Theoretical Models of Culture Shock and Adaptation in International Students in Higher Education
Theoretical Models of Culture Shock and Adaptation in International Students in Higher Education
Theoretical Models of Culture Shock and Adaptation in International Students in Higher Education
To cite this article: Yuefang Zhou, Divya Jindal-Snape, Keith Topping & John Todman (2008)
Theoretical models of culture shock and adaptation in international students in higher education,
Studies in Higher Education, 33:1, 63-75, DOI: 10.1080/03075070701794833
Introduction
Students attending universities in a culture different from their own have to contend with novel
social and educational organisations, behaviours and expectations – as well as dealing with the
problems of adjustment common to students in general. This is difficult enough when the
newcomer is aware of the differences in advance, but even more difficult when the newcomer is
unaware and falsely assumes that the new society operates like their home country. Newcomers
easily become ‘lost in translation’. The collective impact of such unfamiliar experiences on
cultural travellers in general has been termed ‘culture shock’. Student sojourners are an example
of such travellers, increasing in numbers in many English-speaking countries.
There are estimated to be more than a million students and scholars attending institutions of
higher education abroad (Hayes 1998; Taylor 2005). The quality of the psychological, sociocul-
tural and educational experiences of this large group of people is important, not least in promoting
global intercultural understanding. It is no surprise that the literature has been concerned with
students’ adaptation problems. Student sojourners are probably the best-researched group of
cross-cultural travellers, as they tend to be easily accessed as research participants. Many studies
have explored social and friendship networks (related to culture learning theory), social skill
acquisition (connected to stress and coping theory), and inter-group perceptions and relations
(linked with social identification theories).
This article reviews the development of theories of culture shock, considers their relevance to
the process of adaptation in student sojourners, and seeks to clarify and extend them in relation
to this group.
tend to be smaller. Systematic research on overseas students only appeared after the 1950s, when
there was a flood of research on their social and psychological problems (Ward, Bochner, and
Furnham 2001).
In describing and analysing students’ adaptation problems, researchers have been influenced
by the traditional perspectives on migration and mental health. In the past, two general explana-
tions were proposed to account for the association between migration and psychological prob-
lems. The first argued that there were predisposing factors that could lead to selective migration,
such as various characteristics of individuals, grief and bereavement (movement as response to
loss and possibly resulting in further loss), fatalism (abandonment of control or, in contrast, a
reactive attempt to seize control), and selective expectations of enhancement of life quality (that
might be more or less realistic). The second argued that mental health changes might be a conse-
quence of migration experiences, including negative life events, lack of social support networks
and the impact of value differences. Theoretical components of these two generalities (illustrating
the differences in origins and conceptual formulation) are listed in Table 1. This also includes
reference to a further formulation (’social skills and culture learning’), which goes beyond culture
shock and can be viewed as an intermediate approach with strong connections to contemporary
theories.
Many studies in the migration literature highlighted the negative aspects of exposure to
another culture, and this was perpetuated in much of the student sojourner literature. Ward,
Bochner and Furnham (2001: 36) observed that ‘the early theories applied to the study of inter-
national students were clinically oriented and strongly related to medical models of sojourner
adjustment’. There followed a gradual movement away from medical models, and researchers
started to question the implicit assumption that cross-cultural contact is so stressful as to necessi-
tate medical treatment (e.g. Bochner 1986).
By the 1980s, a different view had emerged that regarded sojourning as a learning experience
rather than a medical nuisance. It followed that appropriate positive action would include prepa-
ration and orientation, and the acquisition of skills relevant to the new culture (Bochner 1982;
Klineberg 1982). This new perspective viewed sojourning as a dynamic experience, both for
students and members of the host culture. The social skills and culture learning perspective began
to lay the foundation for the development of the culture learning model, which is explored below.
Culture learning
Furnham and Bochner (1986) strongly advocated the social skills/culture learning model, for its
theoretical robustness and because it also led to training methods. This approach developed into
contemporary ‘culture learning’ theory. It has its origin in social psychology, focusing primarily
on behavioural aspects of intercultural contact and regarding social interaction as a skilled and
mutually organised performance (Argyle 1969). ‘Shock’ is understood as the stimulus for acqui-
sition of culture-specific skills that are required to engage in new social interactions. The
process of adaptation is influenced by a number of variables, including: general knowledge
about a new culture (Ward and Searle 1991); length of residence in the host culture (Ward et al.
