Attachment, Abuse,: Among
Attachment, Abuse,: Among
Attachment, Abuse,: Among
Stephen W. Smallbone
1.3 and Billee-Anne 2
McCabe
Written autobiographies of48 incarcerated adult male sexual offenders (22 rapists,
13 intrafamilial child molesters, and 13 extrafamilial child molesters) were used to
generate retrospective self-report measures of their childhood maternal and pater-
nal attachment, childhood sexual abuse experiences, and onset of masturbation.
Contrary to expectation, the offenders as a combined group more often reported
secure than they did insecure childhood maternal and paternal attachment. There
were no differences between the three offender subgroups with respect to maternal
attachment; however the rapists and the intrafamilial child molesters were more
likely to report insecure paternal attachment than were the extrafamilial child mole-
sters. There were no differences between these offender subgroups in the frequency
with which childhood sexual abuse was reported. However, offenders with insecure
paternal attachment were more likely to report having been sexually abused than
were those with secure paternal attachment. Sexually abused offenders in turn re-
ported earlier onset of masturbation than did those who were not sexually abused.
These results are consistent with contemporary attachment models linking insecure
childhood attachment to childhood sexual abuse, and with traditional conditioning
models linking childhood sexual abuse, early masturbation, and sexual offending.
KEY WORDS: attachment; sexual abuse; masturbation; sexual offenders.
(1998, 2000) have further proposed that early disorganization of the childhood
attachment behavioral system may lead to reduced functional separation between
the adult attachment, sexual, and parenting behavioral systems, allowing other-
wise innocuous internal (e.g., subjective distress) and external cues (e.g., physical
contact with a child) to trigger inappropriate and coercive sexual behavior. Prelim-
inary empirical investigations suggest that childhood attachment insecurity may
be a general risk factor for criminality, and that paternal attachment insecurity may
be a more specific risk factor for sexual offending (Smallbone & Dadds, 2000).
Nonetheless, many sexual offenders report secure attachment relationships with
their parents.
Childhood attachment insecurity has been associated with a wide range of
negative sequelae, including conduct and substance abuse disorders; narcissistic,
borderline, and antisocial personality disorders; and aggressive behavior problems
(Jones, 1996; Lyons-Ruth, 1996). Childhood attachment insecurity has also been
theoretically linked with childhood sexual abuse. In particular, Alexander (1992)
has argued that parental unavailability and rejection leaves the child with reduced
parental supervision and protection, and with fewer psychological resources with
which to resist sexual abuse. Alexander (1992) has also argued that the negative
consequences of sexual abuse for children with insecure attachment relationships
are likely to be more severe than for children with secure attachment relationships,
because insecure children will have a less functional family support system avail-
able to them. Thus, childhood attachment insecurity may both increase the risk of
sexual abuse, and lead to more negative outcomes.
Although the prevalence and significance of childhood sexual abuse among
sexual offenders remains controversial (see e.g., Freund, Watson, & Dickey, 1990),
empirical evidence from correctional surveys and clinical studies suggests that sex-
ual offenders are more likely than both nonoffenders and nonsexual offenders to
report having been sexually abused (Bureau of Justice Statistics, 1997; Dawhan
& Marshall, 1996). Psychodynamic explanations linking childhood sexual abuse
and later sexual offending typically involve the operation of complex, unobserv-
able intrapsychic processes, such as victim identification with the offender and
psychological resolution of sexual trauma (Groth, 1979). Learning models are po-
tentially more easily tested, although evidence is scarce. Laws and Marshall (1990)
have described a range of classical conditioning, operant conditioning, and social
learning processes that may be involved in the acquisition of deviant sexual inter-
ests, illustrated with reference to early sexual experiences. One prominent model
involves the role of masturbation in facilitating learned associations between de-
viant sexual imagery and sexual arousal (McGuire, Carlisle, & Young, 1965). This
model suggests that childhood sexual abuse may serve to introduce the victim to
deviant sexual images that are subsequently used during masturbation. It is thus
plausible that childhood sexual abuse may trigger premature masturbation, which
may in turn provide a means to acquire and maintain deviant sexual interests.
Several testable hypotheses may be derived from the attachment and con-
ditioning theory formulations discussed above. Specifically, the following three
hypotheses were tested in a sample of convicted sexual offenders:
1. Sexual offenders will be more likely to have experienced insecure than
secure childhood attachment.
2. Sexual offenders who have experienced insecure childhood attachment
will be more likely to have been sexually abused than will those who have
experienced secure childhood attachment.
3. Sexual offenders who have been sexually abused will have begun mastur-
bating earlier than will those who have not been sexually abused.
METHOD
Participants
Written autobiographies of 48 incarcerated adult male sexual offenders
(22 rapists, 13 intrafamilial child molesters, and 13 extrafamilial child molesters)
were used to generate measures of childhood attachment, childhood sexual abuse,
and onset of masturbation. The offenders had submitted their autobiographies as a
age between the intrafamilial child molesters, extrafamilial child molesters, and
rapists.
