
cognitive capacity.
LEARNING OUTCOMES
Figure 1. Adolescents practice their developing abstract and hypothetical thinking skills, coming up with
alternative interpretations of information.
In the last of the Piagetian stages, a child becomes able to reason not only about
tangible objects and events, but also about hypothetical or abstract ones. Hence it has
the name formal operational stage—the period when the individual can “operate” on
“forms” or representations. This allows an individual to think and reason with a wider
perspective. This stage of cognitive development, termed by Piaget as formal
operational thought, marks a movement from an ability to think and reason from
concrete visible events to an ability to think hypothetically and entertain what-if
possibilities about the world. An individual can solve problems through abstract
concepts and utilize hypothetical and deductive reasoning. Adolescents use trial and
error to solve problems, and the ability to systematically solve a problem in a logical and
methodical way emerges.
WATCH IT
This video explains some of the cognitive development consistent with formal operational
thought.
School is a main contributor in guiding students towards formal operational thought. With
students at this level, the teacher can pose hypothetical (or contrary-to-fact) problems:
“What if the world had never discovered oil?” or “What if the first European explorers had
settled first in California instead of on the East Coast of the United States?” To answer such
questions, students must use hypothetical reasoning, meaning that they must manipulate ideas
that vary in several ways at once, and do so entirely in their minds.
The hypothetical reasoning that concerned Piaget primarily involved scientific problems. His
studies of formal operational thinking therefore often look like problems that middle or high
school teachers pose in science classes. In one problem, for example, a young person is
presented with a simple pendulum, to which different amounts of weight can be hung (Inhelder
& Piaget, 1958). The experimenter asks: “What determines how fast the pendulum swings: the
length of the string holding it, the weight attached to it, or the distance that it is pulled to the
side?” The young person is not allowed to solve this problem by trial-and-error with the
materials themselves, but must reason a way to the solution mentally. To do so systematically,
he or she must imagine varying each factor separately, while also imagining the other factors
that are held constant. This kind of thinking requires facility at manipulating mental
representations of the relevant objects and actions—precisely the skill that defines formal
operations.
As you might suspect, students with an ability to think hypothetically have an advantage in many
kinds of school work: by definition, they require relatively few “props” to solve problems. In this
sense they can in principle be more self-directed than students who rely only on concrete
operations—certainly a desirable quality in the opinion of most teachers. Note, though, that
formal operational thinking is desirable but not sufficient for school success, and that it is far
from being the only way that students achieve educational success. Formal thinking skills do not
insure that a student is motivated or well-behaved, for example, nor does it guarantee other
desirable skills. The fourth stage in Piaget’s theory is really about a particular kind of formal
thinking, the kind needed to solve scientific problems and devise scientific experiments. Since
many people do not normally deal with such problems in the normal course of their lives, it
should be no surprise that research finds that many people never achieve or use formal thinking
fully or consistently, or that they use it only in selected areas with which they are very familiar
(Case & Okomato, 1996). For teachers, the limitations of Piaget’s ideas suggest a need for
additional theories about development—ones that focus more directly on the social and
interpersonal issues of childhood and adolescence.
Hypothetical and abstract thinking
One of the major premises of formal operational thought is the capacity to think of
possibility, not just reality. Adolescents’ thinking is less bound to concrete events than
that of children; they can contemplate possibilities outside the realm of what currently
exists. One manifestation of the adolescent’s increased facility with thinking about
possibilities is the improvement of skill in deductive reasoning (also called top-down
reasoning), which leads to the development of hypothetical thinking. This provides
the ability to plan ahead, see the future consequences of an action and to provide
alternative explanations of events. It also makes adolescents more skilled debaters, as
they can reason against a friend’s or parent’s assumptions. Adolescents also develop a
more sophisticated understanding of probability.
Metacognition
However, more often than not the audience is imaginary because in actual social
situations individuals are not usually the sole focus of public attention. Elkind believed
that the construction of imaginary audiences would partially account for a wide variety of
typical adolescent behaviors and experiences; and imaginary audiences played a role in
the self-consciousness that emerges in early adolescence. However, since the
audience is usually the adolescent’s own construction, it is privy to his or her own
knowledge of him/herself. According to Elkind, the notion of imaginary audience helps to
explain why adolescents usually seek privacy and feel reluctant to reveal themselves–it
is a reaction to the feeling that one is always on stage and constantly under the critical
scrutiny of others.
