TEL - Volume 5 - Issue 1 - Pages 45-71
TEL - Volume 5 - Issue 1 - Pages 45-71
TEL - Volume 5 - Issue 1 - Pages 45-71
45-71
Abstract
The present study attempted to investigate the impact of
sentence-level grammar instruction, explicitly and through
consciousness-raising, on Iranian EFL learners’ lexical
inferencing behavior and subsequent learning of inferred
vocabulary. It also aimed at examining the relationship
between the number of used knowledge sources and success
in lexical inferencing. To address these issues, a quasi-
experimental study with a mixed design involving two
treatment groups and one control group was conducted. The
participants were 47 adult learners at intermediate
proficiency level. A counterbalanced design was utilized to
eliminate the effect of the topic familiarity on the learners’
performance. Retrospective data were collected from eight
participants, as well. Further, the eight participants were
divided into two groups (successful and less successful) in
1
Corresponding author at: Islamic Azad University, North Branch
Email address: [email protected]
46 TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011
Consciousness-raising versus explicit grammar teaching
1. Introduction
The most preferable strategy among EFL learners is lexical
inferencing when exposed to incomprehensible input (Dycus, 1997;
Fraser, 1999; Paribakht, 2004; Paribakht & Wesche, 1999).
Nevertheless, learners’ lexical inferencing is not always successful
and sometimes goes astray (Bensoussan & Laufer, 1984; Frantzen,
2003; Huckin & Bloch, 1993; Hulstijn, 1992; Kaivanpanah & Alavi,
2008a; Nassaji, 2003). Determining a wrong meaning for an
unknown word and consequently misunderstanding the whole text is
not the only inherent drawback of lexical inferencing. The worse is
that learners “may learn the wrong meaning which they have to
unlearn subsequently” (Hulstijn, 1992, p.114). Another problem
touched upon by Hulstijn (1992) is that although the possibility of
learning the meanings of inferred vocabularies is higher than
learning those words which their meanings have been presented to
learners, they still have a slight chance of recalling the meanings of
words upon a single exposure.
With regard to these studies, the present study is an attempt to
make an empirical contribution to the assumption proposed by
Bengeleil and Paribakht (2004) that teaching learners the knowledge
TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011 47
Rashtchi and Jalili
2. Background
EFL learners have different aims for learning English such as
performing better on academic tasks, learning more about subject
matters, or improving language skills. Whatever their goal might be,
if they want to be autonomous language learners, reading is the
main means (Grabe & Stoller, 2001) and is the most important way
for transmitting academic knowledge (Huckin & Bloch, 1993).
Reading as a language skill used for obtaining knowledge is of high
importance for students. As Richards and Renandya (2002) state,
reading comprehension is the only skill that most EFL students ever
need during their lives. Furthermore, most foreign students rely on it
to compensate for the weaknesses in their speaking ability during a
course of study (Huckin & Bloch, 1993). Lee and Krashen (as cited
in Krashen, 2003) contend that the more you read, the less you have
writing apprehension. One reason for such significance can be that
it provides learners with various models of language and introduces
new cultural issues to them. Further, through reading, learners are
exposed to vocabulary, grammar, idioms, proverbs, and culture that
are the essential components of language. Therefore, it is an
advantageous way for bringing EFL learners into contact with
language and consequently increasing the chance of learning new
materials. Moreover, learners can do reading activities on their own
at any time and wherever they are. However, when learners are
48 TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011
Consciousness-raising versus explicit grammar teaching
3. Method
3.1 Participants
A total number of 47 university undergraduates (13 males and 34
females) between the ages of 19 and 24 in three intact classes were
selected based on convenience sampling and randomly assigned to a
TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011 53
Rashtchi and Jalili
3.2 Instrumentation
To check the homogeneity of the participants, Interchange/Passages
Placement Test was used. Since the focus of this study was on
reading, the reading subsection of the ESOL Preliminary English
Test (PET) was also run. Two texts namely ‘Big City Dreams’ and
‘Genetic Engineering’ developed at the same difficulty level by
Paribakht (2005) were used as the pre and posttests to measure
lexical inferencing success. Similar to Nassaji’s (2004) study, in
each text 10 unknown words were used to focus on EFL learners’
inferencing behavior. The target words were all content words
consisting of nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs. To make sure
that the words were unfamiliar to the participants, a list of the words
was given to the participants before the pre and posttests and they
were asked to write down their meanings (either in Persian or
English). Subsequently, the known words were omitted for those
participants.
Moreover, Vocabulary Knowledge Scale (VKS) developed by
Paribakht and Wesche (1996, 1997) was used to monitor the effect
of grammar instruction, both explicitly and through consciousness-
raising, on the retention of inferred vocabularies by EFL learners
and were given ample time to complete the task. This instrument,
utilized by Wesche and Paribakht (1996), elicits self-perceived and
demonstrated knowledge of the target words in written form from
learners with five self-report and performance items. The scale
ratings range from complete unfamiliarity, through recognition of
the word and recall of its meaning, to the ability to use the word
productively in a sentence with grammatical accuracy and semantic
appropriateness. According to Paribakht and Wesche (1996), the
strength of VKS lies in the fact that, unlike most tests of vocabulary
54 TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011
Consciousness-raising versus explicit grammar teaching
3.3 Procedure
As mentioned earlier, Interchange/Passages Placement Test was
administered to three intact classes to examine whether they
belonged to the same population. Since the construct of interest in
TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011 55
Rashtchi and Jalili
this study was reading, the reading subsection of the PET was also
administered to ensure that the participants were homogeneous in
their reading ability. Furthermore, to eliminate the effect of
teachers’ experience, teachers were chosen from novice English
teachers. To this end, Tschannen-Moran and Hoy’s (2007) cut-off
point for distinguishing between novice and experienced teachers
(i.e. three years of teaching experience) was followed. Thus, the
teachers’ experience ranged from nine months to two years.