Forty percent (40%) of the offenders had previously been convicted of at
least one sexual offense, and 25% had previously been convicted of a nonsexual
violent offense. Extrafamilial child molesters (69.2%) were more likely than either
the intrafamilial child molesters (30.8%) or the rapists (27.3%) to have previously
been convicted of a sexual offense, X (2, N = 48) 6.59, p < .04. There were
=
Measures
1. In what ways did your mother/father show that she/he accepted you?
2. In what ways was she/he rejecting of you?
3. Was your mother/father inconsistent (sometimes warm, sometimes not)?
4. Was your mother/father ever abusive or violent toward you?
5. When did you start to masturbate?
6. Before you were 16 years old, did you ever have sexual contact with an
adult? What were the circumstances of this?
RESULTS
Attachment Insecurity
The first hypothesis predicted that the sexual offenders would be more likely
to report insecure than secure childhood attachment. Almost one third of the of-
fenders (31.3%) had experienced insecure maternal attachment, and 40.5% had
experienced insecure paternal attachment. Thus, as a combined group, the offend-
ers in this study were not more likely to have experienced insecure than secure
maternal or paternal attachment. There were no differences between the extrafa-
milial child molesters, intrafamilial child molesters, and rapists in maternal at-
tachment security. However, the intrafamilial child molesters (58.3%) and rapists
(47.4%) were more likely than were the extrafamilial child molesters (9.1 %) to
report insecure paternal attachment, X (2, N = 42) = 6.46, p < .05.
Because approximately 38% of the general population is likely to have ex-
perienced insecure childhood attachment (Campos, Barrett, Lamb, Goldsmith, &
Stenberg, 1983), the present sample of incarcerated sexual offenders cannot as a
whole be considered to have experienced more childhood attachment insecurity
than the general population.
chi-square analyses were used to compare the frequency of childhood sexual abuse
among secure maternal (N 33) versus insecure maternal attachment (N = 15),
=
The third hypothesis that childhood sexual abuse would be associated with
early onset of masturbation was tested by conducting an independent groups t test.
Presence (N 21 ) or absence (N 26) of childhood sexual abuse was used as the
= =
independent variable, and age of onset of masturbation was used as the dependent
variable. Results showed that those offenders who had been sexually abused had
begun to masturbate significantly earlier (11.0 years) than had those who had not
been sexually abused (13.4 years), t (45) 2.46, p < .02.
=
DISCUSSION
1. &dquo; ,
Contrary to our expectations, the sexual offenders in this study were on the
whole no more likely to have experienced insecure than they were to have ex-
perienced secure childhood attachment. Moreover, the frequency with which the
present sample reported insecure childhood attachment was no greater than would
be expected in the general population (see e.g., Campos, et al., 1983). There were,
however, significant differences between subgroups of offenders, with rapists and
intrafamilial child molesters more likely than extrafamilial child molesters to report
insecure attachment relationships with their fathers. The finding that extrafamilial
child molesters experienced secure maternal and paternal attachment is consistent
with previous findings that this subgroup tends to report positive childhood attach-
ment experiences (Smallbone & Dadds, 1998). It is not clear why this should be the
case, because extrafamilial child molesters are often, as in this study, shown to be
more chronic offenders (Marshall & Barbaree, 1990), and are more likely to have a
diagnosable sexual disorder (Barbaree & Seto, 1997), than rapists and intrafamil-
ial child molesters. Further research is clearly needed to identify developmental
antecedents among extrafamilial child molesters. In particular, measures that are
relatively free from subjective biases may be needed to identify developmental
problems in this subgroup.
The importance of insecure paternal attachment has been suggested by previ-
ous studies, which have found rapists to report more disturbed relationships with
their fathers than do nonoffenders, nonsexual offenders, and other types of sexual
offenders (Smallbone & Dadds, 1998), and insecure paternal attachment to pre-
dict coercive sexual behavior in nonoffenders (Smallbone & Dadds, 2000). In the
present study, insecure paternal attachment, but not insecure maternal attachment,
was found to be associated with childhood sexual abuse. Less is known about the
Among the sexually abused group, onset of masturbation was on average less than
1 year after the sexual abuse had occurred. It is thus plausible that images associated
with sexual abuse experiences may be introduced into early masturbatory fantasies.
Through the process of second-order classical conditioning (see e.g., Laws &
Marshall, 1990), these fantasies may be partly responsible for the acquisition of
deviant sexual interests in some boys who are sexually abused.