1998); language or communication competence (Furnham 1993); quantity and quality of contact
with host nationals (Bochner 1982); friendship networks (Bochner, McLeod, and Lin 1977);
previous experience abroad (Klineberg and Hull 1979); cultural distance (Ward and Kennedy
1993a, b); cultural identity (Ward and Searle 1991); acculturation modes (Ward and Kennedy
1994); temporary versus permanent residence in a new country (Ward and Kennedy 1993c); and
cross-cultural training (Deshpande and Viswesvaran 1992). This model leads to practical
guidelines for intervention in preparation, orientation and (especially) behavioural social skills
training.
Theoretical Intervention
Theory Theoretical origin Conceptual framework premise Factors affecting adjustment guidelines
Stress and Coping Social psychology – stress, Cross-cultural travellers Life changes are Adjustment factors involving Training people to
(Affect) appraisal and coping need to develop coping inherently both personal (e.g. life change, develop stress-
(Lazarus & Folkman strategies to deal with stressful personality) and situational management skills
1984); life events (Holmes stress (e.g. social support)
and Rahe 1967)
Culture Learning Social and experimental Cross-cultural travellers Social interaction Culture-specific variables such Preparation,
(Behaviour) psychology – social skills need to learn culturally is a mutually as: knowledge about a new orientation and culture
and interpersonal relevant social skills to organised and culture, language or learning, especially
behaviour (Argyle 1969) survive and thrive in their skilled communication competence, behavioural-based
new settings performance cultural distance social skill training
Social Identification Ethnic, cross-cultural and Cross-cultural transition Identity is a Cognitive variables such as: Enhancing self-esteem,
(Cognition) social psychology – self may involve changes in fundamental knowledge of the host culture, overcoming barriers to
(Deaux 1996; Social cultural identity and issue for the mutual attitude between hosts inter-group harmony,
Identity Theory, e.g. inter-group relations cross-cultural and sojourners, cultural emphasising inter-
Phinney 1990) travellers similarity, cultural identity group similarities
Studies in Higher Education 67
use to enhance self-esteem and overcome barriers to inter-group harmony include raising
awareness of the potentially negative aspects of the process, emphasising inter-group similarities
rather than differences, and getting people to imagine themselves in the role or identity of other
persons – ‘walk a mile in their shoes’.
Overall, the cognitive (C) perspective of the social identification theories complements the
behavioural (B) analysis provided by the culture learning approach and the affective (A) aspect
in the stress and coping framework. These three perspectives together offer a foundation for a
comprehensive model of cultural adaptation.
Acculturation model
Acculturation refers to the process of intercultural adaptation, though the definition of intercul-
tural adaptation is controversial (e.g. Mumford 1998). However, Ward and his colleagues finally
proposed that intercultural adaptation can be broadly divided into two categories: psychological,
mainly situated in a stress and coping framework, and sociocultural adaptation, situated
within the culture learning framework (Searle and Ward 1990; Ward and Kennedy 1992). The
acculturation model presented in Figure 1 (adapted from Ward, Bochner, and Furnham 2001)
Studies in Higher Education 69
Figure 1. The acculturation process (adapted from Ward, Bochner and Furnham 2001).
links the stress and coping perspective with the culture learning perspective, and distinguishes
psychological, sociocultural and cognitive outcomes while emphasising their interaction. This is
why we have adapted Ward, Bochner and Furnham’s model to include arrows from cognition in
the responses domain to both ‘psychological’ and ‘sociocultural’ in the outcome box.
This interactive and dynamic model sees cross-cultural transition as a significant life event
Figure 1. The acculturation process (adapted from Ward, Bochner and Furnham 2001).
that involves adaptive change. The major task facing individuals in cultural transition is the devel-
opment of stress-coping strategies and culturally relevant social skills. This will involve
responses in affect, behaviour and cognition for both stress-management and social skill acquisi-
tion, and should result in psychological adjustment and sociocultural adaptation. The model
incorporates a wide range of micro and macro level variables, with implications for future
research. At the micro-level, characteristics of both person and situation may be important. Indi-
vidual variables such as personality, language competence and cultural identity, and situational
factors such as length of cultural contact, cultural distance and social support are all relevant. At
the macro-level, society of origin and society of settlement are also important, and social,
political, economic and cultural factors are included.