Elkind also addressed that adolescents have a complex set of beliefs that their own
feelings are unique and they are special and immortal. Personal fable is the term
Elkind created to describe this notion, which is the complement of the construction of
imaginary audience. Since an adolescent usually fails to differentiate their own
perceptions and those of others, they tend to believe that they are of importance to so
many people (the imaginary audiences) that they come to regard their feelings as
something special and unique. They may feel that only they have experienced strong
and diverse emotions, and therefore others could never understand how they feel. This
uniqueness in one’s emotional experiences reinforces the adolescent’s belief of
invincibility, especially to death.
This adolescent belief in personal uniqueness and invincibility becomes an illusion that
they can be above some of the rules, disciplines and laws that apply to other people;
even consequences such as death (called the invincibility fable). This belief that one
is invincible removes any impulse to control one’s behavior (Lin, 2016). [1] Therefore,
adolescents will engage in risky behaviors, such as drinking and driving or unprotected
sex, and feel they will not suffer any negative consequences.
TRY IT
Piaget emphasized the sequence of thought throughout four stages. Others suggest
that thinking does not develop in sequence, but instead, that advanced logic in
adolescence may be influenced by intuition. Cognitive psychologists often refer to
intuitive and analytic thought as the dual-process model; the notion that humans have
two distinct networks for processing information (Kuhn, 2013.) [2] Intuitive thought is
automatic, unconscious, and fast, and it is more experiential and emotional.
In contrast, analytic thought is deliberate, conscious, and rational (logical). While these
systems interact, they are distinct (Kuhn, 2013). Intuitive thought is easier, quicker, and
more commonly used in everyday life. As discussed in the adolescent brain
development section earlier in this module, the discrepancy between the maturation of
the limbic system and the prefrontal cortex, may make teens more prone to emotional
intuitive thinking than adults. As adolescents develop, they gain in logic/analytic thinking
ability and sometimes regress, with social context, education, and experiences
becoming major influences. Simply put, being “smarter” as measured by an intelligence
test does not advance cognition as much as having more experience, in school and in
life (Klaczynski & Felmban, 2014). [3]
Risk-taking
Because most injuries sustained by adolescents are related to risky behavior (alcohol
consumption and drug use, reckless or distracted driving, and unprotected sex), a great
deal of research has been done on the cognitive and emotional processes underlying
adolescent risk-taking. In addressing this question, it is important to distinguish whether
adolescents are more likely to engage in risky behaviors (prevalence), whether they
make risk-related decisions similarly or differently than adults (cognitive processing
perspective), or whether they use the same processes but value different things and
thus arrive at different conclusions. The behavioral decision-making theory proposes
that adolescents and adults both weigh the potential rewards and consequences of an
action. However, research has shown that adolescents seem to give more weight to
rewards, particularly social rewards, than do adults. Adolescents value social warmth
and friendship, and their hormones and brains are more attuned to those values than to
long-term consequences (Crone & Dahl, 2012). [4]
Figure 2. Teenage thinking is characterized by the ability to reason logically and solve hypothetical problems
such as how to design, plan, and build a structure. (credit: U.S. Army RDECOM)
Some have argued that there may be evolutionary benefits to an increased propensity
for risk-taking in adolescence. For example, without a willingness to take risks,
teenagers would not have the motivation or confidence necessary to leave their family
of origin. In addition, from a population perspective, there is an advantage to having a
group of individuals willing to take more risks and try new methods, counterbalancing
the more conservative elements more typical of the received knowledge held by older
adults.
Relativistic Thinking
Adolescents are more likely to engage in relativistic thinking—in other words, they are
more likely to question others’ assertions and less likely to accept information as
absolute truth. Through experience outside the family circle, they learn that rules they
were taught as absolute are actually relativistic. They begin to differentiate between
rules crafted from common sense (don’t touch a hot stove) and those that are based on
culturally relative standards (codes of etiquette). This can lead to a period of questioning
authority in all domains.