Besides, all the three teachers held M.A. in English-related majors.
The control group was taught by one of the researchers.
The three intact classes were randomly assigned to a control
group and two experimental groups. In sessions one to three all of
the participants in the three groups took the placement test, were
trained on lexical inferencing, and took the pretest respectively. The
instruction on lexical inferencing strategy focused on developing
students’ awareness of the use and viability of lexical inferencing.
They were taught how to use inferencing as a strategy in their
reading when encountering unfamiliar words using Clarke and
Nation’s (1980) model:
nine words, six words and the participant with the knowledge of the
three words were excluded from the study. Eventually, this stage
was carried out with total number of eight participants and 19
words.
Accordingly, all the answer-sheets gathered from 47 participants
were scored by one of the researchers. To measure the degree of
success in lexical inferencing in the three passages (i.e. the pretest,
posttest, and think-aloud task), a three-point scale (0 to 2)
representing unsuccessful, partially successful, and successful
inferences respectively was used. Successful inferences were
defined as those that were semantically, syntactically, and
contextually appropriate. They could be synonyms or definitions.
Answers that were semantically appropriate but syntactically
deviant, or vice versa, or were approximate were classified as
partially successful. Responses were evaluated as unsuccessful
when they did not meet any of these conditions or no meaning was
offered at all. In this way, the maximum score possible for each
participant was 20, the results of which were compared after adding
them up.
To score VKS items, Paribakht and Wesche’s (1997) guidelines
for scoring VKS were followed. The minimum and maximum
scores possible for any participant were 20 and 100. It is noteworthy
that more or less all students inferred the meaning of some target
words approximately or even wrongly but they remembered exactly
those inferred meanings in the vocabulary test. In scoring VKS, all
inferred meanings which students could recall successfully were
considered correct even if they were approximate or incorrect
meanings since what counted was the retention of inferred meanings
no matter whether they were right or wrong. The logic behind is that
of Fraser’s (1999) framework, who asserted that vocabulary
learning from inferred vocabulary happened to the extent that
learners could recall those meanings which they assigned to
unknown words in reading before. However, in scoring students’
answer-sheets in the pretest, posttests, and VKS those target words
which participants had indicated as known were deleted for those
participants.
TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011 59
Rashtchi and Jalili
4. Results
The data obtained from Interchange/Passages placement test,
reading component of the PET, and the pretest to three separate one-
way ANOVAs confirmed the homogeneity of the three groups with
respect to their general proficiency, reading ability, and lexical
inferencing prior to the instruction. Table 1 displays the mean
scores and standard deviations for the three groups’ performance on
the pretest, posttests, and the VKS.
significant the Scheffe test was performed. The results revealed that
the mean scores for the ETG and CRG were significantly different
from the CG. The ETG and CRG did not differ significantly from
each other. The main effect for text type [F (2, 88) = 14.22, p =
0.0005] was also significant. The obtained η2 value was 0.139 which
can be said to be a large value. According to post-hoc comparisons
of the three groups the CG was significantly different from both
ETG and CRG although ETG and CRG did not differ significantly
from each other.
SS df MS F Sig.
Between
15.61 2 7.80 2.5 .09
Groups
Pretest
Within Groups 137.48 44 3.12
Total 153.1 46
SS df MS F Sig.
Between
409.63 2 204.814 5.63 .007
Groups
VKS
Within Groups 1601.95 44 36.408
Total 2011.58 46
used more knowledge sources than the less successful group (139
vs. 93).
5. Discussion
The findings of the present study revealed that teaching sentence-
level grammar both explicitly and through consciousness-raising
result in learners’ significant success in applying lexical
inferencing. These findings lend support to Paribakht (2004) and
Kaivanpanah and Alavi’s (2008b) findings that grammatical
knowledge contributes to lexical inferencing success. Paribakht
(2004) in her study discussed the possible effect that grammatical
64 TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011
Consciousness-raising versus explicit grammar teaching
6. Conclusion
The present study supports the findings of the previous studies that
sentence-level grammatical knowledge contributes to lexical
TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011 67
Rashtchi and Jalili
References
Alavi, S. M., & Kaivanpanah, S. (2009). Examining the role of
individual differences in lexical inferencing. Journal of Applied
Sciences, 9(15), 1-6.
Bengeleil, N. F., & Paribakht, T.S. (2004). L2 reading proficiency
and lexical inferencing by university EFL learners. The
Canadian Modern Language Review, 61(2), 225-249.
Bensoussan, M., & Laufer, B. (1984). Lexical guessing in context
in EFL reading comprehension. Journal of Research in
Reading, 7(1), 15-32.
Bonk, W. (2000). Second language lexical knowledge and listening
comprehension. International Journal of Listening, 14, 14-31.
Clarke, D. F., & Nation, I. S. P. (1980). Guessing the meaning of
unknown words from context: Strategy and techniques. System,
8(3), 211-220.
Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A
framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning &
Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684.
Craik, F. & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the
retention of words in episodic memory [Electronic version].
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268-294.
De Bot, K., Paribbakht, T. S., & Wesche, M. B. (1997). Toward a
lexical processing model for the study of second language
vocabulary acquisition: Evidence from ESL reading. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition, 19, 309-329.
68 TELL, Vol. 5, No. 1, 2011
Consciousness-raising versus explicit grammar teaching