This model is quite efficient in explaining the acculturation process. However, the relation-
ship between psychological adjustment and sociocultural adaptation is still not very clear. For
70 Y. Zhou et al.
example, how does this model explain a student who fits successfully into a different system of
teaching and learning, but still feels bad about the transition? Furthermore, within Ward, Bochner
and Furnham’s model the cognitive aspects of acculturation seem not well integrated with the
whole acculturation process. Finally, the relationship between students’ pedagogical adaptation
and their psychological and sociocultural adaptation requires further clarification.
Affective adaptation
Social support also impacts on affective outcomes, although research on friendship networks
places greater emphasis on the quantity and quality of actual support than the mere number of
networks. Social support from both host and co-nationals can contribute to enhancement of
student psychological well-being (e.g. Tanaka et al. 1997). Social support also alleviates ‘home-
sickness’ (Hannigan 1997). However, the relationship between psychological adjustment and
academic adaptation is not very clear. For example, how much do communication skills learned
in friendly interactions with host students contribute to effective formal communication with host
teachers, where patterns of affect might be quite different? Further research into how the
psychological well-being and sociocultural adaptation of international students impede or
enhance their academic success is needed.
Cognitive adaptation
The literature on cognitive aspects of acculturation in student sojourners has concentrated on
inter-group perceptions and relations. Many international students perceive prejudice and
discrimination during their interaction with host nationals (e.g. Sodowsky and Plake 1992). Some
studies have even indicated that increased contact can in some cases lead to a sharpening of nega-
tive inter-group stereotypes over time (e.g. Stroebe, Lenkert, and Jonas 1988). This is a reminder
that contact theory, which hypothesises that increased contact improves inter-group perceptions
and relations, might only work under certain circumstances.
Bond’s (1986) study of local Chinese and American exchange students in Hong Kong
revealed comparatively positive inter-group perceptions. His analysis included consideration of
auto-stereotypes (in-group perceptions), hetero-stereotypes (out-group perceptions) and reflected
stereotypes (how the out-group is perceived to view the in-group). Bond argued that the stereo-
types accurately reflected significant differences in the behavioural characteristics of the two
groups. Both Chinese and Americans perceived Chinese students as conservative and obedient,
while both also perceived American students as questioning and independent. Such stereotypes
might consistently influence interactions. Pratt (1991) commented that in America teachers are
regarded as facilitators who promote learner autonomy, while in China students see teachers as
authority figures, and are used to accepting academic assertions without questioning them. It
seems that cross-cultural stereotypes, that is, cognitive aspects of the acculturation process for
students, may have particularly important effects on the culture of learning, a concept proposed
by Cortazzi and Jin (1997). The concept includes cultural beliefs and values about teaching and
learning, and expectations about classroom behaviours. These ideas lead on to consideration of
issues specifically concerned with what is going on in intercultural educational settings, and
issues about student sojourners’ pedagogical adaptation.
simply expecting sojourning students to assimilate host nation ways, because these aspects of
culture were deep-rooted, and change could be seen as a profound threat to identity. Instead, they
proposed a process of ‘culture synergy’, asking for mutual efforts from both (host) teachers and
(sojourning) students to understand one another’s culture.
The proposed concept of culture synergy has clear advantages. First, many learning-related
problems in intercultural classrooms might result from mismatched expectations between teach-
ers and students. Second, the introduction of the concept of culture synergy suggests a mutual and
reciprocal process – teachers may learn from students by understanding the students’ cultural
traditions.
However, merely asking for mutual understanding is not enough without understanding the
processes involved. Recently, as a result of the rapid increase in the number of international
students, both students and host teachers have been becoming more aware of pedagogical
differences in one another’s culture. Further research is, therefore, needed to clarify current
teacher and student expectations in order to learn how mismatches occur, and to begin to
explore how they might be resolved. Figure 2 illustrates one way in which we suggest future
research on the relevant processes might be extended, using the example of Chinese students
studying in the UK.