Taken together, the results of this study suggest that many (but by no means
all) sexual offenders experience insecure childhood attachment, that offenders with
insecure paternal attachment are more likely to have been sexually abused, and that
sexually abused offenders tend to have engaged prematurely in masturbation. This
sequence of childhood and early adolescent experiences, which was predicted from
a combination of established attachment and conditioning theory formulations,
Although the hypotheses tested in this study were theoretically derived, and
were in a number of respects supported by the results, the above conclusions should
be considered tentative. First, the small numbers of offender autobiographies avail-
able for the present investigation provided relatively low statistical power. This was
a more serious problem for examining differences between offender subgroups;
adequate power was available for analyses of differences within the combined
offender group. Second, given that the data were self-reported in a treatment set-
ting, substantial biases are likely to have confounded the results. Collecting data
in a treatment setting will also, of course, limit the extent to which the find-
ings can be generalized. Third, although the offender autobiographies represent a
unique and potentially rich source of data, the measures derived from this source
lack standardization, and provide only face validity. Finally, the pattern of results
found in this study needs to be replicated before more confident conclusions can
be drawn.
These limitations notwithstanding, this study provides empirical evidence for
positive associations between insecure paternal attachment and childhood sexual
abuse, and between childhood sexual abuse and early onset of masturbation. Be-
cause the testing of developmental models has clear and important implications for
prevention and early intervention, we would hope that this study provides a sound
basis for further theoretical and empirical exploration of developmental factors in
sexual offending. In particular, differences in maternal and paternal influences on
the development of sexual offending warrant further attention.
REFERENCES
Alexander, P. (1992). Application of attachment theory to the study of sexual abuse. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 185-195.
Barbaree, H. E., & Seto, M. C. (1997). Pedophilia: Assessment and treatment. In D. R. Laws, &
W. O’Donohue (Eds.), Sexual deviance: Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 175-193). New
York: Guilford.
Bureau of Justice Statistics. (1997). Sex offenses and offenders: An analysis of data on rape and sexual
assault. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.
Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg, C. (1983). Socioemotional
development. In M. M. Haith &J. J. Campos (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy
and psychobiology (pp. 783-915). New York: Wiley.
Cohen, M. L., Seghorn, T., & Calmas, W. (1969). Sociometric study of sex offenders. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 94, 249-255.
Dawhan, S., & Marshall, W. L. (1996). Sexual abuse histories of sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse: A
Journal of Research and Treatment, 8, 7-15.
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and research. New York: Free Press.
Freund, K., Watson, R., & Dickey, R. (1990). Does sexual abuse in childhood cause pedophilia: An
exploratory study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 557-568.
Groth, A. N. (1979). Men who rape: The psychology of the offender. New York: Plenum.
Groth, A. N., Burgess, A. W., & Holstrom, L. L. (1977). Rape: Power, anger, and sexuality. American
Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1239-1243.
Jones, E. E. (1996). Introduction to the special section on attachment and psychopathology: Part 1.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 5-7.
Laws, D. R., & Marshall, W. L. (1990). A conditioning theory of the etiology and maintenance of
deviant sexual preference and behavior. In W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.),
Handbook of sexual assault: Issues, theories, and treatment of the offender (pp. 209-229). New
York: Plenum.
Lyons-Ruth, K. (1996). Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior problems:
The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
, 64-73.
64
Malamuth, N. M., Heavey, C. L., & Linz, D. (1993). Predicting men’s antisocial behavior against
women: The interaction model of sexual aggression. In G. C. N. Hall, R. Hirschman, J. R. Graham,
& M. S. Zaragoza (Eds.), Sexual aggression: Issues in etiology, assessment and treatment (pp. 63-
97). Washington, DC: Taylor and Francis.
Marshall, W. L. (1989). Intimacy, loneliness and sexual offenders. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
27,491-503.
Marshall, W. L., & Barbaree, H. E. (1990). An integrated theory of the etiology of sexual offending.
In W. L. Marshall, D. R. Laws, & H. E. Barbaree (Eds.), Handbook of sexual assault: Issues,
theories, and treatment of the offender (pp. 257-275). New York: Plenum.
McGuire, R. J., Carlisle, J. M., & Young, B. G. (1965). Sexual deviations as conditioned behavior: A
hypothesis. Behavior Research and Therapy, 2, 185-190.
Smallbone, S. W., & Dadds, M. R. (1998). Childhood attachment and adult attachment in incarcerated
adult male sex offenders. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 13, 555-573.
Smallbone, S. W., & Dadds, M. R. (2000). Attachment and coercive sexual behavior. Sexual Abuse: A
Journal of Research and Treatment, 12,
3-15.
Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., & Marshall, W. L. (1996). Attachment style in sex offenders: A preliminary
study. The Journal of Sex Research, 33, 17-26.
Ward, T., Hudson, S. M., Marshall, W. L., & Siegert, R. J. (1995). Attachment style and intimacy
deficits in sexual offenders: A theoretical framework. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and
Treatment, 7, 317-335.