As we continue through this module, we will discuss how this influences moral
reasoning, as well as psychosocial and emotional development. These more abstract
developmental dimensions (cognitive, moral, emotional, and social dimensions) are not
only more subtle and difficult to measure, but these developmental areas are also
difficult to tease apart from one another due to the inter-relationships among them. For
instance, our cognitive maturity will influence the way we understand a particular event
or circumstance, which will in turn influence our moral judgments about it, and our
emotional responses to it. Similarly, our moral code and emotional maturity influence
the quality of our social relationships with others.
Secondary Education
Adolescents spend more waking time in school than in any other context (Eccles &
Roeser, 2011). Secondary education is traditionally grades 7-12 and denotes the
school years after elementary school (known as primary education) and before college
or university (known as tertiary education). Adolescents who complete primary
education (learning to read and write) and continue on through secondary and tertiary
education tend to also have better health, wealth, and family life (Rieff, 1998).
[5]
Because the average age of puberty has declined over the years, middle
schools were created for grades 5 or 6 through 8 as a way to distinguish between early
adolescence and late adolescence, especially because these adolescents different
biologically, cognitively and emotionally and definitely have different needs.
Transition to middle school is stressful and the transition is often complex. When
students transition from elementary to middle school, many students are undergoing
physical, intellectual, social, emotional, and moral changes (Parker, 2013). [6] Research
suggests that early adolescence is an especially sensitive developmental period (McGill
et al., 2012).[7] Some students mature faster than others. Students who are
developmentally behind typically experience more stress than their counterparts (U.S.
Department of Education, 2008).[8] Consequently, they may earn lower grades and
display decreased academic motivation, which may increase the rate of dropping out of
school (U.S. Department of Education, 2008). For many middle school students,
academic achievement slows down and behavioral problems can increase.
Bullying is unwanted, aggressive behavior among school aged children that involves a real or
perceived power imbalance. The behavior is repeated, or has the potential to be repeated, over
time. Both kids who are bullied and who bully others may have serious, lasting problems. It is a
prevalent problem during the middle and high school years, exacerbated by access to
technology and the means to easily spread damaging information online. These are some key
statistics about bullying from StopBullying.gov:
Been Bullied
Bullied Others
Seen Bullying
o 70.6% of young people say they have seen bullying in their schools.
o 70.4% of school staff have seen bullying. 62% witnessed bullying
two or more times in the last month and 41% witness bullying once a
week or more.
o When bystanders intervene, bullying stops within 10 seconds 57% of
the time.
o
Figure 3. Cyberbullying comes in many forms.
Been Cyberbullied
Types of Bullying
o The most common types of bullying are verbal and social. Physical
bullying happens less often. Cyberbullying happens the least
frequently.
o According to one large study, the following percentages of middle
schools students had experienced these various types of bullying:
name calling (44.2 %); teasing (43.3 %); spreading rumors or lies
(36.3%); pushing or shoving (32.4%); hitting, slapping, or kicking
(29.2%); leaving out (28.5%); threatening (27.4%); stealing
belongings (27.3%); sexual comments or gestures (23.7%); e-mail or
blogging (9.9%).
High School
As adolescents enter into high school, their continued cognitive development allows
them to think abstractly, analytically, hypothetically, and logically, which is all formal
operational thought. High school emphasizes formal thinking in attempt to prepare
graduates for college where analysis is required. Overall, high school graduation rates
in the United States have increased steadily over the past decade, reaching 83.2
percent in 2016 after four years in high school (Gewertz, 2017). [13] Additionally, many
students in the United States do attend college. Unfortunately, though, about half of
those who go to college leave without a degree (Kena et al., 2016). [14] Those that do
earn a degree, however, do make more money and have an easier time finding
employment. The key here is understanding adolescent development and supporting
teens in making decisions about college or alternatives to college after high school.
LINK TO LEARNING
What do you think, is college necessary? Is it worth the investment? Read the article “Is
College Necessary?” from Psychology Today geared towards parents who can help their
teenager decide if college is right for them.