In Figure 2, the terms ‘match’ and ‘mismatch’ should be understood as implying approximate,
Figure 2. Relationships between the educational expectations of UK teachers and Chinese students.
not exact, matching. The process of mutual adjustment by both teachers and students towards a
maximised academic outcome will not necessarily occur to the same degree in both directions. In
some cases, Chinese students might adapt more to the host way of teaching and learning, and in
other cases it might be the other way round. This process of adaptation might be influenced by a
number of factors, such as individual differences in both teachers and students, and situational
factors such as Chinese students coming as a group or as individuals. The focus here is on the
interaction between teacher and student perspectives and reciprocal adaptations. One implication
!
Figure 2. Relationships between the educational expectations of UK teachers and Chinese students.
Studies in Higher Education 73
of this approach is that it suggests the possibility of preparations by teachers and students to facil-
itate mutual adaptations both before and after departure.
Conclusion
The pedagogical adaptation of international students in higher education is a subset of the ‘culture
shock’ experienced by a wide range of cultural travellers. Early models of ‘culture shock’ were
often based on medical perspectives and focused on mental health issues, including both predis-
posing factors and consequences of migration. Later models were based on wider social, psycho-
logical and educational theories, and regarded the traveller as an active agent rather than the
victim of pathology. Component variables and interactive processes within ‘culture learning’,
‘stress and coping’ and ‘social identification’ aspects were identified from many research studies.
These addressed the affective, behavioural and cognitive (ABC) aspects of adaptation. Together,
they offered a more complex but more robust and predictively powerful model, and suggested
practical action that was actually deliverable on a large scale. Indeed, interventions developed
from this model were researched and found to be effective. The current article adapted the culture
synergy model to focus on the pedagogical adaptation of international students in higher
education. Our focus on the match/mismatch of pedagogical expectations has the merit of leading
not only to interesting research possibilities, but also to implications for the pre- and post-depar-
ture preparation of both teachers and students that may lead to more fruitful adaptations by each.
The many variables identified suggest pathways for helping international students and their
teachers to enhance the quality of their overall experiences. It follows that institution-wide poli-
cies for awareness-raising, guiding and supporting international students and their teachers
should be comprehensive, easily accessible and actually put into practice.
Are these issues the same for all source cultures and all host cultures? We have focused partic-
ularly on Chinese students coming to the UK. It seems possible that the experiences of students
from other cultures and/or with different destinations will differ. However, there is some evidence
(e.g. Redmond and Bunyi 1993) that Asian students sojourning in the UK and USA experience
the greatest differences in cultural expectations. For that reason they may constitute a useful
‘extreme case’ for research purposes in relation to student sojourners in general.
References
Abe, J., D.M. Talbot, and R.J. Geelhoed. 1998. Effects of a peer program on international student
adjustment. Journal of College Student Development 39: 539–47.
Adelman, M.B. 1988. Cross-cultural adjustment: A theoretical perspective on social support. International
Journal of Intercultural Relations 12: 183–204.
Argyle, M. 1969. Social interaction. London: Methuen.
Argyle, M., and A. Kendon. 1967. The experimental analysis of social performance. In Advances in
experimental social psychology, ed. L. Berkowitz, vol. 3, 55–98. New York: Academic Press.
Berry, J.W. 1994. Acculturation and psychological adaptation. In Journeys into cross-cultural psychology,
ed. A.-M. Bouvy, F.J.R. van de Vijver, P. Boski, and P. Schmitz, 129–41. Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger.
———. 1997. Immigration, acculturation and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review
46: 5–34.
Bochner, S. 1982. The social psychology of cross-cultural relations. In Cultures in contact: Studies in
cross-cultural interaction. ed. S. Bochner, 5–44. Oxford: Pergamon.
———. 1986. Coping with unfamiliar cultures: Adjustment or culture learning? Australian Journal of
Psychology 38: 347–58.
Bochner, S., B. McLeod, and A. Lin. 1977. Friendship patterns of overseas students: A functional model.
International Journal of Psychology 12: 277–97.
Bond, M.H. 1986. Mutual stereotypes and the facilitation of interaction across cultural lines. International
Journal of Intercultural Relations 10: 259–76.
74 Y. Zhou et al.
Olmeda, E.L. 1979. Acculturation: A psychometric perspective. American Psychologist 34: 1067–70.
Park, R.E. 1928. Human migration and the marginal man. American Journal of Sociology 33: 881–93.
Phinney, J.S. 1990. Ethnic identity in adolescents and adults: Review of research. Psychological Bulletin
108: 499–514.
Pratt, D.D. 1991. Conceptions of self within China and the United States: Contrasting foundations for adult
education. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 15: 285–310.
Pruitt, F.J. 1978. The adaptation of African students to American society. International Journal of Intercul-
tural Relations 2: 90–118.
Ramirez, M. 1984. Assessing and understanding biculturalism-monoculturalism in Mexican-American
adults. In Chicano psychology, ed. J.L. Martínez and R.H. Mendoza, 2nd ed., 77–94. New York:
Academic Press.
Redmond, M.V., and J.M. Bunyi. 1993. The relationship of intercultural communication competence with
stress and the handling of stress as reported by international students. International Journal of Intercul-
tural Relations 17: 235–54.
Rohrlich, B.F., and J.N. Martin. 1991. Host country and re-entry adjustment of student sojourners.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations 15: 163–82.
Rotter, J.B. 1966. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement.
Psychological Monographs 80, no. 609 (whole issue).
Searle, W., and C. Ward. 1990. The prediction of psychological and sociocultural adjustment during cross-
cultural transition. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 14: 449–64.
Sodowsky, G.R., and B.S. Plake. 1992. A study of acculturation differences among international people
and suggestions for sensitivity of within-group differences. Journal of Counselling and Development
71: 53–9.
Stroebe, W., A. Lenkert, and K. Jonas. 1988. Familiarity may breed contempt: The impact of student
exchange on national stereotypes and attitudes. In The social psychology of intergroup relations, ed.
W. Stroebe, D. Bar-Tal and M. Hewstone, 167–87. New York: Springer.
Tajfel, H. 1981. Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge Press.
Tajfel, H., and J. Turner. 1986. The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour. In The social psychol-
ogy of intergroup relations, ed. W. Austin and S. Worchel, 7–24. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Tanaka, T., J. Takai, T. Kohyama, T. Fujihara, and H. Minami. 1997. Effects of social networks in cross-
cultural adjustment. Japanese Psychological Research 39: 12–24.
Taylor, M. 2005. Catch ’em while you can. Education Guardian, February 8.
Ward, C., S. Bochner, and A. Furnham. 2001. The psychology of culture shock. 2nd ed. Hove: Routledge.
Ward, C., and A. Kennedy. 1992. Locus of control, mood disturbance and social difficulty during cross-
cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 16: 175–94.
———. 1993a. Psychological and sociocultural adjustment during cross-cultural transitions: A compari-
son of secondary students at home and abroad. International Journal of Psychology 28: 129–47.
———. 1993b. Where’s the culture in cross-cultural transition? Comparative studies of sojourner adjust-
ment. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology 24: 221–49.
———. 1993c. Acculturation and cross-cultural adaptation of British residents in Hong Kong. Journal of
Social Psychology 133: 395–97.
———. 1994. Acculturation strategies, psychological adjustment and sociocultural competence during
cross-cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 18: 329–43.
Ward, C., Y. Okura, A. Kennedy, and T. Kojima. 1998. The U-curve on trial: A longitudinal study of
psychological and sociocultural adjustment during cross-cultural transition. International Journal of
Intercultural Relations 22: 277–91.
Ward, C., and W. Searle. 1991. The impact of value discrepancies and cultural identity on psychological and
sociocultural adjustment of sojourners. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 15: 209–24.
Wells, D.C. 1907. Social Darwinism. American Journal of Sociology 12: 695–716.
Westwood, M.J., and M. Barker. 1990. Academic achievement and social adaptation among international
students: A comparison groups study of the peer-pairing program. International Journal of Intercul-
tural Relations 14: 251–63.
Zimmerman, S. 1995. Perceptions of intercultural communication competence and international student
adaptation to an American campus. Communication Education 44: 321–35.