Academic Achievement
Figure 4. Adolescents’ moral development gets put to the test in real life situations, often along with peer
pressure to behave or not behave in particular ways.
Adolescents are receptive to their culture, to the models they see at home, in school
and in the mass media. These observations influence moral reasoning and moral
behavior. When children are younger, their family, culture, and religion greatly influence
their moral decision-making. During the early adolescent period, peers have a much
greater influence. Peer pressure can exert a powerful influence because friends play a
more significant role in teens’ lives. Furthermore, the new ability to think abstractly
enables youth to recognize that rules are simply created by other people. As a result,
teens begin to question the absolute authority of parents, schools, government, and
other traditional institutions (Vera-Estay, Dooley, & Beauchamp, 2014) [15] By late
adolescence, most teens are less rebellious as they have begun to establish their own
identity, their own belief system, and their own place in the world.
Unfortunately, some adolescents have life experiences that may interfere with their
moral development. Traumatic experiences may cause them to view the world as unjust
and unfair. Additionally, social learning also impacts moral development. Adolescents
may have observed the adults in their lives making immoral decisions that disregarded
the rights and welfare of others, leading these youth to develop beliefs and values that
are contrary to the rest of society. That being said, adults have opportunities to support
moral development by modeling the moral character that we want to see in our children.
Parents are particularly important because they are generally the original source of
moral guidance. Authoritative parenting facilitates children’s moral growth better than
other parenting styles and one of the most influential things a parent can do is to
encourage the right kind of peer relations. [16] While parents may find this process of
moral development difficult or challenging, it is important to remember that this
developmental step is essential to their children’s well-being and ultimate success in
life.
LINK TO LEARNING
Parenting has the largest impact on adolescent moral development. Read more here in this
article, “Building Character: Moral Development in Adolescence” from the Center for Parent
and Teen Communication.
TRY IT
GLOSSARY
adolescent egocentrism:
a characteristic of adolescent thinking that leads young people (ages 10-13) to focus on
themselves to the exclusion of others (according to David Elkind)
analytic thought:
thought that results from analysis, such as a systematic ranking of pros and cons, risks
and consequences, possibilities and facts. Analytic thought depends on logic and
rationality
proposes that adolescents and adults both weigh the potential rewards and
consequences of an action. However, research has shown that adolescents seem to
give more weight to rewards, particularly social rewards, than do adults
constructivist perspective:
deductive reasoning:
reasoning from a general statement, premise, or principle, though logical steps to figure
out (deduce) specifics. Also called top-down processing
divided attention:
the ability to pay attention to two or more stimuli at the same time; this ability improves
during adolescence
the notion that two networks exist within the human brain, one for emotional processing
of stimuli and one for analytic reasoning
the fourth and final stage of Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, characterized by
more systematic logical thinking and by the ability to understand and systematically
manipulate abstract concepts
hypothetical thought:
reasoning that includes propositions and possibilities that may not reflect reality
imaginary audience:
the other people who, in an adolescent’s egocentric belief, are watching and taking note
of his or her appearance, ideas, and behavior. This belief makes many adolescents very
self-conscious
information-processing perspective:
derives from the study of artificial intelligence and explains cognitive development in
terms of the growth of specific components of the overall process of thinking
intuitive thought:
thoughts that arise from an emotion or a hunch, beyond rational explanation, and are
influenced by past experiences and cultural assumptions
invincibility fable:
metacognition:
refers to “thinking about thinking” and it is relevant in social cognition and results in
increased introspection, self-consciousness, and intellectualization during adolescence
middle school:
a school for children in the grades between elementary school and high school. Middle
school usually begins with grade 6 and ends with grade 8
mnemonic devices:
mental strategies to help learn and remember information more efficiently; improves
during adolescence
personal fable:
relativistic thinking:
secondary education:
the period after primary education (elementary or grade school) and before tertiary
education (college). It usually occurs from about ages 12 to 18, although there is some
variation by school and by nation
selective attention:
the process by which one focuses on one stimulus while tuning out another; this ability
improves during adolescence